1 Identity and Intimacy Dr. Shraga Fisherman Academic Dean Orot Israel College Elkana, D.N. Harei Ephraim Israel 44814 Fax 972-3-9362288 Email: shraga@orot.ac.il Feb. 7. 07 Dr. Jack Seymour Religious Education Dear Professor Dr. Jack Seymour. Enclosed please find a diskette and three copies of my article entitled “Identity and Intimacy in Religiously Observant and Non-ReligiouslyObservant Adolescents and Young Adults in Israel” which I would like to submit for publication in “Religious Education”. Please confirm receipt of manuscript. Your attention to this matter will be much appreciated. Sincerely yours, Shraga Fisherman Ph.D. Merkaz Shapira Israel 79411 shraga@orot.ac.il 2 Identity and Intimacy Running head: Identity and Intimacy in Religiously and Non-Religiously Adolescents. Identity and Intimacy in Religiously Observant and Non-Religiously-Observant Adolescents and Young Adults in Israel Shraga Fisherman Orot Israel College 3 Identity and Intimacy ABSTRACT Ego identity and intimacy are two sequential stages, according to Erikson’s theory. The objective of this study is to examine the relationship between ego identity and intimacy, as two continuous multidimensional variables among religious and nonreligious individuals. Eight sample groups (age x gender x religious observance) comprising 308 subjects responded to The Adolescent’s Ego Identity Scale (AEIS) (Tzuriel, 1974, 1984, 1992) and to The Sharabany Intimacy Scale (1974). There was a difference in the ego identity variables’ predictive profile for the various intimacy dimensions and the general intimacy score among the religious groups and among the non-religious groups, both for the two age groups (adolescents and adults) and for the two genders. A characterization of each group results from understanding these differential relationships . 4 Identity and Intimacy Theoretical Background This study deals with two central developmental stages in the lives of adolescents and young adults: ego identity and intimacy. What follows is a brief survey of the central theories dealing with these two phases, and with the relationship between them. Ego Identity Ego identity was descriptively defined by Erikson (1958) as integration of childhood identities with experiences and specializations that develop from inborn abilities. An individual’s aggregate of identities is, on the one hand, an objective and independent entity, and, on the other, an internal subjective sense that includes knowledge of oneself and of one’s life goals. Consolidation of identity is a process connected to interaction with society. In adolescents, the sense of identity includes mastery of childhood problems and true readiness to face the challenges of adolescence. Erikson (1968) saw ego identity as a one-dimensional and continuous variable. Marcia (1980) perceived it as composed of two primary components, exploration and commitment. He used these to create a now well-accepted dichotomous and discontinuous typology: achieved identity, diffuse identity, moratorium and foreclosure. This typology assists therapists and researchers in determining a client’s/subject’s status. However, the typology certainly does not permit creation of individual identity profiles. It does not make possible a deep examination of personal style or perception of nuances and slight differences. In certain cases, there are small gaps between details defined as belonging to a certain status because they were inappropriate to the other categories, and details which were assigned to other statuses. 5 Identity and Intimacy Because of the aforementioned deficiencies, Tzuriel (1984, 1992) proposed a multidimensional and continuous approach to ego identity. This approach yielded seven dimensions, found through factor analysis: “Commitment and purposefulness” represents the extent of an individual’s vocational and ideological commitment. “Solidity and continuity” is a sense of stability and inner equilibrium, expressed, for instance, as “I am happy to be what I am right now.” “Social recognition” is the degree to which an individual perceives society as valuing her or his talents and abilities. A person might say “It looks like nobody understands me properly.” “Meaningfulness versus alienation” subsumes people’s perceptions of their lives as meaningful, and their sense of involvement, interest and belonging. Alienation is lack of meaningfulness. A typical statement is “I frequently ask myself who and what I am, and what I want out of life.” “Physical identity” refers to the degree to which individuals accept their external appearance and behavior. For example, “I often feel the need to change the way I look, to look different.” “Genuineness and truthfulness” is the extent to which people see themselves as behaving naturally or pretending. “Self-control” is the degree to which persons see themselves as able to control themselves, even in the face of stresses and temptations. One might muse “When I let myself speak freely, I say things that I later regret.” The perception of ego identity as a multi-component variable has been validated by many researchers, including Tzuriel (1984, 1990, 1992a), Even Chen (1993), Nir (1995), and Fisherman (1995). Identity and Religious Observance Beit Halakhmi (1991) draws a parallel between three levels of theoretical use of the concept of identity and religious functions. On the first level, Beit Halakhmi 6 Identity and Intimacy includes the collective identity, on the second level is social identity and on the third level is ego identity. Beit Halakhmi chose the theory of Erikson (1958) to represent the third level, and in this context, religious identity is a source of support and integration for different portions of the ego identity. Marcia (1980) extended Erikson’s (1968) theory and claimed that ego identity comprises two dimensions: commitment and exploration. Marcia mentions two types of commitment—vocational and ideological—but focuses more in his research on vocational commitment and career development. A number of scholars investigated the relationship between ideological commitment in its religious context and ego identity. Hood, Riahinejad, & White (1986) did not find any significant relationship between religious commitment and ego identity. Other researchers, on the other hand, did find a relationship between ego identity and religiosity (King, 2003), between ego identity and intrinsic religiosity (Sherman, 2001; Lee, Miller & Chang, 2006), between religious commitment and ego identity (Schenkel & Marcia, 1972; St. Clair & Day, 1979; Archer, 1982; Philipchalk & Sifft, 1985; Greenfield, 1998; McKinney & McKinney, 1999), between religious maturity and ego identity (Boerio, 1997), and between religious motivation and ego identity (Junkin, 2001). In an early study (Fisherman, 2004), the relationship between religious belief identity, a subcategory of religious identity, and ego identity in religious adolescents of both genders was examined. Throughout the entire sample, there was a significant and positive correlation between belief and general ego identity and between belief and the identity dimensions “solidity and continuity,” “meaningfulness vs. alienation,” “genuineness and truthfulness,” and “physical identity.” Among the girls, there was a significant and positive relationship between belief and the dimensions “social recognition,” “commitment and purposefulness,” 7 Identity and Intimacy “meaningfulness vs. alienation,” “solidity and continuity,” and “genuineness and truthfulness,” as well as the general ego identity score. The explanation for the relationship between spiritual identity and the social dimension of the ego identity can be explained by the centrality of the social dimension among girls, and in the intensity of social relationships in a dormitory setting. The dimensions “commitment and purposefulness” and “meaningfulness vs. alienation” involve ideology, which explains the relationship. The relationship between spiritual identity and the dimensions “genuineness and truthfulness” and “solidity and continuity” is explained by the importance of religious behavior according to religious norms and beliefs. Among the boys, there was a significant and positive relationship between belief and the “meaningfulness vs. alienation” and “genuineness and truthfulness” dimensions of the ego identity. The explanation for the relationship in the case of the boys is similar to that among the girls, except that among the boys, no relationship was found between spiritual identity, the social dimension, and the continuity dimension, due to the social dimension’s lack of centrality among the boys, and their lower degree of sensitivity to changes. Intimacy The sixth stage in Erikson’s epigenetic model (1958), relates to the conflict of intimacy. Erikson (1958) defines intimacy as the ability to commit oneself to living with a concrete partner, to develop moral strength, and to hold fast to such commitment, even when this demands sacrifice and compromise. Theoreticians