Attachment and Supervision 1 © 2006 American Psychological Association "This article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the APA journal. It is not the copy of record." To access the full text of this publication, please visit the Professional Psychology: Research and Practice journal home page at http://www.apa.org/journals/pro/. Attachment Processes in the Supervisory Relationship Shelley Riggs and Karen Bretz University of North Texas Regular Mail: Shelley A. Riggs, Ph.D. Department of Psychology P.O. Box 311280 University of North Texas Denton, TX 76203-1280 Telephone: Facsimile: (940) 565-2672 (940) 565-4682 Electronic Mail: riggs@unt.edu RUNNING HEAD: ATTACHMENT AND SUPERVISION Attachment and Supervision 2 Abstract How do relational variables influence the quality of the supervisory relationship from the supervisee perspective? Recently, it has been suggested that attachment theory might provide some insight into this question, so we explored adult attachment processes and the early parentchild attachment experiences of supervisees in relation to the supervisory working alliance. Findings highlight the relevance of multiple attachment constructs in the supervision process. Results suggest that a history of parental indifference, compulsive self-reliance in supervisees and supervisor attachment style may be particularly important in shaping the supervisory working alliance. Implications for training and supervision are discussed. Attachment and Supervision 3 Attachment Processes in the Supervisory Relationship Clinical supervisors are responsible for the education, guidance and encouragement of the trainees they supervise (Ramos-Sanchez, Esnil, Goodwin, Riggs, Touster, Wright, Ratanasiripong, & Rodolfa, 2002). Because supervisory relationships during training shape the emerging professional identities of young psychologists, they may be the most formative relationships of our professional lives. The foundational nature of these supervisory relationships makes it critical that these experiences be positive for the trainee. Yet, regardless of similarities between supervisors and their trainees in theoretical orientation, personality or relational style, sometimes a positive supervisory working alliance between trainees and their supervisors can remain elusive. Paradoxically, at other times, in spite of seemingly important individual differences between supervisor and supervisee, the supervisory dyad develops into an exceptionally positive relationship. Very little empirical research has explored the impact of individual and interpersonal variables on the supervisory alliance, possibly due to the lack of a suitable theoretical framework. Recently, experts have suggested that attachment theory may provide a strong theoretical foundation from which to conceptualize the supervisory relationship (Pistole & Watkins, 1995;Watkins, 1995). The current project was guided by the key question: In what ways do attachment processes influence the quality of the supervisory working alliance? Bordin (1983) suggested that there was a correlation between the quality of the supervisory alliance and supervisee development. Subsequently, theorists focused on formulating developmental models to explicate the underlying processes of both supervisee and supervisor development (Hess, 1986; Stoltenberg & Delworth, 1987; Watkins, 1990). These theorists hoped that developmentally matched pairings of supervisors and supervisees would enhance the learning experience for both supervisors and supervisees. However, the concept of Attachment and Supervision 4 supervisory developmental levels can be useful in facilitating positive matches only if those levels are identifiable. In an analogue study of 48 supervisors, Chagnon and Russell (1995) examined the ability of supervisors to accurately determine supervisee developmental level based on taped counseling sessions. Regardless of their own experience level, supervisors demonstrated the most difficulty correctly identifying the mid-levels of development (striving for independence, needing a supervisory environment low in structure but high in support). Results indicated that supervisors tended to make judgments about their supervisee’s developmental level by making referent comparisons across cohort groups of supervisees rather than by attending to individual supervisee characteristics. Borders (1989) suggested that individual differences, such as supervisee thoughts and emotions, are relevant to supervision because they may moderate the normative anxiety a supervisee experiences during supervision and counseling. Borders suggested that supervisee characteristics potentially influencing the supervisory alliance include interpersonal maturity, clinical experience level, and relational style. He posited that in tailoring the supervision experience to the needs of individual trainees, an understanding of individual differences across supervisees may be far more informative than the general descriptions posited by developmental stage models in the prediction of a positive supervisory working alliance. To elucidate individual factors contributing to the supervisory experience, researchers have examined variables such as gender (Lichtenberg & Goodyear, 2000; Twohey & Volker, 1993), identity issues (Ellis & Douce, 1994), and trainee and supervisor emotional well-being (White & Queener, 2003). In a recent national survey, psychology interns who reported a negative event in supervision indicated that the experience deleteriously impacted their training and the supervisee-client relationships (Ramos-Sanchez et al., 2002). Qualitative analyses indicated that the majority of Attachment and Supervision 5 negative experiences reported by participants involved interpersonal stylistic differences between supervisees and supervisors. The current study extends Ramos-Sanchez et al.’s research to a quantitative analysis of individual differences in interpersonal style, conceptualized as attachment style, in relation to the supervisory alliance. Supervisory Working Alliance Bordin (1983) argued that the quality of the working alliance between supervisor and trainee was more vital to positive outcomes for the trainee than the particular supervision model or approach to which the supervisor subscribed. He theorized that the relationship between trainees and their supervisors could best be described as a type of attachment bond based on a foundation of mutual trust, which facilitates agreement and understanding of the tasks and goals of supervision. Consequently, the attachment processes activated in the supervisory relationship may play a crucial role in determining the effectiveness of the supervisory working alliance (Epps, 1999). Bordin (1976, 1983) identified three main components to the working alliance: (1) consensus and shared understanding regarding the goals of the change process; (2) the identified tasks of each partner; and (3) the bonds founded upon the level of trust between partners, which is necessary to sustain the working alliance. Bordin postulated that these three distinct dimensions of the working alliance were not equally important during all phases of the helping relationship. His work helped to generalize the concept of the working alliance to all changeproducing relationships, including the therapist-client relationship and the clinical supervisory relationship (Horvath & Greenberg, 1986). Horvath and Greenberg (1986) refined the operational definitions of Bordin’s concepts in order to develop an instrument measuring the working alliance of change-producing Attachment and Supervision 6 relationships, which they called the Working Alliance Inventory. They defined goals as the outcomes mutually agreed upon by supervisor and supervisee as the targets of intervention. Secondly, they described the supervisory tasks as the behaviors within session perceived by both members as relevant and efficacious, and for which each member accepts responsibility. The final contributor to the working alliance is the relationship bond, which they defined as involving “issues of mutual trust, acceptance and confidence” (Horvath & Greenberg, 1986, p. 224). Attachment Theory Bowlby (1979) defined attachment behavior as any action that seeks to attain proximity to a preferred individual, who is considered stronger and more capable of coping with the world. Through early attachment experiences, the individual develops internal working models, or a set of beliefs about the extent to which the individual