Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 1 Cognitive Consequences of Individual Differences in Arousal Asymmetry Thomas Holtgraves Ball State University Keywords: arousal asymmetry, creativity, semantic networks June 5, 2013 CORRESPONDING AUTHOR: Thomas Holtgraves Dept. of Psychological Science Ball State University Muncie, IN 47306 Email: 00t0holtgrav@bsu.edu Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 2 Abstract Prior research has demonstrated that semantic organization in the right hemisphere (RH) is more diffuse and specialized for distant semantic associates than is semantic organization in the left hemisphere (LH). The present research explored individual differences in this regard. If the RH is more specialized for distant semantic associates, then individuals with a more active RH should display greater activation of distant semantic associations. Two experiments were conducted to examine this issue. In both studies a line bisection task was used to assess arousal asymmetry. In Experiment 1, greater RH activation was associated with the ability to generate remote associates to three word stimuli. In Experiment 2, relatively greater RH activation was associated with enhanced priming of distant semantic associates. Taken together, these experiments demonstrate that arousal asymmetry is an individual difference variable that is related to variability in semantic organization and retrieval. Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 3 Cognitive Consequences of Individual Differences in Arousal Asymmetry The two hemispheres of the brain are specialized for different functions. For example, performance on a lateralized word recognition task tends to be superior for the left hemisphere (LH) relative to the right hemisphere (RH). In addition to hemispheric functional differentiation, however, there also appear to be individual differences in arousal asymmetry (Levy, Heller, Banich & Burton, 1983), that is, greater tonic activation of one hemisphere relative to the other. Several lines of research suggest that are different cognitive and emotional tendencies associated with the two hemispheres. Research on hemisphericity, for example, has demonstrated trait-like preferences for different cognitive modes that are associated with each of the two cerebral hemispheres, and that hemispheric preference can be reliably identified via performance on a variety of biophysical tasks (Morton, 2001; 2002; 2003). Moreover, hemispheric preference is related to variability in Corpus Callosum size with right brain oriented individuals having substantially larger CCs than left brain oriented individuals (Morton & Rafto, 2006). Other researchers have focused on some of the emotional concomitants of arousal asymmetry (Sutton & Davidson, 1997; Wheeler, Davidson, & Tomarken, 1993), demonstrating, for example, an association between LH activation and positive mood. The present research contributes to these lines of inquiry by examining some of the cognitive consequences of individual differences in arousal asymmetry. Hemispheric Specialization, Semantic Priming, and Creativity The present approach builds upon research demonstrating hemispheric functional differences in semantic representation and retrieval. One of the earliest and most common techniques for demonstrating hemispheric differences in semantic organization involved semantic priming. In these studies semantic priming was found to be roughly equivalent in the Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 4 two hemispheres when activation was relatively automatic (short stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA), masked primes, etc., Neely, 1991) and the prime target pairs were strongly related and members of the same semantic category (e.g., sofa-chair) (Chiarello, Burgess, Richards, & Pollock, 1990; Richards & Chiarello, 1995). However, when the stimuli were not associated or weakly associated category members (e.g., lamp-chair), priming tended to occur only in the RH and not in the LH (Chiarello, 1985; Chiarello et al., 1990; Chiarello, Maxfield, & Swann, 1996). These findings suggest that there is passive activation of weakly related category members within the right but not the left hemisphere (Chiarello, 1998). Further support for this idea was provided by Beeman and colleagues who demonstrated hemispheric differences in summation priming (Beeman, Friedman, Grafman, Perez, Diamond, & Lindsay, 1994). They presented three word primes prior to presenting a target (e.g., scratch). The primes were either weakly related (cat, attack, paws) or unrelated to a target (church, grease, hurdle), with the target lateralized to either the right or the left hemisphere. They found greater summation priming in the RH than in the LH for the weakly related primes, suggesting that the RH is specialized for the detection of more distant semantic associates. Overall, then, these studies suggest that both hemispheres activate closely related semantic information, but that they differ in the nature of that activation. The LH engages in close semantic coding, activating a restricted set of meanings and quickly settling on a dominant meaning. In contrast, the RH engages in more diffuse semantic coding, activating many different meanings and leaving them activated for a longer period of time. These differences are consistent with a variety of neurological data regarding differences between the right and left hemisphere (e.g., Jung-Beeman, 2005). Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 5 Although there is support for greater activation of distant semantic associates in the RH relative to the LH, the possibility that individuals with a more active RH might display course semantic coding to a greater extent than others has not been examined. There is one line of research, however, that suggests the existence of such a link. Specifically, there is research demonstrating a relationship between dispositional RH activation and proneness to paranormal beliefs, and it has been argued that this link is caused by the coarse semantic processing properties of the RH. For example, Pizzagalli and colleagues demonstrated stronger indirect priming in the RH for individuals with strong paranormal beliefs relative to nonbelievers (Pizzagalli, Lehmann, & Brugger, 2001) as well as greater RH activation (Beta waves assessed with resting EEG) for strong paranormal believers relative to nonbelievers (Pizzagalli et al., 2000). Relatedly, Taylor, Zach, and Brugger (2002) reported significant correlations between magical ideation scores and a leftward shift in a line bisection task (and hence greater right hemisphere activation). It’s possible, then, that the link between RH activation and paranormal beliefs is partly a function of the tendency of the RH to engage in course semantic processing. Assessing Arousal Asymmetry One method for assessing arousal asymmetry is to compare alpha-band EEG readings for the right and left hemispheres (e.g., Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Senulis, & Friesen, 1990). However, faster and less cumbersome techniques have been developed that involve behavioral tasks. One well-known measure in this regard is the line bisection task. With a line bisection task, participants are asked to locate the center of a line by marking the line to indicate what they perceive to be the middle point. The logic of the task is that participants tend to over-attend to a stimulus when it is processed by the more active hemisphere. Hence, when bisecting a line, those with a more active RH will tend to bisect to the left of the midpoint (because they over-attend to Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 6 the left side); those with a more active LH bisect to the right of the midpoint. An alternative procedure is to ask participants to indicate which half of an evenly bisected line is larger (sometimes referred to as the Landmark task), a task that is entirely perceptual. Some evidence for the validity of line bisection task comes from the observation that people with RH damage consistently under-perceive the left side of a line and hence err to the right of center when making line judgments (Cappa, Guariglia, Messa, Pizzamiglio, & Zoccollotti, 1991; Damasio & Damasio, 1980; Vallar & Perani, 1986). In contrast, intact controls tend to err to the left side reflecting an overall RH attention bias, referred to as pseudoneglect (Jewell & McCourt, 2000). Electrophysical/imaging research suggest that this is due to the role of the RH in early visual attention (Çiçek, Deouell,& Knight, 2009). Importantly, however, there is considerable individual variability in line bisection performance (Manning, Halligan, & Marshall, 1990), and this variability is associated with differential hemispheric activation (Bjoertomt, Cowey, & Walsh, 2002; Oliveri, Bisiach, Brighina, Piazza, La Bua, Buffa, & Fierro, 2001). Most relevant for the current research, Marsh, Fogle, Simon-Dack, Davis, Lemann, Parkison, & Holtgraves (2012) and Nash, Mcgregor, & Inzlicht (2010) have demonstrated that participants whose line bisection performance indicated greater RH activation displayed significantly lower alpha (an inverse indicator of cerebral activity) in the RH (but not LH) relative to other participants. Moreover, research has demonstrated that individual differences associated with negative affect (He, Chai, Zhang, Yu, Chen, & Wang, 2010) and motivation (Nash, Mcgregor, & Inzlicht (2010 ) are significantly related to performance on the line bisection task. Regarding the former, He et al. report a significantly greater leftward bias in Generalized Anxiety participants relative to healthy participants. Regarding the latter, Nash et al., demonstrated an increased rightward line bisection Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 7 bias for high (but not low) self-esteem participants when they were challenged (paralleling EEG measures of prefrontal asymmetry when challenged). Hence, individual differences and situational variables associated with hemispheric asymmetries can be detected with