in this area have emphasized various aspects of intimacy. Rogers (1972) stressed appropriate communication between the two partners, the commitment, expectations, and the continuing growth of each of the partners as a unique individual. Other 8 Identity and Intimacy researchers emphasized such components as empathy, openness, feelings of caring and love, responsibility, commitment, and sexual relations (Orlofsky, Marcia, & Lasser, 1973). Sharabany (1974) focused on friendships, voluntary mutual choice, and personal communication and affection, honesty and spontaneity, trust and loyalty, and truly knowing the friend, with sensitivity to his desires, needs, and feelings. Sharabany and Wiseman (1993) described intimacy as a psychological construct relating to various topics in the human life cycle. In their view, this construct undergoes hardly any changes throughout a person’s lifetime. It may develop and its object may change, but it is not a new product of adolescence or adulthood. In childhood, the primary objects of intimacy are the mother and father, followed by the siblings, friends, and peer groups, until intimate relations are formed with a member of the opposite sex and a new couple relationship is formed. According to this description, intimacy is not the direct result of the resolution of the conflict of identity. Rather, it develops from a younger age, and at the end of adolescence, its object switches from a close friend to a member of the opposite sex. What is novel in Sharabany’s perception of intimacy is that it focuses not only on the process of the growth of intimacy, but it also sees intimacy as a variable composed of various elements, producing a rich and diversified system of relationships. Sharabany suggested these dimensions as characterizing friendships with members of the same gender in the pre-adolescent stage. Various studies have found that these dimensions are also useful and relevant for adolescent friendships with the opposite sex (Sharabany, Gershony, & Hofman, 1981; Lev-Ran & Sharabany, 1981; Fisherman, 1995), as well as for adult relationships (Hershlag, 1984). Sharabany (1974) proposed eight components for the concept of intimacy: 9 Identity and Intimacy 1) Honesty and spontaneity—relates to the degree to which open and honest relationships between the friends in relation to themselves and others exist. These relationships include the conveying of both pleasant and less pleasant information, and sharing feelings, fears, hopes, and plans. 2) Familiarity, knowledge, and emotion—expresses the extent of one friend knowing facts about the other as well as his inclinations, preferences, needs, and feelings. Knowledge about the other (and thus, sharing feelings with him) results from two sources of knowledge: that which is passed on by the friend, and perceptions and feelings obtained by means of observation of the friend and empathy. 3) Connection, touch, and seeking closeness—the affection one of the friends feels for the other, missing him when he is not there, and feeling that the relationship has importance and value. 4) Exclusivity and privacy—includes the degree to which the two friends seek opportunities to be together, without other friends, in private. 5) Giving, helping, and sharing—the degree to which one friend expresses emotional support and tends to give instrumental assistance to the other friend. 6) Taking and asking for help—one friend receives help, and feels free to ask his friend for help or advice. 7) Doing things together—the extent of working and playing together and deriving enjoyment therefore. 8) Trust and loyalty—one friend’s confidence that his comrade will not deceive him, that he will keep promises and secrets, and that he will act in his best interest, even when he is not present. 10 Identity and Intimacy In keeping with Sharabany’s approach, this study relates to intimacy as a multi-component variable. This approach makes it possible to compare individuals and groups, not only with respect to the general intimacy score, but also to the different dimensions of intimacy. Gender Differences with Respect to Ego Identity and Intimacy Since, according to Erikson’s theory, ego identity and intimacy are two sequential developmental stages, the question arose: does the relationship between them entail any gender differences? Paterson, Sochting, and Marcia (1992) point out that Erikson (1968) distinguished between the male and the female identity in three principal areas: the content, timing, and order of ego identity conflict resolution. Content refers to different emphases of the two genders in their ego identity. In this area, the findings of Douvan and Adelson (1966) and Josselson (1987) are noteworthy. They report that employment- and career-related issues enjoy a central position in the ego identity of men, while in the case of women, interpersonal relationships predominate. In an early study (Fisherman, 1995), a different approach toward the study of the content of intimacy and identity in the two genders was taken. It was found that a controlled identity in men was related to calculated intimacy rather than spontaneous intimacy. Among women, authentic, spontaneous identity and spontaneous intimacy were interrelated. The timing of resolutions of the intimacy conflict relates to the question: do the two genders resolve the conflicts of ego identity and intimacy synchronously or chronologically? The order of ego identity conflict resolution refers to the differences between the genders with regard to the extent to which the two developmental stages are 11 Identity and Intimacy sequential—first ego identity, followed by intimacy. In other words, how dependent is a positive resolution of the intimacy conflict upon a positive resolution of a conflict of ego identity? According to Erikson (1968), ego identity in women cannot be complete until an intimate partner relationship is formed. Erikson thus shares the view of Douvan and Adelson (1966), who argue that the development of intimacy in women precedes that of identity, which may indicate why the development of intimacy appears less related to the development of identity in women than in men. Josselson (1987) infers from this that a woman cannot define herself until she has chosen her partner. In keeping with this view, a number of scholars note that the female adolescent places great emphasis on how attractive she is, her image of her ideal man, and decisions concerning her body (Paterson et al., 1992). Under Erikson’s influence, several researchers have concluded that men are concerned with matters of ego identity before intimacy, while women reverse this order (Douvan & Adelson, 1966; Josselson, Greenberger, & McConochie, 1977; Orlofsky, 1977; Hodgson & Fischer, 1979; Marcia, 1980). Paterson et al. (1992) notes that girls cope with the issues of identity and intimacy simultaneously, while boys must attain a consolidated identity before they can begin to resolve conflict in the area of intimacy. Schiedel and Marcia (1985) find that men developed a heightened level of intimacy only after they had attained a high level of identity. In contrast, a third of the women with a low identity status were high in intimacy. Matteson (1993) reports a finding similar to Schiedel and Marcia with regard to women, but different with regard to men. According to him, over a third of the women and about a quarter of the men attain a high level of intimacy even if the level of their identity is low. 12 Identity and Intimacy Berliner (2000), on the other hand, finds no gender differences in the relationship between identity and intimacy. Another topic that has been studied is gender differences in the actual resolution of conflict in intimacy. Various studies have indicated that girls rank higher in intimacy than boys (Lev-Ran & Sharabany, 1981; Jones & Dembo, 1989; Clase, 1992; Gorcyca, 1993). Matteson (1993) also indicates that six of the seven studies that he surveyed found that women rank higher in intimacy than men (Matteson, 1979; Hodgson & Fischer, 1979; Kacerguis & Adams, 1980; Archer, 1989; Schiedel & Marcia, 1985; Whitbourne & Tesch, 1985), and only one study found no significant differences (Tesch & Whitbourne, 1982). The longitudinal studies dealing with gender differences regarding intimacy and order in resolving the two conflicts, identity and intimacy, deserve special attention. In 1963, Kahn, Zimmerman, Csikszentmihalyi, and Getzels (1985) interviewed adolescents, and re-interviewed them in 1981. The researchers concluded that a consolidated ego identity in adolescence was important in the first stage of marriage (the establishment of the marriage) among the men in the study. Regarding women, attaining a consolidated ego identity in adolescence was important for the stability of the marriage at a later stage, and for a feeling that their marriage gave them a feeling of satisfaction in adulthood. Another longitudinal study is that of Adams and Fitch (1983), who argue that there is a