1 Manuscript in press Psychophysiology fMRI in an oddball task: Effects of target-to-target interval Michael C. Stevens1,2, Vince D. Calhoun1,2 and Kent A. Kiehl1,2 1 Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, The Institute of Living, Hartford, CT 2 Yale University, New Haven, CT Corresponding Author: Kent A. Kiehl, Ph.D Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Whitehall Building The Institute of Living / Hartford Hospital Hartford, CT 06106 (860) 545-7552 Ph (860) 545-7997 Fax email: kent.kiehl@yale.edu Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 2 1 ABSTRACT 2 The amplitude of the P3 event-related potential (ERP) elicited by task-relevant target (“oddball”) 3 stimuli has been shown to vary in proportion to the length of time between targets. Here we use 4 functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to identify neural systems modulated by target 5 interval in a large sample of healthy adults (n=100) during performance of an auditory oddball 6 task that included both target and novel stimuli. A positive relationship was found between 7 target interval and hemodynamic activity in the anterior cingulate and in bilateral lateral 8 prefrontal cortex, temporal-parietal junction, postcentral gryi, thalamus and cerebellum. This 9 modulation likely represents updating of the working memory template for the target stimuli. 10 There was no such effect of novel interval, suggesting that neuronal modulation may only occur 11 for task-relevant stimuli, possibly in the service of strategic resource allocation processes. 12 13 KEY WORDS: ODDBALL, FMRI, NOVELTY, SEQUENCE, TARGET, P3 14 Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 3 15 fMRI of target-to-target interval 16 17 Oddball tasks often require detection of infrequent target stimuli within the context of 18 frequently presented standard stimuli (Polich & Kok, 1995; Sutton, et al., 1965) – for example, 19 the detection of a high-pitched tone in a sequence of low-pitched tones. This produces a 20 characteristic ERP waveform with a prominent positive peak at approximately 300 msec (i.e., the 21 P3). In oddball tasks that require a response to a particular infrequent stimulus type, the P3 is 22 thought to reflect cognitive processes necessary for updating working memory representations of 23 task-relevant stimuli (Donchin & Coles, 1988). Oddball tasks sometimes also include a low 24 probability task-irrelevant stimuli, such as novel, non-repeating random noises, which are 25 thought to elicit automatic attentional orienting responses (Friedman, et al., 1993). 26 There is great interest in determining what factors modulate the neural response to 27 oddball target and various types of nontarget stimuli. Several experiments have demonstrated 28 that global target probability (Duncan-Johnson & Donchin, 1977), the inter-stimulus interval 29 (Polich, 1990), or the order of nontarget and target stimuli (Polich & Bondurant, 1997) modulate 30 the P3. However, these manipulations also change the absolute time between targets (i.e., 31 referred to as target-to-target interval, or target interval). Gonsalvez and colleagues have argued 32 that target interval may better explain target-P3 modulation than other factors (Croft, et al., 2003; 33 Gonsalvez, et al., 1995; Gonsalvez, et al., 1999; Gonsalvez & Polich, 2002). Because 34 intervening novel stimuli do not affect target P3 amplitude (Courchesne, et al., 1977; Johnson & 35 Donchin, 1980), this modulation might occur only for task relevant stimuli. However to date, no 36 study has explored whether novel-to-novel interval influence neural activity, despite the 37 observation that preceding sequence length influences nontarget P3 in a manner analogous to Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 4 38 target interval effects on the target P3 (Johnson & Donchin, 1980; Sams, et al., 1983; Verleger & 39 Berg, 1991). 40 Because of the difficulty inferring the location of ERP neural generators in general 41 (Baillet & Garnero, 1997; Pascual-Marqui, et al., 1994) and the P3 in particular (Halgren, et al., 42 1995a; Halgren, et al., 1995b; Halgren, et al., 1998), it is not clear which brain structures might 43 be modulated by target intervals. The measurement of brain hemodynamics using fMRI 44 provides a means to identify which structures are influenced by oddball target interval 45 manipulations. One of the largest fMRI auditory oddball studies to date (Kiehl, et al., 2005) 46 replicated and extended over a dozen previous fMRI oddball target detection studies, showing 47 that hemodynamic activity is elicited in numerous, widespread cortical and subcortical brain 48 structures during target detection and novelty processing. This study also showed that activation 49 elicited by target and novel stimuli was extremely reliable in the vast majority of these regions. 