THE SECOND ANNUAL M AR C H 2 2 2 0 0 8 A T T H E D E P AR T M E NT O F P SY C H O L O G Y U N I V E R S I T Y O F M I C H I G A N, A N N AR B O R 1|Page SATURDAY, MARCH 22, 2008 BREAKFAST 9:00 am ANNOUNCEMENT & INTRODUCTION 9:30-9:45 am OPENING ADDRESS 9:45-10:00 am SESSION 1: SELF AND OTHER 10:00-11:30 am Vera Sacharin begins this section by discussing how American and German define and perceive friendship differently. Next, Krishna Savani and Young Kim examine how individuals’ self-perception and choices are influenced by other people’s perceptions and expectations. AMERICAN AND GERMAN FRIENDS-SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES. Vera Sacharin, University of Michigan RESPONSIVENESS TO OTHER PEOPLE’S EXPECTATIONS: CULTURAL VARIATION AND SITUATIONAL AFFORDANCE. Krishna Savani, Stanford University THE JURY AND ABJURY OF THE PEERS: THE ROLE OF PUBLIC REPRESENTATION IN DEFINING THE SELF. Young-Hoon Kim, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign SESSION 2: ESSENTIALISM 11:30-1:00pm Expanding out of the lay theory tradition, in this section Melody Chao and Karl Dach-Gruschow examines how lay beliefs about race and national characteristics can influence individuals’ social categorization processes, national identification, and intercultural perceptions in a culturally diverse society. ESSENTIALIZING RACE: ITS IMPLICATION TO SOCIAL CATEGORIZATION AND INTERCULTURAL PROCESSES. Melody Chao, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign NATIONAL IDENTITY, NATIONALISM, AND RACE. Karl Dach-Gruschow, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign 2|Page LUNCH BREAK & POSTER SESSION 1:00-2:30 pm SESSION 3: COGNITIVE PROCESSES & SOCIAL COGNITION 2:30-4:00 pm Presentations in this section examine the newest theoretical and empirical findings in domain of social cognition. Xuezhao Lan begins this section by discussing East-West differences on inhibition, attention, and executive control. Then, Jinkyung Na examines whether cross cultural differences in holistic and analytic modes of thought can be reduced to differences at the individual level. Finally, Igor Grossmann presents his work on interdependence and holistic cognition among Russian. EAST-WEST DIFFERENCES ON INHIBITION. Xuezhao Lan, University of Michigan CAN CULTURAL DIFFERENCES BE REDUCED TO INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES? HOLISTIC VS. ANALYTIC MODE OF THINKING AS CULTURAL SYNDROMES. Jinkyung Na, University of Michigan INTERDEPENDENCE AND HOLISTIC COGNITION: IS RUSSIA EASTERN OR WESTERN? Igor Grossmann, University of Michigan COFFEE BREAK 4:00-4:15 pm SESSION 4: CULTURAL CHANGE AND COMPARISON 4:15-5:45 pm In this session, Nick Bowman begins by examining subjective well-being in a changing society (the Slovak Republic). Next, Shirley Cheng explores the emergence of cultural community from a communication perspective. Finally, Chi-yue Chiu discusses effects of performance forecast in high versus low occupational mobility contexts. SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING AND ADJUSTMENT TO A CHANGING SOCIETY IN THE SLOVAK REPUBLIC. Nick Bowman , University of Michigan A COMMUNICATION PERSPECTIVE TO THE EMERGENCE OF A CULTURAL COMMUNITY. Shirley Cheng, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign PERFORMANCE FORECAST IN HIGH VERSUS LOW OCCUPATIONAL MOBILITY CONTEXTS. Jing (Julie) Chen & Chi-yue Chiu, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign CLOSING ADDRESS 5:45-6:00 pm 3|Page CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY CONFERENCE PRESENTATION ABSTRACTS SESSION 1: SELF AND OTHER Vera Sacharin begins this section by discussing how American and German define and perceive friendship differently. Next, Krishna Savani and Young Kim examine how individuals’ self-perception and choices are influenced by other people’s perceptions and expectations. American and German Friends - Similarities and Differences. VERA SACHARIN Differences in friendship behavior between German and Americans are a re-appearing issue in the management literature (Robinson, 2005). Lewin (1948) describes how Americans show more ease when making friends and less depth in friendships compared to Germans. Underlying this lack of depth could be that for Americans, the term 'friend' seems to have a broad meaning and does not imply closeness (Fischer, 1982). Rather than on intimacy, American friendships seem to be based on sociability (Fischer, 1982; Wierzbicka, 1997). We expected that Americans would be less close to their friends than Germans, particularly on measures of intimacy. In study 1, we show that Germans define 'friends' and 'acquaintances' closer than Americans on an IOS-scale type measure (Araon, Aron, & Smollan, 1992). In study 2, we assess participants' closeness to their friends in more detail. Americans report more sociability with their 'friends,' while Germans report more empathy. Relationships to 'acquaintances' were more similar to relationships to 'friends' for Germans, but not as close as American 'friends.' Germans' closeness to 'acquaintances' is helpful for explaining why Germans named fewer 'friends' than Americans in past research (Sleeth-Keppler, 2005). The study shows that the anecdotally observed lack of depth in American friendships might be particular to intimacy, and not generalize across all facets of friendship. 