positive relationship between the ego identity status of men in their first year of university study and the status of their intimacy in their second year. No such relationship was found after their second year in college. No relationship between the status of ego identity and of intimacy was found among the women. Matteson (1993) ends his survey with the conclusion that 13 Identity and Intimacy the difference between the genders with regard to the states of identity related to intimacy, as noted in early studies, is becoming increasingly blurred. In an early study (Fisherman, 1995), age and gender differences regarding ego identity and intimacy were examined. The abundant results of the research showed significant gender differences. A controlled identity was found to be related to intimacy that is not spontaneous, but rather, carefully considered. Girls showed that an authentic, spontaneous identity was related to authentic and spontaneous intimacy. The objective of this study was to examine the relationship between ego identity and intimacy as two multi-dimensional variables in eight groups (gender x age x religious observance): religiously observant adolescent boys, non-religiouslyobservant adolescent boys, religious adult males, non-religious adult males, religious adolescent girls, non-religious adolescent girls, religious adult females, and nonreligious adult females. The Adolescents Ego Identity Scale (AEIS) (Tzuriel, 1974, 1984, 1992) The Scale comprises 38 items, half of which are worded in the negative and half in the positive. The scale of answers is of the Likert type, with five rankings, ranging from “definitely not true” (1) to “definitely appropriate” (5). The Scale is based partly on earlier questionnaires, among them that of Rasmussen (1964). Tzuriel (1984) reports general reliability of .77, using the Cronbach alpha method. Tzuriel (1992) performed a factor analysis on a sample of 932 high school students in grades 10-12, and found seven factors. These seven factors (hereinafter “dimensions”) are: commitment and purposefulness, solidity and continuity, meaningfulness vs. alienation, physical identity, social recognition, genuineness and truthfulness, and self-control. These dimensions (with the exception of the commitment and 14 Identity and Intimacy purposefulness dimension) were found to differentiate significantly between adolescents from a clinical group that either attempted suicide or threatened to attempt it, on the one hand, and a group of normative adolescents, on the other (Tzuriel & Bar Yoseph, 1986). In addition, dimensions of ego identity and general ego identity were found to be related to social gender roles (Tzuriel, 1984), cognitive complexity (Fisherman, 2001), and nationality identification and ethnocentrism (Tzuriel & Klein, 1977). The average of the responses provides a measurement for every dimension of the ego identity, as well as for the general ego identity. According to the Cronbach alpha formula, the present study yielded measurements of internal consistency. The internal consistencies were as follows, according to the Cronbach alpha formula in the present study: commitment and purposefulness (.71), solidity and continuity (.79), meaningfulness vs. alienation (.76), physical identity (.70), genuineness and truthfulness (.56), social recognition (.60), and self-control (.59). Sharabany Intimacy Scale (Sharabany, 1974) The Intimacy Scale examines intimacy as a multi-dimensional personality variable. This questionnaire was originally developed for pre-adolescent children (ages 10-12) and was later validated for adolescents and young adults as well (Fayence, 1978; Herz-Lazarovitz, Sharabany, & Tal, 1983; Herschlag, 1984; Sharabany, Gershoni, & Hofman, 1981; Lev-Ran & Sharabany, 1981; Sharabany, Arnon, & Kav-venaki, 1981; Sharabany & Toren, 1982). The scale comprises eight four-item dimensions, for a total of 32 items, in random order. The subjects ranked each item on a Likert-type scale, from “not at all true” (1) to “very true” (6). The general intimacy score is calculated on the basis of 15 Identity and Intimacy responses to all 32 items on the scale. A high score reflects a high level of intimacy. The dimensions of intimacy are: honesty, familiarity, closeness, giving, asking for help, doing things together, exclusivity, and trust. The scale was validated by HerzLazarovitz et al. (1983), according to how three psychologist judges classified the factors, according to the content dimension definitions. For 28 of the 32 items, there was 88% agreement. The reliability of the general score examined in Sharabany’s study (1974) ranged from .90-.94. Following are the reliability indices of internal consistency, according to the Cronbach alpha formula, for the present study: honesty (.73), familiarity (.73), closeness (.78), giving (.68), asking for help (.63), doing things together (.51), trust (.53). The internal consistency of the exclusivity dimension was found to be lower than .50; therefore, our study omitted this dimension from the scale. Sample Two age groups including a total of 308 subjects were examined: 1) late adolescents, 18-19 years old (51%); and 2) young adults aged 24-25 (49%). Table 1 shows the distribution by sample groups. TABLE 1—Distribution of the Sample religious adolescent boys non-religious adolescent boys religious men non-religious men religious adolescent girls non-religious adolescent girls religious women non-religious women TOTAL n 30 41 % 9.7 13.3 42 32 47 39 13.6 10.4 15.3 12.7 29 48 308 9.4 15.6 100% 16 Identity and Intimacy Results In order to identify the identity profile (a combination of the identity dimensions) that can best predict the general intimacy score and the various intimacy dimensions in the research population, stepwise regression analyses were carried out for the eight different sub-samples of the entire research population: 1) religious adolescent boys, 2) religious adolescent girls, 3) religious men, 4) religious women, 5) non-religious adolescent boys, 6) non-religious adolescent girls, 7) non-religious men, and 8) non-religious women. The dependent variable in each analysis was the intimacy dimension and the general intimacy score (each of these separately), and the independent variables were the identity dimensions. The tables that follow show the results of the stepwise regression analyses in the different populations. 17 Identity and Intimacy TABLE 2—Results of Stepwise Regression Analyses of Intimacy Dimensions, According To Ego Identity Dimensions in Adolescent Boys Religious Adolescent Boys Non-Religious Adolescent Boys Intimacy Identity F dimensions dimensions df=1,29 (predicted) (criterion) (criterion) Honesty _____ social Beta R² Identity F dimensions df=1,40 Beta 5.88* R² .36 13 6.31* .37 14 6.65* .38 14 recognition Familiarity genuineness and 11.00** .52 28 truthfulness Closeness social recognition physical identity 10.11** -.51 26 social recognition Giving commitment and 5.47* .40 16 ____ purposefulness Asking for help self- _____ 6.13* -.41 control, social 25 5.85* .34 recognition Doing things 4.48* self-control .37 13 ____ together Trust _____ General intimacy commitment and score purposefulness *P<.05 **p<.01 self-control 5.21* .39 15 5.98* .36 13 ______ ***p<.001 Table 2 shows that “commitment and purposefulness” predicts the general intimacy score among religiously observant adolescent boys, while no ego identity dimension was found for non-religiously observant adolescent boys that could predict this score. It bears pointing out that “physical identity” negatively predicted the “closeness” dimension in religious adolescent boys. A detailed explanation will be found in the Discussion section. In the non-religious adolescent boys, “social recognition” predicted four out of seven intimacy dimensions, while in the case of 18 Identity and Intimacy their religious counterparts, no specific ego identity dimension was found that was conspicuously predictive. TABLE 3-- Results of Stepwise Regression Analyses of Intimacy Dimensions, According To Ego Identity Dimensions in Adolescent Girls Religious Adolescent Girls Non-Religious Adolescent Girls Intimacy Identity F dimensions dimensions df=1,46 Honesty Solidity and 9.53** .41 17 ______ 15.22** .50 25 ______ Beta R² Intimacy F Beta R² dimensions continuity Familiarity Solidity and continuity Closeness Solidity and 5.18* .32 10 ______ 6.44* -.35 12 Self-control 11.43 ** .45 20 _______ 2.42*** .46 21 _______ 14.22** -.49 24 Commitment continuity Giving Meaningfulnes 4.77* -.33 11 7.92** .22 45 s vs. alienation Asking for help Social recognition Doing things Solidity and together continuity Trust Meaningfulness and vs. alienation purposefulness Self-control, 5.24* -.55 Social 5.49* .38 recognition, Genuineness 5.05* .33 and truthfulness General intimacy Solidity and score continuity *P<.05 **p<.01 6.01*** .51 26 ______ ***p<.001 Table 3 shows that the ego identity dimensions predicted all of the intimacy dimensions, as well as the general