50 Few studies have examined the influence of the interval length between target stimuli on 51 hemodynamic activity. Horovitz and colleagues (2002) found that the amplitude of the 52 hemodynamic response elicited by target stimuli was positively correlated with amplitude of the 53 P3 in left supramarginal gyrus, thalamus, bilateral insulae, and right medial frontal gyrus. 54 Similarly, Casey, et al. (2001) reported that activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex increased as 55 the target stimulus probability decreased. Because lower target probability increases target 56 interval, this latter finding may be related to target interval. 57 Here we examine the influence of the length of target-to-target intervals on hemodynamic 58 activity elicited by target and novel stimuli by re-analysis of a large (n=100) fMRI dataset 59 (Kiehl, et al., 2005). Based on previous studies (Casey, et al., 2001; Horovitz, et al., 2002), it 60 was hypothesized that target interval length would be positively correlated with the amplitude of Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 5 61 hemodynamic response in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, supramarginal gyrus, bilateral inferior 62 frontal gyri, and thalamus. Because the response to target stimuli is thought to reflect updating 63 of working memory representations, it also was predicted that target interval length would 64 modulate right inferior parietal lobule and inferior frontal gyrus activity (Fletcher & Henson, 65 2001). A secondary aim was to examine the effect of novel interval length on hemodynamic 66 response to novel stimuli. Because intervening task-irrelevant infrequent stimuli do not 67 influence target P3 ERP (Courchesne, et al., 1977; Johnson & Donchin, 1980) and novel stimuli 68 elicit different cognitive processes from target detection, we did not anticipate novel interval to 69 modulate hemodynamic activity in the same manner as target interval. However, previous ERP 70 studies have not directly addressed this question. Therefore, specific hypotheses were not made. 71 72 73 74 Methods Participants One-hundred healthy right-handed volunteers (53 men and 47 women, mean age 29.2 75 years (range: 18 – 62; SD 10.03) participated in the study. Further demographic and sampling 76 details are presented it Kiehl et al. (2005). 77 78 79 Task and Procedure The experimental task was a three-stimulus auditory oddball task previously used in both 80 ERP and fMRI studies (Kiehl, et al., 2001a; Kiehl, et al., 2001b; Kiehl, et al., 2005). The 81 standard stimulus was a 1000 Hz tone (probability, P ~0.80), the target stimulus was a 1500 Hz 82 tone (P ~0.10), and the novel stimuli (P ~0.10) were non-repeating random digital noises (e.g., 83 tone sweeps, whistles). There were 196 standard stimuli, 24 target stimuli, and 24 novels stimuli Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 6 84 in each of two runs, for a total of 48 target and 48 novel stimuli per subject. Participants were 85 instructed to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible with their right index finger when a 86 target tone occurred and not to respond to other stimuli. Stimuli were presented for 200 msec 87 with a 2000 msec stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). Target intervals ranged from 8 to 68 88 seconds (Session 1 mean=18.7, SD=10.95; Session 2 mean=20.4, SD=14.95). Novel intervals 89 ranged from 8 to 62 seconds (Session 1 mean=20.4, SD=13.68; Session 2 mean=19.7, 90 SD=12.55). In total, there were 15 separate target intervals and 17 novel intervals. There were 91 between 1 to 10 events in each target interval (mean=3.42, SD=2.93) and between 1 to 10 events 92 in each novel interval (mean=2.82, SD=2.74). Within each target interval, between 0 to 4 novels 93 occurred. In each novel interval, there were between 0 to 5 targets. Target and novel stimuli 94 always were preceded by between 3 to 5 standard stimuli (8 to 12 seconds). 95 96 Imaging Parameters, Image Processing and fMRI Modeling 97 Functional image volumes were collected in axial orientation to the AC-PC line using a 98 gradient-echo sequence sensitive to the BOLD signal (TR/TE 3000/40 ms, flip angle 90o, FOV 99 24×24 cm, 64×64 matrix, 62.5 kHz bandwidth, 3.75×3.75 mm in plane resolution, 5 mm slice 100 thickness, 29 slices) effectively covering the entire brain (145 mm). The two runs consisted of 101 167 time points each, prefaced by a rest period of 12 seconds that was discarded prior to 102 subsequent processing to remove T1 stabilization effects. 103 Functional images were processed using techniques described in greater detail in Kiehl, 104 et al. (2005). Briefly, images were reoriented to the anterior commisure/posterior commisure 105 plane, corrected for motion using an algorithm unbiased by local signal changes (INRIAlign; 106 (Freire & Mangin, 2001; Freire, et al., 2002), spatially normalized to standardized MNI space Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 7 107 (Friston, et al., 1995), and smoothed with a 12 mm3 FWHM kernel (see Kiehl et al, 2005, for a 108 comparison of different smoothing kernels) using Statistical Parametric Mapping 2 software. 