Reaction time in cognitive tasks in relation to age Kotsavasiloglou C., Nousi Α., Baloyannis S. Abstract Processing speed is an age depended property of the brain functionality. There is a decline of the processing speed with the advance of the age in healthy subjects. Many diseases affect the reaction time of patients in various cognitive tasks as well. The aim of this study was the quantitative assessment of the reaction time in various stimuli in healthy people in relation with the age. In order to investigate this quantitative aspect of specific brain areas, a set of cognitive tasks was created, with the use of a software tool. Seventy four subjects were divided in three groups based on the age limits of 40 and 60 years old. The findings showed statistically significant differences of the reaction time in almost all of the under investigation parameters between the groups. The use of this tool which measures the reaction time in cognitive tasks may be very useful in the evaluation of the progression of the decline of the cognitive performances in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. KeyWords : ageing, reaction time, cognitive functions Introduction The daily human cognitive behavior is a very complicated procedure. The human brain accomplishes its high level cognitive faculties integrating elementary informations. The results of these information processes are elaborated according to internal algorithms. In normal conditions when a person communicates with another a lot of elementary neuronal connections are activated in milliseconds in various contiguous and not contiguous areas of the brain. For the investigation of a complex cognitive task a reasonable approach is to find the elementary cognitive tasks which compose it. For every elementary cognitive task, the investigation must be oriented both to the qualitative and the quantitative aspects of this task. During a conversation between two persons the person who hears must translate the changes in the physical sound to phonemes, the phonemes to words and the words to meanings. The same is valid in the case of a person which reads a book. The person must translate the optical properties of the content of a page in words and then must translate these words to meanings. In the first case sounds are the “medium” that carries the information and the translation is held in the brain area specialized for the sound’s perception in the temporal lobe. In the second case, light is the “medium” that carries the information and the translation is held in another brain area specialized in the elaboration of the images which is located in the occipital lobe. The localization of the brain areas specialized to the elaboration of a specific type of information and the pattern of its activation, are examples of qualitative aspects of investigation. A quantitative aspect of an elementary cognitive function is the time a brain area takes, to accomplish a specific function. 1 Both aspects are useful in combination. The pattern of activation of a brain area or the time that area takes to complete an elementary task, are parameters which allow us to investigate the limits between the normal and the pathologic state. An indirect index of the processing speed of a specific brain area is the reaction time to various stimuli wich activates this area. The human brain as a complex dynamic system which interacts continuously with the environment receives flows of information. All these information are elaborated and responses to the environment are generated. The responses to the environment are based on the purposes of the person and are modulated by emotional factors. An external stimulus which is considered by a person as a threat generates a very intense and rapid response. Other parameters which influence reaction times are personality traits. Personality traits modulates response times allocating attentional resources (Hainaut, 2005). On the other hand in depressed individual the reaction time is longer than in normal persons. Depressed individuals were slow to name the emotionality of positive information and displayed greater sustained processing in the negative aspects of the information (Siegle, ). Compared to normal controls, the AD and depressive patients exhibited a signifcantly impaired performance in manual motor coordination discrimination reaction time and visual patternmatching tasks. This suggests that these patients are handicapped by deficient basic operational functions and central information processing depression in the elderly (HOFMAN) In normal conditions there is a statistical range of Reaction times for every elementary cognitive task. This range of Reaction times (RT) is related to the age. It is well known from empirical observations that the RT of a person to external stimuli is proportional to the age. Data from investigations using evoked potentials suggests a sensorimotor slowing with aging and with task complexity (Yordanova, 2004). In more complicated procedures where semantic and gender priming was investigated, longer reaction times were registered during the tasks performed