line bisection tasks. The Present Research Two experiments were conducted to examine individual differences in arousal asymmetry and its likely relation to differences in the activation of semantic associates. In both experiments arousal asymmetry was assessed with a computerized line perception task. The first experiment examined the relationship between arousal asymmetry and the ability to generate remote associates to three word stimuli. The second experiment examined differences in summation priming (Beeman et al., 1994) as a function of arousal asymmetry. If the RH is specialized for distant semantic associates as prior research suggests, then individuals with a more active RH should display greater activation of distant semantic associates relative to other individuals. Experiment 1 In this study the relationship between arousal asymmetry and the ability to generate distant semantic associates was examined. To do this, participants completed a version of the line bisection task and attempted to solve remote associates problems (modeled after the Remote Associates Task, Mednick, 1962), a widely used measure of creative thinking and insight. For the latter task, participants were asked to produce a solution word (e.g., air) in response to three stimulus words (e.g., force, line, mail). It was expected that individuals with a more active RH would be better at this task than would others. Method Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 8 Participants. Participants (N = 78; 48 Females) were undergraduate students enrolled in Introductory Psychology courses who participated for partial course credit. All participants spoke English as their first language. The data from four participants (two males and two females) were excluded because they failed to provide judgments on at least two of the ten critical lines. In preliminary analyses there were no main effects or interactions involving participant sex and this variable was excluded from subsequent analyses. Materials. A computerized line perception task was developed for the current research. The line bisection task consisted of 20 bisected lines presented on the computer screen. Ten of the lines were bisected in the middle and constituted the critical trials. Filler trials included 5 lines that were bisected 1/16 in. to the right and 5 lines that were bisected 1/16 in. to the left. Lines not bisected in the center were included so as to identify participants who were responding randomly (no participants were excluded due to random responding) and to prevent participants from realizing that all stimuli were the same and responding randomly without given a true decision (Luh, 1995). The 20 lines were presented (four each) in lengths of 2 in., 3 in., 4 in., 5 in., and 6 in. Degree of RH activation was defined as the number of times a participant chose the left half of an equally bisected line. A 20 item associates test was constructed for this study. Ten of these items were compound associates and consisted of sets for which the solution created a compound word (e.g., cream, skate, water – ice). These sets were randomly selected from a list provided by Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003) and were of moderate difficulty (in the Bowden and Jung-Beeman study the mean percentage of these items that were solved was 54.7%). The other ten items were remote associates and had solutions that were conceptually related but did not need to form compound words (e.g., strike, same, tennis – match). These items were randomly selected from Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 9 the items used in Bowers, Regehr, Balthazard, and Parker (1990) and were more difficult than the compound problems with a reported solution rate of 21.9%. All items are included as supplementary material. Finally, because of some suggestions in the literature regarding a correlation between handedness and arousal asymmetry (Hellige, 1993) it was deemed wise to assess handedness and treat it as a covariate. Hence, participants completed the Edinburgh measure of handedness (Oldfield, 1971). Procedure. Participants first completed the Edinburgh measure of handedness (Oldfield, 1971), followed by the line bisection task and then the associates test. For the latter two tasks participants were seated approximately 57 cm in front of a computer screen and their head stabilized and held in position with the UHCOTech HeadSpot. Participants were told that lines would appear on the screen, and that each of these lines was bisected at a point near (but not exactly at) the middle. They were told to look at each line and indicate which of the two halves of the line they perceived to be the longest. Participants proceeded through the task at their own pace and stimulus presentation order was randomized for each participant. Participants then completed the 20 item associates task. For this task, the three stimulus words were presented on the screen for up to 15 sec., during which participants were to type a word that was common to all three. Presentation