is lovable and others are trustworthy (Neswald-NcCalip, 2001). Although attachment-based working models of self and others are subject to revision over time, they continue to be salient throughout the life course and are often generalized to alternative attachment figures in new relational contexts (Bowlby, 1988). This is particularly true in times of stress or emergency, which is precisely when individuals who have had negative experiences with early caregivers may exhibit signs of insecure or pathological attachment. Based on the idea that individuals seek security and comfort in attachment relationships (Ainsworth, 1989), the principles of attachment theory have been expanded beyond the infant-caregiver bond to adult romantic relationships (Hazan & Shaver, 1987), therapistclient relationships (Dolan, Arnkoff & Glass, 1993; Dozier, Cue & Barnett, 1994; Farber, Lippert & Nevas, 1995; Ligiero & Gelso, 2002), and supervisory relationships (Neswald-McCalip, 2001; Pistole & Watkins, 1995; Watkins, 1995; White & Queener, 2003). Attachment and Supervision 7 Hazan and Shaver (1987) argued that romantic love involves affective bonds rooted in attachment processes. Based on Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters and Wall’s (1978) description of three infant attachment classifications, Hazan and Shaver (1987) developed a forced-choice, selfreport measure of attachment consisting of short paragraphs identifying relationship features characteristic of the three primary attachment styles. Bartholomew (1990) subsequently expanded Hazan and Shaver’s work, adding a fourth category of adult attachment. Using Bowlby’s (1973) notion that attachment processes are reflected in mental representations of the self and other important people, Bartholomew conceptualized adult attachment style as an interaction between positive or negative internal working models of self and other. Working models of self combine with working models of other to define four adult attachment styles: (a) Secure adults have positive self and other models, so the self is seen as worthy of love and others are seen as trustworthy and available, (b) Dismissing-avoidant adults have a positive model of self, but they have a negative model of others so that they view others as unavailable or rejecting, (c) Preoccupied adults have a negative model of self and view themselves as unlovable and unworthy, but they have a positive model of others, and (d) Fearful-avoidant adults have a negative model of both self and other. Based on this new four-category typology, Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991) developed a measure of adult attachment called the Relationship Questionnaire, which requires participants to self-select one of four paragraphs that best describes their relational style and also to rate each paragraph on a 7-point Likert scale according to how well it describes them. Bowlby (1977, 1980) also suggested that there were distinct forms of pathological attachment behavior: compulsive self-reliance, compulsive care-giving, angry withdrawal and compulsive care-seeking. Compulsive self-reliance is a pattern of behavior that involves doing Attachment and Supervision 8 everything for oneself whatever the conditions. Bowlby theorized that compulsively self-reliant individuals experience poor early attachment relationships and subsequently develop a tendency to inhibit attachment feelings and behavior, demonstrating deep mistrust and fear of depending on others. As an extreme form of avoidant attachment behavior, compulsive self-reliance involves self-sufficiency and distancing behavior. In contrast, compulsive care-giving is reflected in a pattern of exclusively taking on care-giving roles while simultaneously being unable or unwilling to receive care. Although individuals who develop a compulsive care-giving pattern have frequent feelings of martyrdom and self-sacrifice, they nevertheless prioritize others needs before their own whether or not it is requested or even desired by the recipient (West & SheldonKeller, 1994). Compulsive care-seeking, which is analogous to extremely anxious attachment as it was first discussed by Bowlby (1977), involves a pattern of overactive seeking and excessive reliance on attachment figures for care and assistance. Lastly, angry withdrawal was identified by Bowlby as a pattern of negative reactions (e.g., anger, withdrawal) that surface when the attachment figure is perceived as unavailable or unresponsive. West and Sheldon-Keller (1994) developed the Reciprocal Attachment Questionnaire (RAQ) to measure pathological attachment behavior. Although limited, research using the RAQ has addressed several psychological issues with different populations. Using a sample of undergraduate college students, Lapsley, Varshney, and Aalsma (2000) demonstrated that RAQ pathological attachment scales were significantly related in theoretically expected ways with adult attachment style, assessed by Bartholomew and Horowitz’s (1991) Relationship Questionnaire, although the two instruments appeared to tap different aspects of the attachment system. Secure adult attachment style was negatively correlated with compulsive self-reliance, and moderately positively correlated with compulsive care-giving. Fearful attachment was Attachment and Supervision 9 positively correlated with angry withdrawal and compulsive self-reliance, while dismissing attachment was negatively correlated with compulsive care-seeking and positively correlated with compulsive care-giving. Preoccupied attachment was positively associated with compulsive care-giving, angry withdrawal, and compulsive care-seeking. Furthermore, RAQ pathological attachment behavior was significantly associated with adjustment problems, pathology of separation-individuation, and psychopathological symptoms. In another study examining pathological attachment in cases of spousal bereavement, Field and Sundin (2001) found that compulsive care-seeking was correlated with an inability to cope and the experience of emotional numbness. Compulsive self-reliance was correlated with positive thoughts about the spouse, whereas angry withdrawal was correlated with negative thoughts about the spouse; both were correlated with unwanted thoughts and feelings. In addition, higher scores of psychopathology were associated with both compulsive care-seeking and angry withdrawal. Similarly, in their study comparing the attachment processes of eating disorder patients with non-eating disorder patients controls, Ward, Ramsey, Turnbull, Benedettini, and Treasure (2000) found that eating disorder patients reported more compulsive care-seeking and compulsive self-reliance behaviors than controls. Overall, the scarce literature available indicates that individuals demonstrating high levels of pathological attachment are likely to be insecure (Lapsley et al., 2000) and may be more vulnerable to adjustment difficulties and psychopathological behaviors (Field & Sundin, 2001; Ward et al., 2000). Attachment Processes and Clinical Supervision Pistole and Watkins (1995) have argued that attachment theory is useful for conceptualizing interpersonal relationships, as well as counseling and supervision processes, because the theory is based on the principle that emotionally significant relationships play a Attachment and Supervision 10 critical role in development. The primary goal of clinical supervision is to facilitate the supervisee’s professional development, which often corresponds with the supervisee’s personal development. Consequently, important elements of an individual’s attachment style may emerge in the context of supervision. Similar to the role of the clinician in the therapeutic alliance, the supervisor should function as a secure base from which supervisees can freely explore and develop their professional identities as therapists, knowing that a dependable and reliable supervisor/attachment figure will be available to support them (Pistole & Watkins, 1995). Yet, even when the supervisor provides safe and reliable support, supervisees’ insecure attachment processes may surface in various forms during the supervision process (Pistole & Watkins, 1995). For example, the compulsively self-reliant supervisee may be distant and have difficulty asking for or receiving supervisory assistance, perhaps even challenging the supervisor’s authority but feeling wronged by perceived supervisor criticism. Supervisees manifesting an angry withdrawal pattern may be hyper-vigilant to signs that the