intimacy score for the religiously observant adolescent girls. For the non-religiously observant adolescent girls, in contrast, the ego identity dimensions predicted only two of the seven intimacy dimensions. No 19 Identity and Intimacy significant predictor for the general intimacy score was found. The explained variance was generally higher for the religious than the non-religious adolescent girls. It is worth noting that “trust” in the non-religious adolescent girls was predicted by four ego identity dimensions, explaining 45% of the variance. TABLE 4-- Results of Stepwise Regression Analyses of Intimacy Dimensions, According To Ego Identity Dimensions in Adult Males Religious Adult Males Non-Religious Adult Males Intimacy Identity F dimensions dimensions df=1,41 Honesty Social 11.72** .47 22 physical identity 5.48* .39 15 7.51** .39 15 Commitment and 5.50* .39 15 6.48* .42 17 Beta R² Identity F dimensions df=1,31 Beta R² recognition Familiarity Solidity and continuity Closeness physical identity purposefulness 6.95* .38 14 Genuineness and truthfulness Giving Self-control 16.90** .54 29 _______ Asking for help Social 8.71* .42 18 _______ 5.22* .34 11 _______ 7.07** .38 15 _______ 8.12** .41 17 _______ recognition Doing things Meaningfulness together vs. alienation Trust Social recognition General Meaningfulness intimacy score vs. alienation *P<.05 ** p<.01 *** p<.001 Table 4 shows that the ego identity dimensions predicted all of the intimacy dimensions and the general intimacy score for the religiously observant men, whereas in the case of the non-religiously observant men, ego identity dimensions predicted only three of the seven intimacy dimensions. No significant predictor of the general 20 Identity and Intimacy intimacy score was found. The explained variance for the religiously observant men was generally higher than for their non-religious counterparts. TABLE 5-- Results of Stepwise Regression Analyses of Intimacy Dimensions, According To Ego Identity Dimensions in Adult Females Religious Adult Females Non-Religious Adult Females Intimacy Identity F dimensions dimensions df=1,28 Honesty Solidity and 10.22** .52 27 5.87* .42 17 4.65* .38 14 5.28* .40 16 19.75*** .64 41 Beta R² continuity Familiarity Solidity and continuity Closeness Meaningfulness vs. alienation Giving Social recognition Asking for Solidity and help continuity Doing things _______ together Trust _______ General Solidity and intimacy continuity 6.75* .45 19 Identity dimensions F Genuineness and truthfulness Social recognition 9.06** .40 16 9.84** .42 17 Genuineness and truthfulness Social recognition 4.29* 8 11.29** .44 19 Genuineness and truthfulness Commitment and purposefulness Social recognition Social recognition Genuineness and truthfulness 4.90* .31 9 6.34** .31 20 4.97* .30 4.24* .29 8 8.26** .39 15 Beta R² df=1,47 .29 score *P<.05 ** p<.01 *** p<.001 It is evident from Table 5 that the ego identity dimensions predicted all of the intimacy dimensions, as well as the general intimacy score, for non-religiously observant women, but only five out of the seven (and the general intimacy score) for their religious counterparts. For the non-religious women, the significant ego identity predictors were “genuineness and truthfulness” and “social recognition”; for religious women, no one specific ego identity predictor of intimacy was found. Identity and Intimacy 21 Table 6 provides an inclusive and comprehensive depiction of the regression analyses for all of our study’s sub-samples. TABLE 6—Summary of Stepwise Regression Analyses of Intimacy Dimensions and General Intimacy Score, by Ego Identity Dimensions in the Various Sample Groups Intimacy dimension religious adolescent boys non-religious adolescent boys religious adolescent girls non-religious adolescent girls religious adult Honesty _______ Social recognition Genuineness & truthfulness physical identity Solidity & continuity Solidity & continuity Solidity & continuity Meaningfulness vs. alienation Social recognition _______ Familiarity Social recognition Social recognition Social recognition _______ _______ _______ Solidity & continuity physical identity Self-control Closeness Giving Asking for help Commitment & purposefulness _______ non-religious adult males religious adult females non-religious adult females Self-control physical identity Commitment & purposefulness Genuineness & truthfulness _______ Solidity & continuity Solidity & continuity Meaningfulness vs. alienation Social recognition _______ Social recognition _______ Solidity & continuity Genuineness & truthfulness Social recognition Genuineness & truthfulness Social recognition Genuineness & truthfulness Solidity & continuity _______ Meaningfulness vs. alienation _______ _______ Commitment & purposefulness Self-control Social recognition Genuineness & truthfulness _______ Social recognition _______ _______ Meaningfulness vs. alienation _______ Solidity & continuity Doing things together Self-control Self-control Social recognition _______ Trust _______ Self-control Meaningfulness vs. alienation General intimacy score Commitment & purposefulness _______ Solidity & continuity males Commitment & purposefulness Social recognition Social recognition Genuineness & truthfulness 22 Identity and Intimacy If we summarize the series of regressions described in tables 2-6, we can see the ego identity profile that can best predict the various intimacy dimensions in the sample groups. In the various comparisons of religiously observant and nonreligiously-observant groups, none of the analyses showed that the intimacy dimension was predicted by the same dimension of ego identity. This result indicates two completely separate predictive profiles for these two sub-samples. This result will be explained in the discussion. Various Intimacy Dimensions Four out of seven intimacy dimensions predicted the ego identity dimensions among religiously observant adolescent boys. A specific dimension that could consistently predict a large number of intimacy dimensions was not found. The explained variance was also relatively low for this group, varying from 4.48% to 11%. Among non-religious adolescent boys, the "social recognition" dimension of ego identity predicted four out of seven intimacy dimensions, and the "self-control" dimension predicted two. A further difference was that the explained variance was relatively high among the non-religiously-observant adolescent boys, ranging from 12.8% to 24.6%. In the case of the religious adolescent girls, the ego identity dimensions predicted all of the intimacy dimensions. The ego identity dimensions that consistently predicted the greatest number of intimacy dimensions were “solidity and continuity” and “meaningfulness vs. alienation.” Among non-religious adolescent girls the ego identity dimensions predicted only two of the intimacy dimensions. There was no specific ego identity dimension that was conspicuous in its consistent predictability. Noteworthy among the non-religiously-observant adolescent girls was 23 Identity and Intimacy the intimacy dimension of “trust,” which was predicted by the following ego identity dimensions: “commitment and purposefulness,” “self-control,” “social recognition,” and “genuineness and truthfulness.” The explained variance was 45%. Among religiously observant adult males, the ego identity dimensions predicted all of the intimacy dimensions. The ego identity dimension that was conspicuous in its consistent predictability was that of “social recognition,” unlike the non-religious men, among whom the ego identity dimensions predicted only three of the seven intimacy dimensions, and no specific ego identity dimension was found with any conspicuously consistent predictability. The ego identity dimensions predicted five out of seven intimacy dimensions among religiously observant women. The conspicuous ego identity dimension for consistent predictability was “solidity and continuity,” a predictor of three intimacy dimensions. The ego identity dimensions of the non-religious women predicted all of the intimacy dimensions, and their conspicuously consistent predictive ego identity dimensions were those of “social recognition” (which predicted four intimacy dimensions) and “genuineness and truthfulness” (which predicted three). Predictability of the General Intimacy Score A further differentiator among the various sample groups emerging from the regression analyses presented in the above tables was the ego identity profile best suited for predicting the general intimacy score. Among religiously observant adolescent boys, this score was predicted by “commitment and purposefulness” (the explained variance was 15%); in their non-religious counterparts, we did not find any ego identity dimension that predicted this score. 