Supplementary Online Material Social Laughter is Correlated With an Elevated Pain Threshold 5 Dunbar, R., Baron, R., Frangou, A., Pearce, E., van Leeuwen, E.J.C., Stow, J., Partridge, P., MacDonald, I., Barra, V., & van Vugt, M. 10 15 20 25 Methods Participants were screened to exclude any who were pregnant, lactating or suffering from a medical condition, or who had drunk alcohol or smoked within two hours prior to the experiment. Diabetics were specifically excluded because of their lack of pain sensitivity (McKinney et al., 1996). All designs were approved by local ethics boards, and all subjects provided informed consent. In Experiments 1 and 2, we used a frozen vacuum wine cooler sleeve placed over the forearm as the assay for pain tolerance. Subjects were asked to say when they could no longer stand the pain, and the duration noted (subject to a maximum of 180 sec to avoid skin damage). These sleeves are open to the criticism that they might have been less cold (and hence more bearable) in the second assay (after the video) than in the first (before the video). To check for this, Experiment 3 used a more robust assay for pain, namely a mercurial sphygmomanometer. Pilot trials suggested that the two assays correlated, but that the sphygmomanometer produced less variable results than the wine cooler sleeve. To induce ischemic pain, the cuff was inflated on participants’ non-dominant arm above the elbow at a steady rate (10mmHG/sec) by gentle pumping to a maximum pressure of 260-280 mmHg. Subjects were again asked to say when they could no longer stand the pain. The pressure at the point at which the subject declared the pressure painful was noted to the nearest 5mmHg. 30 35 40 Video material Experiment 1 The comedy videos included excerpts from You’ve Been Framed, Mr Bean, and a performance by the comedian Eddie Izzard; the control videos included Barking Mad (a pet training documentary), excerpts from TV News and other factual TV documentaries, and the religious programme Songs of Praise. Experiment 2 The comedy videos included Father Ted, Friends and The Simpsons, while the control videos included excerpts from a golfing programme, TV news and Eastenders (a drama serial). Experiment 3 1 45 The comedy video was an excerpt from episode 95 of South Park; who watched a 31min video lecture on substance abuse (Substance Abuse: Current Topics: Episode 5, “Understanding How Drugs Work”; Governors State University, June 2000). 50 Experiment 4 The comedy video was from a performance DVD of the British comedian Michael McIntyre; the neutral video was from a golfing instruction DVD. 55 60 65 70 75 80 85 Experiment 5 The clips were either neutral (an excerpt from the BBC natural history series Walking with Dinosaurs), affect positive (an excerpt showing lush, pleasant scenes and animals from the Jungles episode of the BBC series Planet Earth) or funny (excerpts from a performance by Dutch stand-up comedian Ronald Goedemondt and from Funniest Home Videos). Experiment 6 We made use of several Oxford-based troupes performing at the Fringe: the Oxford Imps (an improvised stand-up comedy group) and the caste of two drama playlets (Greek, a modern adaptation of the Oedipus story by Steven Birkhoff, and Same Time Next Week, a modern drama about male friendships in the 21st Century, written by Chris Leask and Martin West). The Comedy condition involved performers and audience members at performances of the Oxford Imps. Performances comprised a set of 7-8 improvised scenes and games performed by 7 performers (6 improvisers and one Master of Ceremonies), in the style of the improvised comedy TV series “Who’s Line Is It Anyway?” The comedy and drama shows were 50 and 75 mins long, respectively. The show was performed in the same venue at the same time (3 pm) every day to similar sized audiences. Each performer was in three or four scenes per show and so all had roughly equal amounts of stage time. Audience members were non-performing members of the troupe present at the same performances (technical crew members or performing members not on stage that day who came to watch the show). In the control (non-comedy) condition, the performers were the actors from the playlets Greek and Same Time Next Week: each actor performed the experiment three times. Audience participants at these two plays were recruited ad hoc from the Imps crew. Statistical analyses The distribution of pain threshold differences was significantly different from a normal distribution in only one of the 16 conditions in the 6 experiments: onesample Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests, 0.039p0.999; pooling the tests using Fisher’s meta-analysis [Sokal & Rolf 1969, pp. 621-624] gave 2=24.76, df=32, p=0.857, indicating that there was no underlying trend for the data to be significantly different from a normal distribution). However, a similar analysis for rate of laughter yielded two individual conditions that differed from normality (0.001p0.911), and an overall pattern across the set of 12 conditions in which laughter occurred at all that differed significantly from normality (Fisher’s meta-analysis: 2=47.17, df=24, 2 90 95 100 105 110 p=0.003). An ln-transformation of [laughter rate +1] (to account for zero rates) yielded an overall distribution that did not differ significantly from a normal distribution (2=28.38, df=24, p=0.244). Parametric statistical tests were therefore used on raw data for pain threshold and ln-transformed data for laughter rates. Since there may be gender differences in pain threshold, we include gender as a factor in the statistical analyses. The underlying hypothesis we are testing is that laughter elevates pain thresholds, and we need to show that the change in threshold from pre- to postintervention is significantly δ>0 after laughter, but not in the control condition when there is no laughter. Since this is a directional hypothesis (we ask how likely it is that the observed statistic is greater than 0 by chance alone), we use a one-sample, onetailed, directional t-test: when δ>0, p-values are one tailed as conventional, but when δ<0 the likelihood that δ>0 is p=1-(0.5*pobs), where pobs is the conventional 2-tailed pvalue associated with the observed value of the test statistic. Where appropriate, we use Fisher’s meta-analysis (Sokal & Rolf 1969, pp. 621-624) to test for consistent underlying trends in datasets from different experiments. The test asks how likely it is to get a set of p-values as extreme as the observed set by chance alone. Fisher showed that the absolute value of twice the sum of the ln-transformed one-tailed directional p-values for individual experiments is distributed as χ2 with df equal to twice the number of tests being combined (irrespective of actual sample size). Thus, if three tests are combined, df=6. References 115 McKinney, C., Tims, F.C., Kumar, A.M., & Kumar, M. 1996. The effect of selected classical music and spontaneous imagery on plasma β-endorphin. J. Behav. Med. 20, 85-99. Sokal, R.R. & Rolf, F.J. 1969: Biometry. Freeman, San Francisco. 120 3 125 Figure S1. Median (50% and 95% ranges) laughter rates for control (neutral video: white bars) and experimental (comedy video: dark grey bars) groups in Experiments 1, 2 and 4. In Experiment 4, subjects watched a neutral video alone (white bar) and the comedy video alone (light grey bar) or in a group (dark grey bar). Sample sizes are (left to right): 18, 17, 16, 16, 22, 24, 12. 4