supervisor is inattentive to their needs and paradoxically distance themselves from the supervisor. In contrast, the compulsively care-giving supervisee may be overly solicitous of the supervisor or clients, may have difficulty in receiving help from the supervisor, seeking instead to be of assistance to the supervisor. Conversely, the compulsively care-seeking supervisee may seek to retain proximity to the supervisor, and may expect the supervisor to take primary responsibility for the supervisee’s professional development. Because attachment processes are so fundamental and mostly unconscious, a close examination of the supervisory relationship may uncover pathological attachment patterns that are affecting the working alliance. According to Watkins (1995), when a clinical supervisor begins questioning his or her professional judgment, Attachment and Supervision 11 agonizing over lack of supervisory progress, or experiencing strong emotions in the supervisory relationship, insecure attachment may be present in the supervisory relationship. In spite of the increasing number of theorists and researchers who have recommended the exploration of relational variables in supervision (e.g., Pistole & Watkins, 1995; Epps, 1999), few studies have examined the role of attachment processes in the context of the clinical supervisory relationship. Fewer still have used Bordin’s (1983) theory of the working alliance to operationalize supervision variables and none have explored the potential influence of childhood attachment experiences or pathological attachment behaviors on supervisory processes. However, Epps (1999) examined adult attachment style and supervisory rapport among master’s level supervisee/supervisor dyads using the Supervisory Working Alliance Inventory (Efstation, 1990; SWAI). Epps found that securely attached supervisees rated supervisory rapport higher than insecurely attached supervisees. Of the supervisory dyads, 39% had secure supervisors and secure supervisees, 24% had insecure supervisors and supervisees, 20% of the dyads had insecure supervisors and secure supervisees, and 17% of the dyads had insecure supervisors and secure supervisees. Findings indicated that dyads in which one or both members were insecurely attached had greater variations in scores than those dyads in which both were securely attached. Compared to insecure supervisees, securely attached supervisees perceived a better supervisory bond with both secure and insecure supervisors, suggesting that securely attached supervisees may experience more positive relational interactions regardless of their supervisors’ attachment styles. Similarly, Kim (1997) found that trainee attachment characteristics accounted for 11% of the variance in the trainees’ satisfaction with supervision, and 19% of the variance in the trainees’ rating of the quality of the supervisory bond. Results also indicated that trainee attachment security explained a significant amount of the variance in trainee perceptions of the Attachment and Supervision 12 supervisor’s interpersonal style. Trainees who worried that others would not care for them as much as they cared for others tended to be less satisfied with supervision, while trainees secure in their ability to relate to others were more likely to be satisfied with supervision. Some research suggests that the supervisor characteristics may be more important than supervisee characteristics in determining the quality of the supervisory relationship than the supervisee characteristics. In a sample of 67 supervisor-supervisee dyads, White and Queener (2003) reported that the supervisors’ ability to make healthy adult attachments predicted both their own and their trainees’ perceptions of the supervisory working alliance. Furthermore, the supervisor’s ability to make attachments and accept social support was more predictive of the quality of the supervisory alliance than the same characteristics in supervisees. Similarly, Summerel and Borders (1996) found that the strength of the supervisory working alliance was more highly correlated with the supervisor’s interactional style than individual supervisee characteristics. In summary, while empirical evidence is slowly emerging to support the notion that attachment processes actively contribute to the quality of supervision and the supervisory relationship, many questions remain regarding the contribution of various attachment processes to different aspects of the supervisory working alliance. We were interested in several different attachment constructs that theoretically are likely to influence the supervisory working alliance (goal, task, and bond), including parent-child attachment experiences, pathological attachment behaviors, and adult attachment style. Based on the assumption that early childhood attachment experiences continue to affect relational behavior throughout the lifespan, particularly in relationships that in many ways mirror the hierarchical organization of the parent-child relationship, we explored the potential associations between childhood attachment and the Attachment and Supervision 13 working alliance. Next, we examined Pistole and Watkins’ (1995) hypotheses regarding the impact of supervisee pathological attachment behavior on the supervisory alliance. Then, we tested the predictions related to supervisor and supervisee adult attachment styles: (a) we expected that both supervisor and supervisee adult attachment style would be associated with aspects of the working alliance, especially supervisory bond, and (b) we expected that working alliance ratings for supervisory dyads with secure supervisors and supervisees would be higher than ratings for supervisory dyads composed of at least one (or both) insecure member. After these preliminary analyses, we explored a path model linking the various attachment processes to the three components of the working alliance (See Figure 1). National Supervision Study Attachment and Supervision 14 Two hundred internship sites from all 50 states were randomly selected from the 2003 APPIC manual. The internship directors of these sites were sent email, which described the study and requested that they forward to their current interns an invitation to participate and the online address for the survey. The directors were also asked to reply to the principal investigators with the number of interns who had received the invitation to participate in the study. Sixty-one directors complied with this request (30% response rate for directors), indicating that a total of 173 interns received the invitation to participate. The interns were given 2 months to complete the survey. Approximately one month after the original message, the directors were sent a second email asking them to remind their interns of the deadline for participation. A total of 87 interns completed the online survey before the two-month deadline elapsed. After one case was deleted due to missing data, a final sample of 86 participants produced a 50% response rate for the interns. A five-part survey requiring approximately 30 minutes to complete included the following: (a) a demographic form; (b) the Working Alliance Inventory (WAI; Horvath & Greenberg, 1994) to assess the task, bond and goal components of the supervisory working alliance; (c) the Relationship Questionnaire (RQ; Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991) to assess respondents’ adult attachment styles (i.e., Secure, Fearful, Dismissing, Preoccupied) and their perceptions of their supervisors’ attachment styles; (d) the Memory of Parental Styles (MOPS; Parker et al., 1997) to assess early attachment experiences with parents, including parental indifference and over-control; and (e) the Reciprocal Attachment Questionnaire (RAQ; West & Sheldon-Keller, 1994) to assess pathological attachment behaviors, including compulsive selfreliance, compulsive care-giving, compulsive care-seeking and angry withdrawal. Participants Attachment and Supervision 15 Survey data were collected from psychology interns at their internship placement in the Spring of 2004 (N=86). The total sample consisted of 66 women (77%) and 20 men (23%), who were predominantly of Caucasian (78.2%) descent, ranging in age from 25 to 54 (M = 32.6). Forty-five percent of respondents were completing their Ph.D.’s in Clinical Psychology, 17.2% in Counseling Psychology, 2.3% in School Psychology, and 33.3% were completing a Psy.D. in Clinical Psychology. Approximately 30% of respondents were working at a Veteran’s