NOTICE: this is the author’s version of a work that was accepted for publication in The Concept of Self in Psychology, Nova Publishing. Changes resulting from the publishing process, such as peer review, editing, corrections, structural formatting, and other quality control mechanisms may not be reflected in this document. Changes may have been made to this work since it was submitted for publication. A definitive version was subsequently published in Travis, F.T. (2006) From I To I: Concepts of Self on an Object-Referral/ Self-Referral Continuum, In A. P. Prescott, Ed. The Concept of Self in Psychology, Nova Publishing FROM I TO I: CONCEPTS OF SELF ON A OBJECT-REFERRAL/ SELF-REFERRAL CONTINUUM Fred Travis Center for Brain, Consciousness and Cognition Maharishi University of Management 1000 North Fourth Street, FM 1001 Fairfield, IA 52557 641 472 7000 x 3309 (phone) 641 472 1123 (fax) Email: ftravis@mum.edu Key words: Self, Transcendental Meditation, self-referral, object-referral, levels of mind, cognition Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum ABSTRACT Concepts of self in the 100-year history of psychology can be placed along an objectreferral/self-referral continuum. For instance, William James’s self-as-known, or “me,” is more object-referral and his self-as-knower, or “I,” is more self-referral. Current neural imaging studies have investigated 3rd person-perspectives of self (object-referral) and 1st person-perspectives of self (self-referral). The object-referral/self-referral continuum is based on analysis of individuals practicing the Transcendental Meditation (TM) technique. This meditation technique from the Vedic tradition of India takes the attention from surface perception and thinking, to more abstract levels of thought, to a level of pure self-awareness at the source of thought. Our research has yielded subjective and objective markers of the experience of pure self-awareness or content-free self-awareness. Subjectively, pure self-awareness is characterized by the absence of time, space and body sense. Time, space and body-sense are the framework for waking experiences. During the experience of pure self-awareness, this framework that gives meaning to waking consciousness is absent. Objectively, pure self-awareness is characterized by spontaneous periods, 10-40 sec long, of slow inhalation along with heightened brain wave coherence. These data support the Vedic descriptions of pure selfawareness as “the fourth”—a state of consciousness in addition to waking, dreaming and sleeping. Our research has also yielded subjective and objective characteristics of the permanent integration of pure self-awareness with customary experiences during waking, dreaming, and sleeping. This state has been traditionally called enlightenment. A content analysis of inner experiences of these subjects and two comparison groups has yielded descriptions of 2 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum the ends of this object-referral/self-referral continuum. On the object-referral end, individuals described themselves in terms of cognitive and behavioral processes. They exhibited lower values of the consciousness factor, lower frontal EEG coherence during tasks, lower alpha and higher gamma power during tasks, and less efficient cortical preparatory. In contrast, individuals reporting the experience of enlightenment described themselves as an abstract continuum underlying thought, feeling and action— self-referral descriptions. The experience of self-awareness has been written with a capital “S”—Self— to differentiate from more active experiences. These subjects exhibited higher values of the consciousness factor, higher frontal coherence, higher alpha and lower gamma power during tasks, and heightened cortical response to tasks. This object-referral/self-referral continuum describes the progressive de-embedding of the Self from the machinery of perception, thought, and behavior. In the coming century, psychology should take advantage of meditation technologies— technologies of consciousness—to explore more expanded experiences of Self to understand who we are and what it means to be fully human. 3 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum INTRODUCTION The concept of self has been a core concept throughout the 100-year history of psychology. For instance, Wilhelm Wundt, the father of experimental psychology, defined the self as a unitary concept: “…the feeling of the interconnection of all single psychical experiences.” ((Wundt, 1897) pg. 221). In contrast, William James, the father of American Psychology, delineated a range of possible concepts of self. These ranged from the self as knower, the “I,” to the self as known, the “me” (James, 1962). The self-asknown includes our material processions, social status, thoughts and feelings (James, 1962). Current neural imaging studies have investigated a similar continuum from 1st person to 3rd person perspectives of sense-of-self. Visualizing ones’ physical traits and bodily gestures, a 3rd person perspective, activated medial parietal cortices including the precuneus and angular gyrus (Kjaer et al., 2001; Taylor, 2001). Stories containing 1st person pronouns activated medial frontal areas including the anterior cingulate (Vogeley et al., 1999). Similarly, medial frontal activation was reported during 1st person self-referential judgments of pictures (Gusnard et al., 2001) and of trait adjectives (Kelley et al., 2002). Neural imaging data suggest that a distributed frontal-parietal system supports our experience of self-awareness, with 1st person experiences activating predominately frontal midline structures and 3rd person experiences activating predominately parietal midline structures. Our proposed object-referral/self-referral continuum of sense-of-self adds a level of self that is independent of thinking and acting. This object-referral/self-referral continuum (Travis et al., 2002) was based on interview data and EEG data of individuals practicing the 4 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Transcendental Meditation (TM®) technique1. This meditation technique is from the Vedic tradition of India. It takes one’s attention from surface perception and thinking to a level of pure consciousness at the source of thought (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1963). Pure consciousness is the experience of consciousness itself—self-awareness without customary thoughts, feelings or perceptions (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1969; Travis et al., 2000). Pure self-awareness is the state in which the self is only aware of itself. This contrasts with object-referral awareness, in which the self is identified with the processes of knowing— feelings, decisions, planning, thinking, comparing, perception and action. This state of pure self-awareness is often written with a capital “S” to distinguish this more universal, unbounded state of Self from the experience of being absorbed in thinking, planning and activity. SECTION 1: WHAT IS THE NATURE OF THE EXPERIENCE OF PURE SELF-AWARENESS? We’ll begin with the Oxford English Dictionary definition of awareness. The first five definitions in the dictionary define awareness with specific content: (1) interpersonal cognitive relations, (2) remembering on a first-hand basis ones past actions or experiences, (3) awareness of any object; (4) immediate awareness of ones mental processes, and (5) the totality of mental experiences that constitute our conscious being. These definitions characterize self-awareness is terms of the content of experience. The last definition is qualitatively different: “…awareness that is distinct from the content that makes up the stream of consciousness.” 1Transcendental Meditation is registered in the U.S. Patent and Trademark Office, licensed to Maharishi Vedic Education Development Corporation, and used under sublicense. 