order was randomized for each participant. Results and Discussion Scores on the total associates test varied between 1 and 11 (out of a maximum possible score of 20) with a mean of 6.01 (SE = .32). Scores on the line bisection task (number of times left side was chosen as larger) varied between 0 and 10 (out of a maximum possible score of 10) with a mean of 5.62 (SE = .24). Consistent with past research (Bowers & Heilman, 1980), there Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 10 was a left side (RH) bias (i.e., pseudoneglect) as indicated by an overall mean of 5.62 that was significantly different from the scale midpoint of 5, t(73) = 5.62, p < .05). The relationship between performance on the line bisection task and the associates test was analyzed using both correlational techniques and mean differences. In terms of the former, partial correlations (with handedness treated as a control variable) between line bisection performance and associates test performance were computed. There was a significant positive correlation between line bisection performance and the overall associates test, r = .31, p < .01. Hence, the more frequently the left side was chosen as larger the greater the number of associates that were correctly generated. Subsequent analyses indicate that this relationship was significant for the compound items (r = .32, p < .01) but not for the non-compound items (r = .153, p < .20). Next, on the basis of a median split, participants were classified as RH active if they chose the left side of the equally bisected line on seven or more trials (N = 29; 15 females). Performance on the associates test was analyzed with a 2 (Active Hemisphere; Right Active, Non-Right Active) X (2) (Stimulus Type; Compound, Non-compound) Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) with repeated measures on the second factor and scores on the Edinburg handedness measure treated as a covariate. The covariate was significant in this analysis, F(1, 70) = 9.7, p < .01. There was a marginally significant main effect for hemisphere, F(1,70) = 3.62, p = .06. Right hemisphere active participants performed marginally significantly better on the associates test (M = 6.74; SE = .49) than did Non-Right Hemisphere Active participants (M = 5.53; SE = .40). In this study, then, increasing RH tonic activation, as assessed with the line bisection task, was associated with an increased ability to generate associates that were related to each of three words in a stimulus set. Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 11 Experiment 2 This experiment examined performance on a summation priming task as a function of individual differences in arousal asymmetry as assessed with performance on the line bisection task. The summation priming task was a modification of the task used by Beeman et al. (1994). In their study, three word primes were presented simultaneously in the center of the screen, followed by a target that was presented to either the left visual field (LVF) and hence initially processed by the RH, or to the right visual field (RVF) and hence initially processed by the LH. In the present study, the targets were presented centrally rather than being lateralized to the right or left hemisphere, and performance on the lexical decision task was examined as a function of individual differences in arousal asymmetry. If the RH is specialized for detecting diverse semantic associates as demonstrated by Beeman et al. (1994), then it follows that individuals who display greater arousal in the RH should show relatively greater priming effects with this task. Hence, although an overall summation priming effect was expected, it was also expected that participants with a more active RH would show greater summation priming effects than others. Method Participants. Participants (N=130; 86 Females) were undergraduate students enrolled in Introductory Psychology courses who participated for partial course credit. All participants spoke English as their first language. The data from two participants was excluded because of missing judgments on at least two of the ten critical lines in the line bisection task. Materials. The line bisection task was the same one used in Experiment 1 (10 critical evenly bisected lines and 10 filler lines). The materials for the summation priming task were the summation (N = 24) and unrelated (N = 24) triads taken from Beeman et al. (1994). The Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 12 summation sets involved three words (e.g., cat, attack, paws) weakly related to the target (e.g. scratch); the unrelated sets involved three words that were unrelated to the target. Two lists of prime – target pairs were constructed and constituted two versions of the task. Participants were randomly assigned one of the two versions. The lists were mirror images of each other: if the target was preceded by the summation primes in one list it was preceded by the unrelated primes in the other list. In this way, across the experiment, each of the 48 targets was