4|Page Responsiveness to other people’s expectations: Cultural variation and situational affordance. KRISHNA SAVANI In all cultures, people encounter many situations in which another person expects them to behave in a certain way or to make certain choices. But cultural contexts might differ in the extent to which people are generally responsive to other people's expectations and in the particular situations in which people are most responsive. We hypothesize that people in Indian contexts will adjust their choices merely after reflecting upon a significant others' expectation, even with the other person is not present, which American participants will not. Two studies show that Indian women choose more conservative shirts when their parents' expectations were made salient, while American participants show no movement. These differences in psychological tendencies are afforded by the interpersonal situations that people typically encounter in Indian and American contexts. A situation sampling study found that compared to American situations, significant others in Indian situations try to influence with a benevolent rather than a self-serving motive, and accommodation in Indian contexts is more likely to strengthen the relationship. The Jury and Abjury of the Peers: The Role of Public Representation in Defining the Self . YOUNG-HOON KIM We outline the way Face cultures tend to give priority to knowing oneself from the outside, whereas Dignity cultures tend to give priority to knowing oneself from the inside. In Experiment 1, Face culture participants absorbed into their self-definition whatever was publicly known among a group, whereas Dignity culture participants did not absorb such judgments. In Experiment 2, for people from a Face culture, simply having one other person know a piece of information about the self was enough to enhance the validity of that information, making it more influential for self-definition, but not for those from a Dignity culture. In Experiment 3, we used a modified version of the Ross et al. (1977) quiz bowl task where the quiz bowl contestant would look relatively ignorant in this task, simply as a function of their role. For those from a Face culture, the public performance of this ignorant role affected their self-definition whereas their private performance of this role did not. For those from a Dignity culture, the public vs. private performance distinction did not matter so much. 5|Page SESSION 2: ESSENTIALISM Expanding out of the lay theory tradition, in this section Melody Chao and Karl Dach-Gruschow examines how lay beliefs about race and national characteristics can influence individuals’ social categorization processes, national identification, and intercultural perceptions in a culturally diverse society. Essentializing Race: Its Implication to Social Categorization and Intercultural Processes. MELODY CHAO Racial essentialism is the belief that racial groups possess distinctive essence. Previous research has found consistent associations between the endorsement of essentialist beliefs and negative attitudes towards ethnic minority groups (Jayaratne et al., 2006; Keller, 2005). However, little is known about the basic cognitive processes underlying essentialists' racial perceptions. Going beyond the intuitive notion that endorsers of essentialist beliefs are essentially negative, this presentation presents findings from three studies suggesting that essentialist beliefs setup mindsets through which individuals interpret their social experiences. Specifically, the findings demonstrate that individuals who endorse essentialist beliefs are more sensitive to subtle racial differences (Study 1) and acquire racial categorization rules that define group membership more readily (Study 2). In addition, the findings suggest that essentialists are more likely to make inference about group characteristics based on racial group membership. More importantly, they do not necessarily make negative inferences towards a particular ethnic group. Instead, they were simply more likely to make race-based inferences in general; whether they evaluate a group positively or negatively depend on the information they have about the specific ethnic group (Study 3). National Identity, Nationalism, and Race. KARL DACH-GRUSCHOW In the United States, there is a broad assumption that national ingroup members are White and that non-Whites are less American. This assumption can be seen as both an example of the perpetual foreigner stereotype and as a motivated effort to exclude non-Whites. The latter relationship is consistent with Social Dominance Theory, which predicts a fusion of Racial and National Identity for dominant group members. We present a new measure of the American = White assumption stemming from this theoretical framework. Moreover our preliminary data suggests that for White Americans, racial prejudice is related to the American = White assumption mainly when the individual also holds nationalistic beliefs. 