5 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum A number of authors have defined states that could fall into this last definition of awareness. O’Shaughnessy suggested that awareness itself can be "...distinct from particular consciousnesses or awarenesses” ((O'Shaughnessy, 1986), p. 49). He proposed that pure self-awareness is like an “empty canvas” that can not be viewed representationally, but makes possible and is physically necessary to view a painted picture. Woodruff-Smith discussed “the inner awareness that makes an experience conscious...a constituent and constitutive feature of the experience itself” ((WoodruffSmith, 1986) p. 150). Baar’s theater metaphor includes an attention director or deep self that provides a context (framework) to connect one conscious event with another (Baars, 1997). The prevailing Western view is that an individual cannot be aware or conscious without being conscious of something (Natsoulas, 1997); that no subjective state can be its own object of experience ((James, 1962) pg. 190). However, the subjective traditions of the East—the Vedic tradition of India (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1969), and the Buddhist traditions of China (Chung-Yuan, 1969) and Japan (Reps, 1955)—include systematic meditation techniques predicted to lead to the state of "pure self-awareness" without customary content of awareness. For instance, the Maitri Upanishad 6:19 (Upanishads, 1953) states: When a wise man has withdrawn his mind from all things without, and when his spirit of life has peacefully left inner sensations, let him rest in peace, free from the movements of will and desire…Let the spirit of life surrender itself into what is called turya, the fourth condition of consciousness. For it has been said: There is something beyond our mind, which abides in silence within our mind. It is the supreme mystery beyond thought. Let one’s mind . . . rest upon that and not rest on anything else. Maharishi Mahesh Yogi (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1969), responsible for bringing the TM technique to the West from the Vedic tradition of India, explains: 6 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum When consciousness is flowing out into the field of thoughts and activity it identifies itself with many things, and this is how experience takes place. Consciousness coming back onto itself gains an integrated state….This is pure consciousness. (pg. 25) Pure consciousness is “pure” in the sense that it is free from the customary processes and contents of knowing. It is a state of awareness in that the knower does not black out. There is no gap in time. Being or pure self-awareness is maintained. The content of this experience is self-awareness. The Self serves both as the experiencer and the object of experience. In contrast, during customary waking experience, the experiencer and the object of experience are different. During customary waking experience, there is an inner reality who is experiencing outer objects or inner thoughts and feelings. The Buddhist tradition does not speak of an expanded state of Self-awareness. They characterize deep experiences during their meditation practice to be “Nothingness” or “Emptiness” (Shear et al., 1999). In contrast, the Vedic tradition characterizes pure consciousness as the expansion of the individual self to its most universal status (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1963; Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1969). Thus, to investigate the nature of an expanded sense of Self, we studied individuals who practiced the TM technique, which is based in the Vedic tradition. TM subjects are also well suited to this study because individuals quickly master transcending during TM practice—after a few months practice (Travis et al., 1999; Travis et al., 2002; Travis et al., in review) in comparison to years of practice required with other meditation techniques before becoming expert (Compton et al., 1983; Lutz et al., 2004). Thus, most TM subjects would be having similar experiences reducing variability in the data. Focusing research subjects practicing a single meditation technique, controls for different meditation experiences, and allows well-defined research into the nature of self-awareness. This paper first summarizes previously published research on subjective and objective markers of the experience of pure self-awareness in TM subjects. It then reviews subjective and objective markers of the permanent integration of pure self-awareness with 7 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum waking, dreaming and sleeping. Following this review of the literature, the proposed object-referral/self-referral continuum of self-awareness is discussed in detail. SECTION II: PURE SELF-AWARENESS DURING MEDITATION PRACTICE Physiological Correlates Changes in EEG, breath rate, skin conductance, and heart rate characterize the state of pure self-awareness during TM practice. Refined breathing was the first published marker of this experience. Farrow and Hebert (Farrow et al., 1982) and later Badawi and colleagues (Badawi et al., 1984) observed suspension of normal respiration from 10 to 40 seconds during pure self-awareness experiences. This type of breathing, while initially termed “respiratory suspension,” is actually an example of slow, prolonged inspiration (Kesterson et al., 1989). This type of breathing is supported by different respiratory drive centers in the brain stem then those that drive breathing during waking {Travis, 1997 #27; Plum, 1980 #115}. A second reliable marker of this state are skin conductance responses at the onset of breath changes (Travis et al., 1997). This autonomic response is similar to that seen during orienting—attention switching to environmental stimuli that are novel (Sokolov, 1963; O'Gorman, 1979) or significant (Maltzman, 1977; Spinks et al., 1985). These autonomic responses could mark the transition of awareness from active thinking processes to pure self-awareness devoid of customary content. A third marker, which is less obvious but has been reported in most studies, is a trend for increasing frequency of the peak power in the EEG. Compared to the period prior to the experience of pure self-awareness, the frequency of peak power of the EEG increases from 0.5 to 1.5 Hz. Fluctuations in frequency of peak power follow fluctuations in alertness. For instance, during sleep 1 Hz EEG is seen, while during very focused tasks 40 Hz EEG 8 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum activity is reported. The observed increase in frequency of peak power during respiration suspensions could be associated with increased alertness during this experience. These changes in EEG power occur on the background of high global EEG coherence. Coherence represents level of connectedness between different brain areas (Florian et al., 1998; Pfurtscheller, 2001). Higher EEG coherence is associated with higher emotional stability, higher moral reasoning, more inner motivation, and lower anxiety (Travis et al., 2004). Alpha EEG coherence rises to a high level in the first minute of TM practice and continues at that high level through the rest of the session (Travis et al., 1999). Recent research also reports higher frontal/parietal phase synchrony during TM practice (Hebert et al., in press). This unique constellation of physiological patterns is unlike any seen in normal waking, sleeping, or dreaming. Phenomenological Analysis Fifty-two University students, twenty-six males and twenty-six females, were asked: "Please describe the fine details of your deepest experiences during practice of the Transcendental Meditation technique. Please describe them in your own words, just as if you were describing the experience of eating a strawberry — its sweetness, juiciness, etc." (Travis et al., 2004). We emphasized that we were interested in what they experienced; in how it felt to them, and that we were not interested in other people’s descriptions of these experience. We did not ask for descriptions of pure self-awareness. That would have elicited biased responses reflecting more what they knew about pure consciousness, rather than their direct experience of this state. They were allowed to write as much and for as long as they wished. These subjects were an average age of 22.5 yrs (SD 6.9). They had been practicing the TM technique for an average of 5.4 yrs (SD 5.0). 9 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Data analyses The descriptions were analyzed using the guidelines for systematizing phenomenological analysis proposed by Hycner (Hycner, 1985). This procedure begins with reading the passage (all 52 descriptions) many times to get "a sense of the whole." Next, Hycner recommends reading through and bracketing out "units of meaning"—words or phrases which express a unique and coherent idea. Once the units of meaning had been identified, then, as Hycner recommends, units that were clearly redundant were eliminated. Next, the units were clustered by "shared meanings." This was done by explicit and implicit meaning. From these clusters of shared meaning, general themes were identified. The final step was to re-read the descriptions and tally the occurrence of the themes. This yielded the number of subjects who included that theme in their descriptions, expressed as a percentage of total subjects. Results Three major themes emerged from this analysis. Sixty-eight percent of the subjects explicitly characterized deep experiences during practice of the Transcendental Meditation technique by the absence of space, time, or body-sense. The other two themes—peaceful (32%) and unboundedness (20%)—implicitly include the lack of boundaries of space, time and body-sense, but further describe the experience when these boundaries were absent. For instance, one subject described the experience of pure self-awareness as: ...a couple of times per week I experience deep, unbounded silence, during which I am completely aware and awake, but no thoughts are present. There is no awareness of where I am, or the passage of time. I feel completely whole and at peace. The content analysis of deep experiences during practice of the TM technique suggest that pure self-awareness, consciousness itself, is qualitatively different than other waking