preceded by the summation primes and unrelated primes an equal number of times. Procedure. Participants completed, in order, the Edinburgh handedness inventory (Oldfield, 1971), the line bisection task and the summation priming task. The first two tasks were identical to those used in Experiment 1. For the priming task, participants first heard a 500-Hz tone and saw an orienting screen with a fixation cross in the center for 250 ms. The three prime words (stacked in a vertical order) then appeared in the center of the screen for 2000 ms. This was followed by a 300 ms screen with a fixation cross accompanied by a 500-Hz tone. The target was then presented in the center of the screen. Participants were instructed to indicate, as quickly as possible, whether or not the target was a word. The manual response options consisted of two keys on a response box marked Yes/No. Participants were instructed to push the Yes key if the letter string was a word and the No key if the letter string was not a word. Participants had 3000 ms to respond. For 48 trials (24 summation prime trials and 24 unrelated prime trials) the target was a word and hence the correct answer was yes. To ensure that participants did not develop the expectation that the target string was always a word, there were 48 non-word trials in which the target string was a pseudo-word (e.g., pister, clins, smates) and hence the correct answer was no. The format of the non-word trials was identical to that of the 48 critical trials. Immediately after making a judgment, feedback (correct/incorrect and response Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 13 time) was provided on the screen for 1500 ms. Feedback was provided in order to motivate participants. Presentation order was randomized for each participant. Results and Discussion As in Experiment 1, a participant was considered to be right hemisphere activate (N = 56) if she or he judged the left side to be longer on seven or more trials, and scores on the handedness measure were treated as a covariate. Overall accuracy for this experiment was 96.4% and did not differ as a function of prime (96.1% vs. 96.8%), F < 1, or hemisphere activation (96% vs. 96.8%), F < 1. The overall priming effect was reliable as participants were significantly faster judging targets following primes (M = 538 ms; SE = 6.73) than following unrelated words (M = 555 ms; SE = 7.24), F(1,125) = 8.52, p < .01. The handedness covariate was significant, F(1,125) = 4.16, p < .05 and there was no difference in response time as a function of hemisphere activation, F(1,125) = 1.92, p > .15. However, as expected, there was a significant Hemisphere X Prime interaction, F(1,125) = 6.45, p < .05, and the difference between unrelated and summation primes was significant for right active participants (M = 569; SE = 10.99 vs. M = 544; SE = 10.21), F(1,54) = 14.65, p < .001, but not for non-right active participants (M = 541; SE = 9.67 vs. M = 533; SE = 8.98), F< 1. In other words, participants with a more active RH demonstrated distant semantic priming; other participants did not. An alternative analysis was conducted in which only strongly right-handed participants (defined as participants with a score of 0 on the left-hand scale of the Edinburgh) were included and handedness as a covariate was dropped. The Hemisphere X Prime interaction remained significant in this analysis, F(1, 57) = 7.73, p < .01. As in Experiment 1, correlation analyses were also conducted. In this case the correlation between priming (mean response time for the Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 14 unrelated trials minus mean response time for the summation trials) and performance on the line bisection task (number of trials the left side was selected as larger) was computed. This relationship was in the predicted direction but not significant (r = .14, p < .14). General Discussion The present research was designed to investigate some of the cognitive consequences of individual differences in arousal asymmetry. Prior research has demonstrated that semantic organization is more diffuse in the RH than in the LH. Specifically, when weakly related or nonassociated primes are used, stimuli presented initially to the RH typically produce greater priming than stimuli presented initially to the LH. The present research sought to test the hypothesis that there is an individual difference corollary to this effect. Specifically, it was expected that people with a more active RH should be more likely to display the RH course semantic effects typically found in divided field studies. The present studies provide some support for this extension. Experiment 1 suggests that greater RH activation may be associated with an enhanced ability to generate remote associates to three word stimuli. And in Experiment 2, participants demonstrating greater RH activation displayed enhanced priming of distant semantic associates relative to other participants. Taken together, these experiments demonstrate that arousal asymmetry is an individual difference