6|Page SESSION 3: COGNITIVE PROCESSES & SOCIAL COGNITION Presentations in this section examine the newest theoretical and empirical findings in domain of social cognition. Xuezhao Lan begins this section by discussing East-West differences on inhibition, attention, and executive control. Then, Jinkyung Na examines whether cross cultural differences in holistic and analytic modes of thought can be reduced to differences at the individual level. Finally, Igor Grossmann presents his work on interdependence and holistic cognition among Russian. East-West Differences on Inhibition. XUEZHAO LAN Previous research has shown that East-Asians have holistic parallel cognition and westerners have analytic sequential cognition (Kopecky, Kitayama, Meyer, under review; Kitayama, Duffy, & Uchida 2007; Nisbett, 2003; Nisbett & Miyamoto, 2005.) Recently, it has been proposed that underlying these differences in higher-level cognitive tasks are cultural differences in basic cognitive processes (Boduroglu, Shah, & Nisbett , 2008). Westerners were found to adopt sequential strategies while East Asians adopted parallel strategies when multi-tasking (Kopecky, Kitayama, Meyer, under review). The present studies investigated the influence of different cognitive styles on lower-level inhibition including response inhibition, task switching and interference resolution. 38 American and 36 Asian students who have been to the U.S. for less than two years from the University of Michigan participated on a battery of inhibition tasks in experiment one; 47 American students from the University of Michigan and 48 Chinese students from Beijing Normal University participated in Experiment two. Results from two experiments revealed that American students consistently outperformed their East Asian/Chinese counterparts on task switching (switching between the two tasks), and response inhibition tasks (inhibiting proponent responses) as indicated by their performance on task-switching, go-no-go and Anti-saccade tasks. In addition, Asian students living in the U.S. tend to outperform their peers living in China. However, East Asians and Americans did not show strong differences in interference resolution tasks (ignoring irrelevant information) as indicated by their performance on Flanker and Shape matching tasks. This study provided important evidence in explaining the possible cross-cultural differences in attention and executive control. 7|Page Can Cultural Differences be Reduced to Individual Differences? Holistic vs. Analytic Mode of Thinking as Cultural Syndromes. JINKYUNG NA Past decade of research on holistic-analytic mode of thinking has shown that first, there are several aspects of holism (e.g. diffused vs. focused attention and situational vs. dispositional attribution) and second, these aspects of holism are correlated at cultural level such that in a culture where diffused attention is common, the other aspects of holism (e.g. situational attribution) are also pervasive. In other words, holism is a coherent and unitary concept at the level of culture. However, it is still an empirical question whether the coherence of holism is maintained at the level of individual. We hypothesized holism as cultural syndromes (or complexes) and therefore proposed independence between cultural and individual level analysis. At cultural level, a holistic (or analytic) culture provides a complete set of syndromes and hence coherence would result. However, such coherence is broken at individual level because each individual samples only some aspects of holistic (or analytic) mode of thinking. Consequently, we expected that correlations among aspects of holism would become notably weak at the level of individual. A competing hypothesis, however, argues that holism is another global trait which can be applied to both individual and cultural level in the same way. The alternative hypothesis predicts holism would be a coherent and unitary concept at individual level because it is so at cultural level. To test our hypothesis, we tested 136 participants with nine holistic-analytic tasks. Supporting our hypothesis, we found correlations become notably weak at the level of individual. Implications for cultural differences and holism are discussed. Interdependence and Holistic Cognition: Is Russia Eastern or Western? IGOR GROSSMANN Previous research found that East Asians are more holistic or less analytic in cognitive style than North Americans. These differences have been explained in terms of social orientation, with East Asians more socially interdependent than North Americans. The current research extended this literature by examining Russian culture, which may be assumed to fall between East Asia and North America in terms of social orientation and the modal style of reasoning. We conducted a series of eight studies to test the prediction that Russians would be more holistic than North Americans. As predicted, Russians were relatively interdependent in self-construal. Moreover, they were more likely than Americans 1) to reason about the social world dialectically, i.e. when presented with descriptions of a person’s behavior (e.g. hiding a medical mistake), Russians attributed contradictory characteristics to this person (e.g. honest) more often than Americans; 2) to predict changes in events; 3) to categorize objects thematically (rather than taxonomically); 4) to detect changes in context; 5) to make situational (vis-à-vis dispositional) attributions; and 5) to categorize behaviors (e.g. “joining the army”) in terms of the global orientation (“helping the nation’s defense”) rather than action orientation (“signing up”). Overall, the present findings provide converging evidence for holistic style of thinking and interdependent social orientation in Russia. 