Část 6. kapitoly nazvané „Character Structures, Empathy and Interest“ z knihy Eda S. Tana EMOTION AND THE STRUCTURE OF NARRATIVE FILM (Film as an Emotion Machine). Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers: New Jersey 1996, s. 153-171. Character Structures, Empathy, and Interest INTRODUCTION OF TERMS There are various contexts in which the viewer has the sensation of being "in the film," a sensation characteristically experienced by the natural audience of the traditional feature film. As a viewer I do not only entertain the illusion that I am present in the scene —the diegetic effect—I may even feel that to a greater or lesser degree the adventures of the protagonists are actually happening to me. This experience can take many different forms, which makes it somewhat difficult to describe. In La Peau Douce (1964) we fill in Nicole's thoughts after Lachenay has turned his back on her in the street in order to keep their love secret. We have a different experience, when Reuven is struck full in the face by a baseball in The Chosen (1981); we almost feel as if we ourselves are the victim. And watching King Kong (1933), one sympathizes with both the girl and the huge gorilla as the animal majestically undergoes his fate, amid a rain of bullets fired by stupid, insensitive human beings. In describing such experiences, people often speak of identification or empathy. Although based on the diegetic effect, these phenomena are clearly distinct from it. It is quite conceivable, for example, that the illusion of being present in the fictional world is absolute without the viewer experiencing any appreciable involvement with the events taking place on the screen. The exact meaning of the terms identification and empathy differs according to the context and the field of research; the result is an assortment of widely differing concepts commonly referred to by the same name. (For recent surveys 153 154 6 CHAPTER undertaken from a broader perspective, see Schoenmakers, 1988; van Vliet, 1991; and Zillmann, 1991b.) We are fortunate in being able to examine the phenomena of identification and empathy within quite a limited framework, focusing on viewer emotion and, more particularly, interest. Before doing so, however, we must mention one special manner of participating in a feature film. We all know that it is possible to feel drawn into or borne along by the movement of the camera or the objects in the scene, or by the stream of images created by montage and reinforced by the music; numerous lyrical passages in feature films could be cited as examples. By analogy with the use of music and the visual arts in psychology, it is possible to speak of empathy. Lipps (1906) referred to the total immersion ("projection") of the self in an object as characteristic of total empathy. Michotte (1953/1991) used the term "motor empathy" for the imitation of the rhythmic movements observed on the screen. (For a general overview, see also Kreitler & Kreitler, 1972, pp. 264-281). Following on the terms introduced in chapter 3, A—artefact—empathy may be seen as the opposite of F— fiction—empathy. We will not be examining A empathy here, as this would involve a separate study of not inconsiderable proportions. Moreover, according to our model, the processes of F identification /empathy involved in the viewing of a traditional feature film in themselves make up a large proportion of the determinants of interest. As we know from the Principles laid down in chapter 4, interest is determined by the dynamics of anticipations and outcomes, called up and reinforced by the fictional events. Anticipations and resolution may be seen as a form of costs and benefits. In chapter 5 we saw that one of the sources of expectations and anticipations consists in thematic structures, and we examined their role in the cognitive processing of feature films. A second source of investment and return, (i.e., another determinant of interest), is to be found in the expectations and anticipations that are rooted in the viewer's own knowledge of, and feelings toward, the characters of a film. Our description of these determinants is based on two simple assumptions. First, we assume that the watching of a feature film is accompanied by homogeneous experiences, (i.e., that the experiences of various viewers are comparable), the reason being that the effects intended by the maker are operative in all viewers. The sadness expressed by a particular viewer when a sympathetic protagonist contracts a fatal disease is less relevant for our purposes where that sadness is caused by the fact that a close relative has been similarly struck down. While this fact is extremely important for the viewer in question and for the manner in which he or she experiences the film, explanations based on life experiences and personality factors can at most help to account for the preference that certain natural viewers of the traditional feature film display for a particular genre. Thus when we speak of identifica- CHARACTER STRUCTURES, EMPATHY, AND INTEREST 155 tion/empathy, we will be referring to processes that in principle operate in all viewers in response to a certain film. As regards our second assumption, during the discussion of interest in chapter 4 we emphasized the activity of the viewer and the fact that active participation that respects the rules of the game is rewarded. This point of departure clashes with certain views on identification that have attained some popularity within contemporary film theory. I would like to begin with a few remarks on this subject. IDENTIFICATION IN CONTEMPORARY FILM THEORY: THE TRAGIC VIEWER Under influence of the psychoanalytical orientation in French film studies, which has been evident in the last two decades, identification has become a prominent subject of theorizing (see also the discussion of the etat filmique in chap. 2). Metz (1975a) introduced a distinction between primary and secondary identification: the former refers to the identification of the viewer with him or herself, the latter to the identification with the film character. Identification with oneself has to do with the fact that the viewer is forced to share the perspective of events presented by the camera. You identify with a certain view, which ultimately becomes your own. The basis of the expression "identifying with one's own view" is not entirely clear. We assume that Metz is referring here to the cornerstone of the diegetic effect, the illusion that the viewer is actually present in the fictional world as an (invisible) spectator. A similar reading of Metz was put forward by Aumont, Bergala, Marie, and Vernet (1983). According to these authors, primary identification is identification with the camera. On logical grounds, I have chosen to distinguish the diegetic effect from identification/empathy. In its more general sense, the secondary identification referred to by Metz is an identification with characters. It is possible that what primary and secondary identification have in common is the reflection of an imaginary, absent object, based on a lack of some kind, a shortcoming that is at once compensated for and constantly recalled. Film technique denies the existence of this lack, creating the illusion of presence; in this sense it is a fetish, a surrogate, which is enjoyed not only by the cinephile, but also by the average filmgoer. This tragic representation of affairs is most clearly defined in the formulation of primary identification by Aumont, Bergala, Marie, and Vernet (1983). The viewer is prompted by a desire the precise nature of which . . . est certainement a chercher du cote d'un etat d'abandon, de solitude, de manque: le speetateur de cinema est toujours plus ou moins un refugie pour qu'il s'agit de reparer quelque perte irreparable, serait-ce au prix d'une regres- 156 CHAPTER 6 sion passagere, soeialement reglee, le temps d'une projection. (Aumont et al., 1983, p. 172) . . . is assuredly to seek a state of abandon, solitude, want. The cinema spectator is always something of a fugitive, whose concern is to redress some irreparable loss, even at the cost of a passing regression, socially controlled, lasting only as long as a projection, [author's translation]. For Metz, establishing a paradox between what viewers want and what they ultimately get is the crux of the argument, which is in itself an amusing point. In the view of Aumont et al., this obscures the issue of the operations that constitute identification and the circumstances under which they are carried out. Aumont et al. propose several such operations that are quite useful, as we will see. First, there is the operation that consists in following the narrative in search of the Restoration of Balance (chap. 3). In chapter 5 we saw that something resembling empathy is involved in understanding the various events, which are themselves governed by themes. This was abundantly clear from the analysis of the process of understanding the film Punishment. Second, Aumont et al. mention identification with the character through the operation of a type; third identification with a character due to the structure of the situation; fourth identification at the level of the surface structure of the film (the decoupage); and fifth identification with the narrative. It is not entirely clear whether some of the factors are identical to or merely influenced by identification. I hope that the following exposition will help to clarify this point. One fundamental idea put forward by Aumont et al. is deserving of our attention, namely, the fact that identification requires no in-depth psychological explanation, because, given the diegetic effect, it flows directly from the viewer's attention to, and comprehension of, the film. It may be added that the same goes for other affective attitudes toward film characters as well. Let us begin with the viewer's comprehension of characters. UNDERSTANDING CHARACTERS Because viewers have the feeling that they are spectators in the fictional world, a world that has at least some similarity with the real world, it is conceivable that many well-known mechanisms of social cognition are applicable to their experience of characters in a film. Thus we are justified in seeing the comprehension of characters as a guided impression formation that extends to the entire film narrative. There is reason to believe that our comprehension of fictional characters takes place in the same way as our comprehension of people in the everyday world or in that of the psychological laboratory (Hoffner & Cantor, 1991). We examine first a number of insights related to fictional characters, and second, impression formation by actual individuals, in an attempt to establish possible similarities. CHARACTER STRUCTURES, EMPATHY, AND INTEREST 157 Literary Theories on Characterization: Type Versus Person In narrative theory there have been a number of attempts to arrive at a system for categorizing the characters that appear in traditional feature films. The proposed systems are in line with recent social psychological views on impression formation. No doubt the best known distinction that has been put forward in the field of literary analysis is that between flat and round characters, as launched by E. M. Forster (1927). This distinction also appears in drama theory, for example, in the contrast between one-dimensional and multidimensional characters (Pfister, 1977). According to Forster flat characters or types appear in strong plots, such as those employed by Dickens and Wells. The strength of the plot precludes the possibility of the reader seeing through the shallowness of the characterization. On the other hand, one will search in vain for types in a tragedy. Forster contrasts the type with the round character, who carries within him or her the unpredictability of life itself. Characters like this predominate in the novels of Tolstoy and Dostoevsky. In general one would do well to exercise a certain restraint in applying this distinction to whole genres. However, the usefulness of such a dimension, say, from type to person to individual (Pfister, 1977), is, in the light of our objectives, undeniable, if only because it implies a kind of trade-off, in terms of complexity, between the course of the narrative events on the one hand and the characters on the other. In certain genres, such as the tale or the novel, the complexity of the narrative is concerned mainly with the way the plot develops. The themes that feed the plot contain a multitude of possibilities for reversals of expectation, countless shifts and turns, and an abundance of ironic twists (see chap. 5). In other genres, such as novels or tragedies, the complexity, that is, the postponement of the major outcomes of the narrative, is rooted in the development of the characters and their interrelationships. A similar dichotomy in genres is to be found in Barthes (1966). He proposed a distinction that recognizes, on the one hand, functional narratives, which rely heavily on linear sequences of action, guided by narrative functions, such as those distinguished by Propp and Bremond. The popular fairy tale is a case in point. On the other hand, there are the indicial narratives, such as the psychological novel, in which the emphasis is on atmosphere and characterization.89 This distinction may overlap that between the popular feature film on the one hand, and the quality film on the other. 89 Obviously, there are a great many conceivable transitional forms. Thus it is quite possible that what we designated earlier (chap. 5) as a quality film, notably the so-called psychologically realistic drama, meets Auerbach's (1946) description of the great realist literary work dating from before the 20th century, which combines a tragic involvement with the individual, regardless of his or her particular place in the hierarchy, and a comic interest in society, all of which results in an apt representation of the actual conditions. Examples include Host sonaten/Autumn 158 CHAPTER 6 In chapter 5 we said that in the traditional feature film the major focus is on the thematic action, while less attention is given to an insightful examination of character, especially in comparison with the so-called modern art cinema, with its subjective realism (Bordwell, 1985). Pfister (1977) pointed out that the limited psychological treatment of the characters has a long cultural and historical tradition, while the opposite approach did not make its appearance until the Sturm und Drang period. This does not mean that the viewer of the traditional feature film in general, and the popular action film in particular, is deprived of in-depth psychological examinations of character. For one thing, even types are to some extent individualized. As Scholes and Kellogg (1966) noted, Mr. Pickwick and Sam Weller have quite striking traits of their own, despite their admitted resemblance to other famous literary duos, such as Quixote/Panza and Holmes/Watson. And although it would be misleading to speak of an exploration of character in the strict sense of the word, the viewer must at the very least go along with a characterization that often transcends routine typification. Harvey (1965) maintained that in everyday life "the bore bores us" and we are repelled by the hypocrite (pokrytec), and yet when they appear in the fictional world we cannot get enough of them. In the second place, the interaction between the various types makes possible a greater degree of depth. According to Harvey (1965), the protagonist acquires a semblance of depth precisely because there are other characters playing out their roles in the background. Let us look now at an example of relationships between different types. Cards are relatively static and predictable characters, although not necessarily simple, who are often at once comical and pathetic. (For example, the faithful Cheyenne in C'era una volta il West/Once Upon a Time in the West (1969), who is invariably turning up at the side of the hero, the "Harmonica Man," and in the end dies a heroic death, or the faithful Lieutenant Bates in The Third Man (1949), who catches Harry Lime's first shot, intended for the hero Martins.) Harvey referred to Cards as "chemically pure," whereas drama theorist Beckerman (1970) characterized them as "narrow," in a reference to the extent of their potential for development throughout the drama as a whole. A second example is the Ficelle, the representative of the reader/viewer in the fictional world. He or she is contrasted with the protagonist by virtue of the