variable that is related to variability in semantic organization and retrieval. The present results have implications for several areas of research. First, the concept of hemisphericity suggests that there are different thinking and behavior styles associated with which of the two hemispheres predominates for an individual. One cognitive dimension of hemisphericity is the association of the right brain with global processing and the left brain with local processing. The activation and generation of distant semantic associates, as examined in the Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 15 two experiments reported here, can be considered instances of global processing. Hence, the present finding of more distant semantic processing being associated with greater RH activation is consistent with this conceptualization of hemisphericity. Although research on hemisphericity fell out of favor for a period of time due to the lack of reliable tasks for assessing left-brain vs. right-brain orientation, newer assessment techniques and conceptualizations (e.g. Morton, 2001, 2002, 2003) have provided some empirical support for this concept. Second, the present findings provide some support for a mechanism that may underlie the relationship between RH activation and a proneness to paranormal beliefs. Past research has demonstrated that people with a proneness to paranormal beliefs demonstrate enhanced RH activity (Pizzagalli et al., 2000). But why? It has been suggested that a proneness to paranormal beliefs may reflect a more active network of loose semantic associations, in other words course semantic coding (Pizzagalli, Lehmann, & Brugger, 2001). The present results provide some empirical support for this reasoning by demonstrating an association between dispositional RH activation and the ability to generate and comprehend distant semantic associates. Further research could examine this more directly by examining the relationship between paranormal belief proneness and course semantic coding in the two hemispheres as assessed with the tasks used in the current research. Third, the tendency for the RH to engage in more diffuse processing suggests that it may play a particularly important role in certain types of creativity. Although the empirical literature on the neural correlates underlying creativity is relatively unclear (Dietrich & Kanso, 2010), there have been some studies reporting RH specialization, in particular the right superior temporal gyrus (STG), for convergent creativity, especially in terms of the experience of insight (Bowden, Jung-Beeman, Fleck, & Kounios, 2005; Jung-Beeman, Bowden, Haberman, Frymiare, Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 16 Arambel-Liu, Greenblatt, Reber, & Kounios, 2004; Kounios, Frymiare, Bowden, Fleck, Subramaniam, Parrish, & Jung-Beeman, 2006). For example, participants in the Jung-Beeman et al. study attempted to solve verbal problems (i.e., generating semantic associates) and indicated whether they were solved with or without insight. Imaging results demonstrated that problems solved with insight were associated with increased activity in the right hemisphere anterior superior temporal gyrus. EEG readings in a second experiment demonstrated a burst of gamma activity in the same area immediately prior to insight solution. This area is associated with the making of connections between distant semantic associates and hence is consistent with the present findings. The present results suggest several additional issues for further research. One issue is whether functional differentiation and arousal asymmetry would combine in an additive manner. For example, Levine, Banich, & Koch-Weser (1984) demonstrated a right visual field (RVF)/LH advantage on a word recognition task and a left visual field (LVF)/RH advantage on a face recognition task. However, individuals differed with respect to the size and direction of these advantages; individuals with a more active LH showed a stronger LH advantage for the word recognition test, and a smaller RH advantage for the face recognition task, relative to participants with a more active RH. Also, Tamagni, Mantei, & Brugger (2009) examined emotion word recognition as a function of both visual field presentation and individual differences in hemispheric attention (assessed with a line bisection task). Superior recognition of negatively valenced over positively valenced words occurred for participants who were left bisectors (and hence individuals who rely more on RH functions). Moreover, these individuals displayed better overall recognition accuracy for words presented to the LVF than words presented to the RVF. Applying this to the present research, if the targets in Experiment 2 were lateralized to the right Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 17 or left visual field rather than presented centrally, the expectation would be that even though there would be an overall tendency for superior RH performance (functional differentiation), individuals with a more active RH would display this effect to a greater extent than others. Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 18 References Beeman, M. J., Friedman, R., Grafman, J., Perez, E., Diamond, S., & Lindsay, M. (1994). Summation priming and coarse coding in the right hemisphere. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 26-45. doi:10.1162/jocn.1994.6.1.26 Bjoertomt, O., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2002). Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 125, 20122022. doi:10.1093/brain/awf211 Bowden, E. M., & Beeman, M. (1998). Getting the right idea: Semantic activation in the right hemisphere may help solve insight problems. Psychological Science, 9, 435-440. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00082 Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Aha! Insight experience correlates with solution activation in the right hemisphere. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 730-737. doi:10.3758/BF03196539 Bowden, E. M., Jung-Beeman, M., Fleck, J., & Kounios, J. (2005). New approaches to demystifying insight. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 322-328. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.012 Bowers, K. S., Regehr, G., Balthazard, C., & Parker, K. (1990). Intuition in the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 72-110. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(90)90004-N Brugger, P. & Mohr, C. (Eds.) (2008). Special issue on Neuropsychology of Paranormal Experiences and Beliefs, Cortex, 44, 1291-1396. Burgess, C., & Simpson. G. (1988). Hemispheric processing of ambiguous words. Brain and Language, 33, 86-104. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(88)90056-9 Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 19 Cappa, S. F., Guariglia, C., Messa, C., Pizzamiglio, L., & Zoccollotti, P. (1991). Computed tomography correlates of chronic unilateral neglect. Neuropsychology, 5, 195-204. Chiarello, C. (1985). Hemisphere dynamics in lexical access: Automatic and controlled priming. Brain and Language, 26, 146-172. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(85)90034-3 Chiarello, C. (1998). On codes of meaning and meaning of codes: Semantic access and retrieval within and between the hemispheres. In M. Beeman & C. Chiarello (Eds.), Getting it right: The cognitive neuroscience of right hemisphere language comprehension. (pp. 141-160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Chiarello, C., Burgess, C., Richards, L., & Pollock, A. (1990). Semantic and associative priming in the cerebral hemispheres: Some words do, some don’t…sometimes, some places. Brain and Language, 38, 75-104. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(90)90103-N Çiçek, M., Deouell, L. Y., & Knight, R. T. (2009). Brain activity during Landmark and line bisection tasks. Frontiers of Human Neuroscience, 3:7. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.007.2009 Damasio, A. R., & Damasio, H. (1980). Neglect following damage to frontal lobe or basal ganglia. Neuropsychologia, 18, 123-132. Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. A., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approachwithdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology I. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 330-341. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.330 Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 822-848. doi: 10.1037/a0019749 Fiore, S. M., & Schooler, J. W. (1998). Right hemisphere contributions to creative problem solving: Converging evidence for divergent thinking. In M. Beeman, C. Chiarello, M. Beeman, C. Chiarello (Eds.), Right hemisphere language comprehension: Perspectives from Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 20 cognitive neuroscience (pp. 349-371). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. He, W., Chai, H., Zhang, Y.C., Yu, S.H., Chen, W., & Wang, W. (2010). Line bisection performance in patients with generalized anxiety disorder and treatment-resistant depression. International Journal of Medical Sciences, 7, 224-231. Hellige, J. B. (1993). Hemispheric asymmetry: What’s right and what’s left. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Jewell, G., & McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 93-110. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00045-7 Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., Frymiare, J. L., Arambel-Liu, S., Greenblatt, R., Reber, P. J., & Kounios, J. (2004). Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS Biology, 2, 0500-0510. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020097 Jung-Beeman, M. (2005). Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 712-718. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.009 Kounios, J., Frymiare, J. L., Bowden, E. M., Fleck, J. I., Subramaniam, K., Parrish, T. B., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2006). The prepared mind: Neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. Psychological Science, 17, 882-891. Leonhard, D., & Brugger, P. (1998). Creative, paranormal, and delusional thought: A consequence of right hemisphere semantic activation? Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 11, 177-183. Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 21 Levine, S. C., Banich, M. T., & Koch-Weser, M. (1984). Variations in Patterns of Lateral Asymmetry among Dextrals. Brain and Cognition, 3, 317-334. doi:10.1016/02782626(84)90024-1 Levy, J., Heller, W., Banich, M.T., & Burton, L.A. (1983). Asymmetry of perception in free viewing of chimeric faces. Brain and Cognition, 2, 404-419. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.9.3.329 Luh, K. E. (1995). Line bisection and perceptual asymmetries in normal individuals: What you see is not what you get. Neuropsychology, 9(4), 435-448. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.9.4.435 Manning, L., Halligan, P. W., & Marshall, J. C. (1990). Individual variations in line bisection: a study of normal subjects with application to the interpretation of visual neglect. Neuropsychologia, 28, 647-655. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(90)90119-9. Marsh, L., Fogle, K., Simon-Dack, W., Davis, A., Lemann, E., Parkison, P., & Holtgraves, T. (2012, May). An exploratory study of line perception performance and resting alpha power asymmetries. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Psychological Science. Chicago, IL. Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220-232. doi:10.1037/h0048850 Morton, B. E. (2001). Large individual differences in minor ear output during dichotic listening. Brain and Cognition, 45, 229-237. Morton, B. E. (2002). Outcomes of hemisphericity questionnaires correlate with unilateral dichotic deafness. Brain and Cognition, 49, 63-72. Morton, B. E. (2003). Phased mirror tracing outcomes correlate with several hemisphericity measures. Brain and Cognition, 51, 294-304. Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 22 Morton, B. E., & Rafto, S. E. (2006). Corpus callosum size is linked to dichotic deafness and hemisphericity, not sex or handedness. Brain and Cognition, 62, 1-8. Nash, K., Mcgregor, I., & Inzlicht, M. (2010). Line bisection as a neural marker of approach motivation. Psychophysiology, 47, 979-983. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00999x Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition. A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264-336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Oldfield, R. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburg inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 Oliveri, M. M., Bisiach, E. E., Brighina, F. F., Piazza, A. A., La Bua, V. V., Buffa, D. D., & Fierro, B. B. (2001). rTMS of the unaffected hemisphere transiently reduces contralesional visuospatial hemineglect. Neurology, 57, 1338-1340. Papousek, I., & Schulter, G. (1998). Different temporal stability and partial independence of EEG asymmetries from different locations: Implications of laterality research. International Journal of Neuroscience, 93, 87-100. doi:10. /00207459808986415 Pizzagalli, D., Lehmann, D. & Brugger, P. (2001). Lateralized direct and indirect semantic priming effects in subjects with paranormal experiences and beliefs. Psychopathology, 34, 75-80. doi: 10.1159/000049284 Pizzagalli, D., Lehmann, D., Gianotti, L., Koenig, T., Tanaka, H., Wackermann, J., & Brugger, P. (2000). Brain electric correlates of strong belief in paranormal phenomena: intracerebral EEG source and regional Omega complexity analyses. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 100, 139-154. Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 23 Richards, L., & Chiarello, C. (1995). Depth of associated activation in the cerebral hemispheres: Mediated versus direct priming. Neuropsychologia, 33, 171-179. doi:10.1016/00283932(94)00102-U Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry: A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychological Science, 8, 204-210. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00413.x Tamagni, C., Mantei, T., & Brugger, P. (2009). Emotion and space: Lateralized emotional word detection depends on line bisection bias. Neuroscience, 162, 1101-1105. Taylor, K.I., Zäch P., & Brugger P. (2002). Why is magical ideation related to leftward deviation on an implicit line bisection task? Cortex, 38, 247–252. Vallar, G., & Perani, D. (1986). The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions: a clinical/CT scan correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia, 24, 609-622. Wheeler, R. E., Davidson, R. J., & Tomarken, A. J. (1993). Frontal brain asymmetry and emotional reactivity: A biological substrate of affective style. Psychophysiology, 30, 82-89. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03207. Arousal Asymmetry and Cognition 24 Author notes The author wishes to thank Adam Felton, Kevin Cappaert, and Joe Waters for their assistance in collecting data used in this study. This research was partially supported by a grant from the Ball State University Aspire grants program.