109 The regressors for each participant’s fMRI model were derived by extracting stimulus onset 110 timings for all target, novel, or standard stimuli. The effects of target and novel interval were 111 evaluated using 1st order parametric terms, following the analysis framework described in detail 112 in (Buchel, et al., 1998). This orthogonalized expansion term models brain activity relative to 113 target or to novel stimuli that shows linear increases in the amplitude of hemodynamic response 114 over time. For each target (or novel) stimulus, the parametric modulator was the time in seconds 115 since the previous target (or novel) stimulus. The first target or novel stimulus in the timeseries 116 used the time since session start for the parametric modulator. Each participant’s model included 117 terms for 1) standard stimuli relative to implicit baseline, 2) target stimuli relative to implicit 118 baseline, 3) novel stimuli relative to implicit baseline, 4) target stimuli × target interval linear 119 change, and 5) novel stimuli × novel interval linear change. The implicit baseline represents 120 variance in the timeseries that is not explicitly modeled, including error. 121 For group analyses, images containing latency variation-corrected (Calhoun, et al., 2004) 122 effects of target or novel interval on the amplitude of the hemodynamic response were entered 123 into separate one-sample t-test random effects analyses. Statistical significance was evaluated 124 using a threshold of p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons using the familywise error rate 125 (Worsley, et al., 1996). 126 Significant regions were examined further by using a cluster analysis to group regions 127 with similar patterns of covariation across target interval lengths. A hierarchical cluster analysis 128 (SPSS 12.0) examined the average between-groups linkage based on a Pearson correlation 129 similarity measure. This measure was selected because it is less sensitive to size effects that Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 8 130 often arise in distance functions. Clustering was performed using extracted mean peak 131 amplitudes for each target interval length. 132 133 Mean reaction time for each target interval was compared using repeated-measures ANOVA (Greenhouse-Geiser corrections) and post hoc tests for linear and quadratic trends. 134 135 136 137 Results Behavioral Performance Mean target interval reaction time data was available for 99 of 100 subjects. Mean 138 reaction time differed across target interval lengths (F(1,98) = 5.426, p = .022) and there was a 139 significant linear trend (F(1,98) = 14.225, p = .0003), such that longer target intervals were 140 associated with slower reaction times. Reaction time means (SD) for each target interval are 141 listed in Table 1. 142 143 144 Hemodynamic Response Modulation by length of Target-to-Target or Novel-to-Novel Interval Figure 1 illustrates the location of target interval effects in the brain images and Table 1 145 lists peak voxel locations, t-scores, and parametric contrast beta coefficients. Target interval 146 linear effects were found in numerous brain structures, including bilateral superior/middle and 147 inferior frontal gyrus, bilateral insulae, medial frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate, bilateral pre- and 148 postcentral gyri, bilateral inferior and superior parietal lobules, left precuneus, posterior 149 cingulate, thalamus, bilateral middle temporal gyri, left cuneus/lingual, thalamus, right putamen 150 and bilateral cerebellum. All of the above regions showing significant target interval effects 151 previously have been observed to be active during target detection (Kiehl, et al., 2005; Stevens, Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 9 152 et al., in press). There were no brain regions that showed an inverse relationship between target 153 interval length and the amplitude of the hemodynamic response. 154 At the thresholds appropriate for this large sample (p < .05, corrected for multiple 155 comparisons to search the entire brain volume using the familywise error rate), novel interval did 156 not modulate the hemodynamic response to novels in any brain region. At liberal thresholds (p < 157 .001 uncorrected for multiple comparisons), a linear effect of novel interval was found in 158 cingulate gyrus (x,y,z = -4, 4, 44, t99 = 3.95), right inferior parietal lobule (x,y,z = 60, -36, 24, t99 159 = 4.45), left superior temporal gyrus (x,y,z = -64, -24, 0, t99 = 4.56), and right middle temporal 160 gyrus (x,y,z = 60, -32, 0, t99 = 3.99). 