24 Identity and Intimacy Among the adolescent girls, a similar difference between the religious and non-religious groups was found: in the case of the former, “solidity and continuity” predicted the general intimacy score (26% explained variance) for the religiously observant girls; no identity dimension was found that significantly predicted this score for the non-religious girls. “Meaningfulness vs. alienation” predicted the general intimacy score among the religious men (17% explained variance). No ego identity dimension was found to predict this score in non-religious men. “Solidity and continuity” predicted the general intimacy score of the religious women (19% explained variance), while “genuineness and truthfulness,” with a 15% explained variance, was the predictor for the non-religious women. Discussion The objective of this study was to examine the relationship between ego identity and intimacy among the religious and non-religious. The comparison was made between the two genders and between two age groups (adolescents and young adults). The study’s principal finding indicates that the religious and the non-religious exhibit completely different ego identity profiles for prediction of the different dimensions of intimacy, as well as the general intimacy score. This difference exists in all the research groups (age x gender). An explanation of the differences between the religiously observant and non-religiously-observant subjects in all the groups follows. 25 Identity and Intimacy Religious and Non-Religious Adolescent Boys In the religiously observant adolescent boys’ group, we found that the “commitment and purposefulness” dimension significantly predicted the general intimacy score. In the case of their non-religiously-observant counterparts, there was no ego identity dimension that predicted the general intimacy score. For the religious adolescent boys, the identity dimensions predicted the following four intimacy dimensions: “familiarity,” “closeness,” “giving,” and “doing things together,” although no ego identity dimension was found that significantly and conspicuously predicted a large number of intimacy dimensions. Among nonreligiously-observant male adolescents, the ego identity dimensions predicted most of the intimacy dimensions (five out of seven), the most conspicuously predictive of them being “social recognition.” The “commitment and purposefulness” dimension refers to the degree of the individual’s professional and ideological commitment, as well as to his world-view and religious ideology. In all probability, the fact that “commitment and purposefulness” predicts the general intimacy score reflects the religiously observant adolescent’s preoccupation with issues of faith and religious identity, unlike his nonreligious counterpart (Fisherman, 1998). Since for religiously observant adolescents intimacy and ego identity are interrelated, “commitment and purposefulness” predicted the general intimacy score. Conflict in the area of intimacy arises and assumes a central position in the individual’s life as soon as any conflict of ego identity is resolved. A resolution of a conflict in the area of ego identity encompasses “commitment and purposefulness,” and the very self-definition of these adolescents as being religiously observant is linked to this dimension. Moreover, the religious adolescent labors under the constant tension between his feelings towards members of 26 Identity and Intimacy the opposite sex and his desire to form close relationships with them, on the one hand, and the many prohibitions regarding such relationships, on the other (Smilansky, 1990). The religiously observant adolescent’s ability to cope with such tension is largely dependent on his faith and his life-view. A religious adolescent whose religious belief (related to “commitment and purposefulness”) is weak will find it very difficult to abide by the behavioral limits relating to intimacy. Therefore, it was specifically among the religiously observant adolescents that “commitment and purposefulness” predicted the general intimacy score. A non-religious adolescent, committing himself to no external limitations regarding intimacy with girls, does not feel such tension between his feelings toward his partner and his physical attraction to her. As such, his sense of “commitment and purposefulness” is not necessarily related to intimacy. Regarding the intimacy dimensions profile, among religiously observant adolescent boys, a relationship exists between ego identity and intimacy, but this relationship cannot be characterized by a specific ego identity dimension; whereas among non-religious adolescent boys, “social recognition” can be used to characterize the relationship. The non-religious adolescent boy probably considers his relationship with his girlfriend part of his social network. Smilansky (1989) notes that most (nonreligiously-observant) boys are interested in sexual relationships as such or as a means to achieve social recognition. In this connection, he quotes Simon, Berger, and Gagnon (1972), who showed that some 60% of male university students reported their sexual skills to their friends within the first month of having sexual relations. A nonreligious adolescent boy who feels confident of his popularity will also have feelings of “honesty,” “familiarity,” and “closeness” toward his girlfriend, and will be prepared to “ask for help,” since he relates to his girlfriend as a friend. 27 Identity and Intimacy Among religiously observant adolescent boys, the relatively high negative correlation coefficient (β=-.51) of “physical identity” with “closeness” should be noted. It may be possible to explain this finding by the religiously observant adolescent boy’s attitude toward intimacy in general, and toward “closeness” in particular. “Closeness” expresses the degree with which one friend likes the other, misses her when she is not around, and considers their friendship important. The religious adolescent, prohibited from having close physical relations with his girlfriend, and particularly intercourse, feels tension between his mature adult physical identity (gender identity included) and the norms that forbid him to physically act on the affection he feels for her. The more satisfied and in harmony he is with his physical identity, the greater the tension he feels regarding the relationship of closeness that he has with his girlfriend. This explains why the correlation coefficient between the two was negative. Religious and Non-Religious Adult Males Among religiously observant adult males, the “meaningfulness vs. alienation” dimension significantly predicted the general intimacy score. Among the nonreligious men, no identity dimension that predicted this score was found. Among religious men, the ego identity dimensions predicted each of the intimacy dimensions, but none of them did so conspicuously. In the case of the nonreligiously-observant men, three ego identity dimensions (“physical identity,” “commitment and purposefulness,” and “genuineness and truthfulness”) predicted only three of the seven intimacy dimensions (“honesty,” “familiarity,” and “closeness”). The result that relates to the general intimacy score indicates that, for the religious man who has resolved the ego identity conflict, starting a family—a mission 28 Identity and Intimacy which for him combines a religious/ideological obligation with his personal desire and with the resolution of the intimacy conflict that he faces at this age (24-25)— represents an important part of the meaning of his identity. For such a person, the meaning of intimacy represents more than a close warm relationship between the genders. For him, intimacy is the key to realizing an important part of his religious identity, namely, raising a family. The fulfillment of this element represents a segment of the utmost importance within the “meaningfulness vs. alienation” dimension. As a result, this dimension predicted the general intimacy score among the religiously observant men. From the point of view of the non-religious man, the resolution of the intimacy conflict is not necessarily part of any ideological outlook. In other words, while intimacy is an important part of his life and linked to his identity, it does not seem to be necessarily connected to the sense of meaning that he ascribes to his life. In this connection it is worth comparing, from a developmental standpoint, the religiously observant adolescent boys and young men. Among the latter, “meaningfulness vs. alienation” predicted the general intimacy score, while the predictor of this score among the former was “social recognition.” The religiously observant man is different from the religious adolescent boy in that for the adolescent, intimacy is linked to “commitment and purposefulness,” which expresses a sense of obligation to an ideology and a world-view, while for the religious young man, intimacy is related to “meaningfulness vs. alienation,” due to the centrality of intimacy in his personality. 