experiences. Time, space, and body sense are the framework for understanding waking experience. Experiences either occur inside or outside of the body. Experiences contain 10 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum changing perceptual content that change over time and occur in different points of space. During the experience of pure self-awareness, both the fundamental framework—time, space and body sense—and the content of waking experience were absent. This suggests that pure self-awareness is not an "altered" state of waking. It is not a distorted waking experience. Rather, pure self-awareness is marked by the absence of the customary qualities and characteristics of waking experience. Pure self-awareness is Self-awareness, the Self, isolated from the processes and objects of experience. Pure self-awareness is subjectively and objectively distinct from experiences during customary waking, sleeping, and dreaming. In the Vedic tradition, pure self-awareness is called “the fourth” (Upanishads the Principle Upanishads, 1953). It is described as a state of consciousness that underlies waking, dreaming and sleeping. An Integrated Model of States of Consciousness: The Junction Point Model A junction point model has been proposed, an “ocean metaphor,” which conceptualizes pure self-awareness as a fundamental state (an ocean), which sometimes appears as (a wave of) waking consciousness, sometimes as dreaming consciousness, and sometimes as deep sleep consciousness (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1972; Travis, 1994). In this model, pure self-awareness underlies the activity of waking, dreaming and sleeping, and can be objectively identified and subjectively experienced at the junction point between each state, i.e., where waking has ceased and sleep has not yet begun. According to this model, the activities of waking, sleeping, and dreaming are like currents that hide the silent nature of pure self-awareness—the surface waves covering up the silent depths of the ocean of consciousness. EEG research supports this model. Similar EEG patterns—theta/alpha activity (6-10 Hz)—are seen during Transcendental Meditation practice, and during the waking/sleeping junction, the sleeping/dreaming junction, and the dreaming/waking junction (Travis, 1994). 11 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum One prediction of this model is that pure self-awareness is not only is an underlying continuum, but that it can be integrated with waking, dreaming and sleeping. The next section tests this prediction. It presents subjective and objective characteristics of the integration of pure self-awareness with waking, dreaming and sleeping. SECTION III: INTEGRATION OF PURE SELF-AWARENESS WITH WAKING, DREAMING, AND SLEEPING Repeated experience of the state of pure self-awareness alternated with customary waking activity gives rise to a new integrated brain state in which pure self-awareness coexists with waking, dreaming and sleeping (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1969; Travis et al., 2002). In this new integrated state, pure awareness is experienced as a foundational state that gives rise to ongoing experience (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1969). It is analogous to the vastness of the ocean that is not lost with each rising wave of daily life. Research has identified physiological and phenomenological markers of this integrated state during sleep and during problem solving. Physiological Markers Brainwave (EEG) and muscle tone (EMG) patterns during deep sleep Mason and colleagues compared brain wave (EEG)S patterns and muscle tone (EMG) during deep sleep—stage 3 and 4 sleep—in three groups of 11 subjects: non-meditating subjects, meditating subjects not reporting this integrated experience, and meditating subjects who reported the permanent integration of pure self-awareness with waking and sleeping (Mason et al., 1997). Self-awareness during sleep is the critical test of the integration of pure self-awareness with waking, dreaming, and sleeping, because it spans 12 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum the widest possible continuum of consciousness—from wide-awake, inner awareness to the inertia of deep sleep. The data were visually scored for sleep stages. Physiological patterns were analyzed during Stages 3 and 4 of sleep, because these are the deepest stages of sleep, and so represent a more stringent test of the experience of inner wakefulness during sleep. The amount of delta activity in these three groups, which indexes depth of sleep (Feinberg, 1999), was similar. All subjects appeared to have similar requirements for the restorative value of sleep, which recent research suggests is for cellular repair following normal day-to-day wear and tear on the brain (Huber et al., 2004). These groups did, however, differ in the level of theta/alpha activity—6-10 Hz— during sleep. During deep sleep, theta/alpha activity, characteristic of the experience of pure self-awareness during Transcendental Meditation practice, increased with years TM practice. Theta/alpha activity was lowest in the non-meditating subjects, medium in the short-term meditating subjects, and highest in the subjects reporting pure self-awareness during sleep. It is noteworthy that the subjective experience of the co-existence of pure self-awareness and sleep was associated with the coexistence of the EEG patterns seen during the experience of pure self-awareness and during sleep. Muscle tone (EMG) levels decreased during slow wave sleep with years of TM practice. They were highest in the non-TM group and lowest in the long-term TM group. Reduced EMG during slow wave sleep has not been reported before. Future research is needed to probe the significance of this observed physiological marker during sleep in these subjects EEG patterns during computer tasks 13 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Travis and colleagues (Travis et al., 2002) investigated EEG and ERP patterns in three groups of individuals distinguished by their self-reported experience of the integration of pure self-awareness with waking and sleeping. The three groups included: a Non-TM group (N=17, age = 39.7±11.5 yrs),2 who did not practice a meditation technique and rarely if ever reported the experience of pure self-referral consciousness; a Short-term TM group (N= 17; age = 42.5±11.5 yrs), who had practiced TM for about eight years (7.8±3.0 yrs), and reported pure self-awareness during TM but only occasionally during daily life; and a Long-term TM group (N=17; age = 46.5±7.0 yrs), who had practiced TM for about 25 years (24.5 ±1.2 yrs) and reported the continuous experience of pure self-awareness throughout daily life. (Four of the subjects in the last group also participated in the earlier sleep research.) The age differences between groups were not statistically significant, F(2,48) = 1.90, p = .160) Each group comprised eight females and nine males. The subjects were part of the larger Fairfield, Iowa community. Subjects were in good health with no history of serious accidents, hospitalization, or psychiatric diseases that would affect their EEG. They were free of prescription or nonprescription drugs. All subjects were right-handed by self-report. Informed consent was obtained before the testing, and the University Institutional Review Board approved the experimental protocol. Table 1 represents these subjects’ demographics. As seen in this table, subjects in each group spanned levels of education (high school to graduate training) and vocation. The “Professions” category included architects, engineers, lawyers, researchers, computer professionals, pharmacists, and university administrators and faculty. These were 2 Data are reported as mean ± standard deviation. 