161 A cluster analysis on target interval data indicated that there were two patterns of 162 hemodynamic change across target intervals. Representative graphs depicting peak 163 hemodynamic activity across the 15 target interval time bins are displayed in Figure 2. One 164 cluster comprised regions that showed a sharp rise in the amplitude of hemodynamic response 165 across target intervals from ~30 to ~50 seconds. The second cluster showed a relatively smooth 166 increase of peak hemodynamic activity across all target interval lengths tested. Cluster 167 memberships are listed in Table 1. 168 169 170 Discussion This study tested the hypothesis that longer intervals between target stimuli would be 171 associated with increased hemodynamic activity to subsequent targets. Consistent with 172 hypotheses, target interval effects were observed in anterior cingulate, medial frontal gyrus, 173 bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex regions, left supramarginal gyrus, bilateral inferior frontal 174 gyri, and thalamus. In addition, target interval modulated hemodynamic activity in bilateral Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 10 175 temporal lobe structures, bilateral postcentral gyri, and bilateral cerebellum. Therefore, the 176 present results replicate and extend previous studies. The current study differed from previous 177 reports in that it examined the relationship of target interval to hemodynamic activity rather than 178 correlating hemodynamic activity to peak ERP amplitude (Horovitz, et al., 2002). The present 179 study also employed a wider range of target intervals and a larger sample than used in previous 180 studies. At appropriate statistical thresholds, there was no evidence that hemodynamic activity 181 elicited by novel stimuli was modulated by novel-to-novel interval. 182 P3 ERPs elicited by oddball stimuli are commonly interpreted within the theory of 183 contextual updating proposed by Donchin and Coles (1988). In this theory, increased neural 184 activity with longer target intervals likely reflects the process of updating a working memory 185 model as the environmental context is altered by ongoing changes. Alternatively, Gonsalvez and 186 colleagues (Croft, et al., 2003; Gonsalvez, et al., 1995; Gonsalvez, et al., 1999; Gonsalvez & 187 Polich, 2002) suggest that target interval effects reflect working memory template restoration 188 following systematic degradation over time. This account is similar to the contextual updating 189 model, but temporal factors are emphasized over subjective or global probability, which are 190 important for contextual updating theory. Activity of brain regions within at least one of the two 191 clusters may reflect target working memory model updating. This cluster includes regions 192 believed to mediate working memory and attentional control, particularly for salient stimuli, 193 including bilateral middle frontal gyri, bilateral insulae, right inferior frontal gyrus, bilateral 194 inferior parietal lobule, anterior and posterior cingulate (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002). The 195 anterior cingulate increases in activation during conditions involving conflict (Carter, et al., 196 1998) or error monitoring (Gehring, 1993; Kiehl, et al., 2000). Therefore, this activity may 197 reflect a cumulative process of increasing target expectancy (Donchin & Coles, 1988; Squires, et Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 11 198 al., 1976). Alternatively, several researchers have suggested that lateral prefrontal and anterior 199 cingulate activity might influence norepinephrine modulation of P3 in oddball contexts 200 (Nieuwenhuis, et al., 2005). Nieuwenhuis and colleagues have proposed that phasic 201 norepinephrine activity driven by the outcome of response decision-making may serve to 202 enhance future top-down mediated selective attention for salient stimuli. They predict that 203 target-to-target interval would enhance neural response in brain areas active during task-relevant 204 target processing, while no modulation would be seen to motivationally insignificant (i.e., task- 205 irrelevant novel) stimuli, which is the pattern of results found in this study. This raises the 206 possibility that part of the working ‘memory template restoration’ process may be to tune the 207 neural network, facilitating the ability to process future target stimuli. 208 Gonsalvez and colleagues also propose that target and nontarget stimuli working memory 209 templates might be independently represented in the brain, rather than jointly contributing to a 210 general “context” for cognitive processing (Croft, et al., 2003). The current results are consistent 211 with this proposal. The lack of modulation of the hemodynamic response by novel interval, 212 particularly in the presence of modulation by target stimuli, is consistent with the proposed 213 independence of working memory templates. The fact that both target and novel stimuli were 214 infrequent (i.e., both P ~0.10) strongly suggests task salience, rather than global stimulus class 215 probability, was the more influential factor modulating neural response to infrequent stimuli. 