by the elderly group (Manenti, 2004) The age is not the only factor which affects processing speed. Many diseases of the CNS have more or less significant impact on the reaction times depending on their state. In patients with mild cognitive disorders the reaction times are longer than old aged persons when performing the same tasks (Ritchie, 2001). Brain pathology which affects performances in processing speed, involves cortical and subcortical brain areas. The smallvessel disease contributes to cognitive decline by affecting processing speed and executive function (Prins, 2005). In other studies that investigates White matter hyperintesities (WMHs) and cognitive functions an increase in processing time for various tests like the Stroop test is recorded (Soderlund). WMLs are related to impairment of cognitive functions, in particular those that involve a speed component (de Groot,). 2 Processing speed is the most substantial area of cognitive impairment in subjects suffering from CADASIL (Cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencefalopathy) (Peters 2005). The RT evaluation is an important index of all the levels of the brain functionality. The knowledge of a quantitative relation between the age and the RT is valuable in defining the limits of the normal and the pathological response in relation to the age. There is no one specific RT for the brain as a hole. Each brain function has its own RT index which is related with the age. Various methods and procedures are used by different research groups for the investigation of the processing speed in various cognitive and not cognitive tasks. The last 20 years researchers have been dealing with various aspects of the brain functionality using computers. The computers promoted the research not just as number elaborating machines but as tools for creating sophisticated models and interfaces. In this paper we use a software tool developed in our clinic for the evaluation of the cognitive functions. One set of tests of this software tool is oriented to the measurement of the reaction time in various stimuli. The aim of this study was the quantitative assessment of the RT in various stimuli in relation to the age in healthy people and people without any pathological condition involving directly the brain. Methods Subjects Seventy five subjects participated in this study. All these persons were either healthy or had diseases that not affect directly the brain functions. The subjects with a disease were five. Four of them had well controlled Hypertension under treatment and one had Diabetes type II under treatment. One of the subjects (72 years old) with hypertension was operated 9 years ago after an ischemic heart attack (triple bypass). During the tests the systolic and diastolic blood pressure and the glycemia levels were normal. None of the subjects suffered from any form of depression. All subjects of the third group had a detailed neurological examination with normal findings. The subjects were divided in three groups based on age. Group A had 41 persons with age between 20 and 39 years old. Group B had 23 subjects with age between 40 and 59 years old. Group C there were 9 persons over 60 years old. The persons of the third group had a lower educational level. The possible implications of this parameter in the analysis of the data are discussed in the Conclusion section of this paper. Table 1 shows more analytical data about the groups. 3 Table 1. Groups Group A Group B Group C Age Number Mean age 20 – 39 40 – 59 60 - 80 41 23 9 31,65 47,25 69,15 Standard deviation 4,42 5,2 6,14 All subjects agreed to participate in this study after a detailed explanation of the procedure and the scope. The five subjects with a specific pathology, but in a very good mental status, where included because in the elderly people some pathology is the rule and not the exception. Of course, many of the diseases of the elderly, affect indirectly the brain and this is a factor to keep in mind when discussing the results. Procedure For the evaluation of the RT we used a software tool developed in our clinic. This software tool comprises a specific battery of tests for the measurement of the time of reaction in specific stimuli. This battery consists of 5 tests. In all tests (except the first) the subjects respond to the apparition of a specific visual stimulus on the screen of a computer by hitting a button. The subject can use its right or its left hand. In this paper the data are from the use of the “dominant” hand. In all tests (except for the first preliminary test) there is an elementary sequence of an “Apparition of a stimulus” and a “Response of the subject”. This elementary sequence lasts a mean time of 3 seconds. A mean time of 3 seconds separates two elementary sequences. Every test comprises 20 elementary sequences. The time counter starts with the apparition of the stimulus and stops when the subject hits the button. In the first preliminary test the subject pushes the “ENTER” button of a computer keyboard rapidly many times. The computer stores the time in milliseconds between two hits. This test serves as a