14 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum combined into the “Professionals” category to simplify this table. These people were part of the community. They had not been sequestered in an ashram. Table 1. Education Levels and Vocation of the Subjects Group Non-TM Education 2 High School 9 Some College 6 Graduate/Professional Short-term TM 9 Some College 8 Graduate/Professional Long-term TM 4 Some College 13 Graduate/Professional Vocation 2 Artisans 2 Military 4 Business & Management 7 Professionals 2 Student 7 Business & Management 9 Professionals 1 Student 2 Artisans 4 Business & Management 11 Professionals EEG patterns were recorded from all subjects during two paired-stimulus reaction time tasks. Both tasks contained a pair of stimuli 1.5 seconds apart. We measured the rise in the EEG baseline before the second stimuli. This is called the contingent negative variation (CNV) (Walter, 1964). CNV amplitude 200 msec before the expected stimulus, called the late CNV, reflects proactive preparatory processes, including mobilization of motor (Brunia, 1993; van Boxtel et al., 1994) perceptual, cognitive, and attention resources (Tecce et al., 1993). The first task was a simple CNV task—asterisk/ tone/ button-press to stop the tone. The second task was a choice CNV task—two numbers were sequentially presented 1.5 seconds apart. Subjects responded with a left/right button press to indicate which number was larger. Three brainwave measures calculated during the choice CNV trials distinguished individuals who reported the integration of pure self-awareness with waking and sleeping. These measures were (1) higher broadband frontal EEG coherence, (2) higher alpha and 15 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum lower gamma power, and (3) a better match of the timing and magnitude of CNV with task demands. Broadband (8-45 Hz) frontal coherence during tasks. Broadband frontal task EEG coherence was highest in the Long-term group (Travis et al., 2002). The Non-TM group had lowest task coherence, and the Short-Term group exhibited intermediate values of coherence. The frontal cortices, which are reciprocally connected with nearly all other cortical, subcortical, and brainstem structures (Fuster, 1993; Fuster, 2000), are important circuits for emotion regulation (Davidson, 2002), moral reasoning (Moll et al., 2001; Moll et al., 2002), decision making and planning (Fuster, 1993; Fuster, 2000), and self-concept (Vogeley et al., 1999; Ben Shalom, 2000). Broadband coherence, in contrast to narrow band coherence such as theta or alpha, may reflect large-scale cortical integration thought necessary for the unity of subjective experience (Varela et al., 2001). Broadband frontal coherence observed in subjects reporting this integrate state may characterize the largescale neural integration necessary to support the coexistence of pure self-awareness with ongoing problem solving. Increased alpha and decreased gamma power. The pattern of EEG power spectra during tasks was also different between groups (Travis et al., 2002). The Long-term T group exhibited higher alpha (8-10 Hz) EEG, which is associated with long-range, top-down processes, and lower gamma (25-55 Hz) EEG, which is associated with local, bottom-up, sensory processing (von Stein et al., 2000). Higher alpha and lower gamma EEG in Longterm TM subjects suggests that they process information differently: Inner, self-awareness may play a greater role in cognitive processing. 16 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Different CNV patterns. The CNV patterns in the three groups support the proposition that the Long-term subjects processed tasks differently. CNV in the Long-term group better suited the task demands. In the Long-term group, late CNV was higher during simple trials and lower during choice trials. Thus, in the simple trials, Long-term TM subjects activated cortical response circuits after the first stimulus because they what the correct response would be—a button press. On the other hand, in the choice trials, Long-term TM subjects remained balanced and did not activate cortical response circuits after the first stimulus because they did not have enough information to determine a correct response. In contrast, the Non-TM subjects exhibited the opposite response. They responded less during the easier simple tasks, and more during the complex choice tasks. These responses, however, were not appropriate. For instance, they activated response circuits before seeing the second number in the choice task. They did not have enough information to make a correct response. This is a more object-referral mode, reflecting overall task characteristics. Frontal and central cortical areas participate in generating the CNV waveform (Tecce et al., 1993). Appropriate timing of CNV activation suggests more appropriate timing of frontal executive processes in the Long-term TM subjects. This CNV finding complements the finding of higher levels of frontal EEG coherence during tasks in these subjects. Thus, frontal areas, whose functioning are critical for generating levels of self-awareness (Vogeley et al., 1999; Hobson et al., 2002) appear to function differently in these three groups. 17 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum The inner experience of these subjects also markedly differed. The last section reports results from an open-ended interview and paper-and-pencil tests of these three groups of subjects. Phenomenological Analysis of Interview Data The three groups of subjects tested during the computer tasks were given an open ended interview (Travis et al., 2004). Subjects were asked three questions. (1) “Please describe experiences during the reaction time tasks;” (2) “Please describe experiences during sleep;” and (3) “Please describe yourself.” Questions were always in that order. The first question allowed the interview to gain rapport with the subjects. The second and third questions were analyzed. During the interview, the interviewer asked the person to explain any comment that did not seem obvious. For instance, when one person said: “I think I'm not as happy as I used to be.” The interviewer asked: “You used to be ... does that mean two years ago or 10 years ago?” Or the interviewer asked: “You talk about connectedness, could you expand on that concept?” The interview ended when the person had no further comments to make. These taped interviews were transcribed for later analysis. Response to the Query: Please Describe Your Experiences during Sleep During sleep, the non-TM group described typical sleep. For instance, one subject said: Most times sleep is just that. I go to bed around 9:30 or 10:00 and put my head on the pillow. It doesn't take me very long to fall asleep. I'm not aware of anything till 5 or 6 o'clock in the morning when it's time to get up. Notice there is a gap in time between eyes-closed at night, and waking up in the morning. This is a good night’s sleep. The Long-Term TM subjects described an entirely different experience during sleep. They described a continuum of self-awareness that was independent of time, space and 18 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum body sense, which was the ground for all experiences. One person described their experience of falling sleep in terms of layers settling down: When I go to bed at night, first layers of the body settle down. I notice when the body is asleep. Lots of dreams come and go or just fatigue leaves the body, and then 5-6 hours later the body wakes up again in gradual layers and being aware of the other side…hearing sounds and feeling the covers. First there’s no impulse to attend to it, then I wake up. Another person used the analogy of soda fizzing up and settling down to describe his experience during sleep: …there’s a continuum there. It's not like I go away and come back. It's a subtle thing. It's not like I'm awake waiting for the body to wake-up or whatever… It's me there. I don't feel like I'm lost in the experience. That's what I mean by a continuum… you know its like the fizzing on top of a soda when you've poured it. It's there and becomes active so there's something to identify with. When I'm sleeping, it's like the fizzing goes down. Response to the Query: Describe Your Self These responses were analyzed using Atlas-ti software (Muhr, 2004). Atlas-ti is an interactive software program designed for content analysis of interview data. In this software, a section of transcribed text is opened in an Atlas-ti window. The experimenter manually highlights phrases or sentences that contain a single idea. For instance, “I believe anything is possible.” With a mouse click, the highlighted section is added to Atlas-ti’s lists of “quotations” for that group. After going through the entire text, the experimenter begins with the first quotation and generates a single word or phrase that encapsulates the unit of meaning in that quotation. In Atlas-ti, these are called “codes.” The codes are connected to the quotations in Atlas-ti so that double-clicking any code brings up all quotations connected with it. These codes were not generated before hand, but were generated from the data itself. This “grounded theory” approach (Sommer, 1991) was used to discover inner experience of the integration of pure self-awareness with waking, sleeping 19 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum and dreaming, rather than test apriori hypotheses. Next the codes are structured into hierarchical networks within subjects to create a picture of their inner world of meaning. For instance, in the Non-TM group the networks centered on the descriptions (codes) of the self as a (1) belief system, (2) cognitive style, (3) feelings, and (4) social roles. These networks were then assigned a supercode. In the example of the Non-TM group, the supercode was: “Self is identified with thoughts, feelings and actions.” The responses to the question: “Describe yourself.” are summarized in Table 2. This table reports average total words in each interview, average number of quotations selected from each interview, total codes for each group and the resulting supercode. The total codes are reported because the same code often summarized quotations from different subjects in each group. Table 2: Means and standard deviations of average number of words, quotations, total codes and the supercode for each group from the content analysis. Group Average Word Count Average Quotations Total Codes Non-TM 554±459 9.5 ± 5.0 57 Short-Term 850±760 9.3 ± 3.2 56 Long-term 1156 ±1180 10.1 ± 6.2 59 Supercodes Self is identified with thoughts, feelings, and actions Self is the director of thoughts, feelings and actions Self is underlying and independent of thoughts, feelings and actions The interviews varied in number of words. However, this difference was not statistically significant (F(2,48) = 2.07, p = .108) due to high variability within groups. The average number of quotations and total codes were very similar between groups. 