216 However, it also might reflect an absence of a stimulus-bound memory template for the class of 217 continually-changing ‘novel’ stimuli. This could be tested in future studies by contrasting the 218 effect of nontarget interval between infrequent stimuli that are novel versus those that are 219 repeated. Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 12 220 The other cluster of brain regions with a target interval effect showed a marked increase 221 in activity after approximately 30 seconds. This cluster comprised brain regions known to be 222 involved in motor preparation (e.g., premotor, supplementary motor cortex) and execution 223 (bilateral cerebellum) and top-down influences on attentional shifting (e.g., bilateral superior 224 parietal lobule) (Behrmann, et al., 2004; Cunnington, et al., 2003; Rushworth, et al., 2003). 225 These brain regions might show additional activity when the brain must engage additional neural 226 resources to prepare a new motor response following sufficient passage of time. Many of these 227 brain regions recently have been linked to two motor preparation processes – motor readiness 228 potentials (Cunnington, et al., 2003) and contingent negative variation processes (Nagai, et al., 229 2004). However, these latter effects typically are observed following a cue or a decision for 230 volitional movement, which might differentiate them from the apparently automatic phenomenon 231 captured by the target interval analysis. The relationship of activity in these brain regions and 232 reaction time is not yet clear, but both show a significant linear increase over the intervals 233 measured in this experiment. Therefore, it is possible there is a relationship between increased 234 motor preparation neural activity and reaction time. 235 It also is possible that the current findings might be related to well-described contextual 236 factors other than target interval that also influence P3 amplitude. Although the current 237 paradigm controlled infrequent event global probability (P ~ .10), interstimulus interval (2 238 seconds), and preceeding nontarget structure (range 3 to 5 standards), it did not control for other 239 sequential structure issues such as local target/novel stimulus probability. Local nontarget 240 probability might alter participants’ subjective sense of the likelihood of an upcoming target 241 stimulus (Donchin & Coles, 1988; Squires, et al., 1976). Therefore, the current results should be 242 interpreted cautiously until such time as data are available that address this possibility. These Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 13 243 findings also might not reflect the same phenomena observed in published ERP studies because 244 the range of target and novel intervals in this experiment was greater than that typically used 245 (e.g., <16 seconds). A comparison of how brain activity is modulated by short target intervals 246 (i.e., 1-10 seconds) versus long target intervals (i.e., 8–60 second range) would help to clarify the 247 relationship between EEG and fMRI target interval-modulation. It also should be noted that the 248 reaction time analyses may be limited because several target intervals averaged few 249 observations, which raises the possibility that these means were less stable than others. Finally, 250 because previous studies focus on target interval effects for P3 ERP amplitude and latency, it is 251 not conclusively known if target interval alters other ERP components. Because the 252 hemodynamic response to targets reflects neural activity to multiple target-elicited ERPs, it is 253 possible that the current findings may be related to other cognitive processes (i.e., not those 254 reflected by the P3). 255 256 Conclusion 257 The present experiment demonstrates that longer target intervals are associated with 258 increased hemodynamic activity in a large network of brain structures, possibly containing two 259 functional subnetworks. Although additional research is needed to more conclusively link fMRI- 260 measured target interval-modulations to the target interval modulations of ERP data, the current 261 results likely reflect the same neuronal phenomenon previously identified in ERP research. The 262 results support the hypothesis that target interval modulates neural activity in diverse brain 263 structures by updating of working memory processes. 264 Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 14 265 References 266 Baillet, S., & Garnero, L. (1997). A Bayesian approach to introducing anatomo-functional priors 267 in the EEG/MEG inverse problem. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng, 44(5), 374-85. 