baseline reference. In the second test the subject hits the same button every time a red full colored circle (0.7 mm of diameter) appears in random positions on the screen in random time intervals. The random positions of the object on the screen in all tests were chosen for keeping the attention of the subject. In the third test there are two circles, a red and a green one and the subject must hit the button only when the green circle appears on the screen. These two circles alternate randomly on the screen. The circle appears 20 times with the green one appearing no less than 7. In the fourth test the subject sees on the screen two words alternating each other randomly one at a time. The subject must hit the button only if one of the two words appears on the screen. These two words are phonetically similar and consist of three syllables. They have in common the first and the third syllable and they differ in the second syllable. The two words appear 20 times with the target word appearing no less than 7. 4 The last test is more complex. In the screen appears randomly in random positions one of 10 words at a time. Five of these words mean “PAST” and five means “FUTURE”. The subject must hit the button when in the screen appears a word meaning “PAST”. The word appears 20 times. Words meaning “PAST” appear no less than 8 times. For every person for every test and for every single response the software stored each reaction time separately. The times were measured in milliseconds. They were short for the first test and increased gradually seeking the maximum measured time in the last test. In the third and the forth test the exclusion criteria was the 10% of wrong choices. The measured parameters for every test are: 0. Basic hit time (BHT): It was used to measure the time of the physical movement of the finger hit on the button and the eye fixation movements on the screen. 1. Reaction Time for one color (RT1C): This parameter was calculated in a 2 phase process. For every subject the mean value of the BHT was subtracted from the mean value of the measures of the second test. The mean value for every group was calculated from these resulting values. 2. Reaction Time for the recognition of one color between two colors (RT2C): This parameter was calculated in a 2 phase process. For every subject the mean value of the BHT was subtracted from the mean value of the measures of the third test. The mean value for every group was calculated from these resulting values. 3. Reaction Time for the recognition of a word among two similar words (RTW): This parameter was calculated in a 2 phase process. For every subject the mean value of the BHT was subtracted from the mean value of the measures of the forth test. The mean value for every group was calculated from these resulting values. 4. Reaction Time for the perception of the meaning of a word (RTM): This parameter was calculated in a 2 phase process. For every subject the mean value of the BHT was subtracted from the mean value of the measures of the fifth test. The mean value for every group was calculated from these resulting values. The data from the 3 groups, collected by the computer, were compared according to the following three combinations. 1) Group A versus group B. 2) Group B versus group C. 3) Group A versus group C. These comparisons between the three age periods for the above mentioned parameters offer a good evaluation of the decline of the performances in relation to the age. For the statistical evaluation the Independent Samples t-Test were used. For the statistically significant difference the Levene’s test for Equality of variances was considered. The statistically significant difference was evaluated at 0.05 and 0.01. 5 Results Group analysis Table 2. Reaction Time for one color (RT1C) Groups A B C Subjects Mean Std. (msec) Deviation 41 128.21 75.43 24 195.16 91.45 9 238.33 114.25 Std. Error Mean 11.78 18.66 38.08 Comparisons Group A – Group B Group A – Group C Group B – Group C P < 0.05 P < 0.05 No diff. In the test “Reaction time for one color” the subject must find the target shape that appears in random positions on the screen. This test revealed statistically significant difference between the first and second group and between the first and the third group. The comparison among the second and the third group revealed no difference. The results for this test may suggest that there is a significant decline around the age of 40 years old that is followed by a normal decline. Table 3. Reaction Time for the recognition of one color among two colors (RT2C) Groups A B C Subjects Mean Std. Std. Error (msec) Deviation Mean 41 207.65 89.28 13.94 23 263.13 92.76 19.34 9 344.55 145.82 48.60 Comparisons Group A – Group B Group A – Group C Group B – Group C P < 0.05 P < 0.05 No diff. In the next test (Reaction Time for the recognition of a color among two colors) the subject must find the target shape on the screen and then press the button if the shape is green. The results from this test are similar to the results of the previous test. This means that the reactions times for the discrimination of the two