20 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum The super codes for the three groups are presented in Table 3, along with sample quotations. This gives the reader a sense of the responses of subjects in each group that generated the final super-codes. Table 3. Results of Content Analysis: Super Codes and sample responses from the three groups. Group and Sample Responses/Quotations Super Code N1: I guess I'm open to new experiences, and I tend to appreciate those things Non-TM Group: that are different Self is N2: I kind of like to forge my own way identified with N3: I am open to change and new ideas…I’m an adventuress. I like to go thoughts, out...and experiment with new ideas. feelings, and N4: I tend to appreciate those things that are different, even in my style of actions dress. I like something usually because its odd or strange or something that other people absolutely wouldn't wear. N5: I’m happy, caring, helpful, I like people who like to help other people; I hate seeing anyone in trouble. S1: I’m my own awareness. My ability to perceive and be aware. I’m my own Short-Term Group: potential, my own power, Self is the S2: I’m my own capabilities; my ability to learn; my ability to do things…in director of it’s essential nature — my ability to act. thoughts, S3: There are many different levels to who I am. I'm a sister, a daughter, a feelings, and friend, an athlete, a nature lover, a seeker of the truth. I'm a very spiritual actions person. I believe that I can do and accomplish anything that I set my mind to. S4: I am a little bit more silent, more reserved, and thoughtful than most, with a deep desire to just succeed in all activities and at the same time to develop spiritually very quickly. S5: Who I am is who I am inside. How I think. What I believe. How I feel. How I react. L1: We ordinarily think my self as this age; this color of hair; these hobbies … Long-term Group: my experience is that my Self is a lot larger than that. It’s immeasurably vast... Self is on a physical level. It is not just restricted to this physical environment. independent of L2: It's the “I am-ness.” It's my Being. There's just a channel underneath that's and underlying just underlying everything. It's my essence there and it just doesn't stop where I thoughts, stop…by “I,” I mean this 5 ft. 2 person that moves around here and there. feelings, and L3: I look out and see this beautiful divine Intelligence ...you could say in the actions sky, in the tree, but really being expressed through these things… and these are my Self. L3: I experience myself as being without edges or content…beyond the universe…all-pervading, and being absolutely thrilled, absolutely delighted with every motion that my body makes. With everything that my eyes see, my ears hear, my nose smells. There's a delight in the sense that I am able to penetrate that. My consciousness, my intelligence pervades everything I see, feel and 21 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum think. L5: When I say "I" that's the Self. There's a quality that is so pervasive about the Self that I'm quite sure that the “I” is the same “I” as everyone else's “I.” Not in terms of what follows right after. I am tall, I am short, I am fat, I am this, I am that. But the “I” part. The “I am” part is the same “I am” for you and me. The super-codes derived from the content analysis delineated three quite different descriptions of self-awareness in healthy adults. As seen in Table 3, the Non-TM group, who rarely if ever reported the experience of pure self-awareness, described themselves predominantly in terms of their thoughts, feelings and behavior: “I guess I’m open to new experiences…” or “I tend to appreciate those things that are different…” or “I kind of like to forge my own way.” This group described their sense-of-self as predominately identified with their thoughts, feelings, and actions. Individuals in the Short-term group, with infrequent experiences of pure self-awareness during daily life, described themselves predominately as that which directed thoughts, feelings and actions. While their sense-ofself was less object-referral, it was still in terms of active processing. “I’m my awareness. My ability to perceive and be aware.” or “I’m my own capabilities; my ability to learn.” Individuals in the Long-term group, who reported the continuous experience of pure self-awareness co-existing with waking and sleeping activity, described themselves as underlying and independent of thoughts, feelings and actions. This group recognized space-time boundaries. For instance, “And in certain contexts that has some value, like when I tell my wife, I'm going to bed now.” However, they predominately described themselves as existing outside of space-time causation—“my Self is immeasurably vast…on a physical level;” “all-pervading;” “beyond speech;” or “My Self doesn’t stop where I stop.” 22 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum The subjects described three very different inner states. Subjects in the Non-TM Group described themselves in terms of their thoughts, feelings, and actions. In contrast, subjects in the Long-term Group described themselves in terms of an independent, underlying reality—a continuum that was outside time and space. To further explore the nature of the inner experience of these subjects, they were administered standardized paper-and-pencil instruments of personality and psychological measures. Standardized Psychological Tests After the above research, the subjects were re-contacted and were mailed four pencil-andpaper instruments measuring personality, inner/outer orientation, moral reasoning, and anxiety. The mailing was followed up by phone calls to confirm receipt and participation. Individuals returned their tests by mail in an enclosed self-addressed stamped envelope. Personality. The International Personality Item Pool (IPIP) was used to measure personality. The IPIP is the result of an international effort to develop and continually refine a set of personality inventories, whose items are in the public domain, and whose scales can be used for both scientific and commercial purposes. The IPIP items are freely available on the Internet (http://ipip.ori.org/ipip/). This website also provides sub-scales and their correlations with proprietary instruments such as the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory or the California Personality Inventory. We used 100 items in the IPIP that index the five personality constructs in the “Big Five” model of personality: extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and openness to experience (Goldberg 1992). Extraversion represents the tendency to be social, assertive and active, including the two dimensions of dominance and sociability; Agreeableness is the tendency to be trusting, caring and gentle; 23 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Conscientiousness includes achievement and dependability; Emotional Stability includes good emotional adjustment, high self-esteem, low anxiety, and high security and easiness with others; and Openness to experience is the disposition to be imaginative, nonconforming and unconventional (Judge et al., 2002; Judge et al., 2002). Consensus is emerging that this five-factor model of personality may describe the most salient aspects of personality (Goldberg, 1990). Inner/Outer Orientation. Baruss developed this scale to quantify a subject’s worldview along an outer/inner, material/transcendental dimension (Baruss et al., 1992). Subjects are given 38 statements like: “My spiritual beliefs determine my approach to life.” Subjects respond on a 7-point Likert Scale. This instrument has high item-total correlations (r = .56.62) and high Cronbach alpha coefficients (r = .82-.95) (Baruss et al., 1992). Scores on this scale correlate highly with positive inner growth and meaningfulness of life (Baruss et al., 1992). This scale yields a single number, which ranges from –114 (materialistic: conceptualizing consciousness in terms of information processing) to +114 (transcendental: emphasize subjective features of consciousness and declare it’s ontological primacy). Moral Reasoning. Gibbs Socio-Moral Reflection Measure- Short Form (SMR-SF) presents moral statements and asks subjects to describe why a moral act may be important to them. For instance: “Keeping promises is important because...”; or “Helping one’s friend is important because....” Gibbs has written an extensive reference manual to aid in categorizing responses into moral maturity levels (Gibbs et al., 1992). The