268 Behrmann, M., Geng, J.J., & Shomstein, S. (2004). Parietal cortex and attention. Curr Opin 269 270 271 272 Neurobiol, 14(2), 212-7. Brown, J.W., & Braver, T.S. (2005). Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science, 307(5712), 1118-21. Buchel, C., Holmes, A.P., Rees, G., & Friston, K.J. (1998). Characterizing stimulus-response 273 functions using nonlinear regressors in parametric fMRI experiments. Neuroimage, 8(2), 274 140-8. 275 Calhoun, V.D., Stevens, M.C., Pearlson, G.D., & Kiehl, K.A. (2004). fMRI analysis with the 276 general linear model: removal of latency-induced amplitude bias by incorporation of 277 hemodynamic derivative terms. Neuroimage, 22(1), 252-7. 278 Carter, C.S., Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., Botvinick, M.M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J.D. (1998). 279 Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. 280 Science, 280(5364), 747-749. 281 Casey, B.J., Forman, S.D., Franzen, P., Berkowitz, A., Braver, T.S., Nystrom, L.E., Thomas, 282 K.M., & Noll, D.C. (2001). Sensitivity of prefrontal cortex to changes in target 283 probability: a functional MRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 13(1), 26-33. 284 285 Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G.L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci, 3(3), 201-15. Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 15 286 Courchesne, E., Hillyard, S.A., & Courchesne, R.Y. (1977). P3 waves to the discrimination of 287 targets in homogeneous and heterogeneous stimulus sequences. Psychophysiology, 14(6), 288 590-7. 289 290 291 Croft, R.J., Gonsalvez, C.J., Gabriel, C., & Barry, R.J. (2003). Target-to-target interval versus probability effects on P300 in one- and two-tone tasks. Psychophysiology, 40(3), 322-8. Cunnington, R., Windischberger, C., Deecke, L., & Moser, E. (2003). The preparation and 292 readiness for voluntary movement: a high-field event-related fMRI study of the 293 Bereitschafts-BOLD response. Neuroimage, 20(1), 404-12. 294 295 Donchin, E., & Coles, M.G.H. (1988). Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 357-374. 296 Duncan-Johnson, C.C., & Donchin, E. (1977). On quantifying surprise: the variation of event- 297 related potentials with subjective probability. Psychophysiology, 14(5), 456-67. 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 Fletcher, P.C., & Henson, R.N. (2001). Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain, 124(Pt 5), 849-81. Freire, L., & Mangin, J.F. (2001). Motion correction algorithms may create spurious brain activations in the absence of subject motion. Neuroimage, 14(3), 709-22. Freire, L., Roche, A., & Mangin, J.F. (2002). What is the best similarity measure for motion correction in fMRI time series? IEEE Trans Med Imaging, 21(5), 470-84. Friedman, D., Simpson, G., & Hamberger, M. (1993). Age-related changes in scalp topography to novel and target stimuli. Psychophysiology, 30(4), 383-96. Friston, K.J., Ashburner, J., Frith, C.D., Poline, J.-B., Heather, J.D., & Frackowiak, R.S.J. 307 (1995). Spatial registration and normalization of images. Human Brain Mapping, 2, 165- 308 189. Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 16 309 310 311 Gehring, W.J. (1993). The error-related negativity: Evidence for a neural mechanism for errorrelated processing, U Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, US. Gonsalvez, C.J., Gordon, E., Anderson, J., Pettigrew, G., Barry, R.J., Rennie, C., & Meares, R. 312 (1995). Numbers of preceding nontargets differentially affect responses to targets in 313 normal volunteers and patients with schizophrenia: a study of event-related potentials. 314 Psychiatry Res, 58(1), 69-75. 315 Gonsalvez, C.J., Gordon, E., Grayson, S., Barry, R.J., Lazzaro, I., & Bahramali, H. (1999). Is the 316 target-to-target interval a critical determinant of P3 amplitude? Psychophysiology, 36(5), 317 643-54. 318 319 320 Gonsalvez, C.L., & Polich, J. (2002). P300 amplitude is determined by target-to-target interval. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 388-96. Halgren, E., Baudena, P., Clarke, J.M., Heit, G., Liégeois, C., Chauvel, P., & Musolino, A. 321 (1995a). Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. I. 322 Superior temporal plane and parietal lobe. Electroencephalography and Clinical 323 Neurophysiology, 94(3), 191-220. 324 Halgren, E., Baudena, P., Clarke, J.M., Heit, G., Marinkovic, K., Devaux, B., Vignal, J.P., & 325 Biraben, A. (1995b). Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and 326 visual stimuli. II. Medial, lateral and posterior temporal lobe. Electroencephalography 327 and Clinical Neurophysiology, 94(4), 229-50. 328 329 Halgren, E., Marinkovic, K., & Chauvel, P. (1998). Generators of the late cognitive potentials in auditory and visual oddball tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 106(2), 156-64. Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 17 330 Horovitz, S.G., Skudlarski, P., & Gore, J.C. (2002). Correlations and dissociations between 331 BOLD signal and P300 amplitude in an auditory oddball task: a parametric approach to 332 combining fMRI and ERP. Magn Reson Imaging, 20(4), 319-25. 333 334 Johnson, R., Jr., & Donchin, E. (1980). P300 and stimulus categorization: two plus one is not so different from one plus one. Psychophysiology, 17(2), 167-78. 335 Kiehl, K.A., Laurens, K.R., Duty, T.L., Forster, B.B., & Liddle, P.F. (2001a). An event-related 336 fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21, 221- 337 240. 338 Kiehl, K.A., Laurens, K.R., Duty, T.L., Forster, B.B., & Liddle, P.F. (2001b). Neural sources 339 involved in auditory target detection and novelty processing: An event-related fMRI 340 study. Psychophysiology, 38, 133-142. 341 342 Kiehl, K.A., Liddle, P.F., & Hopfinger, J.B. (2000). Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: An event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology, 37, 216-223. 343 Kiehl, K.A., Stevens, M.C., Laurens, K.R., Pearlson, G.P., Calhoun, V.D., & Liddle, P.F. (2005). 344 An adaptive reflexive processing model of neurocognitive function: Supporting evidence 345 from a large scale (n=100) fMRI study of an auditory oddball task. Neuroimage, 25(3), 346 899-915. 347 Nagai, Y., Critchley, H.D., Featherstone, E., Fenwick, P.B., Trimble, M.R., & Dolan, R.J. 348 (2004). Brain activity relating to the contingent negative variation: an fMRI investigation. 349 Neuroimage, 21(4), 1232-41. 350 351 Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J.D. (2005). Decision making, the p3, and the locus coeruleus--norepinephrine system. Psychol Bull, 131(4), 510-32. Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 18 352 Pascual-Marqui, R.D., Michel, C.M., & Lehmann, D. (1994). Low resolution electromagnetic 353 tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. Int J 354 Psychophysiol, 18(1), 49-65. 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 Polich, J. (1990). P300, probability, and interstimulus interval. Psychophysiology, 27(4), 396403. Polich, J., & Bondurant, T. (1997). P300 sequence effects, probability, and interstimulus interval. Physiol Behav, 61(6), 843-9. Polich, J., & Kok, A. (1995). Cognitive and biological determinants of P300s: An integrative review. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 103-146. Rushworth, M.F., Johansen-Berg, H., Gobel, S.M., & Devlin, J.T. (2003). The left parietal and premotor cortices: motor attention and selection. Neuroimage., 20(Suppl 1), S89-100. Sams, M., Alho, K., & Naatanen, R. (1983). Sequential effects on the ERP in discriminating two stimuli. Biol Psychol, 17(1), 41-58. Squires, K.C., Wickens, C., Squires, N.K., & Donchin, E. (1976). The effect of stimulus 366 sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. Science, 193(4258), 367 1142-6. 368 369 370 371 372 Stevens, M.C., Calhoun, V.D., & Kiehl, K.A. (in press). Hemispheric differences in hemodynamics elicited by auditory oddball stimuli. Neuroimage. Sutton, S., Braren, M., Zubin, J., & John, E.R. (1965). Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science, 150(700), 1187-8. Verleger, R., & Berg, P. (1991). The waltzing oddball. Psychophysiology, 28(4), 468-77. Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 19 373 Worsley, K.J., Marrett, S., Neelin, P., Vandal, A.C., Friston, K.J., & Evans, A.C. (1996). A 374 unified statistical approach for determining significant voxels in images of cerebral 375 activation. Human Brain Mapping, 4, 58-73. 376 377 Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 20 378 Author Note 379 All authors are affiliated with the Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center at the Institute of 380 Living in Hartford, CT and with the Department of Psychiatry at Yale University. Dr. Kiehl also 381 has an appointment to the Department of Psychology at Yale University. This research was 382 supported in part by the National Institutes of Health, under grants by 1 K23 MH070036-01 383 (MCS), 1 R01 EB 000840-01 (VDC), 1 R01 MH0705539-01 (KAK) and 1 R01 MH072681-01 384 (KAK) and National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD) Young 385 Investigator awards to KAK and VDC. Address reprint requests to Dr. Kent A. Kiehl at the Olin 386 Neuropsychiatry Research Center, 200 Retreat Avenue, Whitehall Building, The Institute of 387 Living, Hartford, CT 06106, or by email at kent.kiehl@yale.edu. 388 389 Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 21 390 List of Tables 391 Table 1. Summary of reaction time means and standard deviations for each target-to-target 392 interval. 393 Table 2. Summary of the brain areas showing a significant positive linear relationship between 394 target interval and amplitude of hemodynamic response to target stimuli. Coordinates 395 represent peak t-score for the target interval effect within each region. Cluster membership 396 also is also shown. Statistical significance was evaluated using the family-wise error rate, 397 corrected for searching the whole brain. 