colors follow the same rules as the test for one color. There is a greater reaction time in the test for the recognition of one color among two randomly appearing colors. This greater reaction time is due to the additional brain neuronal processes for the selection of the green color. Table 4. Reaction Time for the recognition of a word among two similar words (RTW): Groups TEST 3 A B C Comparisons Subjects Mean (msec) 41 384.21 24 572.66 8 908.50 Std. Deviation 107.13 154.68 247.99 6 Std. Error Mean 16.73 31.57 87.68 Group A – Group B Group A – Group C Group B – Group C P < 0.01 P < 0.01 P < 0.05 As in the previous test the subject must perform two sequential sub processes. The first is to find the target word that appears randomly on the screen and the second is the identification of the target word. The first sub procedure involves the same brain structures for the fixation of a target as in the previous tests. The second sub process involves the brain areas for the word recognition. In this test the absolute reaction times are greater than the previous two tests. This suggests that the neural substrate for the word elaboration is more complex from the neural substrate for the recognition of the colors. Therefore it demands more time. Another observation in this test is that there is a relation among the complexity of the neuronal connections and the level of the statically significant differences. In this test the differences are clearer (p<0.01). There are two cut off points. The first is around 40 year’s old age and the second is around 60 years old age. Table 5. Reaction Time for the perception of the meaning of a word (RTM) Groups TEST 4 A B C Subjects Mean 40 23 7 Std. Std. Error Deviation Mean 521.87 192.30 30.40 715.08 220.16 45.90 936.28 173.95 65.74 Comparisons Group A – Group B Group A – Group C Group B – Group C P < 0.01 P < 0.01 P < 0.05 The last test is more complex. The subject must perform three sequential sub processes. The first is to fixate the eyes on the word that appears randomly on the screen. The second is the identification of the word and the third is the assignment of the meaning in that word. The area responsible for the identification of the word is strictly connected to another adjacent area in the posterior portion of the temporal lobe that gives a meaning to that word. The activation of this area demand more time and therefore the reaction times registered are longer. The performance differences between the three groups are statistically significant. In order to clarify the influence of the hypertension and diabetes on the performances of the third group we performed a statistical analysis between the persons of the Group C dividing them in two subgroups. The first subgroup comprised the persons with a known pathology and the second the healthy remains. There was no statistically significant difference between the two subgroups. The number of the subjects was small and therefore this issue must be further evaluated. 7 Correlation analysis For the evaluation of a probable statistical correlation among the age and the performances for every test for all subjects, the Pearson correlation coefficient was calculated. The results are shown in the next table. Table 6. Correlation of the age and the performances for all subjects. Test 1 Number of subjects Pearson Correlation P < 001 75 0.401 Test 2 Number of subjects Pearson Correlation P < 001 73 0.389 Test 3 Number of subjects Pearson Correlation P < 001 74 0.704 Test 4 Number of subjects Pearson Correlation P < 001 71 0.497 These findings suggest a direct relation between the age and the performances of the subjects. They also corroborate the results of the comparisons among the groups. The groups represents periods of age. The statistically significant differences between the groups are differences of performances among periods of age. Conclusions In this paper we investigate the quantitative aspects of 4 specific cognitive tasks in relation to the age. These tasks investigate mainly optical areas of the brain. The last task investigates the time of perception of the meaning of a word. Therefore investigates the functionality of the brain area that assigns to “optical” words their meaning. There is a question to be discussed. The educational level of the third group is lower than the educational levels of the other two groups. May this difference in the educational level have an influence on the reaction times of the subjects of the third group, when they perform the last two tasks? To respond to this question we must consider in what way the education of a person may influence their performances. The education may affect the performances mainly in two ways. 8 The first is the frequency of the use of words of the test and the second is the frequency of the use of this specific brain area by the person. In the third task the person had to react hitting the “Enter” button in the apparition of a specific word. There were only two words alternating randomly. These words were of common use. In the forth task the subjects response were oriented to the classification of a series of