SMR-SF can be group administered as a pencil-and-paper test, takes 15-20 minutes to complete, and can be scored in 25 minutes. In addition, a scorer can gain competency in 25-30 hours of self-study. Gibbs’s SMR-SF has high test-retest reliability (r = .88), and 24 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum high Cronbach alpha coefficients (r = .92). Scores on the SMR-SF are highly correlated with scores on Kohlberg’s Moral Judgment Interview (r = .70) (Gibbs et al., 1992), which is much more intensive to administer and to score. Levels of moral reasoning range from surface considerations to an inner autonomous basis for decision-making. More abstract levels of moral reasoning emerge developmentally and parallel growth in cognitive development and ego development (Gibbs et al., 1992). Anxiety levels. Spielberger’s State/Trait Anxiety (STAI) assesses both transitory feelings of anxiety (state anxiety) and chronic feelings of anxiety (trait anxiety). In epidemiological studies, 17% of adolescents (Wittchen et al., 1998) and 25% of adults (Jacobi et al., 2004; Wittchen et al., 2005) report the occurrence of high anxiety in the past 12 months. The tests of inner/outer orientation, state/trait anxiety, and personality were scored using standard methods. Gibbs’s moral reasoning protocols were sent to trained scorers. The scorers met the requirements for reliability in scoring, set forth in Appendix B and C in Gibbs’s manual (Gibbs et al., 1992). Means, standard deviation, F statistics from MANOVA and p-values of group differences are presented in table 4. P-values smaller than .002 are bolded. As seen in this table, there were highly significant difference between groups in inner/outer orientation, moral reasoning, anxiety, and emotional stability. Table 4. Means (standard deviations), F statistics and p-values for the psychological tests 25 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Test Non-TM Short-Term Long-term F stat (2,26) p value 9.03 .001 Inner/Outer Orientation 60.2(23.8) 70.0(12.4) 84.4(13.9) Moral Reasoning 3.1(0.4) 3.4(0.4) 3.7(0.2) 5.69 .009 State Anxiety 35.9(15.2) 27.1(9.1) 22.3(2.4) 7.66 .002 Trait Anxiety 40.2(15.5) 30.6(7.6) 24.6(4.0) 7.90 .002 IPIP: Extraversion 3.1(0.6) 3.4(0.5) 4.1(0.8) 4.48 .021 IPIP: Agreeableness 4.0(0.5) 4.2(0.4) 4.6(0.4) 3.98 .031 IPIP: Conscientiousness 3.6(0.7) 3.9(0.8) 4.2(0.4) 2.28 ns IPIP: Emotional Stability 3.3(1.0) 3.8(0.8) 4.4(0.4) 10.64 .0004 IPIP: Openness to Experience 4.0(0.4) 4.5(0.4) 4.7(0.4) 3.64 .040 A Pearson Correlation analysis showed that the psychological measures were highly intercorrelated (all r > .5) A principle component analysis (PCA) was conducted to model the variance in the test scores,. The first unrotated component of the PCA has long been used in intelligence research as a measure of general intelligence or “g”—a construct theorized to underlie performance across a range of reasoning and problem solving tests (Spearman, 1904; Jensen, 1980). In the current study, the first principal component of the unrotated PCA of psychological and personality tests may represent a general measure of sense-of-self, a basic quality of self-consciousness or life-orientation, that is reflected in these tests. Table 5 contains the factor loadings for the first and second unrotated components, which accounted for 69% of the variance in test scores. The other components had eigenvalues less than 1. 26 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Table 5: Variable Loadings of the 1st and 2nd Unrotated Components of the PCA Inner/Outer Orientation Moral Reasoning State Anxiety Trait Anxiety Emotional Stability 1st Unrotated Component .71 .64 -.86 -.87 .88 2nd Unrotated Component -.48 -.30 .19 .12 -.11 Variance Accounted for 58% 12% Variable All variables from the psychological tests were converted to z-scores, weighted by their factor loadings on the 1st unrotated component, and summed. This sum was called a Consciousness Factor, because it was theorized to reflect a basic quality of consciousness or life-orientation common to measures of personality and psychological health. An ANOVA was used to test for main effects for group on the Consciousness Factor scores. This analysis yielded significant main effects for group, F(2,26) = 13.2, p < .0001: NonTM: -4.78 ± 1.2; Short-Term: 0.40 ± 1.2; Long-term: 3.59 ± 1.1. Individual comparisons revealed that Consciousness Factor scores for the Short-Term subjects were significantly higher (two-tailed) than those of the Non-TM subjects (t(28) = 3.03, p = .006), and there was a trend for Long-term subjects to be higher than those for the Short-Term subjects (t(28) = 1.96, p = .062). The Consciousness Factor, which represents a common dimension across several tests of personality and psychological health, accounted for over half the variance among groups. As “g” represents a common dimension among all intelligence tests (Spearman, 1904), so the Consciousness Factor may represent a common dimension among measures of personality and psychological health. In light of the phenomenological data above, 27 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum higher scores on the Consciousness Factor may represent greater ‘de-embedding’ of senseof-self from mental processes and behavior. GENERAL DISCUSSION The research reported here presents distinct subjective and objective characteristics of (1) the experience of pure self-awareness during meditation practice and (2) the permanent integration of pure self-awareness with waking, dreaming, and sleeping, a state called enlightenment. This research includes investigation of meditation experience and objective correlates of meditation effects on brain functioning after the practice. As advances in instrumentation have enabled science to probe deeper into matter, so applying the techniques developed by Eastern cultures—meditation practices—may enable science to probe deeper into the individual psyche. The first section discussed the possibility of a state of self-awareness free from customary content of inner thoughts, feelings and outer perceptions. The second section reports subjective descriptions and objective correlates of the experience of pure self-awareness during TM practice. These findings suggest that underlying our turbulent, ever-changing thoughts is a state that is not confined to our head, our heart or to the interior defined by our body. Rather, pure self-awareness is described as being outside of time, space, and body sense. It is a continuum of awareness that underlies all change. It is the deepest aspect of our selves—called the Self. This section ended with a junction point model that places pure self-awareness in relation to waking, sleeping and dreaming. The Object-referral/self-referral continuum: From I to I The range of concepts of self presented in the phenomenological analysis depict the progressive de-embedding or detachment of self-awareness from its machinery to know the 28 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum world—the mind, senses and the body. This process of de-embedding from a more expressed level to a more abstract level of self-awareness is the normal path of development from childhood to adulthood. For instance, Piaget’s stages of cognitive development can be understood as the progressive de-embedding of the center of one’s sense-of-self from sensory, motor, and cognitive processes (Alexander et al., 1990). Thus, one could have a behavioral-centered self in which the person identifies with sensorimotor behavior: “I like to forge my own way;” or “I like to go out and experiment with new ideas.” As one de-embeds from behavior, one could have a more cognitive-centered self in which the person identifies with mental objects and ongoing thoughts and feelings: “I’m open to new experiences.” In turn, one could become more affect-centered, in which one identifies more with feelings and interrelations with others and the environment: “I care deeply for other people;” or “I’m happy, caring, helpful. I like to help other people. This progressive de-embedding of self-awareness from mental contents and processes is a natural process that is shaped by ongoing experience (Alexander et al., 1990; Travis et al., 2001). The process of “de-embedding” is clearly seen in the interview results from these subjects. Subjects in the Non-TM group described themselves predominantly in terms of how they interacted with the world. That is, the self was “embedded” in or identified with the processes by which they experienced the world. This could be characterized as an object-referral style. One is what one does. Subjects in the Short-term group described themselves as directing thinking and behavior—the first stages of the self “de-embedding” or separating from the processes of thinking and behavior. “I’m my own capabilities; my ability to learn.” And another: “I am my ability to perceive and be aware.” Yet, these 29 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum subjects still described themselves primarily in terms of what they did. In contrast, subjects in the Long-term group described themselves as