398 399 Figure Captions 400 Figure 1. Illustration of brain areas showing increasing amplitude of the hemodynamic response 401 to target stimuli with longer target interval. The legend shows t-score value associated with 402 the color map. The statistical parametric map has a threshold of t99 = 4.55, corresponding to 403 p < .05 (familywise error rate, corrected for searching the whole brain). 404 Figure 2. Representative regions showing target interval effect in each cluster. 2A) The first 405 cluster shows relatively sharp increase in peak hemodynamic activity (x-axis) at 406 approximately 30 seconds by target interval (y-axis). 2B) The second cluster shows 407 relatively smooth linear increases in peak hemodynamic activity (x-axis) with increasing 408 target interval (y-axis). Lines represent mean across participants, bounded by standard error 409 of measurement. Non-depicted regions show similar profiles. All cluster memberships are 410 shown in Table 1. 411 412 Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 22 413 Table 1. Summary of reaction time means and standard deviations for each target-to-target interval. Target Interval Mean (SD) (seconds) 8 429.2 (99.32) 10 406.7 (92.59) 12 394.1 (87.53) 16 423.1 (103.45) 18 419.7 (101.90) 20 406.5 (101.10) 24 416.1 (88.84) 28 423.0 (111.00) 30 461.5 (146.52) 32 426.3 (98.12) 38 449.7 (131.06) 40 431.9 (118.64) 42 414.3 (137.18) 54 423.6 (130.41) 68 439.8 (120.25) 414 Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 23 415 Table 2. Summary of the brain areas showing a significant positive linear relationship between target interval and amplitude of hemodynamic response to target stimuli. Coordinates represent peak t-score for the target interval effect within each region. Cluster membership also is shown. Statistical significance was evaluated using the family-wise error rate, corrected for searching the whole brain. Frontal Lobe L Superior / Middle Frontal Gyrus L Middle Frontal Gyrus R Superior / Middle Frontal Gyrus L Inferior Frontal Gyrus R Inferior Frontal Gyrus Medial Frontal Gyrus Anterior Cingulate Gyrus L Insula R Insula L Precentral Gyrus L Precentral Gyrus R Precentral Gyrus Parietal Lobe L Postcentral Gyrus R Postcentral Gyrus L Superior Parietal Lobule R Superior Parietal Lobule L Inferior Parietal / Supramarginal Gyrus R Inferior Parietal / Supramarginal Gyrus Posterior Cingulate Gyrus Precuneus Occipital Lobe L Middle Temporal Gyrus R Middle Temporal Gyrus Occpital Lobe L Lingual Gyrus / Cuneus Other L Thalamus R Thalamus R Putamen / Globus Pallidus L Cerebellum R Cerebellum x y z Target × target interval t-score -24 -40 28 -52 56 0 -4 -48 48 -32 -36 28 48 36 48 4 12 8 12 16 20 -20 -28 -4 24 28 28 12 20 48 44 -8 -12 64 60 56 5.21** 5.24** 6.05*** 4.91* 5.02** 7.71**** 7.80**** 7.26**** 6.89**** 8.30**** 7.68**** 5.82*** 1.71 1.69 2.19 1.62 1.81 4.14 3.62 4.34 4.60 3.31 3.21 1.98 1 2 2 1 2 1 2 2 2 1 1 1 -60 56 -36 24 -64 60 0 -4 -24 -20 -40 -56 -32 -44 -40 -32 16 16 64 64 24 16 28 40 5.45** 6.79**** 5.76*** 4.85* 6.16*** 5.14** 5.02** 5.68*** 2.34 2.50 3.13 2.32 1.89 2.08 3.00 2.52 2 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 -56 56 -64 -28 0 -12 5.21** 5.28** 2.25 2.02 2 1 -4 -72 -8 5.05** 3.18 1 -8 0 32 -8 40 -20 -16 12 -84 -72 8 4 0 -20 -24 5.18** 6.31**** 4.86* 6.26**** 5.87*** 2.44 3.03 1.57 5.19 4.38 2 1 2 1 1 * Target interval β Cluster p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001, **** p < .0001 familywise error rate, corrected for searching the whole brain. 416 Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 24 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 Figure 1. Illustration of brain areas showing increasing amplitude of the hemodynamic response to target stimuli with longer target interval. The legend shows t-score value associated with the color map. The statistical parametric map has a low threshold of t99 = 4.55, corresponding to p < .05 (familywise error rate, corrected for searching the whole brain). Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval 25 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 Figure 2. Representative regions showing target interval effect in each cluster. 2A) The first cluster shows relatively sharp increase in peak hemodynamic response amplitude (x-axis) at approximately 30 seconds by target interval in seconds (y-axis). 2B) The second cluster shows relatively smooth linear increases in peak hemodynamic response amplitude (x-axis) across target interval in seconds (y-axis). Lines represent mean across participants, bounded by standard error of measurement. Non-depicted regions show similar profiles. All cluster memberships are shown in Table 1. Running head: fMRI Oddball target interval