Administration Hospital, 13.8% at a Counseling Center, 10.3% at an Armed Forces Medical Center, 10.3% at a Medical School, 9.2% at State or County Hospital, 8.0% at a Consortium, and the remaining 17.2% were at federal prisons, state hospitals, or at institutions they identified as “other.” In order to protect the anonymity of respondents, information regarding the geographic location or name of facility was not requested. Fifty-four percent of the sample indicated that they were married, 27.6% were dating someone exclusively, 3.4% were dating someone casually, and 13.8% were not presently involved. Findings Preliminary Analyses Prior to testing the path model, preliminary analyses were conducted to provide descriptive statistics (See Table 1), explore interrelations among attachment constructs, and test direct associations between attachment processes and the supervisory alliance. Consistent with previous research using non-clinical samples (e.g., Brennan, Clark & Shaver, 1998; Brennan & Shaver, 1998), over half (59.9%) of the respondents identified themselves as Secure, 18.4% Fearful, 9.2% Avoidant, 5.7% Preoccupied, and six participants did not self-identify a predominant adult attachment style. In terms of the participants’ perceptions of their supervisor’s attachment style, 50.6% identified their supervisor as Secure, 12.6% Fearful, 19.5% Attachment and Supervision 16 Avoidant, 12.6% Preoccupied, and four participants did not indicate supervisor attachment style. Participant adult attachment style was not associated with participant gender, ethnicity, age, or sexual orientation. Contrary to expectations, results of Pearson correlations for the three supervisory alliance scales and both parent-child and pathological attachment scales were non-significant. Subsequently, a series of MANOVAs were performed to assess the relationship of the supervisory working alliance (goal, task, bond) to participant attachment style, supervisor attachment style, and the dyadic configuration of the supervisory alliance (See Table 2). Although results of the MANOVA examining participant attachment style and the supervisory alliance were significant, F (81) = 2.30, p < .02, follow-up univariate tests were non-significant. With respect to perceived supervisor attachment style, MANOVA results indicated that supervisor attachment style was also significantly related to the supervisory alliance, F (83) = 3021, p = .001. In this case, however, follow-up ANOVAs showed significant associations between supervisor attachment style and supervision task, F (83) = 4.29, p = .007, as well as the supervisory bond, F (81) = 5.32, p = .002, but not supervision goal, F (83) = 1.02, p = .38. These findings indicate that participants who perceived their supervisors to have a Secure attachment style rated the supervisory task and bond significantly higher than participants who perceived their supervisors to be Preoccupied or Dismissing. Finally, we conducted a MANOVA comparing secure-secure supervisory dyads (n = 31), insecure-insecure dyads (n=17), and two types of mixed dyads composed of either secure participants/supervisees and insecure supervisors (n = 18) or insecure participants/supervisees and secure supervisors (n = 12). Results showed that dyadic composition was significantly associated with the supervisory alliance, F (78) = 3.82, p < .000. Subsequent univariate tests with post-hoc comparisons indicated that Attachment and Supervision 17 secure-secure dyads had significantly higher scores on supervision task than dyads composed of a secure participant/supervisee and insecure supervisor, F (78) = 3.29, p = .025. Similarly, secure-secure dyads and dyads composed of an insecure participant/supervisee and secure supervisor had significantly higher scores on supervisory bond than dyads with a secure participant/supervisee and insecure supervisor, F (81) = 5.33, p = .002. Path Analysis The Latent Variable Path Analysis with Partial Least squares estimation procedure (PLS) (Lohmoeller, 1984) was used to examine the association between supervisee attachment processes and their supervisory relationships. We chose this approach because PLS allows the examination of hypothesized relationships between theoretical constructs free from the restrictions of measurement and distributional assumptions (Chin, 1998). Additionally, this procedure is ideally suited for use on small samples with multiple measures (Cowan, Cohn, Cowan & Pearson, 1996). In PLS, hypotheses about the directionality of relationships between latent variables are identified based on knowledge of theory and previous research. Based on path coefficients and correlations between latent constructs, as well as the variance accounted for on latent variables, the model(s) explaining the most amount of variance is selected for interpretation. For the present analyses, several models were generated based on the theoretical and empirical literature. Per Falk and Miller’s (1992) recommendations for interpretation of PLS analyses, manifest variables in the outer model demonstrating a low factor loading (e.g., below .55) or negative correlation with other manifest variables measuring the same latent construct were excluded from the analyses. When path coefficients between latent variables in the inner model did not explain greater than 5% of variance, pathways were deleted and the trimmed Attachment and Supervision 18 model was re-run. To establish a more stable path coefficient than the initial analysis, the bootstrapping procedure was run, which involved the model being re-run one thousand times and calculating the mean path coefficient for each pathway in the model. Based on the theoretical and empirical literature, an initial model including the constructs of interest was hypothesized (See Figure 1). The two hypotheses underpinning the initial model were as follows: 1) supervisee’s parent-child attachment experiences shape the supervisees’ adult attachment style, which in turn may influence the development of pathological attachment behavior, and 2) the supervisory bond will predict the goal and tasks of supervision, as well as the supervisees’ perceptions of his or her supervisor’s attachment style. Based on the results of analyses using the initial model, several more theoretically-sound models were subsequently tested. The models exhibiting the maximal fit with the data will be presented. Although PLS output does not include an overall goodness of fit statistic between the model and the data, we utilized the formula for an overall F test of the R² statistic recommended by Falk and Miller (1992) to calculate the goodness of fit statistic. Additionally, we chose to calculate Cohen’s f² effect size statistic for multiple partial correlations to examine the magnitude of the effect present in the model. All models presented can be interpreted as representing the pathways from the supervisee’s parent-child attachment to current adult attachment processes and to the supervisory relationship. MODIFY TO INCLUDE PARTICIPANT ATTACHMENT STYLE. In Figure 1, the latent variables of the supervisee’s experiences of parental attachment (i.e., parental indifference and over-control) and current pathological attachment (i.e., compulsive self-reliance, care-giving, care-seeking, and angry withdrawal) are shown to influence the supervisory bond, which in turn affects the supervisees’ perceptions of the tasks and goals of supervision as well as his or her Attachment and Supervision 19 perceptions of the supervisor’s attachment style. Results indicated that the supervisees’ experiences of parental indifference predicted the manifestation of compulsive self-reliance (path weight [pw] = .492), and compulsive care-seeking (pw = .433), as well as angry withdrawal (pw = .349). However, experiences of parental over-control predicted only compulsive care-seeking (pw = .249). Next, results indicated that the largest path coefficients leading to the supervisory bond were compulsive care-giving (pw = .165) and angry withdrawal (pw = .150). Finally, the path coefficients leading from the supervisory bond to the supervisee’s perceptions of the supervisor’s attachment style, as well as the tasks and goals of supervision were examined. The supervisory bond predicted the supervision tasks (pw = .884), supervision goals (pw = .848), and the supervisee’s perception of the supervisor’s attachment style (pw = .352). However, the F test for the overall goodness of fit indicates non-significance, F (15, 71) = 10.549, ns. Figure 2 