common sense that the human, driven, or spontaneous protagonist lacks or is contrasted with the protagonist in the matter of social background. In 'Round Midnight (1986) Fran9ois, who associates with the "tenor hero" Dexter Gordon, could be a Ficelle, like Scott Fitzgerald's young alter ego in The Great Gatsby (1974) and Thompson, the journalist in Citizen Kane (1941). If no great attention is bestowed upon character in film theory, film critiSonata (1978), Dersu uzala (1975), Under the Volcano (1984), The Dresser (1983), The Dead (1987), Smultronstdllet/Wild Strawberries (1957), and A Woman Under the Influence (1974). CHARACTER STRUCTURES, EMPATHY, AND INTEREST 15 9 cism does highlight the significance of characters. The bridge between literary theories concerned with character reading and psychological theories related to the comprehension of actual people is formed by Bordwell's account of the interpretation of film characters by professional film critics (Bordwell, 1989b). However, the schemas devised by Bordwell, on the basis of which critics interpret characters, are of such a general nature that it may be assumed that they are also employed by the natural viewers of the traditional feature film.90 The comprehension of characters is a major strategy for lending coherence to the film text. According to Bordwell, it consists of two components. First, characters are conceived of as persons in a folk (lidské) psychological sense. They are linked to a body, they display perceptual activity such as self-awareness, they have thoughts and feelings, they are characterized by persistent dispositions, and they are capable of self-generated actions. All these qualities are included in the term "intentionality." In the second place, the image that one has of characters guides one's understanding of all the other elements of the film narrative and the artefact. Thus there are various connections between character and cinematography. For instance, a threatening villain may be filmed from a lower point of view, or irregularities of physiognomy may be emphasized by lighting. Psychological Theories of Impression Formation: Type Versus Person Very little psychological research has thus far been devoted to the comprehension of fictional characters, in comparison with research focusing on the processing of narrative action structures and thematic structures. There is, however, an excellent review study by Hoffner and Cantor (1991), where frequent mention is made of research in which real people rather than fictional characters serve as stimuli. The comparison between the formation of impressions of media characters and that of real people would appear to be a valid one, according to Hoffner and Cantor (1991). Somewhat ironically, the most recent studies on social cognition have for practical reasons employed mainly imaginary individuals as stimuli, which also blurs the distinction between the two types of research. Reviewing the literature on the formation of impressions, we see that the earlier mentioned distinction between typological and individual-based cognitive structures is the subject of several of the more recent contributions. The picture emerging from the literature involved in what it is to understand other people shows a surprising similarity to current views within the field of literary analysis. However, a major difference is the fact that psychological 90 As I noted elsewhere, it is regrettable that Bordwell did not strive for a more general se-manties of characters (Tan, 1990). 160 CHAPTER 6 models strive to capture processes instead of looking only at the final results. This means that not only various levels of detail are distinguished but also different steps in the categorization of stimulus persons. Let us now take a closer look at these processes. The categorization of characters is presumably based on our habit of trying to organize the rich array of sensory impressions confronting us by linking all the different characteristics of the perceived other. It has often been demonstrated that there is a substantial correlation between observed characteristics, such as intelligence and dominance, while that between the same objective or measured characteristics may be almost zero and that between observed and measured characters is likewise negligible (Brunswik, 1956).91 If subjects are asked to mentally assess others on the basis of certain characteristics, the pattern of correlations between traits is quite different from that reported on the basis of direct observation of others (Pryor, 1986; Shweder & DAndrade, 1980). In categorizing others, people apparently make use of person schemas that describe the connections between traits. It would take very little steering of the categorization process by filmic means to produce a significant halo-effect (Hamilton & Rose, 1980). Automatic Categorization At the most elementary level of the encoding of the stimulus, persons are automatically recognized by means of a process that Bruner (1957) called "primitive categorization." It may be assumed that even at this early stage of processing "intentionality" is attributed to the stimulus, thus introducing a distinction between living and inanimate objects. Such basic characteristics as sex and age are also identified. It is more difficult, however, to establish to what extent traits that are important for affect are registered during this stage. But it is logical to assume that the total appeal that an individual has for the subject is not realized in a single instant. In feature films characters are often "introduced," in the sense that a certain amount of time is devoted to presenting them to the audience. The course of the plot also plays a vital role in determining the appeal of a particular character: how she reacts to events determines whether she arouses sympathy or antipathy among viewers. At a very elementary level of processing, however, the visual image of a film character is capable of producing direct appeal. This may be the case if an unlearned emotional stimulus (Frijda, 1986) or innate releaser is presented. Facial and bodily characteristics that are adaptive for the survival of the species or the attainment of a goal by the individual may be emphasized or exaggerated. We are thinking here of such things as signs of sexual readiness, dependence, or approachability (Berry & McArthur, 1986) or a variant 91 A recent survey of research into "illusory correlation" is to be found in Fiske and Taylor (1991). CHARACTER STRUCTURES, EMPATHY, AND INTEREST 161 of appro achability, such as cuteness, as studied by Brooks and Hochberg (1960). These signs call up desire and protective action tendencies respectively. The ecological approach to social perception (Baron, 1980) is of particular importance in explaining the elementary process of impression formation. Here qffordances are sought, which the individual observed offers for a certain behavior and which are directly observable, without any significant intervention of cognitive processes. Frijda (1993) discussed the importance of the direct observation of affordances for the primary appraisal of situations in the emotion process. An example of a social affordance is the impression of infantile helplessness, which makes possible protective behavior and which is immediately observable in such things as, say, the shape of the head (Alley, 1983, 1986). According to Baron (1991), certain emotions, such as anger and fear, are immediately evident in the movements of the model. He suggests that in general the observation of personality dispositions is the same thing as the direct observation of social affordances. This would mean that the usual accounts of impression formation assume the existence of more — and more complicated— cognitive operations than necessary. According to Berscheid (1985), a great many studies attest to the role that physical beauty plays in the attraction people feel for one another. The results of these studies "are such that the physically attractive—across age, sex, race, and all socioeconomic stations— receive numerous preferential social treatments" (pp. 453-454). According to Berscheid, this may be due to the fact that the observer tends to assume the presence of various other attractive qualities behind the appealing outward appearance. Hoffner and Cantor (1991) found support for this assumption during research that focused on children and television. It is, however, probable that other dimensions also play a role in determining the appeal of media personalities. The physical attractiveness of a face is probably related to the degree to which it reflects the average of a particular population. Light, Hollander, and Kayra-Stuart (1981) found that faces described as attractive were also judged to be more common and typical, whereas attractive and typical faces were thought to look more like each other than like other faces. In a later study Langlois and Roggman (1990) actually established that average faces are more attractive. Using computer techniques, the researchers averaged photographs of faces. Stimulus faces that were obtained by averaging a larger number of photographs were judged to be more attractive than those obtained by averaging a smaller number of photographs. Langlois and Roggman believe that this is due to a closer association of the first group of stimuli with prototypical representations. They do, however, note that a random sample of portraits of film stars might well be considered more attractive, even though they were not necessarily prototypical. All in all, it is unclear to what extent the appeal of sympathetic protagonists, portrayed in feature films by attractive young men and women, is based on innate releasers, as opposed to familiarity or 162 6 CHAPTER cultural stereotypes. 92 But there are other possibilities as well. Garello, Grosofsky, Shaw, Pittenger, and Mark (1989) suggested that the assessment of the physical attractiveness of a person's face should not be based on the average of some reference group or other, but rather on the ideal for that particular individual. That ideal is related to a biological function, the chewing function, for example. This hypothesis was confirmed by experiments in which subjects were presented with illustrations of faces in which the shape of the jaw deviated to a large extent, slightly, or not at all, from a functional archetype. It is interesting to note the complications that can be deliberately attached to the display of signs, such as innate releasers and determinants of affor-dances. The example of veiled or partially revealed sexual releasers has become something of a cliche. There are other examples, such as the smallest of the robots in Return of the Jedi (1983). It is cute as far as its shape is concerned, but it appears to be made of a smooth, hard material. This lends it a minimal degree of cuddliness, which is in sharp contrast to the appeal of its shape. A somewhat different example is to be found in Le gendarme de Saint-Tropez (1964), when Louis de Funes plants a passionate kiss on the white helmet of a motorcycle policeman. From the viewpoint of the film narrative, it is conceivable that innate re-leasers are employed when there is a need to strengthen the typing of characters. "Cards" are endearing by virtue of their shortcomings; "Ficelles" are perhaps nondescript because their appearance and behavior lack innate re-leasers. Protagonists are sexually attractive, while antagonists, in particular bad guys, may be characterized by means of innate releasers of aversion and fear. These include such things as a slight deformity, a rasping voice, a perpetual expression of anger, or—less commonly, perhaps—a remote physical resemblance to animals that generally call up a reaction of fear, such as rats, snakes, and scorpions. In short, we may assume that on an elementary level of impression formation by the viewer, characterization makes use of innate releasers or stimuli that, through processes of conditioning, are related to innate releasers. Such stimuli can be realized both by typecasting and by staging. In view of the fact that on the elementary level of impression formation, perceptual and cognitive processing is not susceptible to conscious steering, and proceeds more or less automatically, the effects of such processes of characterization are largely unconscious and often unavoidable. 92 We know that at the moment they are introduced, protagonists tend to be considered attractive, although there need not be complete agreement among viewers on this point. But there is another important question that is deserving of further study, namely, the extent to which the appeal of famous actors for a natural audience at their first appearance in a particular film is founded on their appearances in previous films (or even outside the film world), quite apart from or in spite of any innate releasers. CHARACTER STRUCTURES, EMPATHY, AND INTEREST 163 There are many interesting avenues of research yet to be explored in this area. Do films display innate releasers that do not immediately present themselves to the unsuspecting eye of the viewer? There may be dynamic characteristics—even some of a higher order—in the physical movements, facial expressions, voice parameters, and paraverbal expression that have a more or less automatic effect. Another interesting possibility is that feature films permanently control the viewer's classical learning processes in such a way that the control by innate releasers is shifted to substitute stimuli (Hearst & Jenkins, 1974; Suboski, 1990). For instance, a character with an unconditioned scary face always hums a certain tune or repeats the same fragment of text. This tune or text—heard separately from any image —then becomes scary, even though in itself it is a pleasing or appealing sound, such as a nursery rhyme. In general, we can say that the stylization of characters does not necessarily lower their affect potential; the loss of direct releasers of affect is often compensated for by learning processes. Readers interested in the research into the relationship between the categorization of characters and other, immediately observable, sources of information, such as voice characteristics, dress, and behavior, are referred to the survey by Hoffner and Cantor (1991). The previously mentioned hypotheses on the categorization of characters on the elementary level show that it is quite possible to derive affective investment and return from a process that is focused primarily on comprehension of the narrative. The only thing necessary for the operation of the innate releasers, for example, is that the viewer wants to know what is going on in the fictional world. The viewer's efforts to understand the action taking place in the film will lead almost automatically to awareness of the details that are made salient by the narrative, details that often have a considerable affect potential. Categorization on Less Elementary Levels of Processing Processes of impression formation, which follow logically upon automatic encoding, lead to a cognitive representation of a character that is of varying complexity and degree of integration. Several models, all offering various levels of detail, have recently been proposed for impression formation of other persons (Andersen & Klatzky, 1987; Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Wyer & Srull, 1986). Fiske and Neuberg (1990) suggested that in the categorization of individuals there is a whole range of processes, ranging from rough categorization to "piecemeal integration." If the results on a given level prove unsatisfactory, recategorization takes place on the following, more detailed, level. This pragmatic quality of the categorization process (i.e., progressive refinement) is in accordance with the possibilities offered by the characterization of the protagonists in the film narrative. The dual-process model proposed by Brewer (1988) is more suited to the 164 CHAPTER 6 typologies familiar from literary analysis. Moreover, it is based on a large body of empirical research and fits the conclusions of an extensive survey of the experimental literature carried out by Wyer and Gordon (1984). According to Brewer, the coarse categorization of individuals runs "top-down," that is, it is category-driven and leads to the instantiation of a type representation. The latter is pictoliteral, that is, it is coded in an analogous, visual format. Type categories are hierarchical combinations of roles, features, and behavioral characteristics: "serious