words based on their meaning. The target concept was “PAST” and the stimuli was common words as “Yesterday”, “Tomorrow”, “After”, in Greek. We think that the educational level has little influence on the performances of this task because of the use of simple common words. In papers were the education was evaluated in relation to the progression of cognitive deficits the results suggested more a greater ‘cognitive reserve capacity’ in highly educated subjects than a true protective effect of education against Alzheimer’s disease (Amieva 2005). Nevertheless the educational level is a parameter to be farther evaluated. Another aspect to be discussed is the presence of hypertension and diabetes in five persons of the third group. None of these subjects had any clinical evidence involving the central nervous system. This is a conclusion based on a detailed clinical examination and an accurate anamnesis. The statistical analysis performed in the two subgroups (Subjects with no disease versus subjects with hypertension and diabetes) of the group C revealed no statistically significant difference. The sample size is considered small and therefore this result is not very reliable. Apart from this result the question remains. To what extend hypertension and diabetes affects the performances in the tests? Various studies have shown an association between elevated blood pressure and white matter lesions (De Leeuw, 1999). White matter lesions are correlated with lower performances in various cognitive tests in subjects with Cerebral small vessel disease and multiple sclerosis. Therefore some degree of negative influence exists. These persons were included in the third group because the presence of these two diseases is common in the elderly. The first aim of this work was the quantification of the response times to specific stimuli in order to have an objective evaluation. This was achieved by setting up a procedure with the use of a software tool. The second aim was the definition of the age limits with abrupt decline of the performances. The age limits of 40 and 60 years old, were chosen after evaluation of the results of various combinations of comparisons of age limits. We think that these age limits needs farther evaluation with more subjects. The findings suggest that for the specific tasks the reaction times are increasing with the age. There are statistically significant differences between the groups in almost all the tasks. 9 In the first tests the reaction time to one colored shape (red), appearing randomly on the screen on casual positions is evaluated. Of course this test does not evaluate the time of recognition of the red color because the subject reacts to the apparition of an object that happens to be red. In the same test when other colors were used, there where no statistically significant difference. This means that the subject’s reaction time is not depended by the color of the projected object. In the second test two colored (red, green) shapes alternates randomly on the screen. The subject must hit the button when the green one appears. This is a test for the evaluation of the green color recognition time. The subject must first recognize the green color and then hit the button. The reaction times registered was greater from those of the previous test. This time difference is expected because the selection of the right color demands the involvement of a specific brain area in the occipital lobe for the color recognition. The third test involves again the occipital lobe. Between two randomly appearing words on the screen the subject must select one. The times we registered in this third test were greater than those of the previous one. An indirect conclusion is that the brain area for the recognition of the words is more complicated in some way. It is logical to suppose that the number of neurons and the number of synapses for every elementary volume unit are statistically similar in the area of color recognition and the brain area of the reading. Therefore the greater response time in this test may suggest a longer signal path which corresponds to an extended elaborative brain area involving the occipitoparietal area of the cortex. This seems quite expected. In the recognition of a color the brain has to identify a simple information pattern, a specific wave length, which is repeated in all the elementary points that compose the shape. In the case of the words in the reading area of the brain, the work of the neural network is the identification of a specific sequence of letters. But every letter has its own identity and its own specific characteristics. The elementary units that compose every word are different and therefore more neurons and synapses are required. In the last test the subject must hit the button when the word that appears randomly on the screen has a specific meaning. The reaction times registered in this test were longer than those of the previous test. This is an expected finding as well. This task involves the same reading area as in the previous test but the signal is propagated to a neiberhood area between occipital and