represents another theoretically-sound model developed to explain the data. Two pathways leading to the task component of supervision are shown. The first pathway shows the supervisees’ recollection of indifferent parent-child attachment leading to compulsive selfreliance, whereas the second pathway shows self-reported attachment style leading to perceptions of the supervisor’s attachment style. As shown in Table 3, the supervisees’ early attachment experiences of parental indifference predicted the manifestation of supervisee compulsive self-reliance (path weight [pw] = .43), which then predicted supervisees’ perceptions of the tasks in supervision (pw = .191). Participants’ self-reported adult attachment styles (secure, fearful, anxious, and avoidant) predicted their perceptions of their supervisor’s attachment classification (pw = .359), which then predicted the supervisees’ perception of the tasks of supervision (pw = .361). Contrary to hypotheses, findings indicated that attachment processes most strongly predicted the in-session tasks and behaviors of supervision. In turn, Attachment and Supervision 20 supervisory task ratings strongly predicted the supervisees’ perceptions of shared supervisory goals (pw = .935), and supervisory bond (pw = .894). According to Cohen’s (1992) delineation of the magnitude of effect sizes, the effect size of f² = .555 for the second model qualifies as large. The large effect size in this model indicates that changes in the endogenous latent variables are likely to cause significant change in the latent variables (i.e., task, goal, bond) to which the endogenous variable pathways point. The F test for the overall goodness of fit indicates F (6, 79) = 44.638, p < .0001. The second model was trimmed to include only those manifest variables or indicators loading most heavily on the endogenous latent variable constructs. Because the latent variable of self-reported adult attachment style did not meaningfully predict any aspect of the supervisory working alliance, it was deleted from the model. As a result, the trimmed model included twelve indicators on the parental indifference scale of parental attachment, and six indicators on the compulsive self-reliance scale of pathological attachment. The trimmed model was re-run and the result is presented in Figure 3. As in the previous model, all pathways met statistical significance at the .05 level (see Table 4). Results indicated that experiences of parental indifference predicted compulsive self-reliance in supervisees (pw = .564), which then predicted their perceptions of the supervisor’s attachment style, (pw = .326). In turn, supervisor attachment style predicted the evaluation of the in-session tasks in supervision (pw = -.355), which then predicted the reported quality of the supervisory bond (pw = .874), and goals of supervision (pw = .945). The third model evidences a larger effect size than the previous model at f² = .749. The F test for the overall goodness of fit, F (5, 80) = 59.972, p < .0001, indicates that the model is a very good fit with the data. Implications Attachment and Supervision 21 The main premise of attachment theory is that attachment relationships form the foundation of ongoing development, and that attachment processes are salient throughout the lifespan (Bowlby, 1988). Because the purpose of supervision is to facilitate the growth and development of the supervisee, the supervisory relationship can incorporate important elements of other significant relationships and therefore elicit attachment responses (Pistole & Watkins, 1995), especially when involving issues around autonomy, authority and individuation (Watkins, 1995). The findings of the present study support the theoretical and empirical literature (Epps, 1999; Kim, 1997; Neswald-McCalip, 2001; Pistole & Watkins, 1995; Watkins, 1995; White & Queener, 2003) and extend our understanding of attachment processes at work in the supervisory working alliance by providing empirical evidence that attachment constructs representing diverse levels of experience in childhood and adulthood are relevant to clinical supervision even at advanced stages of training and clinical experience. Specifically, the final path model showed that the a childhood history characterized by parental indifference leads to compulsively selfreliant attachment behavior, which influences perceptions of supervisor attachment styles, which in turn first affects the supervisory task, then the supervisory bond and goal. This model provides a logical representation of the developmental chain that contributes to the supervisee’s experience in the supervisory alliance. The following discussion will consider the implications of current findings for clinical training and supervision. Contrary to expectations, current results indicated that neither parent-child attachment experiences nor pathological attachment behaviors were significantly correlated with ratings for supervisory task, goal, and bond. In addition, while an overall MANOVA was significant, selfrated adult attachment style was not significantly related to any of the supervisory alliance scales in follow-up univariate analyses. Because MANOVA takes into account correlations among Attachment and Supervision 22 variables and considers differences on all variables jointly whereas univariate tests do not (Stevens, 1996), this finding suggests that the significant comparison may involve some linear combination of the three supervisory variables that does not emerge when each scale is examined separately. That is, it appears that the supervisee’s adult attachment style is not directly associated with a specific aspect of the supervisory alliance, but rather is associated with an overall view of the supervisory process that integrates task, goal, and bond, representing the “big picture,” so to speak. An informal examination of scale means in search of patterns among the attachment groups showed that the Dismissing group of supervisees had the lowest mean ratings for all three supervisory alliance scales; they reported less effective in-session task-related behaviors, less consensus regarding supervision goal, and an inferior bond with their supervisor. Consistent with theoretical expectations for Dismissing adults to minimize the importance of relationships, viewing themselves as “perfect” and others as unworthy (Bartholomew, 1990; Bowlby, 1973), this finding suggests that Dismissing supervisees may devalue the fundamental nature of the supervisory alliance, possibly considering themselves “above” supervision and acting independently without regard to supervisory guidance. Supervisors working with these individuals may initially be pleased with the independence they display, but should be cautious in allowing too much independence and may wish to check in regularly to ascertain if the supervisee has incorporated the supervisor’s suggestions in work with clients. Special efforts to strengthen the supervisory relationship and sensitively challenge noncompliance may be necessary to avoid defensiveness and promote a good working alliance with Dismissing supervisees. Although analyses showed no direct linear correlation between the supervisory alliance and parent-child attachment experiences or pathological attachment behavior in adulthood, Attachment and Supervision 23 results of the path analyses indicated that these two attachment constructs contribute indirectly to the quality of the supervisory alliance via their association with the supervisees’ perceptions of their supervisors’ attachment styles. Current findings are consistent with theoretical assumptions regarding the importance of the parent-child attachment relationship in the development of adult attachment behavior. In particular, the model supports theoretical predictions (e.g., Bowlby’s, 1977; West & Sheldon-Keller, 1994) that early parental indifference and/or rejection play a critical role in promoting compulsively self-reliant behavior. Attachment processes may be particularly relevant to the supervisory process when they are pathological in nature (Watkins, 1995). Of the four pathological attachment styles identified by Bowlby (1977), the present results suggest that compulsive self-reliance may have the most impact on the supervisory relationship. This finding is consistent with the pattern of alliance ratings described above for Dismissing individuals, who are theoretically more likely to demonstrate compulsive selfreliance (West & Sheldon-Keller, 1994). According to Watkins (1995), the compulsively selfreliant supervisee refuses help, is defiant towards authority (e.g., disparaging the supervisor, disregards the supervisor’s comments) and actively distances him- or herself from the supervisory relationship. Compulsive self-reliance may have the most impact on the supervisory relationship because it most directly challenges the trainees’ ability to be open to the supervisor’s feedback. Compulsively self-reliant supervisees may experience extreme discomfort with evaluation and alleviate this discomfort by devaluing the quality of supervision they are receiving. In contrast, compulsive care-seekers or care-givers may not be as inclined to discount or subvert the supervision process. Because self-reliance is an adaptive and valued quality in many graduate programs, pathological forms may be less readily perceptible to supervisors and training directors until it overtly intrudes in the supervision process. Once recognized, given Attachment and Supervision 24 childhood experiences with parental indifference and/or rejection, it would be extremely important for the supervisor to avoid repeating these interaction patterns in the supervisory relationship. Direct criticism can be eschewed in favor of constructive feedback, and a balance between the consistent demonstration of empathic guidance and encouragement of the supervisee’s autonomy can be sought. Consistent with findings of White and Queener (2003), results of the current study highlighted the importance of the supervisor’s role in setting the tone of the supervisory working alliance. In addition to path analyses, both multivariate and two univariate follow-up analyses for supervisor attachment style were significant. Supervisees who saw their supervisors as securely attached tended to evaluate the supervisory task and bond more positively than supervisees who saw their supervisors as preoccupied or dismissing. Similarly, results showed that secure-secure dyads were associated with higher ratings on the supervisory task than mixed dyads composed of secure supervisees and insecure supervisors. Insecure participants with secure supervisors rated the supervisory task only slightly lower than their secure counterparts. It is interesting that ratings by secure participants represented the polar extremes of the quality of the supervisory alliance. Perhaps secure individuals are more attuned to interpersonal behaviors, and thus more likely to notice and respond to the attachment styles of others. Importantly, regardless of their own attachment style, supervisees with secure supervisors rated the supervisory bond more positively than supervisees with insecure supervisors. Results of the path analysis also indicated that the supervisee’s assessment of his or her supervisor’s attachment style predicted the supervisee’s assessment of the task of supervision, and indirectly, the supervisory bond and goal. Consistent with preliminary analyses and previous research (Summerel & Borders, 1996; White & Queener, 2003), the final model Attachment and Supervision 25 indicated that rather than the supervisee’s own attachment style, it was his/her perception of the supervisor’s attachment style that had the most direct impact on the supervisory alliance. From a theoretical and applied standpoint, this is an extremely important point that should not be overlooked. Previous theorists have focused on the role of supervisee attachment behavior (e.g., Watkins, 1995); however, current results suggest that it is the supervisor’s attachment style that is critical to the success of the supervision process. Specifically, like the parent-child attachment relationship, the supervisory relationship is inherently a hierarchical one, in which the supervisor has more power in the relationship than the supervisee. Attachment researchers have documented the process of intergenerational transmission whereby a parent’s attachment style influences their caregiving behavior (e.g., responsiveness vs. inconsistency/role-reversal or rejection/neglect), which in turn contributes to the development of secure or insecure attachment in their infants (see van IJzendoorn, 1995 for meta-analytic review). Like a parent, the supervisor’s attachment style sets the tone of the relationship via its impact on the supervisor’s in-session task-related behavior, which in turn influences the attachment bond with the supervisee. By virtue of his/her greater power and knowledge, the bulk of responsibility for the quality of the supervisory alliance lies with the supervisor, not the supervisee. Contrary to theoretical hypotheses that attachment processes would most strongly predict the supervisory bond, in-session behaviors and tasks were found to strongly predict the ability of the supervisee to build a bond with his or her supervisor, and to subjectively assess the extent to which the supervisory relationship is goal-oriented. It is possible that attachment processes may be more directly related to the supervisory bond among less advanced trainees, who presumably require more nurturance and interpersonal support than the clinical interns in this sample. An alternative explanation for this finding is that the supervisor’s in-session task-related behavior is Attachment and Supervision 26 analogous to parental caregiving behavior, which is assumed to mediate the parents’ internal working models of attachment and attachment behavior in their infants. Current findings indicate that in-session behaviors are reflections of the supervisors’ attachment style, and as such may be characterized by “caregiving” behaviors that indicate whether the supervisor can be trusted to respond sensitively to supervisee cues, or conversely demonstrate inconsistency/role-reversal or rejection/neglect. For example, if a supervisee perceives his or her supervisor as being disorganized or inattentive to the agenda during session, the trainee might perceive the supervisor as untrustworthy or unable to supervise effectively, which would negatively affect the supervisory bond and goal. One implication of this finding is that the supervisor should closely monitor their own in-session behaviors because the identification of supervisory tasks that are tailored to supervisee needs and the manner in which those tasks are performed will provide the foundation on which the supervisory bond will be built and the supervisory goals agreed upon. Current findings underscore the need for supervisors to attend to specific aspects of how time in supervision is spent (e.g., the number of times supervisors watch tapes, the quantity of clients discussed, the clarity with which expectations are conveyed and responsibilities discussed, the agreement upon and adherence to an agenda for each supervision meeting), which can provide clarity of purpose within and across supervision sessions that may improve the supervisees’ perception of the supervision sessions as both structured and useful, contributing to a strong supervisory bond and consensus regarding goals. Limitations and Conclusions Current findings should be interpreted in light of several limitations. Most important are limitations related to sample selection. In order to obtain a national sample, supervisors were not included in the study and therefore an important element of the supervisory dyad is absent from Attachment and Supervision 27 these analyses. Because supervisors were excluded from our study, we were only able to include an indirect measure of the supervisor’s attachment style (i.e., asking the supervisee to speculate about his or her supervisors’ attachment orientation). In addition, at the recommendation of several previous researchers interested in attachment and supervision, we focused on supervisees at advanced levels of training (i.e., internship). As a result, these findings may not be generalizable to trainees with less clinical and supervision experience. Future research should include both members of the supervisory dyad and clinicians at all levels of training with their respective supervisors to gain a more comprehensive picture of the role of attachment processes in the context of clinical supervision. Another limitation of the present study involves measurement issues. First, self-report measures were the only type of measurement utilized, which may have introduced subjective bias. Researchers have recommended caution when relying on self-reports with Dismissing/avoidant individuals, who may have overly positive selfimages and defensive strategies designed