separate from what they were thinking or doing—their identities, their selves were completely “de-embedded” from the processes of thinking and behavior. “My Self is immeasurably vast... on a physical level. Not just restricted to this physical environment.” And another: “It's my Being. There's just a channel underneath that's just underlying everything. It's my essence there and it just doesn't stop where I stop.” This style of functioning is a Self-referral style. In this state, the Self has it’s own status. It is defined in terms of its own structure, independent of objects and processes of knowing. Self-awareness ranges from “I” to “I.” It ranges from the Self embedded and identified with the processes of knowing to the Self aware of itself—from self to Self. While its expression changes, it is the same fundamental value—pure self-awareness—expressing itself differently depending on different styles of brain functioning. As experience changes, so brain functioning changes. We begin to naturally appreciate who we are, and appreciate our relation with the world around us. Figure 6 presents the proposed object-referral/self-referral continuum. The boxes at the top and bottom of Fig. 6 present the ends of the proposed continuum. The three boxes on the left side contain the supercodes that defined the three groups. The two boxes on the right operationally define the ends of the continuum as defined by their Consciousness Factor scores and the brain-based measures. As one de-embeds from cognitive and behavioral process or moves towards the Self-referral end of this continuum, an integrated set of mind/body measures appear: scores on the Consciousness factor increase, frontal EEG 6-40 Hz coherence increases during tasks, alpha power increases and gamma power 30 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum decreases during tasks, and there is a better match between task demands and brain preparatory responses as measured by contingent negative variation (CNV). ___________________ Insert Fig 6 about here These subjective and objective findings may characterize the process of self “deembedding” from thinking, feeling and behavior. We suggest that this pattern of selfreport, performance on psychological tests and brain functioning represent the normal range of human development, which is open to everyone born on earth, provided they gain the necessary experiences to rise to the next level. Movie Metaphor This process of de-embedding can be understood in terms of a movie-metaphor. Watching a movie, most individuals are “lost” in the movie. The movie is real. Emotions and thoughts are dictated by the ever-changing sequence of the frames of the film. This is an object-referral mode, where the objects of experience are salient and determine one’s thoughts, feelings and sense-of-self. The meditative experience of transcending—the repeated experience of pure self-awareness—alters this common movie-going experience. Subjectively, the individual begins to “wake up” to his/her own inner status. Although continuing to enjoy the movie, he/she gradually becomes aware that they exist independent from the movie. They experience a value of ‘witnessing’ the activity around them. To these individuals, the ever-changing movie frames are a secondary part of experience because these frames are always changing. The most salient part of their every experience is pure, self-awareness. What is ‘real’ shifts with time from the movie to self-awareness, from the thoughts, feelings and actions to the Self, from object-referral to self-referral awareness. The reported experience of stable states of self-awareness de-embedded from, but coexisting with, the processes of waking, sleeping or dreaming has been called “Cosmic 31 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Consciousness” or “enlightenment” (Maharishi Mahesh Yogi, 1969; Shear et al., 1999). This state has been described in many traditions (Bucke, 1991). Though many scientifically minded people may consider enlightenment to be either imaginary, impractical, or simply outside the boundaries of scientific investigation, the implications of these data are that enlightenment may be operationalized by interviews, psychological tests, and brain patterns during tasks and so brought into the arena of systematic scientific investigation. As advances in technologies in modern science have expanded our understanding of the outer environment, so applying subjective technologies from Eastern traditions could expand our understanding of our inner world and help further our growth along the proposed Object-referral/Self-referral continuum of Self. CONCLUSION In conclusion, these phenomenological and physiological data suggest a range of fundamentally different values of one’s identity or sense-of-self. On one end is the selfreferral experience in which the Self experiences itself through itself. This experience of pure self-awareness is gained during practice the TM technique. At the other end, the Self is embedded in or identified with ongoing perception, thinking, and behavior. Future longitudinal research can investigate the effect of different meditation and spiritual traditions on moving individuals along this continuum. This line of research could dramatically impact our understanding of the possible range of human development, and could help promote a more unified understanding of spiritual development in terms of development of brain integration. 32 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Object-Referral Mode PREDOMINANCE OF COGNITIVE AND BEHAVIORAL PROCESSES “I like to forge my own way.” Self identified with thoughts and actions Lower Consciousness Factor Scores Lower moral reasoning Lower happiness Lower emotional stability An Outer Orientation Higher anxiety Lower Integration Scale Scores Higher frontal EEG coherence Higher alpha/gamma power ratio More efficient cortical preparatory responses Self as director of thoughts and actions Self is independent of thoughts and actions Higher Consciousness Factor Scores Higher moral reasoning Higher happiness Greater emotional stability An Inner Orientation Lower anxiety Higher Integration Scale Scores Higher frontal EEG coherence Higher alpha/gamma power ratio More efficient cortical preparatory responses Self-Referral Mode Predominance of Self “My self is immeasurably vast on a physical level.” Figure 1: Schematic Representation of Sense-of-Self along an Object Referral/Self Referral Continuum. The range of descriptions of sense-of-self extended from Object-referral to Self-referral predominant modes. The three boxes on the left present the supercode from the phenomenological, first person reports, and the two right-most boxes present the psychological (Consciousness Factor scores) and physiological (brain-based Integration Scale scores) correlates of the ends of the proposed continuum. 33 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum REFERENCES Alexander, C., J. Davies, C. A. Dixon, M. Dillbeck, S. Druker, R. M. Oetzel, J. M. Muehlman and D. OrmeJohnson (1990). Growth of Higher Stages of Consciousness: Maharishi Vedic Psychology of Human Development. Higher Stages of Human Development. C. Alexander and E. Langer. New York, Oxford University Press: 259-341. Baars, B. (1997). In the Theater of Consciousness: The Workspace of the Mind. New York, Oxford University Press. Badawi, K., R. K. Wallace, D. Orme-Johnson and A. M. Rouzere (1984). Electrophysiologic Characteristics of Respiratory Suspension Periods Occurring During the Practice of the Transcendental Meditation Program. Psychosomatic medicine. 46(3): 267-76. Baruss, I. and R. J. Moore (1992). Measurement of Beliefs About Consciousness and Reality. Psychology Reports 71: 59-64. Ben Shalom, D. (2000). Developmental Depersonalization: The Prefrontal Cortex and Self- Functions in Autism. Consciousness and Cognition 9: 457-60. Brunia, C. H. M. (1993). Waiting in Readiness: Gating in Attention and Motor Preparation. Psychophysiology 30: 327-339. Bucke, R. M. (1991). Cosmic Consciousness. New York, E.P. Dutton. Chung-Yuan, C. (1969). Original Teachings of Ch'an Buddhism. New York, Vintage Books. Compton, W. C. and G. M. Becker (1983). Self-Actualizations and Experience with Zen Meditation: Is a Learning Period Necessary for Meditation? Journal of clinical psychology. 39(6): 925-9. Davidson, R. J. (2002). Anxiety and Affective Style: Role of Prefrontal Cortex and Amygdala. Biological psychiatry. 51(1): 68-80. Farrow, J. T. and J. R. Hebert (1982). Breath Suspension During the Transcendental Meditation Technique. Psychosomatic medicine. 44(2): 133-53. Feinberg, I. (1999). Delta Homeostasis, Stress, and Sleep Deprivation. Sleep 22: 1021-30. Florian, G., C. Andrew and G. Pfurtscheller (1998). Do Changes in Coherence Always Reflect Changes in Functional Coupling? Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. 106(1): 87-91. Fuster, J. M. (1993). Frontal Lobes. Current opinion in neurobiology. 3(2): 160-5. Fuster, J. M. (2000). Executive Frontal Functions. Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Experimentation cerebrale. 