29 Identity and Intimacy Religious and Non-Religious Adolescent Girls “Solidity and continuity” significantly predicted the general intimacy score for the religiously observant adolescent girls. No predictive ego identity dimension was found for their non-religious counterparts. Regarding the intimacy dimensions, among the religious adolescent girls, the ego identity dimensions predicted all of the intimacy dimensions. The ego identity dimension that was conspicuous in predicting the largest number of intimacy dimensions was “solidity and continuity.” In the case of the non-religiouslyobservant adolescent girls, the ego identity dimensions predicted only two of the seven intimacy dimensions. The religiously observant adolescent girl is quite pre-occupied with the resolution of her intimacy conflict, but the meaning and nature of the conflict is unlike the one characteristic of her non-religious counterpart. The religious girl is interested in establishing social ties with a male partner; however, she sometimes feels that society is not encouraging her to do so. The feeling of religious society (unlike nonreligious society) is that relations of intimacy and establishing a family unit go hand in hand. This society encourages the adolescent girl to marry at a relatively early age, but not as early as the end of high school or during the year of National Service (or its equivalent). That being the case, the religious adolescent girl vacillates between her desire to conform to the pressures of her religious environment, which does not encourage her to become involved intimately at “too early” an age, and her peer and her own pressures urging her to develop social ties with the opposite sex (Fisherman, 2000). In addition, the religiously observant adolescent girl is apprehensive about expressing her feelings in physical relations unacceptable in religious society, and 30 Identity and Intimacy about being swept away by those feelings for a boyfriend. She is afraid of these physical relationships, and this fear is likely to affect both her feeling of intimacy and her feelings about the degree of continuity, stability, and inner balance in closeness with a boyfriend, which find expression in the “solidity and continuity” dimension of her identity. This explanation of the conflict in the societal context helps us understand why the “solidity and continuity” dimension significantly predicted the general intimacy score of the religious adolescent girls. The non-religiously-observant adolescent girl, on the other hand, perceives intimacy as another experience and another dimension in the forming of her ego identity. Non-religious society does not take a negative or a harsh view of physical relations in general, or sexual relations in particular, between adolescents (Shachar, 1977; Ziv, 1984; Smilansky, 1989). Intimate and sexual relations, especially among young people in late adolescence (ages 18-19), are not considered a prelude to building a family, but rather one of the experiences that adolescents have, and perhaps even a contributor to the consolidatation of their ego identities. For this reason, there is not necessarily any relationship between intimacy and the non-religiouslyobservant adolescent girl’s feeling of inner balance. In contrast to the religious adolescent girl, she does not feel tension between her sense of identity and her desire to develop a bond with a member of the opposite sex; therefore, this dimension was not found to predict the general intimacy score. Predicting the intimacy dimensions according to the religious adolescent girl’s ego identity dimensions can be explained in a similar way. “Solidity and continuity” was the dimension that predicted the largest number of intimacy dimensions, due to the great importance the religious adolescent girl ascribes to it. 31 Identity and Intimacy The correlation between the “self control” dimension and the “giving” and “trust” dimensions of intimacy among non-religious adolescent girls was negative. “Giving” is an expression of instrumental and emotional support for another person. Apparently, a non-religious adolescent girl who scores high in “self control” is unwilling to give support, especially emotional support, to her boyfriend. Such a young woman will neither admit any weakness of her own, nor will she understand and accept weakness in her boyfriend easily, and she will regard helping him (especially emotionally) as an affirmation of his weakness. Probably such a young woman, with her understanding of self control in such extreme and absolute terms, will find it hard to give a helping hand to an imperfect friend. She thinks her relationship with a boy who is not perfect threatens the sense of perfection that characterizes her. She feels that, as her self control increases, her feeling of “trust” is also damaged. The dimension of “trust” is related to the individual’s sense that her companion will keep her secrets. The fact that such a girl has a dark secret means that she has something to be ashamed of, and her perfect self-image is therefore without substance. In other words, her confidence in her self-control is diminished. It is worth noting that among the non-religiously-observant adolescent girls, the intimacy dimension “trust” was predicted by four identity dimensions: “commitment and purposefulness,” “self control” (a negative coefficient), “social recognition,” and “genuineness and truthfulness.” The explained variance was over 45%. This point probably reflects the great importance that the non-religious adolescent girl ascribes to “trust”—she probably perceives intimacy as a life experience, or as part of the process of resolving the conflict of her ego identity. In all likelihood, the non-religiously-observant adolescent girl is apprehensive about this perception; that is, she is afraid of the negative image that will be likely to result from 32 Identity and Intimacy switching boyfriends often. Because of this fear, the non-religious adolescent girl emphasizes the dimension of “trust” within the area of “intimacy.” Therefore, perhaps, the intimacy dimension of “trust” was predicted by the dimensions “genuineness and truthfulness” (expressing a feeling of sincerity and a lack of phoniness), “commitment and purposefulness” (expressing commitment to an ideology and a world-view), and “self control” and “social recognition” (the sense that society, including her boyfriend, know her and appreciate her abilities). Religious and Non-Religious Adult Females Among the religiously observant adult females, the general intimacy score was predicted by “solidity and continuity,” while for their non-religious counterparts, “genuineness and truthfulness” was the predictor. Concerning the intimacy dimensions, “solidity and continuity” predicted the highest number of intimacy dimensions (five out of seven) among religiously observant women. Among non-religiously-observant women, the ego identity dimensions predicted all of the intimacy dimensions. The two dimensions that predicted the highest number of intimacy dimensions were “genuineness and truthfulness” and “social recognition.” The fact that among religiously observant women “solidity and continuity” predicted the general intimacy score may be understood in the light of the conservative nature of religious girls. In the section dealing with ego identity, we pointed out that there are many essential changes that occur in the lives of the girls that impact on their identity and on their intimacy more than is the case with the boys. With marriage, girls ordinarily take on a new last name and indicate their personal status (single or married) as an immanent part of their identity. These changes relate to intimacy. This relationship exists particularly for the religiously observant girls 33 Identity and Intimacy because of the conservative attitude toward relations with the opposite sex and premarital sex, characteristic of religiously observant girls (Shachar, 1977; Smilansky, 1989). “Genuineness and truthfulness” predicted the general intimacy score for the non-religious women. This dimension relates to the extent to which a person considers himself as possessing inner honesty, eschewing hypocrisy. The nonreligiously-observant woman at the beginning of adulthood (24-25) sometimes experiences living with a partner of the opposite sex as part of her resolution of the conflict of intimacy. Living together may not be perceived as a prelude to establishing a family or as an alternative to institutionalized marriage, but rather as a life experience and as the desire to closely examine her choice of a partner. This internal test of the interpersonal suitability of the couple, which is sometimes mutual, is intimately connected to the “genuineness and truthfulness” dimension. The young woman is not interested in pretending that she loves her partner so as to continue to maintain her relationship with him, because the purpose of the relationship is to test her feelings. The non-religious young woman is sometimes prepared to live with her boyfriend in order to examine her feelings as well as his, and to see if these feelings can stand the test of time, if they continue to feel deeply enough to raise a family. The partner’s feelings and behavior are constantly being tested, but she also subjects herself to introspection and the most exacting investigation of her own feelings, as manifest in the “genuineness and truthfulness” dimension of the ego identity. For the religiously observant young woman, on the other hand, intimacy is not a testing of a relationship with a partner, but is, rather, an examination of the possibility of spending the rest of her life with the specific partner that she is dating. 