to minimize emotion and self-evaluation, making selfdisclosure less likely (Dozier, 1990; Dozier, Stevenson, Lee, & Villegan, 1991; Kobak & Sceery, 1988; Pianta et al., 1996; Riggs & Jacobvitz, 2002; Rothbard & Shaver, 1994). There are also some important limitations inherent in the correlational and Partial Least Squares data analytic strategies we chose for the present analyses. Because PLS makes no measurement, distributional or sample size assumptions, like the correlational findings, these results cannot be interpreted to suggest causality. The goal of PLS is to make modest predictions rather than causal claims. Despite these limitations, the current study shed some light on the role of attachment processes in clinical supervision and is the first study to examine the associations of early attachment experiences and pathological attachment behavior to the quality of the supervisory alliance. Path analyses generated a model that showed a logical predictive progression of Attachment and Supervision 28 different attachment processes leading to the supervisory alliance. Results suggested that early attachment experiences with parents contribute to pathological attachment behavior that influences the supervisee’s perception of their supervisor’s attachment style, which in turn is a factor in the perceived quality of in-session task-related behaviors that affect the supervisory bond and goal. Of particular significance, findings emphasize the importance of the supervisor’s attachment style in the functioning of the supervisory alliance. Based on these findings, future theory and research concentrating on supervisor qualities that contribute to the ability to create an effective working alliance may provide us with a better understanding of the supervision process and improve training. Moreover, current results highlight the critical importance of formal graduate training in the provision of clinical supervision, a course that is still not required in many graduate clinical and counseling programs. Formal supervision coursework, as well as self-exploration or personal psychotherapy, which educates trainees regarding the potential impact of personal qualities like attachment style on their supervision skills will improve clinical training and may allow some clinicians to override an insecure attachment style in the service of providing effective supervision. Attachment and Supervision 29 References Ainsworth, M. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709–716. Andersson, P., & Eisemann, M. (2004). Parental rearing and substance related disorders—A multi-factorial controlled study in a Swedish sample. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 11, 392–400. Andersson, P., & Perris, C. (2000). Perceptions of parental rearing and dysfunctional attitudes: The link between early experiences and individual vulnerability? Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 54, 405–409. Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment perspective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 147–178. Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of the four-category model. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 61, 226–245. Borders, E. (1989). A pragmatic agenda for developmental supervision research. Counselor Education & Supervision, 29, 16–24. Bordin, E. (1983). Supervision in counseling: II Contemporary models of supervision: A working alliance based model of supervision. Counseling Psychologist, 11, 35–42. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1977). The making and breaking of affectional bonds: I Aetiology and psychopathology in the light of attachment theory. British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 201–210. Bowlby, J. (1979). On knowing what you are not supposed to know and feeling what you are not supposed to feel. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 25, 403–408. Attachment and Supervision 30 Bowlby, J. (1988). Developmental psychiatry comes of age. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 1–10. Brennan, K., Clark, C., & Shaver, P. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. Simpson & W. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York: Guilford Press. Brennan, K., & Shaver, P. (1998). Attachment styles and personality disorders: Their connections to each other and to parental divorce, parental death, and perceptions of parental care-giving. Journal of Personality, 66, 835–878. Carnelley, K., Peitromonaco, P., & Jaffe, K. (1994). Depression, working models of others, and relationship functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 127–140. Chin, W. (1998). The partial least squares approach to structural equation modeling. In G. A. Marcoulides (Ed.), Modern methods for business research (pp. 295–336). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Chin, W., & Newsted, P. (1998). Structural equation modeling analysis with small samples using partial least squares. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Statistical strategies for small sample research (pp. 307–341). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cobb, R., Davila, J., & Bradbury, T. (2001). Attachment security and marital satisfaction: The role of positive perceptions and social support. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1131–1143. Cowan, P., Cohn, D., Cowan, C., & Pearson, J. (1996). Parents’ attachment histories and children’s externalizing and internalizing behaviors: Exploring family systems models of linkage. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 53–63. Dolan, R., Arnkoff, D., & Glass, C. (1993). Client attachment style and the psychotherapist’s Attachment and Supervision 31 interpersonal stance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 30, 408–412. Dozier, M., Cue, K., & Barnett, L. (1994). Clinicians as caregivers: Role of attachment organization in treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 793–800. Ellis, M., & Douce, L. (1994). Group supervision of novice clinical supervisors: Eight recurring issues. Journal of Counseling & Development, 72, 520–525. Epps, M. (1999). The effect of attachment styles on the working alliance in counselor supervision. (Doctoral dissertation, Texas A&M. University, 1999). Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 5833. Farber, B., Lippert, R., & Nevas, D. (1995). The therapist as attachment figure. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 32, 204–212. Feeney, J. A. (1999). Adult romantic attachment and couple relationships. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Ed.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 355–377). New York: Guilford Press. Field, N., & Sundin, E. (2001). Attachment style in adjustment to conjugal bereavement. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 347–361. Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Models of the self and other: Fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 430–445. Hanson, W., Curry, K., & Bandalos, D. (2002). Reliability generalization of working alliance inventory scale scores. Educational and PsychologicalMeasurement, 62, 659–673. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,511–524. Hess, A. (1987). Psychotherapy supervision: Stages, Buber, and a theory of relationship. Attachment and Supervision 32 Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 18, 251–259. Horvath, A., & Greenberg, L. (1986). The development of the working alliance inventory. In L. Greenberg & W. Pinsof (Eds.), Psychotherapeutic process: A research handbook (pp. 529–556). New York. Guilford Press. Horvath, A., & Greenberg, L. (1989). Development and validation of the working alliance inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36,223–233. Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1994). The working alliance: Theory, research, and practice. Oxford, England: Wiley. Kim, S. (1998). Influence of trainee attachment on satisfaction with supervision, the supervisory relationship, and perceptions of supervisory style. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland. Kirkpatrick, L., & Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment styles and close relationships: A four-year prospective study. Personal Relationships, 1, 123–142. Lapsley, D., Varshney, N., & Aalsma, M. (2000). Pathological attachment and attachment style in late adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 137–155. Lichtenberg, J., & Goodyear, R. (2000). The structure of supervisor–supervisee interactions. Clinical Supervisor, 19, 1–24. Ligiero, D., & Gelso, C. (2002). Countertransference, attachment, and the working alliance: The therapist’s contribution. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 39, 3–11. Lohmo¨ller, J.-B. (1989). Latent variable path modeling with partial least squares. Heidelberg, Germany: Physica-Verlag. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Weller, A. (1993). Attachment styles, coping strategies, and posttraumatic psychological distress: The impact of the Gulf War in Israel. Journal of Attachment and Supervision 33 Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 817–826. Murray, S., Holmes, J., & Griffin, D. (1996). The self-fulfilling nature of positive illusions in romantic relationships: Love is not blind, but prescient. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1155–1180. Neswald-McCalip, R. (2001). Development of the secure counselor: Case examples supporting Pistole & Watkins’ (1995) discussion of attachment theory in counseling supervision. Counselor Education & Supervision, 41, 18–27. Parker, G., Roussos, J., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Mitchell, P., Wilhelm, K., & Austin, M. (1997). The development of a refined measure of dysfunctional parenting and assessment of its relevance in patients with affective disorders. Psychological Medicine, 27, 1193–1203. Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. (1979). A parental bonding instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1–10. Pistole, C., & Watkins, C. E. (1995). Attachment theory, counseling process, and supervision. The Counseling Psychologist, 23, 457–478. Ramos-Sanchez, L., Esnil, E., Goodwin, A., Riggs, S., Touster, L., Wright, L., et al. (2002). Negative supervisory events: Effects on supervision satisfaction and supervisory alliance. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 33, 197–202. Scharfe, E., & Bartholomew, K. (1998). Do you remember? Recollections of adult attachment patterns. Personal Relationships, 5, 219–234. Simoneau, T., & Le, P. L. (Eds.). (2003). The internship and postdoctoral programs in professional psychology. Washington, DC: Association of Psychology Postdoctoral and Internship Centers. Stevens, J. (1996). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Attachment and Supervision 34 Erlbaum. Stoltenberg, C., & Delworth, U. (1987). Supervising counselors and therapists: A developmental approach. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Summerel, M., & Borders, L. (1996). Addressing personal issues in supervision: Impact of counselors’ experience level on various aspects of the supervisory relationship. Counselor Education and Supervision, 35, 268–286. Twohey, D., & Volker, J. (1993). Listening for the voices of care and justice in counselor supervision. Counselor & Education, 32, 3–17. van IJzendoorn, M. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 387–403. Ward, A., Ramsay, R., Turnbull, S., Benedettini, M., & Treasure, J. (2000). Attachment patterns in eating disorders: Past in the present. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 370–376. Watkins, C. E. (1990). The separation/individuation process in psychotherapy supervision. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training,27, 202–209. Watkins, C. E. (1995). Pathological attachment styles in psychotherapy supervision. Psychotherapy, 32, 333–340. West, M., & Sheldon-Keller, A. (1994). Patterns of relating: An adult attachment perspective. New York: Guilford Press. White, V., & Queener, J. (2003). Supervisor and supervisee attachments and social provisions related to the supervisory working alliance. Counselor Education & Supervision, 42, 203–218. Attachment and Supervision 35 Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations for Subscales Subscale Correlations (n = 86) Mean 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 __ .46*** -.31** .12 -.49*** -.30** -.02 -.10 -.11 __ __ -.18 .24* -.25* .02 -.04 -.06 -.07 __ __ __ -.02 .45*** .36*** -.03 .05 .10 __ __ __ __ -.22* .10 .07 .02 -.07 __ __ __ __ __ .28** -.16 -.04 .06 __ __ __ __ __ __ -.08 .02 -.05 __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ .81*** __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ __ (SD) 1. Parent 6.53 Indifference (6.95) 2. Parent 11.99 Overcontrol (5.48) 3. Angry 29.06 Withdrawal (4.52) 4. Compulsive 20.77 Care-Giving (2.18) 5. Compulsive 29.14 Self-Reliance (3.82) 6. Compulsive 29.67 Care-Seeking (3.46) 7. Goal 44.13 .84*** .76*** (6.02) 8. Task 48.29 (6.29) 9. Bond 60.90 (10.74) * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001 Attachment and Supervision 36 Table 2 MANOVAs and Follow-up Tests for Attachment Style and Supervisory Alliance Participant Attachment Style N = 81 Supervisory Alliance Secure Fearful Preoccupied M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Dismissing F M (SD) MANOVA 2.30* Goal 43.12 (6.26) 46.62 (3.40) 46.35 (3.89) 42.75 (9.25) 1.73 Task 49.38 (6.19) 48.87 (4.13) 47.83 (5.64) 45.62 (11.65) .49 Bond 60.72(11.29) 63.56 (5.27) 61.33 (8.14) 54.12 (17.25) 1.32 Supervisor Attachment Style N = 83 Secure Fearful Preoccupied M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) Dismissing MANOVA 3.21*** Goal 45.06 (4.80) 45.27 (4.47) 43.18 (6.79) 42.35 (9.01) 1.02 Task 50.16 (4.27)a 49.36 (4.65) 45.00 (6.68)b 44.94 (9.59)b 4.29** Bond 64.57 (6.82)a 60.74 (11.80) 57.64 (9.49)b 53.47 (15.55)b 5.32** Dyadic Configuration N = 78 Insecure SecP/InsS InsP/SecS Secure M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F M (SD) MANOVA 3.82*** Goal 44.48 (5.12) 44.94 (6.79) 41.05 (7.91) 46.32 (3.88) 2.17† Task 50.31 (4.18)a 46.59 (8.22) 44.94 (8.22)b 49.51 (4.74) 3.29* Bond 64.67 (7.76)a 57.95 (13.15) 53.44 (13.69)b 64.29 (4.19)a 5.32* Note. Means with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05 or greater. SecP = Secure Participant/Supervisee, InsS = Insecure Supervisor, InsP = Insecure Participant/Supervisee, SecS = Secure Supervisor. † F M (SD) p < .10, * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001 Attachment and Supervision 37 Table 3 Path Coefficients, Standard Deviations and Confidence Intervals for Predictors of Supervisory Task, Bond, and Goal in Model 2 (N = 86) ______________________________________________________________________________ Pathway Path Coefficient SD 95% Confidence Interval Self Reliance to Task .209* .104 .1, .417 Supervisor Attachment to Task -.350* .108 -.566, -.134 Task to Bond .894* .031 .832, .956 Task to Goal .928* .017 .894, .962 Parent Indifference to Self Reliance -.431* .139 -.153, .-709 Attachment to Supervisor Att .377* .131 .246, .508 ______________________________________________________________________________ Note. * = pathway met statistical significance at the .05 level. Attachment and Supervision 38 Table 4 Summary of Path Coefficients, Standard Deviations and Confidence Intervals for Variables Predicting the Supervisory Task, Bond, and Goal in Model 3 (N = 86) ______________________________________________________________________________ Pathway Path Coefficient SD 95% Confidence Interval Self Reliance to Task .326* .105 .116, .536 Task to Bond .876* .034 .808, .944 Task to Goal .945* .011 .923, .967 Task to Supervisor Attachment -.372* .093 -.186, -.558 Parent Attachment to Self Reliance -.564* .070 -.424, -.704 ______________________________________________________________________________ Note. * = pathway met statistical significance at the .05 level. Attachment and Supervision 39 Figure 1. Latent Variable Path Analysis (path model 1) Figure 1. Latent variable path analysis: Hypothesized full model of the influence of parent-child attachment and pathological attachment on the supervisee’s perception of the supervisory bond which in turn influences how the supervisee perceives his or her supervisor’s attachment style, as well as the tasks and goals of supervision. Attachment and Supervision 40 Figure 2. Latent Variable Path Analysis (path model 2) Figure 2. Latent variable path analysis: Influence of parental attachment and pathological attachment, as well as self-reported attachment style and the supervisee’s perception of the supervisor’s attachment style on the supervisory task, goal, and bond. Attachment and Supervision 41 Figure 3. Latent Variable Path Analysis (path model 3) Figure 3. Latent variable path analysis: Influence of parental indifference and compulsive selfreliance on the supervisory task, goal, bond and perceptions of the supervisor’s attachment style.