133(1): 66-70. Gibbs, J. C., K. S. Basinger and D. Fuller (1992). Moral Maturity. Hillsdale, New Jersey, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An Alternative "Description of Personality": The Big-Five Factor Structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59: 1216-1229. Gusnard, D. A., E. Akbudak, G. L. Shulman and M. E. Raichle (2001). Medial Prefrontal Cortex and SelfReferential Mental Activity: Relation to a Default Mode of Brain Function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98(7): 4259-64. Hebert, J. R., D. Lehmann, G. Tan, F. Travis and A. Arenander (in press). Enhanced Eeg Alpha TimeDomain Phase Synchrony During Transcendental Meditation: Implications for Cortical Integration Theory. Signal Processing Journal. Hobson, J. A. and E. F. Pace-Schott (2002). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Sleep: Neuronal Systems, Consciousness and Learning. Nature Review of Neuroscience 3: 679-93. Huber, R., M. F. Ghilardi, M. Massimini and G. Tononi (2004). Local Sleep and Learning. Nature 430(6995): 78-81. Hycner, R. N. (1985). Some Guidelines for the Phenomenological Analyses of Interview Data. Human Studies 8: 279-303. Jacobi, F., H. U. Wittchen, C. Holting, M. Hofler, H. Pfister, N. Muller and R. Lieb (2004). Prevalence, CoMorbidity and Correlates of Mental Disorders in the General Population: Results from the German Health Interview and Examination Survey (Ghs). Psychol Med 34(4): 597-611. James, W. (1962). Psychology: Briefer Course. New York, Collier Books. Jensen, A. R. (1980). Bias in Mental Testing. New York, The Free Press. Judge, T. A., J. E. Bono, R. Ilies and M. W. Gerhardt (2002). Personality and Leadership: A Qualitative and Quantitative Review. Journal of Applied Psychology 87(4): 765-780. 34 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Judge, T. A. and R. Ilies (2002). Relationship of Personality to Performance Motivation: A Meta-Analytic Review. Journal of Applied Psychology 4(797-807). Kelley, W. M., C. N. Macrae, C. L. Wyland, S. Caglar and S. Inati (2002). Finding the Self? An EventRelated Fmri Study. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 14(5): 785-94. Kesterson, J. and N. F. Clinch (1989). Metabolic Rate, Respiratory Exchange Ratio, and Apneas During Meditation. The American journal of physiology. 256(3) Pt 2: R632-8. Kjaer, T. W., M. Nowak, K. W. Kjaer, A. R. Lou, H. C. Lou and D. K. G. D. John F Kennedy Institute (2001). Precuneus-Prefrontal Activity During Awareness of Visual Verbal Stimuli. Consciousness and cognition. 10(3): 356-65. Lutz, A., L. L. Greischar, N. B. Rawlings, M. Ricard and R. J. Davidson (2004). Long-Term Meditators SelfInduce High-Amplitude Gamma Synchrony During Mental Practice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101(46): 16369-73. Maharishi Mahesh Yogi (1963). The Science of Being and the Art of Living. New York, Penguin. Maharishi Mahesh Yogi (1969). Maharishi Mahesh Yogi on the Bhagavad Gita. New York, Penguin. Maharishi Mahesh Yogi (1972). The Science of Creative Intelligence. Livingston Manor, New York, Age of Enlightenment Press. Maltzman, I. (1977). Orienting in Classical Conditioning and Generalization of the Galvanic Skin Response to Words: An Overview. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106: 111-119. Mason, L. I., C. N. Alexander, F. T. Travis, G. Marsh, D. W. Orme-Johnson, J. Gackenbach, D. C. Mason, M. Rainforth and K. G. Walton (1997). Electrophysiological Correlates of Higher States of Consciousness During Sleep in Long-Term Practitioners of the Transcendental Meditation Program. Sleep. 20(2): 102-10. Moll, J., R. de Oliveira-Souza, P. J. Eslinger, I. E. Bramati, J. Mouräao-Miranda, P. A. Andreiuolo and L. Pessoa (2002). The Neural Correlates of Moral Sensitivity: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Investigation of Basic and Moral Emotions. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 22(7): 2730-6. Moll, J., P. J. Eslinger and R. Oliveira-Souza (2001). Frontopolar and Anterior Temporal Cortex Activation in a Moral Judgment Task: Preliminary Functional Mri Results in Normal Subjects. Arquivos de neuro-psiquiatria. 59(3-B): 657-64. Muhr, T. (2004). User's Manual for Atlas-Ti. Berlin, Scientific Software Development. Natsoulas, T. (1997). Consciousness and Self-Awareness-Part I: Consciousness4, Consciousness5, Consciousness6. Journal of Mind and Behavior 18: 53-74. O'Gorman, J. G. (1979). The Orienting Reflex: Novelty or Significance Detector? Psychophysiology 16: 253262. O'Shaughnessy, B. (1986). Consciousness. Midwest Studies in Philosophy 10: 49-62. Pfurtscheller, G. (2001). Functional Brain Imaging Based on Erd/Ers. Vision research. 41(10-11). Plum, F. and J. B. Posner (1980). The Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma. Philadelphia, F.A. Davis. Reps, P. (1955). Zen Flesh, Zen Bones. New York, Doubleday Anchor book. Shear, J. and R. Jevning (1999). Pure Consciousness: Scientific Exploration of Meditation Techniques. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6(2-3): 189-209. Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. Oxford, Pergamon. Sommer, B. (1991). A Practical Guide to Behavioral Research. New York, Oxford University Press. Spearman, C. (1904). General Intelligence: Objectively Determined and Measured. American Journal of Psychololy 15: 201-292. Spinks, J. A., G. H. Blowers and D. T. L. Shek (1985). The Role of the Orienting Response in the Anticipation of Information: A Skin Conductance Response Study. Psychophysiology 22: 385-394. Taylor, J. G. (2001). The Central Role of the Parietal Lobes in Consciousness. Consciousness and cognition. 10(3): 379-417. Tecce, J. J. and L. Cattanach (1993). Contingent Negative Variation (Cnv). Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. E. Neidermeyer and L. d. Silva. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins: 887-910. Travis, F. (1994). The Junction Point Model: A Field Model of Waking, Sleeping, and Dreaming Relating Dream Witnessing, the Waking/Sleeping Transition, and Transcendental Meditation in Terms of a Common Psychophysiologic State. Dreaming 4(2): 91-104. 35 Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum Travis, F. and A. Arenander (in review). Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Study of Effects of Transcendental Meditation Practice on Interhemispheric Frontal Asymmetry and Frontal Coherence. International journal of neuroscience. Travis, F. and C. Pearson (2000). Pure Consciousness: Distinct Phenomenological and Physiological Correlates of "Consciousness Itself". The International journal of neuroscience. 100(1-4). Travis, F., J. J. Tecce and C. Durchholz (2001). Cortical Plasticity, Cnv, and Transcendent Experiences: Replication with Subjects Reporting Permanent Transcendental Experiences. Psychophysiology 38: S95. Travis, F. and R. K. Wallace (1997). Autonomic Patterns During Respiratory Suspensions: Possible Markers of Transcendental Consciousness. Psychophysiology. 34(1): 39-46. Travis, F. and R. K. Wallace (1999). Autonomic and Eeg Patterns During Eyes-Closed Rest and Transcendental Meditation (Tm) Practice: The Basis for a Neural Model of Tm Practice. Consciousness and cognition. 8(3): 302-18. Travis, F. T., A. T. Arenander and D. DuBois (2004). Psychological and Physiological Characteristics of a Proposed Object-Referral/Self-Referral Continuum of Self-Awareness. Consciousness and Cognition. 13: 401-420. Travis, F. T., J. Tecce, A. Arenander and R. K. Wallace (2002). Patterns of Eeg Coherence, Power, and Contingent Negative Variation Characterize the Integration of Transcendental and Waking States. Biological psychology. 61: 293-319. Upanishads the Principle Upanishads (1953). London, Allen and Unwin. van Boxtel, G. J. and C. H. Brunia (1994). Motor and Non-Motor Aspects of Slow Brain Potentials. Biological psychology 38: 37-51. Varela, F., J.-P. Lachaux, E. Rodriguez and J. Martinerie (2001). The Brainweb: Phase Synchonization and Large-Scale Integration. Neuroscience 2: 220-230. Vogeley, K., M. Kurthen, P. Falkai and W. Maier (1999). Essential Functions of the Human Self Model Are Implemented in the Prefrontal Cortex. Consciousness and cognition. 8(3): 343-63. von Stein, A. and J. Sarnthein (2000). Different Frequenices for Different Scales of Cortical Integration: From Local Gamma to Long Rnage Alpha/Theta Synchronization. International Journal of Psychophysiology 38: 301-313. Walter, W. G., Cooper, R., Aldridge, V.J., McCallum, W.C., Winter, A. L. (1964). Contingent Negative Variation. An Electric Sign of Sensorimotor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain. Nature 203: 380-384. Wittchen, H. U. and F. Jacobi (2005). Size and Burden of Mental Disorders in Europe-a Critical Review and Appraisal of 27 Studies. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. Wittchen, H. U., C. B. Nelson and G. Lachner (1998). Prevalence of Mental Disorders and Psychosocial Impairments in Adolescents and Young Adults. Psychol Med 28(1): 109-26. Woodruff-Smith, D. (1986). The Structure of (Self-)Consciousness. Topoi 5: 149-156. Wundt, W. (1897). Outlines of Psychology. Leipzig, G.E. Stechert. 36