professionals with I-dare-you-to-challenge-my-opinion attitudes," as one subject put it, or "Barbara Walters types, gossipers, nosey, yet sly and slightly sluggish" (Brewer, 1988, p. 17). In this sense, they correspond to some extent to the flat characters of Forster (1927). Through individuation, subtypes then arise in which the information deviates somewhat from the instanced type. In the type of feature films where the action plays a fairly significant role in the narrative, as compared to character psychology, the cognitive investment related to character categorizations may consist mainly of trying out and discarding increasingly specific individuation hypotheses. This occurs after a particular character has been introduced and a rapid categorization made on the basis of pattern recognition and innate releasers. This first categorization displays a strong primacy effect (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). The return of ongoing categorization is formed by improvements in the fit and a more complex representation of the character. One of the characteristics of the process of impression formation may well be that in the processing of the new features—derived from the actions of the characters—which are continually being offered by the film narrative, the main objective is confirmation. Minor inconsistencies in the various behaviors go unnoticed, as long as the impression formation is driven by the anticipation of more global features, as is decidedly the case during the watching of feature films. Major inconsistencies, however, do give rise to revisions in the direction of a subtype. Another method of processing is what Brewer referred to as personalization. This runs "bottom-up," in other words, it is steered by the input and results in a prepositional representation focusing on a certain individual, with roles and features subordinated. Specific behavior is first stored as concrete information, and only later integrated—by means of combination and inference—to form features. Here the cognitive investment consists in keeping available a large number of particular instances and the relatively laborious process of integration, given the lack of support afforded by a prototypical category. The return would then consist of a rich and complex structure that may be relatively new. In current film theory there is consensus over the high degree of typing of the protagonists in traditional feature films. The characters who appear in classical cinema are not actually individuals, but can best be described as a collection of traits that are required to realize the prototypical causality of the CHARACTER STRUCTURES, EMPATHY, AND INTEREST 165 action (Bordwell & Thompson, 1986; Chatman, 1978). Because there are no comparative studies dealing with the agreement between the categorization of individuals, on the one hand, and fictional characters, on the other hand, we can only surmise—by way of provisional hypothesis—that the categorization found in traditional feature films depends largely on types, although these are to some extent individualized. The role of personalization is probably greatest in the more sophisticated psychological-realistic genres. A supplement to the dual-process model is to be found in Andersen and Klatzky (1987). These researchers make an interesting differentiation in the concept that Brewer describes as utyping." Trait typing makes use of categories such as outgoing, socially skilled, friendly, nutty, power-loving, self-confident, knowledgeable, and intelligent. Social stereotyping, on the other hand, is more vivid and concrete. The categories are also richer in attributes; according to Andersen and Klatzky, the most important of these extra attributes of social stereotypes are typical behavior and reactions to certain events, characteristic intentions, and goals of the type. It is conceivable that they fulfill exactly the same functions as the thematic action structures described in chapter 5. In other words, they function as a rich source of expectations and predictions concerning events and actions. Social stereotypes meet a more specific collection of constraints than trait types. Once the action has gotten under way, detailed schemas of characters must be generated that coincide with the results that have already become clear from the action of the plot. A social stereotype then functions as a goal-derived ad hoc category, which makes more specific the original rough taxonomic category, as postulated by Barsalou (1991; see also chap. 5). The examples suggested by Andersen and Klatzky include mafioso, clown/comedian, politician/diplomat, bully/gang member, brain/genius, depressed/suicidal, wise man/guru, Ronald Reagan, Woody Allen, and Ghandi. These are all characterizations that, when activated, are capable of evoking quite specific expectations concerning the further course of events. They do so on their own, and most certainly in combination with thematic action structures. The mafioso is about to pull a fast one on someone, Woody Allen will have a hard time coping, the brain/ genius will undoubtedly invent some ingenious device and become involved in a comical scene highlighting his relationship with women. The amendment suggested by Andersen and Klatzky is important to an understanding of how we form our impressions of film characters. In the first place, it helps to explain the wealth of possible predictions offered by the film narrative. The film stimulus unfolds quite gradually, and initially the viewer may tend toward trait characterization and social stereotyping. Global predictions are then succeeded by more precise ones. In the second place, trait characterization—in particular, stereotyping—may go a long way toward explaining why one character is seen as real and the other is not. It will be clear from the examples that stereotypes can refer to both fictional characters and 166 CHAPTER 6 real persons; a combination of the two is also possible, as in the ease of Woody Allen the actor and the film character. The fact that stereotypes make no clear distinction between fictional and real-life characters—a quality that might be referred to as "archetyping"—means that they can render a fictional element, in this case a character, believable or, conversely, contribute to the fictional-ization of a real-life stimulus. In the latter case, for instance, an intimidating male person can be dubbed a "Boris Karloff," in an effort to make it easier to deal with him; in the former, one might try to see grotesque fictional figures in a sympathetic, compassionate, or even tragic light. Repulsive figures are often seen as "sheep in wolf's clothing": The Hunchback of Notre Dame (1939), The Elephant Man (1980), The Phantom of the Opera (1925), and Beauty and the Beast (1991). The more specific the expectations created by such a characterization, especially if they are borne out, the more natural the fictional figure becomes. The models of impression formation described above all have in common that the mode or level of categorization of the stimulus person depends on the aim of the observer. In other words, there are pragmatic limitations to person perception. It is safe to assume that as a rule the observer will do no more than is absolutely necessary—Taylor (1981) referred to "the cognitive miser"—and we are assuming that revision of an established representation will be avoided. Nevertheless, during the watching of feature films the "processing depth" of characters is dictated to a considerable extent by the narrative,93 including in some cases the revision of established impressions. The course of the story determines how detailed the categorization will be and involves working out expectations concerning certain traits of the character. The narrative ensures that the necessity to revise a seemingly completed categorization comes to some degree as a surprise. There are countless, largely sentimental, films in which the plot revolves around a metamorphosis of the protagonist or the relatively late revelation of the true nature of a character. A few arbitrary examples will illustrate this point. The horrid boarding-school headmaster Blanchard (whose nickname was Merleusse or "hake") proves to be a kind and generous man who distributes Christmas presents to the boys who have to stay at school over the holidays (Merleusse, 1935). At the beginning of Ceiling Zero (1935) James Gagney, as the test pilot Dizzy Davidson, is portrayed as an unscrupulous ladies' man who connives to get another pilot sent on a dangerous test flight; in the end, he gives up his girlfriend to the pilot who really loves her and takes over the other's dangerous flight, during which he is killed. A less sentimental variant is the gradual revelation of unsuspected aspects of the character of Hannibal the Cannibal, the beast in 93 The term depth of processing is borrowed from the work of Graik (Graik & Loekhart, 1972; Graik & Tulving, 1975) and Anderson (Anderson, 1976, 1983), concerning encoding strategies and their effect on memory. Stimuli can be subjected to a shallow or a deeper elaboration. CHARACTER STRUCTURES, EMPATHY, AND INTEREST 167 The Silence of the Lambs (1991). However, it is not only the depth of the categorization that is determined by the film narrative. The story places all sorts of restrictions on the fleshing out of traits and types. For example, we know that there is a certain relation between the role that a fictional character plays in relation to other characters and the former's psychological characterization. How could a Ficelle be a dominant personality? Gould we ever visualize the adversary of our hero as a Ghandi rather than the usual mafioso or bully type? And the fact that as the narrative progresses Hannibal the Cannibal is shown to possess a strange kind of sensitivity is bound up with the realization that his opponents —the police and the governor—are being increasingly portrayed as stupid and corrupt. Impression Formation, Sympathy, and the Recognition of Concerns The theory of impression formation not only makes it easier to describe how viewers comprehend protagonists but also helps to explain the affective investment and return that are involved in the watching of a feature film. As Aumont et al. (1983) say, characterization automatically leads to identification. Stereotypical categories are not, in the end, affectneutral. In the first place, impression formation may be determined in part by selfcategorization needs. The theory of social identity (Tajfel, 1982), for example, holds that stereotyping of others is a response to the need of individuals to derive their own identity from the group to which they belong (in terms of sex, profession, religion, etc.), and to see one's own group (the in-group) as more favorable than another (the out-group). Older studies have shown that a certain kind of stereotype—prejudice—springs from people's desire to see themselves in a favorable light by contrasting themselves with one or more groups (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950). These self-categorization needs make it possible to arrange psychological stereotypes on a positive-negative affect or evaluation dimension, on the basis of in-groups and out-groups. Such groups are culturally determined; complicated social dynamics ordain that, in the eyes of the dominant group or the majority of other groups, a particular group is out. It need not surprise us that research into the impression formation of actual people shows that this process converges in quite an early stage in a general assessment as either good or bad, around which other traits are then arranged (Srull & Wyer, 1989; Wyer & Gordon, 1984). According to Srull and Wyer (1989) this general assessment is made as soon as sufficient behavioral information has been collected to make possible an affective categorization. Any revision of a categorization is usually prompted by new information that conflicts with the established likeable-dislikeable assessment, rather than by descriptive information. D'Andrade (1984) showed that the terminology that is culturally estab- 168 CHAPTER 6 lished and commonly used by lay persons—as opposed to professional psychologists, at least hopefully—to describe character includes a highly evaluative component. This led him to state: It should be remembered that the natural language of character terms was created by and for ordinary people, and that one of the major needs of ordinary people is to determine how to react to what other people do—to approach or avoid, to reward or punish, to imitate or correct. The natural language system of character terms seems well suited to these purposes. What is interesting is that we have developed so many ways of being good and bad. (D'Andrade, 1984, p. 332) In a more general sense, social schemas are capable of evoking affect directly. When someone fits into an existing person schema, then not only the meanings that are inherent in the schema are invoked but also the affect that has been attached to the schema by previous experience. Schema-triggered affect has been demonstrated using a variety of person schemas, such as the members of a certain profession (doctor, hotel maid, loan shark), or individuals who are culturally stigmatized, such as schizophrenics (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). The fact that characters are so easy to typify and that types, in particular stereotypes, are in or out lends characters an affective charge. The more the narrative gives rise to the instantiation of types, especially stereotypes, the more the appeal of characters can be directly traced to the process of impression formation. Typical intentions (good or bad) and the ensuing character goals and plans are among the attributes of stereotypical social categories, and these trigger affect in the viewer. The appeal of stereotypical characters is presumably related to the intentions ascribed to them: good guys are sympathetic, bad guys are not (Leifer & Robers, 1972, in Hoffner & Cantor, 1991), and deliberate bad behavior on the part of a character results in a negative evaluation (Berndt & Berndt, 1975, in Hoffner & Cantor, 1975). Motives or intentions would appear to carry more weight than consequences (Hoffner & Cantor, 1991). When we say that good guys have good intentions, we mean that the concerns that they have with respect to the state of affairs in the fictional world corresponds to more or less universal source concerns shared by the natural audience of the traditional feature film. And, as we know, perceived similarity leads to attraction. Similarity between one's own attitudes and views on the important issues of life—in other words, value concerns (Frijda, 1986) —and the views of others more in particular creates a bond between people. There is considerable empirical support for the positive relation between observed attitude similarities and attraction.94 Above all, correspondence between the charac94 See, for example, Seeord, Baekman, and Eaehus (1964). For surveys of research into the relation between attraction and observed similarity of attitudes, see also Berscheid (1985, pp. 455-457). One explanation for the powerful influence of this effect may be the operation of a reverse influence: attraction leads to a heightened awareness of shared attitudes (see Granberg CHARACTER STRUCTURES, EMPATHY, AND INTEREST 169 ter's views on justice to those of the viewer presumably determines the lat-ter's sympathy for that character. The categorization of characters in terms of good and bad is by no means restricted to popular and stereotypical films, as one might expect. Among readers and viewers of contemporary drama (Bordewijk-Knotter, 1988) and political drama intended for a culturally elite audience (Tan & Schoenmakers, 1984), there are striking examples of judgments based on those very same moral categories: good and evil intentions. The categorization of characters on the basis of recognized concerns has a number of consequences. First, there is the heightened relevance for the viewer of the vicissitudes of the characters. Both good guys and bad guys harbor intentions and aims that touch the concerns of the viewer. An obvious example is defense against threats to law and order or to sympathetic characters. This relevance of the character's fate is in itself an important condition for the emergence of empathetic emotions. And second, viewers are inclined to subscribe to the goals of the good guy and to reject those of the bad guy. (Albritton & Gerrig, 1991; Zillmann & Bryant, 1975a; Zillmann & Cantor, 1977). Because intentions and goals are so crucial to the process of categorization, there is presumably a pronounced halo effect attached to them; even impression formation consists to a not inconsiderable degree in establishing consistency between the good or evil intentions observed and other, more superficial traits. When Humphrey Bogart suddenly develops a tic and begins to pull on his ear lobe (Casablanca, 1942), this is charming and the repetition of this mannerism only makes him more attractive. The same might be said of the chest-beating of King Kong. The fact that this Beast has the best interests of his Beauty at heart, and even those of all people of good will, serves to lend a human and pleasant quality to his chest beating and other animal features, notably in the long run (King Kong, 1933). Conversely, once we are aware of the intentions of the villain, his (more often than her) refined manners suddenly become decadent, and there is an ominous significance to the way he sits there, stroking the cat on his lap. The revelation of underlying intentions of characters is a component of the narrative process and the previously mentioned halo effects can be manipulated by the filmmaker in a variety of ways. In general, we may assume that the Complication, which theoretically is embedded in the thematic action of all traditional feature films, is balanced by the development of a character. In fact, the development of the character may even replace the thematic action structure in the sense that it creates expectations and shapes them (chap. 5).95 An early and firmly planted primacy effect consisting mainly of negative & King, 1980; Schoedel, Frederickson, & Knight, 1975). Berscheid (1985, p. 456) warned against an overly simplistic interpretation of the links between observed attitude similarities and attraction. ^Development is also seen by scriptwriters as a desirable trait of dramatic characters. The seeds of growth must be planted at an early stage of the drama (Egri, 1960). 