8|Page SESSION 4: CULTURAL CHANGE AND COMPARISON In this session, Nick Bowman begins by examining subjective well-being in a changing society (the Slovak Republic). Next, Shirley Cheng explores the emergence of cultural community from a communication perspective. Finally, Chi-yue Chiu discusses the effects of performance forecast in high versus low occupational mobility contexts. Subjective Well-Being and Adjustment to a Changing Society in the Slovak Republic. NICK BOWMAN In former Communist states, the proliferation of free-market economies and democratic systems of government has resulted in increasingly independent values, norms, and lay theories of agency. Simultaneously, the increases in economic and social inequality associated with the removal of Communism have made social class much more meaningful and salient within Slovak society. Therefore, we hypothesized that social class differences in subjective well-being (SWB) would be greater among Slovaks than Americans. We conducted studies using data from the Pew Global Attitudes Project, National Surveys of Midlife Development in the United States, original data from the Slovak Republic, and three waves of the World Values Surveys. Consistent with predictions, social class differences (defined by educational attainment) in SWB were generally greater among Slovaks than Americans, suggesting that rapid increases in economic and status inequality—as opposed to continuous long-term disparities—have a particularly adverse effect on the lower and working classes. Furthermore, older Slovaks (who are adjusting to a more individualistic society after spending most of their lives in a collectivistic society) exhibited lower levels of SWB than younger Slovaks and older Americans (who have spent most or all of their lives in a relatively individualistic society). A Communication Perspective to the Emergence of a Cultural Community . SHIRLEY CHENG Culture influences behaviors, but how does a culture start? Decades of (cross-) cultural research have provided numerous examples of culture's influence on behaviors. However, the question of how a culture begins is still awaiting answers from social psychology. On the surface, it seems that when it comes to the question of how a culture starts, social psychologists must defer to historians. Upon further probing, although inventing a world culture in a is still far beyond our psychological imagination, simulating a novel brand culture (like the Apple culture) in a social psychology experiment may still be within our reach. Such experimental studies could be informative because they may help us identify some facilitative conditions for culture formation. Recently, to explore the promises and limits of this research strategy, we have turned our laboratory into a culture incubator. In this talk, I will present the preliminary results from our culture simulation research. The key variables we examined in this research are minority status and serial reproductive communication of narratives about ingroup members. We posit that the confluence of these factors has facilitative effects on the formation of a brand culture. 9|Page Performance Forecast in High versus Low Occupational Mobility Contexts . JING (JULIE) CHEN & CHI-YUE CHIU Should people have Bill Gates' personality to succeed in the information technology industry? Should you have Harry Triandis' personality to become a successful cross-cultural psychologist? How people would answer this question depends on their cultural background. In this talk, we will try to answer two questions: (1) Are there East-West differences in the importance of person-profession fit in making performance forecast? and (2) If yes, how do these differences emerge? In the United States, people tend to believe that a competent employee, irrespective of her personal style, can be successful in her self-chosen career. In Japan, where occupational mobility is relatively low, employees who are competent but do not conform to the social expectations in the company may be assigned to a "peripheral" department (aka the Department of the Losers). Consistent with this observation, the results from five studies show that compared to European Americans, Asians believe more strongly that (1) personality-profession fit (having personality expected from one's profession) is important to future career success, (2) this cultural difference may arise from the stronger belief in fixed reality in Asia (vs. North America), and that (3) experiences with occupational immobility as a structural constraint set up the characteristic cultural belief and performance forecast pattern in Asia. we will use these results to discuss the relationship between society, culture, and cognition. 10 | P a g e