temporal lobe. The function of this brain area is the extraction of the meaning of the words. The signal path is longer and therefore the reaction time is longer. The utility of the quantification of the various cognitive elementary tasks is quite obvious. Every elementary task is accomplished by a specific brain area. The identification of statistically constant reaction times for every elementary task, allows us to investigate the dysfunction states 10 of the brain. In the last years the incidence of people with impairment of their cognitive functions is increasing in time. It is well known that the brain can mask its small areas of dysfunction for long periods of time. It is known that Degenerative diseases like Alzheimer disease are characterized by low performances in various cognitive tasks many years before their onset (Amieva 2005). For a more accurate classification of preclinical states of Degenerative diseases like Alzheimer disease the term Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) has been proposed (Flicker 1991, Petersen 1999). The identification of a preclinical situation like MCI is very important because gives a possibility of intervention in delaying the progression of the disease. In the present study the diagnostic method we used was sensible in detecting the differences in the processing speed in specific cognitive tasks in subjects with age from 20 to 80 years old. This means that may detect decline in performances before all criteria for MCI is fulfilled. The tests we present here are a first approach. They need further elaboration and larger groups of subjects for an objective set up of reaction times. 11 References The 9 year cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: a prospective population-based study. Helene Amieva,1 Helene Jacqmin-Gadda,2 Jean-Marc Orgogozo,1,3 Nicolas Le Carret,1 Catherine Helmer,1 Luc Letenneur,1 Pascale Barberger-Gateau,1 Colette Fabrigoule1 and Jean-Francois Dartigues Brain (2005), 128, 1093–1101 Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E Arch Neurol. 1999 Mar;56(3):303-8. Mild cognitive impairment in the elderly: predictors of dementia. Flicker C, Ferris SH, Reisberg B. Neurology. 1991 Jul;41(7):1006-9. A Follow-Up Study of Blood Pressure and Cerebral White Matter Lesions Frank-Erik de Leeuw, MD,*† Jan Cees de Groot, MD,* Matthijs Oudkerk, MD,‡ Jacqueline C. M. Witteman, PhD,* Albert Hofman, MD,* Jan van Gijn, FRCPE,† and Monique M. B. Breteler, MD* Annals of Neurology Vol 46 No 6 December 1999 ALZHEIMER'S DISEASE, DEPRESSION AND NORMAL AGEING: MERIT OF SIMPLE PSYCHOMOTOR AND VISUOSPATIAL TASKS MARC HOFMAN, ERICH SEIFRITZ, KURT KRAÈ UCHI, CHRISTOPH HOCK, HARALD HAMPEL, ALEXANDRA NEUGEBAUER AND FRANZ MUÈ LLER-SPAHN Int. J. Geriat. Psychiatry 15, 31±39 (2000) Cerebral small-vessel disease and decline in information processing speed, executive function and memory Niels D. Prins, Ewoud J. van Dijk, Tom den Heijer, Sarah E. Vermeer, Jellemer Jolles, Peter J. Koudstaal, Albert Hofman and Monique M. B. Breteler Brain (2005), 128, 2034–2041 Cerebral changes on MRI and cognitive function: The CASCADE study Hedvig Soderlund a,b,., Lars-Goran Nilsson b, Klaus Berger c, Monique M. Breteler d, Carole Dufouil e, Rebecca Fuhrer f,1, Simona Giampaoli g, Albert Hofmand, Andrzej Pajak h, Maria de Ridder d, Susana Sans i, Reinhold Schmidt j, Lenore J. Launer k Neurobiology of Aging 27 (2006) 16–23 Cerebral White Matter Lesions and Cognitive Function: The Rotterdam Scan Study Jan Cees de Groot, MD,* Frank-Erik de Leeuw, MD,*† Matthijs Oudkerk, MD,‡ Jan van Gijn, FRCPE,† Albert Hofman, MD,* Jellemer Jolles, PhD,§ and Monique M. B. Breteler, MD* Annals of Neurology Vol 47 No 2 February 2000 12 Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment. A population-based validation study Karen Ritchie, PhD; Sylvaine Artero, MA; and Jacques Touchon, MD NEUROLOGY 2001;56:37–42 The effects of ageing and Alzheimer’s disease on semantic and gender priming Rosa Manenti, Claudia Repetto, Simone Bentrovato, Alessandra Marcone, Elizabeth Bates, and Stefano F. Cappa1 Brain (2004), 127, 2299–2306 Moderate state-anxiety differently modulates visual and auditory response times in normal- and very low trait-anxiety subjects J.-P. Hainaut, B. Bolmont . Neurosci Lett. 2005 Nov 17 Periventricular Cerebral White Matter Lesions Predict Rate of Cognitive Decline Jan Cees de Groot, MD, Frank-Erik de Leeuw, MD, Matthijs Oudkerk, MD, Jan van Gijn, FRCPE, Albert Hofman, MD, Jellemer Jolles, PhD, and Monique M. B. Breteler, MD1 Ann Neurol 2002;52:335–341 Sensorimotor slowing with ageing is mediated by a functional dysregulation of motorgeneration processes: evidence from high-resolution event-related potentials Juliana Yordanova, Vasil Kolev, Joachim Hohnsbein and Michael Falkenstein Brain (2004), 127, 351±362 The Pattern of Cognitive Performance in CADASIL: A Monogenic Condition Leading to Subcortical Ischemic Vascular Dementia Nils Peters, M.D., Christian Opherk, M.D., Adrian Danek, M.D., Clive Ballard, M.D., Jürgen Herzog, M.D., Martin Dichgans, M.D. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:2078–2085) 13