170 CHAPTER 6 affect brought about by the instantiation of a negative type—through innate releasers, say—can be broken off suddenly or in gradual stages, preceded by subtle clues. Something like this occurs in A Star is Born (1954), where the somewhat mysterious figure of Norman Maine is introduced, a disagreeable lush with a creepy manner. Individuation—and perhaps a degree of personalization as well—leads to the integration of a number of traits that cohere around an ultimately wholly good intention. Then there is the Elephant Man, whose hideous appearance is only revealed to us after several carefully constructed scenes in which we are only allowed a view of a mysterious creature, whose face is heavily shrouded. The viewer is then gradually encouraged to conquer aversion and to discover, with Dr. Treves, the intentionality, sensitivity, and intelligence of the monster (The Elephant Man, 1980). One comes to feel compassion and ultimately admiration for a man who has endured so much at the hands of people who look normal but behave like monsters. A comparable mechanism operates in many whodunnits of the Le Corbeau (1943), Death on the Nile (1978) and Murder on the Orient Express (1974) genre; type categorizations display a dynamics that continually shifts suspicion from one character to the other: Here's the kind of person who must have done it.96 In retrospect, of course, there must be at least a few features discernible in the introduction of the character that are compatible with the ultimate categorization. Examples of a total reversal of affect are rare. Kunda, Miller, and Claire (1990) recently demonstrated that presenting fictional characters with conflicting social stereotypical characteristics automatically leads to a great deal of causal reasoning and the construction of causal narratives. This, in turn, gives rise to the formation of new attributes, that is, traits that were not part of the original stimulus. Examples of conflicting combination stimuli include the blind marathon runner, the leftist businessman, and the carpenter graduated from Harvard. The more unusual the combination of characteristics, the more likely they are to trigger such reasoning. Traditional film narratives, too, often present combinations of more or less conflicting clues to the nature of a particular character. This leads to the construction of causal assumptions that can be used to prefigure the expected revelations. Other investments are rooted in the conflict between characters and stereotypes. Cultural sex stereotypes portray women as virtuous and docile. Pussy Galore, assistant to Goldfinger (Goldfinger, 1964), is the very antithesis of that image. She is on the side of the baddies, presides over a squadron of lady pilots in true military fashion, and fails to evince any interest in James Bond, whom she floors with sheer physical force. The viewer hopes she will % There are, of course, interesting variations on this process. In Le Corheau, for example, the ultimate conclusion is that anyone could have done it, while in Murder on the Orient Express, all suspects have done it. CHARACTER STRUCTURES, EMPATHY, AND INTEREST 171 side with 007, and ultimately she does; the point is that initially almost the only grounds for such hope are provided by the stereotypical view of women. In the same way, deep down Ninotchka (1939) can never be the real communist woman who refuses to allow herself to be seduced by a man. Again, the investment is a form of hope, based on a stereotype under pressure. If it is empirically realistic to apply impression formation theory to the categorization of film personages —and this would have to be established experimentally—then it is easy to see how empathetic emotions originate. Categorization and individuation of characters clearly affect the emotional meaning for the viewer of situations involving that character. For one thing, the relevance component of the situational meaning structure is heightened, because the fate of the characters in the categories "hero" and "villain" calls upon the sympathy and value concerns of the viewer. The first result of categorization is that the viewer recognizes the most important objectives and concerns of the characters and either endorses or rejects them. Subsequently, events befalling the characters evoke emotion in the viewer because they are meaningful for these characters in view of their concerns. The misfortunes of the villain are welcome, those of the hero unwelcome, in that they are either in accordance with or run counter to the wishes of the characters. The events and outcomes of the plot may be seen as more or less desirable from the standpoint of both the character and the viewer, to borrow a term from Ortony, Glore, and Collins (1988). A second result of the categorization of characters is enhanced reality. The more depth there is to the individuation of a character, that is, the sharper the differentiation in terms of subtypes, the more real the character is and the higher the reality parameter of the situational meaning structure. REFERENCES Apter, M. J. (1984). Reversal theory, cognitive synergy and the arts. In W. R. Grozier & A. J. Chapman (Eds.), Cognitive processes in the perception of art (pp. 411-426). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland. Aristotle (1972). Poetics. (Trans. M. E. Hubbard). In D. A. Russell & M. Winterbottom (Eds.), Ancient literary criticism (pp. 85-112). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press. Arnheim, R. (1933). Film. London: Faber. Arnheim, R. (1958/1983). Film as art. London: Faber & Faber. Arnheim, R. (1971). Entropy and art. An essay on disorder and order. Berkeley: University of California Press. Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality (Vols. 1-2). New York: Columbia University Press. Aronson, E. (1961). The effect of effort on the attractiveness of rewarded and unrewarded stimuli. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 375-380. Atkin, C. K. (1985). Informational utility and selective exposure to entertainment media. In D. Zillmann & J. Bryant (Eds.), Selective exposure to communication (pp. 63-92). Hills-dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Atkinson, J. W. (1969). Change of activity: A new focus for the theory of motivation. In T. Mi-schel (Ed.), Human action (pp. 106-133). New York: Academic Press. Auerbach, E. (1946). Mimesis: Dargestellte Wirklichkeit in der abendldndischen Literatur [Mimesis: Represented reality in western literature]. Bern, Switzerland: Francke. Aumont, J., Bergala, A., Marie, M., & Vernet, M. (1983). Esthetique dufilm [Esthetics of film]. Paris: Nathan. Austin, B. (1986, Spring). Motivations for movie audiences. Communication Quarterly, 34, 115-126. Austin, B. (1988). Immediate seating. A look at movie audiences. Belmont: Wadsworth. Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression. An essay on emotion. New York: Springer. Balazs, B. (1938). Zur Kunstphilosophie des Films [On the philosophy of film as art]. Das Wort, 3, 104-119. Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bannerman, J., & Lewis, J. M. (1977). College students1 attitudes toward movies. Journal of Popular Film, 6, 126-139. Baron, R. A. (1977). Human aggression. New York: Plenum Press. Baron, R. A., & Bell, P. A. (1977) Sexual arousal and aggression by males: Effects of type of erotic stimuli and prior provocation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 79-87. Baron, R. M. (1980). Contrasting approaches to social knowing: An ecological perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 590-600. Baron, R. M. (1991, August). The implications of treating personality dispositions as social affordances. Paper presented at the 6th International Conference on Event Perception and Action, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Barsalou, L. W. (1991). Deriving categories to achieve goals. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 27, pp. 1-64). New York: Academic Press. Barthes, R. (1966). Introduction a 1'analyse structurale des recits [Introduction to the structural analysis of stories]. Communications, 8, 1-27. Batson, C. D. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 65-122). New York: Academic Press. Baudry, J.-L. (1986). Ideological effects of the basic cinematic apparatus. In P. Rosen (Ed.), Narrative, apparatus, ideology (pp. 286-298). New York: Columbia University Press. (Original work published 1975) Bazin, A. (1958). Qu'est-ce que le cinema? Tome I. Ontologie et langage. Paris, France: Cerf. REFERENCES 25 5 Bazin, A. (1959). Qu'est-ce que le cinema? Tome II. Le cinema et les autres arts. Paris, France: Gerf. Bazin, A. (1961). Qu'est-ce que le cinema? Tome III. Cinema et sociologie. Paris, France: Gerf. Bazin, A. (1962). Qu'est-ce que le cinema? Tome IV. Une esthetiqiie de la realite. Paris, France: Gerf. Beckerman, B. (1970). Dynamics of drama. Theory and method of analysis. New York: Knopf. Bellour, R. (1973). L'evidence et le code [The obvious and the code]. In D. Noguez (Ed.), Cinema: Theories, lectures, (pp. 219-227). Paris: Klincksieck. Bellour, R. (1979). Segmenter, analyser. In R. Bellour, L'analyse dufilm (pp. 247270). Paris, France: Albatros. (Original work published 1976) Bellour, R. (1979). Sur Tespace einematographique. In R. Bellour, L'analyse dufilm (pp. 64-72). Paris, France: Albatros. Berlyne, D. E. (I960). Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill. Berlyne, D. E. (19(>2). Uncertainly and epistcmic curiosity. British Journal of Psychology, 53, 27-34. Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Perception and Psychophysics, 8, 279-286. Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: AppletonCentury-Crofts. Berlyne, D. E. (1974). Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. New York: Wiley. Berlyne, D. E. (1978). Curiosity and learning. Motivation and Emotion, 2, 97-175. Berry, D. S., Kean, K. J., Misovich, S. J., & Baron, R. M. (1991). Quantized displays of human movement: A methodological alternative to the point-light display. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 81-97. Berry, D. S., & McArthur, L. Z. (1986). Perceiving character in faces: Impact of agerelated crano-facial facial changes on social perception. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 3-18. Berscheid, E. (1985). Interpersonal attraction. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (Vol. 2, 2nd ed., pp. 413-484). New York: Random House. Bever, T. G. (1986). The aesthetic basis for cognitive structures. In M. Brand & R. M. Harnish (Eds.), The representation of knowledge and belief (pp. 314-356). Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Birkhoff, G. D. (1933). Aesthetic measure. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bonitzer, P. (1982). Le champ aveugle [The blind field]. Essais sur le cinema. Paris: Gallimard. Booth, W. G. (1961). The rhetoric of fiction. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press. Borde, R., & Chaumeton, E. (1975). Panorama dufilm noir americain: 1941-1953 [Panorama of the American film noir: 1941-1953], Paris, France: Editions dAujourd'hui. Bordewijk-Knotter, J. M. (1988). Pinter appeal. A comparative study of responses to "The home coming." Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: University of Amsterdam. Bordwell, D. (1979). The art cinema as a mode of film practice. Film- Criticism, 4, 56-64. Bordwell, D. (1985). Narration in the fiction film. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press. Bordwell, D. (1989a). A case for cognitivism. Iris, 9, 11-40. Bordwell, D. (1989b). Making meaning. Inference and rhetoric in the interpretation of cinema. Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press. Bordwell, D., Staiger, J., & Thompson, K. (1985). The classical Hollywood cinema: Film style and mode of production. New York: Columbia University Press. Bordwell, D., & Thompson, K. (1986). Film art: An introduction (2nd ed.). New York: Knopf. Boselie, F. (1982). Overvisuele schoonheidservaring [On the visual experience of beauty]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Catholic University Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Bourdieu, P. (1979). La distinction. Critique sociale dujugement [Distinction. A critique of social judgment). Paris, France: Minuit. 256 REFERENCES Bower, G. H. (1978). Experiments on story comprehension and recall. Discourse Processes, 1, 211-231. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148. Bower, G. H., & Cohen, P. R. (1982). Emotional influences in memory and thinking: Data and theory. In M. S. Clark & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Affect and Cognition (pp. 291-331). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Branigan, E. (1992). Narrative comprehension and film. London & New York: Routledge Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R (1962). Explorations in cognitive dissonance. New York: Wiley Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in Social Cognition (Vol. I, pp. 1-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Brewer, W. E, & Lichtenstein, E. W. (1981). Event schemas, story schemas, and story grammars. In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol. 9, pp. 363379). Hills-dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Brewer, W. E, & Lichtenstein, E. H. (1982). Stories are to entertain: A structural-affect theory of stories. Journal of Pragmatics, 6, 473-486. Britton, B. K., Glynn, S. M., Meyer, B. J. E, & Penland, M. J.(1982). Effects of text structure on use of cognitive capacity during reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 51-61. Britton, B. K., Graesser, R. C., Glynn, S. M., Hamilton, T., & Penland, M. J., (1983). Use of cognitive capacity in reading: Effects of variations in surface features of text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 5, 318-331. Brook, P. (1976). The melodramatic imagination. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Brooks, V, & Hochberg, J. (1960). A psychophysical study of cuteness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 11, 205. Browne, N. (1975). The spectator-in-the-text: The rhetoric of stagecoach. Film Quarterly, 29, 26-38. Brunei; J. S. (1957). Perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 12-52. Brunetiere, E (1914). The law of the drama. New York: Dramatic Museum of Columbia University. (Original work published 1894) Brunsdon, C. (Ed.).(1986). Films for women. London: British Film Institute. Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments (2nd ed.). Berkeley: University of California Press. Bryant, J., & Zillmann, D. (1977). The mediating effect of the intervention potential of comunications on displaced agressiveness and retaliatory behavior. In B. D. Ruben (Ed.), Communication Yearbook (Vol. I, pp. 291-306). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. Bryant, J., & Zillmann, D. (1984). Using television to alleviate boredom and stress: Selective exposure as a function of induced excitational states. Journal of Broadcasting, 28, 120. Burch, N. (1979). To the distant observer. Berkeley: University of California Press. Buckland, W. (in press). Relevance and cognition: Towards a pragmatics of unreliable filmic narration. In J. Miiller (Ed.), Proceedings of the Conference on Film Pragmatics. Miinster, Germany: Nodus. Calhoun, C., & Solomon, R. C. (Eds.). (1984). What is an emotion? Classic readings in philosophical psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. Cantor, J. (1991). Fright responses to mass media productions. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Responding to the screen. Reception and reaction processes (pp. 169-197). Hills-dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cantor, J., Ziemke, D., & Sparks, G. G. (1984). Effect of forewarning on emotional response to a horror film. Journal of Broadcasting, 28, 21-31. Cantor, J. R., Zillman, D., & Bryant, J. (1975). Enhancement of experienced sexual arousal in response to erotic stimuli through misattribution of unrelated residual excitation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 69-75. REFERENCES 257 Garello, G., Grosofsky, A., Shaw, R. E., Pittenger, J. B., & Mark, L. S. (1989). Attractiveness of facial profiles is a function of distance from archetype. Ecological Psychology, 1, 227-251. Carroll, N. (1988a). Philosophical problems of classical film theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Carroll, N. (1988b). Mystifying movies: Fads and fallacies in contemporary film theory. New York: Columbia University Press. Carroll, N. (1990). The philosophy of horror. Or paradoxes of the heart. New York: Routleclge. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origin and functions of positive and negative affect: Control-proces view. Psychological Review, 97, 19-35. Charniak, E. (1972). Toward a model of children's story comprehension (Report No. Al-TR266). Cambridge, MA: MIT, Art. Int. Lab. Chatman, S. (1978). Story and discourse. Narrative structure in fiction and film. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Child, I. L. (1946). Children's preference for goals easy or difficult to obtain. Psvchological Monographs, 60 (4, Whole No. 280). Child, I. L. (1969). Esthetics. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eels.), Handbook of Social Psychology1 (Vol. Ill, pp. 853-916). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Child, I. L. (1978). Aesthetic theories. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of Perception (Vol. 10, pp. 11-131). New York: Academic Press. Choffray, J. M., & Pas, B. (1980). Determinants of market success for commercial movies. Der Markt, 73, 3-13. Christ, W. G., & Mecloff, N. J. (1984). Affective state and selective exposure to and use of television. Journal of Broadcasting, 28, 1—20. Cogan, R., Cogan, I)., \Yaltz, W., & McCue, M. (1987). Effects of laughter and relaxation on discomfort thresholds. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 10(2), 139-144. Cohen-Seat, G. (1958). Essais sur les principes d'une pliilosophie du cinema: Notions fonda-mentales et vocabulaire dcfilmologie [Essays on the principles of a philosophy of the cinema. Fundamental notions and a vocabulary for the study of film]. Paris, France: PUF. Colby, K. M. (1975). Artificial paranoia. New York: Pergamon. Comisky, P., & Bryant, J. (1982). Factors involved in generating suspense. Human Communication Research, 9, 49-58. Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684. Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Human Memory, 104, 268-294. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and curiosity. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1988). Optimal experience. Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Culler, J. (1975). Structuralist poetics: Structuralism, linguistics and the study of literature. Ithaca: Cornell University Press. Cupchik, G. C., & Gebotys, R. J. (1988). The experience of time, pleasure and interest during aesthetic episodes. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 6(1), 1-12. Cutting, J. E. (1987). Rigidity in cinema seen from the front row, side aisle. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 13, 323-334. DAndrade, R. G. (1984). Character terms and cultural models. In J. W. D. Dougherty (Ed.), Directions in cognitive anthropology (pp. 321-343). Urbana and Chicago: University of Ilinois Press. Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., & Friesen, W. F. (1990). Approachwithdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology I. Journal of Personality arid Social Psychology, 58, 330-341. 258 REFERENCE S Davis, M. H., Hull, J. G., Young, R. D., & Warren, G. G. (1987). Emotional reactions to dramatic film stimuli: The influence of cognitive & emotional empathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 126-133. Davis, D. K., & Woodall, W. G. (1982). Uses of TV news: Gratification or edification? Paper presented at the annual meetings of the International Communication Association Convention, Boston, MA. Dawkins, R. (1978). The selfish gene. London: Granada. cle Beaugrande, R. (1982). The story of grammars and the grammar of stories. Journal of Pragmatics, 6, 383-422. de Groot, A. D. (1965). Methodologie [Methodology]. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton. cle Jonge, P. (1994). De heleving van theatrale produkten: Over empathie en identificatie [Experiencing theatrical products: On empathy and identification). Unpublished technical report, Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands. de Kuyper, E. (1984). Filmische hartstochten [Filmic passions]. Weesp, The Netherlands An-twerpen, Belgium: Het Wereldvenster/Standaarduitgeverij. cle Lauretis, T. (1984). Alice doesn't: Feminism, semiotics, cinema. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. den Uyl, M. J., & Frijda, N. H. (1984). Mood, emotion and action: A concern realization system. In Proceedings of the 6th Cognitive Science Conference (pp. 137-141). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. de Rivera, J. (1977). A structural theory of the emotions. Psychological Issues, 10, (Whole Vol.). New York: International Universities Press. De Sousa, R. (1987). The rationality of emotion. Cambridge & London: MIT Press. de Wied, M. A. (1991). The role of temporal structures in the production of suspense and duration experience. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: University of Amsterdam. de Wied, M. A. (1995). The role of temporal expectancies in the production of film suspense. Poetics, 23, (1-2), 107-123. de Wied, M. A. (in press). The role of temporal expectancies in the production of film suspense. In P. Vorderer, H. J. Wulff, & M. Friedrichsen (Eds.), Analysis of suspense. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaurn Associates. de Wied, M. A., Zillmann, D., & Ordman, V. (1995). The role of empathic distress in the enjoyment of cinematic tragedy. Poetics, 23(1-2), 107-124. Dorner, D., & Vehrs, W. (1975). Asthetisehe Befriedigung und Unbestimmtheitsreduktion [Esthetic pleasingness and uncertainty reduction]. Psychological Research, 37, 321-334. Dovidio, J. F., Schroeder, D. A., & Allen, J. L. (1990). Specificity of empathy induced helping: Evidence for altruistic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 249-260. Duclos, S. E., Laird, J. D., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., Stern, L., & van Lighten, O. (1989). Emotion-specific effects of facial expressions and postures of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 100-108. Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58 (Whole No. 5). Durgnat, R. (1967). Films and feeling. London/Cambridge, MA: Faber & Faber/MITPress. Duvignaud J. (1965/1973) Sociologie du theatre. [Sociology of literature and drama]. Paris: PUF. (Original work published 1973) d'Ydewalle, G., & Vanderbeeken, M. (1990). Perceptual and cognitive processing of editing rules in film. In R. Groner, G. d'Ydewalle, & R. Parnham (Eds.), From eye to mind: Information acquisition in perception, search and reading (pp. 129-139). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier/North-Holland Dyer, M. G. (1983). The role of affect in narratives. Cognitive Science, 7, 211-242. REFERENCES 259 Dyer, R. (1979). Stars. London: BFI. Eaton, M. (1981). Laughter in the dark. Screen, 22(2), 21-43. Eberwein, R. T. (1984). Film and the dream screen. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Efran, J, S., & Spangler, T. J. (1979). Why grown-ups cry. A two-factor theory and evidence from "The Miracle Worker." Motivation & Emotion, 3, 63-71. Egri, L. (1960). The art of dramatic writing. New York: Simon & Schuster. (Original work published 1946) Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99, 550-553. Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomous nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221 (4616), 1208-1210. Elias, N., & Dunning, E. (1986). The quest for excitement in leisure. In N. Elias & E. Dunning (Eds.), Quest for excitement: Sport and leisure in the civilising process (pp. 63-90). Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Ellis, J. (1982). Visible fictions. London: Routleclge. Ellsworth, P. G. (1991). Some implications of cognitive appraisal theories of emotion. In K. T. Strongman (Ed.), International Review of Studies in Emotion (Vol. 1, pp. 143162). Chieh-ester: Wiley. Eysenck, H. J. (1942). The experimental study of the "Good Gestalt." A new approach. Psychological Review, 49, 344-364. Eysenck, H. J. (1968). An experimental study of aesthetic preference for polygonal figures. Journal of General Psychology, 79, 3-17. Faber, R. J., O'Guinn, T. G., & Hardy, A. P. (1988). Art films in the suburbs. In B. A. Austin (Ed.), Current research in film audiences, economics and law (pp. 44-53). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Fechner, G. T. (1876). Vorschule der Asthetik [Preliminaries of esthetics]. Leipzig, Germany: Breitkopf und Hartel. Fenigstein, A., & Heycluk, R. G. (1985). Thought and action as determinants of media exposure. In D. Zillmann & J. Bryant (Eds.), Selective exposure to communication (pp. 113-139). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Feshbach, S. (1976). The role of fantasy in the response to television. Journal of Social Issues, 32, 71-84. Feshbach, S., & Singer, R. (1971). Television and aggression. San Francisco: JosseyBass. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row & Petersson. Festinger, L. (1961). The psychological effect of insufficient reward. American Psychologist, 16, 1-11. Fisher, W. A., & Byrne, D. (1978). Individual differences in affective, evaluative and behavioral responses to an erotic film. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 8, 355-365. Fiske, J. (1987). Television culture. London & New York: Methuen. Fiske, S. T. (1982). Schema triggered affect: Application to social perception. In M. S. Clark & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Affect and Cognition (pp. 55-78). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Fiske, S. T. (1988). Compare and contrast: Brewer's dual proces model and Fiske et al.'s continuum model. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in Social Cognition: Vol. I. A dual process model of impression formation (pp. 65-76). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum model of impression formation from category-based to individuality responses: Influences of informatica and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 1-79). New York: Academic Press. Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press. Fokkema, D. W. (1989). The concept of convention in literary theory and empirical research. In 260 REFERENCE S D'Haen, T., Griibel, R., & Lethen, H. (Eds.), Convention and innovation in literature (pp. 89-101). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Coping as a mediator of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 466-475. Forgas, J. P. (1982). Episode cognition: Internal representations of interaction routines. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 15 (pp. 59-101). New York: Academic Press. Forster, E. M. (1927). Aspects of the novel. London: Arnold. Fowles, J. (1992). Why viewers watch (rev. ed.). Newbury Park: Sage. Freud, S. (1900). Die Traumdeutung [The interpretation of dreams]. Leipzig, Germany: Deuticke. Freud, S. (1908). Der Dichter und das Phantasieren [The poet and phantasy]. Neue Revue, 1, 716-724. Freud, S. (1913). Totem und Tabu. Leipzig, Germany: Heller. (Original work published 1912) Freud, S. (1919). Das Unheimliche [The uncanny]. Imago, 5, 297-324. Freud, S. (1940). Der Witz und seine Beziehung zum Unbewussten [The joke and its relations to the unconscious]. In Gesammelte Werke, Vol. 6. London: Imago. (Original work published 1905) Freud, S. (1942). Psychopathic characters on the stage. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 11, 459-464. Freytag, G. (1911). Die Technik des Dramas. Leipzig, Germany: Hirzel. Frijda, N. H. (1956). Het begrijpen van gelaatsexpressies [Understanding facial expressions]. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Van Oorschot. Frijda, N. H. (1981). Menselijke natuur en informatie als determinanten van gedrag [Human nature and information as determinants of behavior]. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologic, 36, 135-150. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotions, cognitive structure and action tendency. Cognition an Emotion, 1, 115-144. Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, 349-358. Frijda, N. H. (1989). Aesthetic emotions and reality. American Psychologist, 44, 15461547. Frijda, N. H. (1993). Appraisal and beyond: The issue of cognitive determinants of emotion [Special issue]. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 357-387. Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212-228. Frijda, N. H., Mesquita, B., Sonnemans, J., & van Goozen, S. (1991). The duration of affective phenomena or emotions, sentiments and passions. In K. T. Strongman (Ed.), International Review of Studies on Emotion (Vol. 1, pp. 187-226). Chichester: Wiley. Frijda, N. H., & Swagerman, J. (1987). Can computers feel? Cognition and Emotion, 1, 235-258. Frith, IL, & Robson, J. E. (1975). Perceiving the language of films. Perception, 4, 97103. Gaver, W. W., & Handler, G. (1987). Play it again Sam: On liking music. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 259-282. Gee, J. P., & Grosjean, F. (1984). Empirical evidence for narrative structure. Cognitive Science, 8, 59-85. Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., & Robertson, R. R. W. (1992). Do readers mentally represent characters' emotional states? Cognition and Emotion, 6, 89111. Gerrig, R. J. (1989). Suspense in the absence of uncertainty. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 633-648. Gerrig, R. J., & Prentice, D. A. (1987, August). Persuasion by fiction. Paper presented at the 95th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, New York City. REFERENCES 261 Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gilbert, D. T., Tofarodi, R. W., & Malone, P. S. (1993). You can't not believe everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 221-234. Gilligan, S. G., & Bower, G. H. (1984). Cognitive consequences of emotional arousal. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotion, cognition & behavior (pp. 547-588). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Godkewitsch, M. (1974). Correlates of humor: Verbal and nonverbal aesthetic reactions as functions of semantic distance with adjective-noun pairs. In D. Berlyne (Ed.), Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation (pp. 279-304). New York: Wiley. Goldman, M., & Hagen, M. A. (1978). The forms of caricature: Physiognomy and political bias. Studies on the Anthropology 1 of Visual Communication, 2, 30-36. Gombrich, E. II. (1977). Art and illusion (5th eel.). London: Phaidon Press. (Original work published 1960) Gombrich, E. H. (1970). The mask and the face: The perception of physiognomic likeness in life and art. In E. H. Gombrich, J. Hochberg, & M. Black (Eds.), Art, perception and reality (pp. 1-46). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Graesser, A. C. (1981). Prose comprehension beyond the word. New York: Springer. Graesser, A. C., & Clark, L. F. (1985). Structures and procedures of implicit knowledge. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Graesser, A. C., Higginbotham, M. W., Robertson, S. P., & Smith, W. R. (1978). A natural inquiry into the National Enquirer: Self-induced versus task-induced reading comprehension. Discourse Processes, 1, 355-372. Granberg, D., & King, M. (1980). Cross4agged panel analysis of the relation between attraction and perceived similarity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 573581. Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Greenwald, A. G. (1981). Self and memory. In G. H. Bower (Eel.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 201-237). New York: Academic Press. Greimas, A. J. (1966). Elements pour une theorie de 1'interpretation du recit mythique [Elements of a theory of the interpretation of mythical stories]. Communications, 8, 2859. Grice, P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 3. Speech acts (pp. 41-58). New York: Academic Press. Grice, P. (1978). Further notes on logic and conversation. In P. Cole (Ed.), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 9. Pragmatics (pp. 113-128). New York: Academic Press. Gunning, T. (1989). An aesthetic of astonishment. Early film and the (In-)credulous spectator. Art & Text, 34, 34-45. Gunter, B. (1985). Determinants of television viewing preferences. In D. Zillmann & J. Bryant (Eds.), Selective exposure to communication (pp. 108-109). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erl-baum Associates. Gunter, B., & Wober, M. (1983). Television viewing and public trust. British Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 174-176. Giinzenhauser, R. (1965). Das asthetische Mass Birkhoffs in informationsasthetischer Sicht [Birkhoff's esthetic measure from the point of view of information esthetics]. In H. Ronge (Ed.), Kunst und Kybernetik (pp. 37-55). Koln, Germany: Dumont. Gurvitch, A. (1973). The sociology of the theatre. In E. Burns & T. Burns (Eds.), Sociology of literature and drama (pp. 71-81). Harmondsworth: Penguin. (Original work published 1956) Hagen, M. A., & Perkins, D. (1983). A refutation of the hypothesis of the superfidelity of caricatures relative to photographs. Perception, 12, 55-61. 262 REFERENCE S Hamilton, O. G., & Rose, T. L. (1980). Illusory correlation and the maintainance of stereotype beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 382-845. Handel, L. M. (1950). Hollywood looks at its audience: A report of film audience research. Ur-bana: University of Illinois Press. Harvey, W. J. (1965). Character and the novel. New York: Oxford University Press. Hearst, E., & Jenkins, H. M. (1974). Sign-tracking: The stimulus-reinforcer relation and directed action. Austin, TX: The Psychonomic Society. Heath, S. (1981). Questions of cinema. London: MacMillan Press. Heider, F., & Simrnel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243-259. Hekkert, P., & van Wieringen, P. G. W. (1990). Complexity and prototypicality as determinants of the appraisal of cubist paintings. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 483-496. Hidi, S., & Baird, W. (1986). Interestingness: A neglected variable in discourse processing. Cognitive Science, 10, 179-194. Hilgard, E. R. (1962). Impulsive versus realistic thinking: An examination of the distinction between primary and secondary processes. Psychological Bulletin, 59, 477-488. Hilgard, E. R. (1986). Divided consciousness (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Hochberg, J. (1970). Attention, organization and consciousness. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Attention: Contemporary theory and analysis (pp. 99-124). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Hochberg, J. (1972). The representation of things and people. In E. H. Gombrich, J. Hochberg, & M. Black (Eds.), Art, perception and reality (pp. 43-44). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Hochberg, J. (1978). Perception (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Hochberg, J. (1986). Representation of motion and space in video and cinematic displays. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of Perception and Human Performance: Vol. L Sensory processes and perception (pp. 22/122/64). New York: Wiley. Hochberg, J., & Brooks, V. (1978). Film cutting and visual momentum. In J. W. Senders, D. F. Fischer, & R. A. Monty (Eds.), Eye movements and the higher psychological functions (pp. 293-313). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Hoffman, M. L. (1982). Development of prosocial behavior: Empathy and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 281-313). New York: Academic Press. Hoffman, M. L. (1984). Interaction of affect and cognition in empathy. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 103-131). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hoffner, G., & Cantor, J. (1991). Perceiving and responding to mass media characters. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Responding to the screen. Reception and reaction processes (pp. 135-168). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Horowitz, M. J. (1976) Stress response syndromes. New York: Jason Aronson. Houston, C. E., & Wegner, D. M. (1993). Believe it or not: Ironic processes in belief formation. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Arlington, VA. Huitema, J. S., Dopkins, S., Klin, C. M., & Myers, J. L. (1993). Connecting goals and actions during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 1053-1060. Humphrey, N. K. (1973). The illusion of beauty. Perception, 2, 420-439. Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of Social Cognition (pp. 179-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory and behavior: A cognitive loop. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1-12. REFERENCES 263 Izard, G. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press, Izard, G. E. (1989). The structure and function of emotions: Implications for cognition, motivation, and personality. In I. S. Cohen (Ed.), The G. Stanley Hall Lecture Series (Vol. 9, pp. 35-73). Washington DC: American Psychological Association. Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion-cognition relations. Psychological Review, 99, 561-565. Izard, G. E. (1993). Four systems for emotion activation: Cognitive and noncognitive processes. Psychological Review, 100, 68-90. James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology: Vol. 2. New York: Dover. (Original work published 1890) Jarvie, I. C. (1970). Movies and reality. New York: Basic Books. Johansson, G. (1950). Configurations in event perception. Uppsala, Sweden: Almquist £ Wiksell. Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Alental models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Jose, P. E., & Brewer, W. F. (1984). Development of story liking: Character identification, suspense and outcome resolution. Developmental Psychology, 20, 911-924. Jowett, G., & Linton, M. J. (1989). Movies as mass communications. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Kaplan, E. A. (1983). Women and film: Both sides of the camera. New York: Methuen. Kaplan, S., & Kaplan, R. (1982). Cognition and environment: Functioning in an uncertain world. NY: Praeger. Kay, P. (1983). Three properties of the ideal reader (Tech. Rep. No. 7). Berkeley CA: Berkeley Cognitive Science Program. Kermode, F. (1967). The sense of an ending: Stories in the theory of fiction. New York: Oxford University Press. Kiewiet, R., & Tan, E. (in preparation). Effects of velocity, temporal contingency and shape on perceived causality and judgment of expressive qualities. Kintsch, W. (1980). Learning from text, levels of comprehension, or: Why anyone would read a story anyway. Poetics, 9, 87-98. Klinger, E. (1971). Structure and function of fantasy. New York: Wiley. Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incentives in people's lives. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Knowles, P. L., & Smith, D. L. (1979). The ecological perspective applied to social perception: Revision of a working paper. Journal of the Theory of Social Behaviour, 12, 53-78. Konijn, E. (1994). Acteurs spelen emoties [Actors acting emotions]. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Boom. Koriat, A., Melkman, R., Averill, J. R., & Lazarus, R. S., (1972). The self-control of emotional reactions to a stressful film. Journal of Personality, 40, 601-619. Kracauer, S. (1947). From Caligari to Hitler. A psychological history of the German film. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Kracauer, S. (1960). Theory of film. The redemption of physical reality. New York: Oxford University Press. Kreitler, II., & Kreitler, S. (1972). Psychology of the Arts. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. Kretsinger, E. A. (1952). Gross bodily movement as an index of audience interest. Speech Monographs, 19, 244-348. Kubovy, M. (1986). The psychology of perspective and Renaissance art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Kuhn, A. (1982). Women's pictures: Feminism and cinema. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Kunda, Z., Miller, D. T., & Claire, T. (1990). Combining social concepts: The role of causal reasoning. Cognitive Science, 14, 551-578. Lang, P. J. (1984). Cognition in emotion: Concept and action. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. 264 REFERENCE S Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition and behavior (pp. 192-226). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lang, R. (1989). American film melodrama. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Langer, S. K. (1953). Feeling and form. New York: Scribner. Langlois, J. H., & Roggman, L. A. (1990). Attractive faces are only average. Psychological Science, 1(2), 115-121. Lanser, S. S. (1981). The narrative act. Point of view in prose fiction. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Lasher, M. D., Carroll, J. M., & Bever, T. G. (1983). The cognitive basis of aesthetic experience. Leonardo, 16, 196-199. Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press. Lazarus, R. S., & Alfert, E. (1964). Short-circuiting of threat by experimentally altering cognitive appraisal. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 195-205. Lazarus, R. S., Speisman, J. C., Mordkoff, A. M., & Davidson, L. A. (1962). A laboratory study of psychological stress produced by a motion picture film. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76(34, Whole No. 553). Lazier, G., Zahn D., & Bellinghiere, J. (1974). Empirical analysis of dramatic structure. Speech Monographs, 41, 381-390. Leech, G. N. (1983). Principles of Pragmatics. London: Longman. Lehnert, W. G. (1981). Plot units and narrative summarization. Cognitive Science, 4, 293-331. Lehnert, W. G. (1982). Plot Units: A narrative summarization strategy. In W. G. Lehnert & M. H. Ringle (Eds.), Strategies for natural language processsing (pp. 375-414). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Lehnert, W. G., & Vine, E. W. (1987). The role of affect in narrative structure. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 299-322. Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum. Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P. E., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity, Psychophysiology, 27, 363-384. Leventhal, H. (1979). A perceptual-motor processing model of emotion. In P. Pliner, K. Blanken-stein, & I. M. Spiegel (Eds.), Perception of emotion in self and others (Vol 5, pp. 1-46). New York: Plenum. Levy, S. G., & Fenley, W. F. (1979). Audience size and likelihood and intensity of response during a humorous movie. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 13, 409412. Lewin, B. (1950). Inferences from the dream screen. The Yearbook of Psychoanalysis, 6, 104-117. Lewis, M. (1964a). Some non-decremental effects of effort. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 57, 367-372. Lewis, M. (1964b). The effect of effort on value: An exploratory study of children. Child Development, 356, 1337-1342. Lichtenstein, A., & Rosenfeld, L. B. (1983). Uses and misuses of gratifications research: An explication of media functions. Communication Research, 10, 97-109. Light, L. L., Hollander, S., & Kayra-Stuart, F. (1981). Why attractive people are harder to remember. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 269-276. Lipps, T. (1906). Leitfaden der Psychologic. Leipzig, Germany: Engelmann. Loewenstein, G. F., & Prelec, D. (1993). Preferences for sequences of outcomes. Psychological Review, 100, 91-108. Long, D. L., Golding, J. M., Graesser, A. C., & Clark, L. F. (1990). Goal, event, and state inferences: An investigation of inference generaton during story comprhension. In A. C. Graesser & G. H. Bower (Eds.), Inferences and text comprehension (pp. 89107). San Diego: Academic Press. REFERENCES 26 5 Lucariello, J. (1994). Situational irony: A concept of events gone awry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 129-145. Lynch, F. D. (1972). Clozentropy: A technique for studying audience response to films. New York: Arno Press. MacCabe, G. (1976). Theory and film: Principles of realism and pleasure. Screen, 17, 3. Handler, G. (1982). The structure of value: Accounting for taste. In M. S. Clark & S. T. Fiske (Eds.) Affect and Cognition (pp. 3-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Handler, G. (1984). Mind and body. New York: Norton. Handler, J. M. (1982). Some uses and abuses of a story grammar. Discourse Processes, 5, 305-318. Handler, J. M. (1984). Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Harkus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology1, 35, 63-78. Harkus, H., & Nevius, P. (1987). Possible selves: The interface between motivation and the self-concept. In K. Yardley & T. lioness (Eds.), Self & identity. Psychosocial perspectives (pp. 157-172). Chichester: Wiley. Hartin, H. (1990). On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 669697. Martin, W. (1986). Recent theories of narrative. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Martindale, C., & Moore, K. (1988). Priming, prototypicality, and preference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 661-670. Matthews, B. (1910). A study of the drama. London: Longman & Green. Me Arthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Toward an ecological theory of social perception. Psychological Review, 90, 215-238. McClelland, D. C. (1955). The psychology of mental content reconsidered. Psychological Review, 62, 297-302. HcGhee, P. E. (1971). Development of the humor response: A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 328-348. HcGhee, P. E. (1972). On the cognitive origins of incongruity humor: Fantasy assimilation versus reality assimilation. In J. H. Goldstein & P. E. HcGhee (Eds.), The psychology of humor (pp. 61-80). London: Academic Press. McGuiness, D., & Pribram, K. H. (1980). The neuropsychology of attention: Emotional and motivational contrast. In M. C. Witrock (Ed.), The brain and psychology (pp. 95-139). New York: Academic Press. McGuire, W. J. (1974). Psychological motives and communication gratification. In J. G. Blum-ler & E. Katz (Eds.), The uses of mass communications. Current perspectives on gratifications research (pp. 167-196). Beverly Hills: Sage. HcKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1990). Dimensions of inference. In A. C. Graesser & G. H. Bower (Eds.), Inferences and text comprehension (pp. 313-328). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. HcKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Inference during reading. Psychological Review, 99, 440-466 HcQuail, D. (1987). Mass communication theory (2nd ed.). London: Sage. Headowcroft, J., & Zillmann, D. (1984). The influences of hormonal fluctuations on women's selection and enjoyment of television programs. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, Gainesville, FL. Meier, N. C. (1950). The Meier audience response machine. American Journal of Psychology, 63, 87-89. Mervis, C. B. (1980). Category structure and the development of categorization. In R. J. Spiro, B. C. Bruce, & W. F. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. 279-303). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrrence Erlbaum Associates. Hessaris, P. (1994). Visual literacy: Image, mind and reality. Boulder: Westview Press. 266 REFERENCES Metz, G. (1968). Essays sur la signification au cinema. Paris: Klincksieck. Metz, G. (1971). Langage et cinema. Paris: Larousse. Metz, G. (1972) Essais sur la signification au cinema (Vol. 2). Paris, France: Klincksieck. Metz, G. (1975a). Le signifiant imaginaire. Communications, 23, Psychanalyse et Cinema, 3-55. Metz, G. (1975b). Le film de fiction et son spectateur. Communications, 23, Psychanalyse et Cinema, 108-135. Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Miall, D. S. (1989). Beyond the schema given: Affective comprehension of literary narratives. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 55-78. Michotte, A. (1946). La perception de la causalite. Louvain, Belgium: Presses Universitaire de Louvain. Michotte, A. (1950). The emotions regarded as functional connections. In M. L. Reymert (Eel.), Feelings and emotions: The Mooseheart Symposion (pp. 114-125). New York: McGrawHill. Michotte, A. (1991). The character of "reality" of cinematographic projections (A. P. Costall, Trans.). In G. Thines, A. Costall, & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Michotte's experimental phenomenology of pereception (pp. 197-209). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. (Original work published 1948) Michotte, A. (1991). The emotional involvemnt of the spectator in the action represented in a film: Toward a theory (A. P. Costall, Trans.). In G. Thines, A. Costall, & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Michotte's experimental phenomenology of perception (pp. 209-217). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrrence Erlbaum Associates. Original work published (1953) Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In J. McV. Hunt (Eel.), Personality and the behavioral disorders. (Vol. 1, pp. 431-465). New York: Ronald Press. Miller, R. S. (1987). Empathic embarrassment: Situational and personal determinants of reactions to the embarrassment of another. Journal of Personality and Social Psycholog\\ 53, 1061-1069. Milner Davis, J. R. (1977). A structured approach to humour and farce. In A. J. Chapman & H. C. Foot (Eds.), It's a funny thing, humour (pp. 391-394). Oxford: Pergamon Press. Mitry, J. (1965). Esthetiqiie et psychologic du cinema. Tome 2: Les formes. Paris, France: Editions Universitaires. Moller, K. E. K., & Karppinen, P. (1983). Consumer motion picture choice. Journal of Economic Psychology, 4, 239-262. Morin, E. (1956). Le cinema, ou Vhomme. imaginaire [The cinema, or imaginary man]. Paris, France: Minuit. Morreal, J. (1983). Humor and emotion. American Philosophical Quarterly, 20, 297-305. Morrow, D. G., Bower, G. H., & Greenspan, S. L. (1989). Updating situation models during narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Learning, 28, 292-312. Morrow, D. G., Greenspan, S. L., & Bower, G. H. (1987) Accessibility and situation models in narrative understanding. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 165-187. Movshon, A. (1990). Visual processing of moving images. In H. Barlow, G. Blakemore, & M. Wes-ton-Smith (Eds.), Images and understanding (pp. 122-137). New York: Cambridge University Press. Moynihan, C., & Mehrabian, A. (1981). The psychological aesthetics of narrative forms. In H. I. Day (Ed.), Advances in intrinsic motivation and aesthetics (pp. 323-340). New York: Plenum. Mulvey, L. (1975). Visual pleasure and narrative cinema. Screen, 16(3), 6-18. Mulvey, L. (1981, Summer). On 'Duel in the sun'. Framework (15/16/17), 12-15. Miinsterberg, H. (1970). The film, a psychological study. New7 York: Dover. (Original work published 1916) Nachbar, J. (1974). Focus on the Western. Englewoods Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. REFERENCES 267 Neale, S. (1980). Genre. London: BFI. Neale, S., & Krutnik, F. (1990). Popular film and television comedy. London & New York: Routledge. Neisser, U., & Becklen, R. (1975). Selective looking: Attending to visually specified events. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 480-494. Nell, V. (1988). Lost in a book. New Haven, GT: Yale University Press. Newell, A., & Simon, H., (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Nomikos, M. S., Opton, E., Averill, J. R., & Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Surprise versus suspense in the production of stress reaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 204208. Nunnally, J. C. (1981). Explorations of exploration. In H. I. Day (Ed.), Advances in intrinsic motivation and aesthetics (pp. 87-129). New York: Plenum. Oatley, K. (1992). Best laid schemes: The psychology of emotion. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Oatley, K. (1995). A taxonomy and theory of the emotions of literary response and a theory of identification in fictional narrative. Poetics, 23(1-2), 53-74. Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspective. New York: Plenum. Ornstein, R. (1969). On the experience of time. Harmonclsworth: Penguin. Ortony, A. (1978). Remembering, understanding and representation. Cognitive Science, 2, 53-69. Ortony, A. (1979). Beyond literal similarity. Psychological Review, 86, 161-180. Ortony, A. (1980). Metaphor. In R. J. Spiro, B. C. Bruce, & W. F. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. 349-366). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. M. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Foss, M. A. (1987). The referential structure of the affective lexicon. Cognitive Science, 11, 361-384. Ortony, A. & Turner, T. J. (1990). What's basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97, 315-331. Palmer, J. (1987). The logic of the absurd. London: BFI. Palmgreen, P., Cook, P. L., Harvill J. G., & Helm, D. M. (1988). The motivational framework of moviegoing: Uses and avoidances of theatrical films. In B. A. Austin (Ed.), Current research in film: Audiences, economics and law (pp. 1-23). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Palmgreen, P., Wenner, L. A., & Rayburn, J. D. (1981). Gratification discrepancies and news program choice. Communication Research, 8, 451-478. Panksepp, J. (1982). Towards a general psychological theory of emotions. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 407-467. Panksepp, J. (1992). A critical role for affective neuroscience in resolving what is basic about basic emotions. Psychological Review, 99, 554-560. Panofsky, E. (1991). Perspective as symbolic form. New York: Zone Books. (Original work published 1927) Parish, J. R., & Pitts, M. R. (1976). The great gangster pictures. Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press. Perkins, V. F. (1972). Film as film. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin. Peters, J. M. (1977). Kijken naar beelden; psychologic van het mechanische beeld [Watching images: The psychology of the mechanic image]. Leuven, Belgium: Centrum voor Communi-catiewetenschappen. Peters, J. M. (1981). Pictorial signs and the language of film. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Rodopi. Peters, J. M. (1989). Hetfilmische denken [Filmic thinking]. Leuven, Belgium: Acco. Petro, P. (1989). Joyless streets. Women and melodramatic representation in Weimar Germany. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 268 REFERENCE S Pfeifer, R. (1982). Cognition and emotion: An information processing approach, (GIF Working Paper No. 436). Pittsburgh: Carnegie Mellon University. Pfister, M. (1977). Das Drama. Miinchen, Germany: Fink Verlag. Philippot, P. (1993). Inducing and assessing differentiated emotion-feeling states in the laboratory. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 171-193. Piet, S. (1987). What motivates stuntmen? Motivation and Emotion, 11, 195-213. Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper & Row. Plutehik, R. (1991). Emotions and evolution. In R. T. Strongman (Ed.), International review of studies on emotion (Vol. 1, pp. 37-58). Ghichester: Wiley. Polti, G. (1977). The thirty-six dramatic situations. Boston: The Writer, Inc. (Original work published 1921) Porfirio, R. G. (1976). No way out: Existential motifs in the film noir. Sight and Sound, 45(4), 212-217. Posner, M. I. (1975). Psychobiology of attention. In M. S. Gazzaniga & C. Blakemore (Eds.), Handbook of psychobiology (pp. 441-480). New York: Academic Press. Potter, W. J. (1988). Perceived reality in television effects research. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 32(1), 23-41. Powdermaker, H. (1950). Hollywood: The dream factory. Boston: Little, Brown & Co. Pratt, M. L. (1977). Toward a speech act theory of literary discourse. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Preminger, A. (Ed.). (1965). Encyclopedia of poetry and poetics. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Pribram, K. H., & McGuiness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation, and the effort in the control of attention. Psychological Review, 82, 116-149. Prokop, D. (1974). Soziologie des Films [Sociology of film]. Darmstadt Neuwied, Germany: Luchterhand Verlag. Pryor, J. B. (1986). The influence of different encoding upon the formation of illusory correlations and group processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 216-226. Purcell, A. T. (1984). The aesthetic experience and mundane reality. In W. R. Crozier & A. J. Chapman (Eds.), Cognitive processes in the perception of art (pp. 189-210). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Purves, A. C., & Beach, R. (1972). Literature and the reader: Research in response to literature, reading interest and the teaching of literature. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Piitz, P. (1970). Die Zeit im Drama. Zur Technik Dramatischer Spannung [Time in drama: On the technique of dramatic tension]. Gottingen, Germany: Vandenhoeck und Ruprecht. Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind's eye tells the mind's brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1-24. Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1984). Computation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Rabbie, J. M., & Visser, L. (1984). Dei'ndividuatie en de ontwikkeling van normen in groepen toeschouwers, een veldexperiment [ Deindividuation and the development of norms in groups of spectators: A field experiment]. In R. van der Vlist (Ed.), Sociale psychologic in Neder-land: Vol. 4. Samenleving en individuen (pp. 102-127). Deventer, The Netherlands: Van Loghum Slaterus. Ramirez, J., Bryant, J., & Zillmann, D. (1982). Effects of erotica on retaliatory behavior as a function of level of prior provocation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 971-978. Reisz, K., & Millar, G. (1968). The technique of film editing. London: Focal Press. Ritchie, R. G. (1986). Momentary affect and attention allocation. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 387-395. Rombouts, H. (1992). Edit verliefd [Really in love]. Meppel, The Netherlands: Boom. REFERENCES 269 Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27-71). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rosch, E., & Mervis, G. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573-605. Ruch, W. (1993). Exhilaration and humor. In M. Lewis & J. N. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (pp. 605-616). New York: The Guilford Press. Ruimsehotel, D. (1987). Explanation, causation and psychological theories. A methodological study illustrated by an analysis of Festinger's theory of cognitive dissonance and Newell & Simon's theory of human problem solving. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Understanding and summarizing brief stories. In D. LaBerge & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Perception and comprehension (pp. 265303). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rumelhart, D. E. (1980). On evaluating story grammar. Cognitive Science, 4, 313-316. Ryan, T., & Schwartz, G. (1956). Speed of perception as a mode of representation. American Journal of Psychology, 69, 60-69. Sartre, J.-P. (1939). Esquisse d'une theorie des emotions. [Outline of a theory of the emotions]. Paris: Gallimard. Sartre, J.-P. (1940). Ulmaginaire. Psychologic phenomenologique de ^imagination. [The imaginary: Phemomenological psychology of the imagination]. Paris: Gallimard. Schachter, S., & Wheeler, L. (1962). Epinephrine, chlorpromazine and amusement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 121-128. Schank, R. G. (1975). Conceptual information processing. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North Holland. Schank, R. G. (1979). Interestingness: Controlling inferences. Artificial Intelligence, 12, 273297. Schank, R. G. (1982). Dynamic memory. A theory of reminding and learning in computers and people. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Schank, R. G., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals & understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Schatz, T. (1981). Hollywood genres: Formulas, filmmaking and the studio system. Philadelphia: Temple LIniversity Press. Scherer, K. (1988). Facets of emotion. Recent research. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Scheff, T. J., & Scheele, S. G. (1980). Humor and catharsis: The effect of comedy on audiences. In P. H. Tannenbaum (Ed.), The entertainment function of television (pp. 165-182). Hills-dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Scheff, T. J. (1979). Catharsis in healing, ritual and drama. Berkeley: University of California Press. Schoedel, J., Frederickson, W. A., & Knight, J. M. (1975). An extrapolation of the physical attractiveness and sex variables within the Byrne attraction paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 3, 527-530. Schoenmakers, H. (1988). To be, wanting to be, forced to be: Identification processes in theatrical situations. In W. Sauter (Ed.), New directions in audience research: Advances in Reception and Audience Research (Vol. 2, pp. 138-163). Utrecht, The Netherlands: Instituut voor Theaterwetenschap. Scholes, R., & Kellogg, R. (1966). The nature of narrative. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Schram, D., & Frijda, N. (Eds.).(1995). Emotion and cultural products [Special issue]. Poetics, 23, 1-2. Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 417458. Secord, P., Backman, G. W., & Eachus, H. T. (1964). Effects of imbalance in the self-concept on perception of persons. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 442-446. 270 REFERENCES Sekuler, R., Anstis, S., Braddick, O. J., Brandt, T., Movshon, J. A., & Orban, G. (1990). The perception of motion. In L. Spillman & J. S. Werner (Eds.), Visual perception: The neurophysi-ological foundations (pp. 205-230). San Diego: Academic Press. Selfridge, O. G., & Neisser, U. (1960). Pattern recognition by machine. Scientific American, 203(3), 60-68. Sesonke, A. (1973). Cinema space. In D. Carr & E. S. Casey (Eds.), Explorations in phenomenology (pp. 399-409). The Hague, The Netherlands: Nijhoff. Shweder, R. A., & DAndrade, R. G. (1980). The systematic distortion hypothesis. In R. A. Shwe-der & D. W. Fiske (Eds.), New directions for methodology of behavioral science (pp. 37-58). San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Silver, A., & Ursini, J. (1974). Brief Encounter (1945). In David Lean and his films (pp. 3951). London: Frewin. Silver, A., & Ursini, J. (1993). The vampire film (2nd ed.). New York: Limelight Editions. Simon, H. A. (1970). The sciences of the artificial. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Simons, R. F., & Zelson M. F. (1985). Engaging visual stimuli and reflex blink modification. Psychophysiology, 22, 44-49. Singer, J. L. (1973). The child's -world of make-believe: Experimental studies of imaginative play. New York: Academic Press. Singer, J. L., & Singer, D. G. (1976). Family television viewing habits and the spontaneous play of preschool children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 46, 496-502. Singer, J. L., & Singer, D. G. (1981). Television, imagination and aggression: A study of preschoolers. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Smith, M. (1995). Character engagement: Fiction, emotion, and the cinema. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Pres. Solomon, R. L. (1980). The opponent process theory of acquired motivation. American Psychologist, 35, 691-712. Sonnemans, J. (1991). Structure and determinants of emotional intensity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: University of Amsterdam. Sorlin, P. (1977). Sociologie du cinema [Sociology of the cinema]. Paris: Autier Montaigne. Souriau, E. (1950). Les deux cent milles situations dramatiques [200,000 dramatic situations]. Paris, France: Flammarion. Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1989). Person memory and judgment. Psychological Review, 96, 58-83. Stam, R. (1989). Subversive pleasures: Bakhtin, cultural criticism, and film. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Sternberg, M. (1978). Expositional modes and temporal ordering in fiction. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Stotland, E. (1969). Exploratory studies of empathy. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 271-314). New York: Academic Press. Stotland, E., Mathews, K. E., Sherman, S. E., Hansson, O. R., & Richardson, B. Z. (1978). Empathy, fantasy and helping. Beverly Hills: Sage. Stotland, E., & Mathews, K. E. (1978). Mrs. Reynolds needs a nurse. In E. Stotland, K. E. Mathews, S. E. Sherman, R. O. Hansson, & B. Z. Richardson (Eds.), Empathy, fantasy and helping (pp. 51-61). Beverly Hills & London: Sage. Strongman, K. T. (1978). The psychology of emotion (2nd ed.). Chichester: Wiley. Suboski, M. D. (1990). Releaser-induced recognition learning. Psychological Review, 97, 271-284. Suh, S., & Trabasso, T. (1993). Inferences during reading: Converging evidence from discourse analysis, talk-aloud protocols, and recognition priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 279-300. Suls, J.M. (1972). A two-step model for the appreciation of jokes and cartoons: An information REFERENCES 271 processing analysis. In J. H. Goldstein & P. E. MeGhee (Eds.), The psychology of humor (pp. 81-100). New York: Academic Press. Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 1-39. Tamborini, R. (1991). Responding to horror: Determinants of exposure and appeal. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Responding to the screen. Reception and reaction processes (pp. 305-328). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tan, E. (1985). Beoordeling en heleving van "de Grens" [Appreciation and experiencing of "The Frontier"] (Technical report on the reception of theatre and the media, 11). Amsterdamt, The Netherlands: Instituten voor Theaterwetenschap. Tan, E. (1986a). Interest when nothing happens. A note on narrative retardation. Proceedings of the Eighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 832842 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tan, E. (1986b). What's in a line? TijdseJirift voor TJieater-icetcnscliap, 4(16-17), 151-185. Tan, E. (1990). Bespreking van D. Bordwcll, Making meaning: Inference and rhetoric in the interpretation of cinema. Skrien, 173, 64. Tan, E. (1991). Filmpsyehologie. In P. Bosnia (Red.), Filmkunde. Een inleiding (pp. 267280). Nijmegen, The Netherlands: SUN. Tan, E. (1994). Story processing as an emotion episode. In H. van Oostenclorp & R. Zwaan (Eds.) Naturalistic text processing (pp. 165-188). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Tan, E. (1995). Film induced affect as a witness emotion [Special issue]. Poetics, 23, (1-2), 7-32. Tan, E. (in press). Constraint and convention in psychological film aesthetics. In J. Muller (Ed.), Proceedings of the Conference on Film Pragmaticy. Minister, Germany: Nodus. Tan, E. (in preparation). Exposing the affect structure of narrative film: Some empirical studies. Tan, E., & de Wied, M. A. (1986). Empirische vaststelling van handelingseenheden in defihn "Straf" [Empirical assessment of action units in the film "Punishment1] (Technical report on the reception of theatre and the media, 18). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Instituten voor Theaterwetenschap. Tan, E., & van den Boom, I. (1992). Explorations in the psychological affect structure of narrative film. In E. Nardocchio (Ed.), Reader response to literature. The empirical dimension (pp. 57-94). Berlin, Germany & New York: De Gruyter-Mouton. Tan, E., & Diteweg, G. (in press). Expectation, suspense and other emotion in film viewing. In P. Vorderer, H. J. Wulff, & M. Friedrichsen (Eds.), Analysis of suspense. Hillsdale (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tan, E., Eggermont, L., & Joosten, P. (1989). On becoming a cinephile. A theoretical and empirical analysis. Tijdschrift voor Theaterwetenschap, 7(27/28), 207-233. Tan, E., Eggermont, L. J. M., Joosten, P. D. M., & Spiiihof, H. (in preparation). Emotion in the film viewer as a function of cinephilia and film genre. Tan, E., & Schoenmakers, H. (1984). "Good guy and bad guy' 1 effects in political theatre. In H. Schmidt & A. van Kesteren (Eds.), Semiotics of drama and theatre (pp. 467-508). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. Tannenbaum, P. H. (1980). Entertainment as vicarious emotional experience. In P. H. Tannen-baum (Ed.), The entertainment function of television (pp. 107-131). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tannenbaum, P. H. (1985). 'Play it again Sam.1 Repeated exposure to television programs. In D. Zillmann & J. Bryant (Eds.), Selective exposure to communication (pp. 225-241). Hills-dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tannenbaum, P. H., & Gaer, E. P. (1965). Mood change as a function of stress of protagonist and degree of identication in a film viewing situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 612-616. 272 REFERENCES Taylor, S. E. (1981). The interface of cognitive and social psychology. In J. Harvey (Ed.), Cognition, social behavior, and the environment (pp. 189-211). Hillsdale (NJ): Lawrence Erl-baum Associates. Tellegen-van Delft, S. (1987). Waarom zou je lezen? [Why would you read?]. Groningen, The Netherlands: Wolters-Noordhoff. Thorndyke, P. W. (1977). Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 77-110. Todorov, T. (1971, Fall). The two priciples of narrative. Diacritics, 39-53. Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness: Vol. 1. The positive affects. New York: Springer. Tomkins, S. S. (1984). Affect theory. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 163-195). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Turner, T. J., & Ortony, A. (1992). Basic emotions: Can conflicting criteria converge? Psychological Review, 99, 566-571. Tversky, B., & Baratz, D. (1985). Memory for faces: Are caricatures better than photographs? Memory & Cognition, 13, 45-49. Uchiyama, F., Hanari, T., Ito, T., Takahashi, T., Okuda, K., Goto, T., & Tsuji, K. (1990). Patterns of psychological and physiological responses for commom affects elicited by films. Psy-chologia -An InternationalJournal of Psychology in the Orient, 33, 36-41. van den Berg, H. (1979). Teksten voor toeschouwers [Texts for spectators]. Muiderberg, The Netherlands: Coutinho. van der Run, J. I. M. (1970). Handelingsaspecten in het drama [Aspects of action in drama]. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Polak & van Gennep. (Original work published 1938) Van der Voort, T. H. A. (1982). Kinderen en TV-geweld; waarneming en heleving [Children and TV violence: perception and experience]. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. van der Wurff, A. (1992). Aard en achtergronden van onveiligheidsgevoelens in de woonom-geving [The nature and background of feelings of insecurity in the residence]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: University of Amsterdam. van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Recalling and summarizing complex discourse. In K. Holker & W. Burg-hardt (Eds.), Text processing (pp. 49-118). Berlin, Germany & New York: De Gruyter. van Dijk, T. A. (1981). Studies in the pragmatics of discourse. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton. van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. van Vliet, H. (1991). De schone schijn [The beautiful apearance]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Utrecht, The Netherlands: University of Utrecht. Vipond, D., & Hunt, R. A. (1984). Point driven understanding: pragmatic and cognitive dimensions of literary reading. Poetics, 13, 261-277. Warren, W. H., & Kurtz, K. J. (1992). The role of central and peripheral vision in perceiving the direction of self-motion. Perception and Psychophysics, 51, 443453. Weaver, J. (1991). Responding to erotica: Perceptual processes and dispositional implications. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Responding to the screen. Reception and reaction processes (pp. 329-354). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wegman, C. (1985). Psychoanalysis and cognitive psychology. London: Academic Press. Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34-52. Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation. Chigaco: Rand McNally. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297-333. Whitfield, T. W. A. (1983). Predicting preference for familiar, everyday objects: An experimental confrontation between two theories of aesthetic behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 3, 221-237. REFERENCES 27 3 Whitfield, T. W. A., & Slatter, P. E. (1979). The effect of categorisation and prototypicality on aesthetic choice in a furniture selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 70, 65-76. Wilensky, R. (1982). Points: A theory of the structure of stories in memory. In W. G. Lehnert & M. H. Ringle (Eds.), Strategies for natural language processing (pp. 345-374). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wilensky, R. (1983a). Planning and understanding. A computational approach to human reasoning. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Wilensky, R. (1983b). Story grammars versus story points. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 579-623. Wilson, S. G., Rinck, M., McNamara, T. P., Bower, G. H., & Morrow, D. G. (1993) Mental models and narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 141-154. Winston, P. II., & Horn. B. K. P. (1981). LISP. Reading. MA: Addison-Wesley. Wollen, P. (1972). Signs and meaning in tlie cinema. London: Seeker & Warburg. Woods, I). .!., Beecher, G. P., <Sc Ris, M. D. (1978). The effects of stressful arousal on conjugate lateral eye movement. Motivation and Emotion, 2, 345-353. Wright, H. F. (1937). The influence of barriers on strength of motivation. Contributions to Psychological Theory, 1( Whole No. 3). Wright, W. (1975). Sixguns and society. Berkeley: University of California Press. Wyatt, R. O., & Badger, D. P. (1988). What newspaper critics value in film and film criticism: a national survey. In B. A. Austin (Ed.), Current research in film (Vol 4, pp. 54-71). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Wyer, R. S., & Gordon, S. E. (1984). The cognitive representation of social information. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 73-149). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wyer, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1986). Human cognition in the social context. Psychological Review, 53, 322-359. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1-27. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 39, 117-123. Zajonc, R. B., Murphy, S. T., & Inglehart, M. (1989). Feeling and facial efference: Implications of the vascular theory of emotion. Psychological Review, 96, 395— 416. Zeegers, W. (1988). Andere tijden, andere mensen. De sociale representatie van identiteit [Changing times, changing people]. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Bakker. Zillmann, D. (1979). Hostility and agression. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D. (1980). Anatomy of suspense. In P. H. Tannenbaum (Ed.), The entertainment functions of television (pp. 133-163). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D. (1983). Disparagement humor. In P. E. McGhee & J. H. Goldstein (Eds.), Handbook of humor research: Vol. 1. Basic issues (pp. 85-107). New York: Springer. Zillmann, D. (1985). The experimental exploration of gratifications from media entertainment. In K. E. Rosengren, L. A. Wenner, & P. Palmgreen (Eds.), Media gratifications research. Current perspectives (pp. 225-239). Beverly Hills: Sage. Zillmann, D. (1988). Mood management: Using entertainment to full advantage. In L. Donohew, H. E. Sypher, & E. Tory Higgins (Eds.), Communication, social cognition and affect (pp. 147-171). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D. (1991a). Television viewing and physiological arousal. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Responding to the screen. Reception and reaction processes (pp. 103-133). Hills-dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D. (1991b). Empathy: Affect from bearing witness to the emotions of others. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Responding to the screen. Reception and reaction processes (pp. 135-168). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 274 REFERENCES Zillmann, D. (1991c). The logic of suspense and mystery. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Responding to the screen. Reception and reaction processes (pp. 281-303). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1974). Effect of residual excitation on the emotional response to provocation and delayed aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 782-791. Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1975a). Viewers' moral sanction of retribution in the appreciation of dramatic presentations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 572-582. Zillmann D., & Bryant, J. (1975b). Effect of residual excitation on the emotional response to provocation and delayed aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 782-791. Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1985). Affect, mood and emotion as determinants of selective exposure. In D. Zillmann & J Bryant (Eds.), Selective exposure to communication (pp. 157-190). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (Eds.). (1989). Pornography: Research advances and policy considerations. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1991). Responding to comedy: The sense and nonsense in humor. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Responding to the screen. Reception and reaction processes (pp. 261-279). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D., Bryant, J., & Sapolsky, B. S. (1989). Enjoyments from sport spectatorship. In J. II. Goldstein (Ed.), Sport, games and play: Social and psychological viewpoints (2nd ed., pp. 241-278). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D., & Cantor J. R. (1976). A disposition theory of humour and mirth. In A. J. Chapman & H. C. Foot (Eds.), Humour and laughter: Theory, research and applications (pp. 93-115). London: Wiley. Zillmann, D., & Cantor, J. R. (1977). Affective response to the emotions of a protagonist. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 155-165. Zillmann, D., Hay, T. A., & Bryant, J. (1975). The effect of suspense and its resolution in the appreciation of dramatic presentations. Journal of Research in Personality, 9, 307-323. Zillmann, D., Mody, B., & Cantor, J. R. (1974). Empathetic perception of emotional displays in films as a function of hedonic excitatory state to prior exposure. Journal of Research in Personality, 8, 335-349. Zillmann, D., Rockwell, S., Schweitzer, K., & Sundar, S. S. (1992). Does humor enhance discomfort tolerance? Paper presented at the ICA, Miami, FL. Zillmann, D., & Wakshlag, J. (1985). Fear of victimization and the appeal of crime drama. In D. Zillmann & J. Bryant (Eds.), Selective exposure to communication (pp. 141-156). Hills-dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zillmann, D., Weaver, J. B., Mundorf, N., & Aust, C. F. (1986). Effects of an opposite-gender companion's affect to horror on distress, delight, and attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 586-594. Zimbardo, P. G. (1969). The human choice: individuation, reason and order versus deindividu-ation, impulse, chaos. In W. J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska Symposion on Motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Zuckerman, M. (1988). Behavior and biology: Research on sensation seeking and reactions t o the media. In. L. Donohew, H. E. Sypher, & E. Tory Higgins (Eds.), Communication, social cognition and affect (pp. 173-194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zusne, L. (1986). Cognition in consonance. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 62, 531-539. Zwaan, R. A. (1993). Aspects of literary comprehension: A cognitive approach. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.