34 Identity and Intimacy The research findings regarding prediction of the intimacy dimensions by means of the ego identity dimensions can be explained in a similar manner. The religious young woman, who sees intimacy as a self-examination of the couple’s interpersonal suitability for purposes of raising a family tries, as a rule, to establish relationships of closeness with a number of men, until she finds her intended. She tests him, as she herself is being tested, in spite of the many limitations that are placed on her relations with men. A young woman who feels “solidity and continuity” despite changes in her social situation is the same woman who will manage to build relationships of “honesty” and empathy with her boyfriend, and who will be prepared to “ask him for help.” The non-religious young woman who sometimes experiments with living with a steady boyfriend before marriage does not feel the great gap that is felt by her religious counterpart between the inner experience (love) and her outward behavior (living together), as manifest in “genuineness and truthfulness.” It bears emphasizing the relatively high degree of predictability among religiously observant young women of the “solidity and continuity” and “asking for help” dimensions of intimacy (41%). “Asking for help” refers both to a willingness to ask the partner for help and to the confidence that it will be forthcoming. The religious young woman, who sees in intimacy an examination of her relationship with her boyfriend in order to raise a family with him will indeed be confident that he will help her, and she is therefore willing to continue working on the relationship with him. Therefore, this young woman is also willing to ask for help. If she is not confident of receiving the help, she will probably break off the relationship with her boyfriend, and her sense of stability will also be undermined. When she feels insecure in her choice and breaks up with the man, her feelings regarding “solidity 35 Identity and Intimacy and continuity” are also likely to be shaken, regardless of social and personal changes she may be undergoing. Conclusion To summarize the data, it appears that each of the research population groups has its own identity profile, evident especially when comparing the religiously observant and the non-religiously-observant. The educational and counseling ramifications of these differences are many. We will here note the key educational implications with regard to each sample sub-group: Religious adolescent boys—There is tension between the desire for sexual experimentation and the boy’s religious obligation; his sense of ideological commitment is what helps him to overcome that urge. Understanding this can be of great help to teachers and counselors seeking to help the religiously observant adolescent boy. The existing tension that these boys are subjected to and their sense of ideological commitment, which tends to help ameliorate their tension, are fertile teaching themes, both for the individual and for the group. Non-religious adolescent boys—Intimacy is part of the social system of these boys. “Social recognition” is the dimension that is related to feelings of intimacy Religious adult males—Intimacy is related to raising a family, which has both personal (ego identity) and religious significance. The personal and ideological meaning of raising a family and the interaction and occasional tension between the two can be useful for personal and family counselors. Non-religious adult males—The relationship between identity and intimacy is less complete and significant among these men. It appears that they have just finished dealing with their identity, and they are not yet especially concerned with the 36 Identity and Intimacy intimacy conflict. They are less pre-occupied with sexual experiences, and they are not yet concerned with starting a family. Religious adolescent girls—“Solidity and continuity” occupies a place of great importance with relation to intimacy. Apparently, these girls are interested in stability and in not being swept away in their relationships with boys. They are afraid of getting carried away and of the fact that friendships with boys can change them. An appreciation of the importance of stability for such girls is likely to shed light on their tensions and fears with respect to themselves and to their relationships with boys. Non-religious adolescent girls—The “trust” dimension was the most predicted (and by a high percentage of variance) of all the intimacy dimensions. “Trust” is what gave the non-religiously-observant adolescent girl the sense of a consolidated identity and a positive self-image, regardless of what she has experienced in the area of intimacy. Teachers and counselors can benefit greatly from an understanding of the mechanism within her that balances her desire to fall in love and her need to trust her partner and her relationship with him. Religious adult females—Like the religiously observant adolescent girls, these women also lean towards “solidity and continuity” within their ego identity in relation to intimacy. We see this in the context of the process of seeking a partner for the purpose of raising a family. The search and the internal conflict that it involves means that a great deal of dating is necessary to meet the potential husband, and many questions touching on “Who am I? What do I expect from my life?” arise. Non-religious adult females—For these women, intimacy is not a matter of gaining experience, but rather of a search for a stable, ongoing relationship requiring inner honesty and relating to the desire for spontaneous relations. 37 Identity and Intimacy REFERENCES Adams, G. R., and Fitch, S. A. 1983. Psychological environments at university departments: Effects on college students’ identity status and ego stage development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 12661275. Archer, S.L. 1982. The lower age boundaries of identity development perspective. Journal of Early Adolescence, 7, 223-225. Archer, S. L. 1989. Gender differences in identity development: Issues of process, domain, and timing. Journal of Adolescence, 12, 117-138. Bar-Joseph, H., and Tzuriel, D. 1990. Suicidal attempts, suicidal inclinations, and ego identity among adolescents. Adolescence, 25, 215-223. Beit-Halahmi, B. 1991. Religion and identity: concept, data, questions. Social Science Information,1,81-95 . Berliner, A. J. 2000. Re-visiting Erikson’s developmental model: The impact of identity crisis resolution on intimacy , generatively formation, and psychological adaptation in never-married, middle-aged adults. Ph.D. Diss., New York: Long Island University. Boerio, R.P.1997. The relationship of self-esteem and identity to Christian religious maturity: A study of student at a small, private, liberal arts university. Dissertation Abstracts International, A 57(7-A). Clase, M. E. 1992. Friendship and personal adjustment during adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 15, 39-55. Constantinople, A. 1960. An Eriksonian measure of personality development in college students. Developmental Psychology, 1, 357-372. Douvan, E., and Adelson. J. 1966. The adolescent experience. New York: Wiley. Erikson, E. H. 1958. Young man Luther. New York: Norton & Co. Erikson, E. H. 1968. Youth: Identity and crisis. New York: Farber and Farber. Even-Chen, B. 1993. The influence of biographical milestones, family functioning, perception of self-ability, and ego identity on suicidal tendencies in adolescents (Hebrew). M.A. Thesis, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan. 38 Identity and Intimacy Fayence, A. 1978. Intimacy between partners and placement of control in adolescents (Hebrew). M.A. Thesis, Tel Aviv University. Fisherman, S. 1995. Ego identity, harmony of identity, intimacy, and family factors (Hebrew). Ph.D. Diss., Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan. Fisherman, S. 1998. The youth of the discarded Kipa (Hebrew). Orot College at Elkana and University of Alabama at Birmingham Publication. Fisherman, S. 2000. Why was it lost? The identity of formerly religious girls (Hebrew). Orot College Israel at Elkana. Fisherman, S. 2001. Cognitive complexity and ego identity in talented Israeli adolescent boys. Gifted Education International, 15, 291-301. Gorcyca, D. A. 1993. Gender differences in relationship development and selfdisclosure. Paper presented at the 79th Annual Meeting of the Speech Communication Association, in Miami Beach. - Greenfield, J.J. (1998). Spritual weel-being and identity status among Catholic college students. Dissertation Abstracts International, A. 59(3A). Hershlag, A. 1984. Intimacy and mutual friendship of emotions in married couples in the city and the kibbutz (Hebrew). M.A. Thesis, University of Haifa. Hertz-Lazarovitz, R., Shaharbany, R. and Tal. M. 1983. The influence of the level of intimacy and the level of cognition on changes in moral judgment (Hebrew). Iyunim Bechinuch, 58-78. Hodgson, J. W. and Fischer, J. L. 1979. Sex differences in identity and intimacy development in college youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 8, 3750. Hood, A. B., Riahinejad, A. R. and White, D. B. 1986. Changes in ego identity during the college years. Journal of College Student Personnel, 27(2), 107-113. Jones, G. P. and Dembo M. H. (1989). Age and sex-role differences in intimate friendship during childhood and adolescence. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 445-462. - 39 Identity and Intimacy Josselson, R. L., Greenberger, E. and McConochie, D. E. D. 1977. Phenomenological aspects of psychosocial maturity in adolescence, Part II—“Girls”. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6, 145-167. Josselson, R. L. 1987. Finding herself: Pathways of identity development in women. New York: Jossey-Bass. Junkin, M. F. 2001. Relationship of religious involvement, commitment, and motivation to identity development in late adolescence. Dissertation Abstracts International, Section B. 62 (2B). Kacerguis, M. A. and Adams, G. R. 1980. Erikson stage resolution: The relationship between identity and intimacy. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 9, 117-126. Kahn, S., Zimmerman, G., Csikszentmihalyi, M. and Getzels, J. W. 1985. Relations between identity in young adulthood and intimacy at midlife. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1316-1322. King, P.E. 2003. Religion and identity: The role of ideological, social and spiritual contexts. Applied Developmental Science, 7(3), 197-204. Lee, J., Miller, L. and Chang, E.S. 2006. Identity among Christian Korean American adolescents. Psychological Reports, 98(1), 43-56. Lev-Ran, A. and Sharabany, R. R. 1981. The development of intimate friendship among kibbutz children: A longitudinal study. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Toronto. Marcia, J. E. 1966. Development and validation of ego identity status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551-558. Marcia, J. E. 1980. Identity in adolescence. In Handbook of adolescent psychology, ed., J. Adelson. 159-187. New York: Wiley. Matteson, D.R. 1979. Training and identity formation: A practical proposal for bridging the worlds of school adulthood. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development. Lun. Sweden. Matteson, D. R. 1993. Differences within and between genders: A challenge to the theory. In Ego identity: A handbook for psychosocial research, ed., J. E. Marcia, A. S. Waterman, D. R. Matteson, S. L Archer, and J. L. Orlofsky. New York: Springer Verlag. 40 Identity and Intimacy McKinney, J. P., and McKinney, K. G. 1999. Prayer in the lives of late adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 22(2), 279-290. Nir, M. 1995. The relationship between solitude, family functioning, and consolidation of ego identity in immigrant youth compared with nativeborn adolescents (Hebrew). M.A. Theses, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan. Orlofsky, J. L. 1977. Sex role orientation, identity formation, and self-esteem in college men and women. Sex Roles, 3, 561-575. Orlofsky, J. L., Marcia, J. E. and Lasser, I. M. 1973. Ego identity status and the intimacy versus isolation crisis of young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 211-219. Paterson, S. J., Sochting, I. And Marcia, J. E. 1992. The inner space and beyond: Women and identity. In Adolescent identity formation. Eds., G. R. Adams, T. P. Gullotta, R. Montemayer. G. R. Adams, T. P. Gullotta, and R. Montemayer. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Philipchalk, R. P., and Sifft, C. R. 1985. Role of religious commitment in occupational and overall identity formation in college students. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 44-47. Rasmussen, J. E. 1964. The relationship of ego identity to psychosocial effectiveness. Psychological Reports, 15, 815-825. Rogers, C. R. 1951. Dealing with breakdowns in communication—Interpersonal and intergroup (Hebrew translation from English). In Hadracha Bachinuch [Counseling in Education], eds., M. Amir and R. Vaknin. Jerusalem: Women’s College (1988). Rogers, C. R. 1972. Becoming partners: Marriage and its alternatives. New York: Dilacerate Press. St. Clair, S., and Day, H. D. 1979. Ego identity status and values among high school females. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 8, 317-326. Schenkel, S., and Marcia, J. E. 1972. Attitudes toward premarital intercourse in determining ego identity status in college women. Journal of Personality, 40 (3), 472-482. Schiedel, D. G., and Marcia, J. E. 1985. Ego identity, intimacy, sex role orientation, and gender. Developmental Psychology, 18, 149-160. 41 Identity and Intimacy Shachar, R. 1977. Expectations regarding the role of sex in Israeli youth (Hebrew). M.A. Thesis, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan. Sharabany, R. 1974. Intimate friendship among kibbutz and city children and its measurement. Ph.D. Diss., Cornell University. Sharabany, R., Arnon, A. and Kav-venaki, S. 1981. Intimate friendship among Israeli kibbutz children: Family vs. community-reared. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, in Boston. Sharabany, R., Gershoni, R. and Hofman J. 1981. Girlfriend, boyfriend: Age and sex differences in intimate friendship. Developmental Psychology, 17, 800-808. Sharabany, R., and Toren, Z. 1982. Socialization setting, parental discipline, and children’s moral judgment. Paper presented at the International Symposium on Normal Education, at the University of Fribourg, Switzerland. Sharabany, R., and Wiseman, H. 1993. Close relationships in adolescence: The case of the kibbutz. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 671-695. Sherman, R. M. 2001. Religious orientation and identity formation: A study of adolescent girls. Dissertation Abstracts International. Vol 61 (11B). Simon, W., Berger, A. and Gognon, J. 1978. Beyond anxiety and fantasy: The vital experience of college youth. J. of Youth and Adolescence, 3, 222302. Smilansky, M. 1989. The Challenge of Adolescence: Sexual Identity Between the Sexes. (Hebrew). Tel Aviv: Ramot. Smilansky, M. 1990. The Challenge of Adolescence: Parent/adolescent relations. (Hebrew). Tel Aviv: Ramot. Tesch, S., and Whitbourne, S. 1982. Intimacy and identity status in young adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1041-1051. Tzuriel, D. 1974. Well-formed ego identity as contrasted with diffuse identity as a function of cognitive complexity, ethnocentrism, and nationality identification among oriental and western adolescents (Hebrew). M.A. Thesis, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan. 42 Identity and Intimacy Tzuriel, D. 1984. Sex role typing and ego identity in Israeli oriental and Western adolescents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 440-457. Tzuriel, D. 1990. Ego identity as contrasted with identity diffusion in adolescence: Developmental aspects and educational implications (Hebrew). Megamot, 4, 484-509. Tzuriel, D. 1992. Ego identity and emotional disturbance at adolescence. Paper presented at the 25th International Congress of Psychology, in Brussels Tzuriel, D. 1992a. Development of ego identity among Israeli Jewish and Arab adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26, 206-232. Tzuriel, D., and Bar-Joseph, H. 1986. Identity development and suicidal tendencies in adolescence: Implications for assessment, prevention, and psychotherapy. Paper presented at the 21st International Congress of Applied Psychology, in Jerusalem. Tzuriel, D., and Birger, R. 1997. Program for enhancement of ego identity (PEEI). Paper presented at the Society for Research on Child Development (SRCD), in Washington, D.C. Tzuriel, D., and Klein, M. M. 1977. Ego identity: Effect of ethnocentrism, ethnic identification, and cognitive complexity in Israeli oriental and Western ethnic groups. Psychological Reports, 40, 1099-1110. Whitbourne, S. K., and Tesch, S. A. 1985. A comparison of identity and intimacy statuses in college students and alumni. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1039-1044. Ziv, A. 1984. Adolescence (Hebrew). Tel Aviv: Masada. 43 Identity and Intimacy Author Note .Shraga Fisherman Ph.D. Academic Dean, Orot Israel College, Elkana, Israel Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shraga Fisherman, Orot Israel College, Elkana, D.N. Harei Ephraim 44814, Israel. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to: shraga@orot.ac.il . 44 Identity and Intimacy