Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide 4 Chapter 4 Force and Motion Recommended class days: 2 When asked to draw a force diagram for some simple situation, most students emerging from any level of introductory physics course are likely to draw objects which look like a porcupine shot by an Indian hunting party—the number and direction of pointed entities being essentially stochastic. Arnold Arons (1979) Background Information The chapter on Physics Education Research has already provided much of the background information on student difficulties with force and motion. The major reference is Halloun and Hestenes (1985b). Other reference are cited in Reddish (1994). This section will summarize the earlier information. There are three basic issues, all of which cause serious difficulties for students: • What is a force? • What is the connection between force and motion? • How are forces between different object related? What is a force? Students don’t have a clear idea of just what a force is. They tend not to distinguish between force, inertia, energy, power, or even velocity, often using these terms interchangeably. In addition: • Some students believe that only animate objects can exert forces. They don’t believe that a table exerts an upward force on an object; the table simply “gets in the way of the object wanting to fall.” • Forces recognized by physicists are often seen by students as simply influences on an object’s motion, not as forces. Thus friction is not a force but merely “what makes it stop.” Gravity is not a force but simply “what makes it fall.” 4-1 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 • A majority of students believe in an impetus theory of motion. In throwing a ball, the hand imparts a “force of the throw” to the ball. This is a property of the ball (an inherent force) and travels with the ball “to keep it moving.” Typically 75% or more of students beginning calculusbased physics thinking that a ball tossed upward has an upward “force of the throw” or “force of your hand” on it after it leaves your hand. • Students tend to view forces from the perspective of the applier of force rather than from the perspective of the object experiencing force. That is, they recognize the pushes and pulls they must apply to move an object, but they don’t recognize that the object may experience additional inanimate forces of friction, gravity, and so on. This is one of the major reasons they think that motion requires a force. How are force and motion connected? The prevailing student belief is that motion requires a force. This belief is based on much common-sense evidence, and it is a belief that is highly resistant to change. More specifically, the “student version” of the laws of motion is: • If there’s no force on an object, the object is at rest or will immediately come to rest. • The converse is not true. An object at rest does not automatically imply no net force. • Motion requires a force or, alternatively, force causes motion. • In general, force is proportional to velocity. These are the most common student beliefs, but not every student necessarily holds every belief. It is important to note that students are not at all consistent in their application of these beliefs; for example, they may apply some beliefs to vertical motion but not to horizontal motion. Even so, recognition of these prevailing alternative conceptions will give instructors better insight into student responses and the difficulties students face. How are forces between two different objects related? Newton’s third law is profound, much more so than the first two. For students, the third law is a subtle and difficult topic, and they will find this to be a challenging subject. However, if you devote ample time to the underlying principles and provide students with ample opportunities for practice and feedback, most will end up finding this a rewarding chapter where the concepts of force and motion suddenly begin to “make sense.” Suppose a large truck and a compact car have a head-on collision. During the collision, is the force of the truck on the car larger, smaller, or equal to the force of the car on the truck? This question, and several similar questions on the Force Concept Inventory, is initially missed by 70% 4-2 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 to 80% of students in typical calculus-based physics courses. Presumably students entering an algebra-based course would fare considerably worse. Halloun and Hestenes (1985b) have characterized student beliefs about interactions in terms of a dominance principle: The larger (or faster or more active) object exerts a larger force than the smaller (or slower or less active) object. Students tend to view an interaction as a “conflict” in which the stronger wins. It’s not hard to understand how this common-sense view comes about. After all, the effect of the collision on the compact car is much larger than its effect on the truck. Different effects would seem to require different causes, hence different amounts of force. The difference in the masses does not appear to students as a significant factor in drawing conclusions about forces. This basic misconception about interaction forces is likely the most persistent and hard to change of all the student misconceptions in mechanics. Some of the more specific difficulties students have with Newton’s third law and with interacting systems are: • Students don’t believe Newton’s third law. It’s too contrary to common sense. • Students have difficulty identifying action/reaction force pairs: They match two forces on the same object. They place forces on the wrong objects. They don’t believe that long-range forces (e.g., gravity) have reaction forces. • Students confuse equal force with equal acceleration. • Students don’t understand tension: They think that tension is the sum of the forces exerted at the two ends of a string. They think that tension exerts a force only in the direction of motion. They think that tension can pass through an object to another string on the other side. • Students often don’t recognize that objects connected by an inextensible string must have accelerations of equal magnitude. Interacting-system problems are difficult and frustrating for students. There are no magic formulas to search for, so students must correctly apply Newton’s second law to each object. But even if they realize this to be the proper approach, they cannot succeed without first identifying all the interaction forces and then making proper use of the third law. 4-3 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 Student Learning Objectives In covering the material of this chapter, students will • Recognize what does and does not constitute a force. • Identify the specific forces acting on an object. • Draw an accurate free-body diagram of an object. • Begin the process of understanding the connection between force and motion. • Understand the relationship between forces between two different objects. • Begin learning how to explain an observation on the basis of physical principles. Pedagogical Approach Chapter 4 is a qualitative overview to dynamics, a prelude to the quantitative analysis of Chapters 5–8. This is analogous to the qualitative introduction to motion in Chapter 1, followed by quantitative kinematics in Chapter 2. This is an extremely important chapter that should not be rushed. Successful learning of Newtonian mechanics requires a major reorientation of thinking for most students. This chapter lays the conceptual groundwork that tends to be overlooked in the typical rush to start using Newton’s laws to solve problems. As the chapter objectives note, students must recognize what a force is, correctly identify the forces on an object, and be able to express their knowledge about forces on a free-body diagram before they can successfully solve dynamics problems that go beyond simple plug-and-chug. Most of the chapter, the Student Workbook exercises, and the end-of-chapter problems are devoted to these issues. Chapter 4 introduces another new idea—that of experimental evidence. The fact that acceleration is proportional to net force is an observation about how the world works. It is important, through lecture demonstrations and laboratory activities, to present students with clear and compelling evidence that force determines an object’s acceleration, not its velocity. This chapter seeks to establish the connection between force and acceleration, then Chapter 5 will follow up on this idea with actual problem solving. Newton’s third law is only introduced at the end of the chapter. Although we ultimately want students to understand forces as interactions, the conceptual hurdles that students face are high enough already, and the topic of interactions adds yet more. The authors have found that students do much 4-4 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 better if instruction concentrates first on how a single object responds to forces. Only after these ideas begin to seem comfortable can students deal with the issue of how two objects interact with each other. Chapter 4 introduces the idea of dividing a problem into the system, which is the object or objects of interest, and the environment, which is everything else. For most of the chapter, the system is a single object and all forces originate in the environment. When the third law is introduced, the system may consist of two or more interacting objects. This system/environment division is a pedagogical device that will be expanded and elaborated on in many chapters to come. Instructors may want to look ahead in the textbook to Figures 10.4 and 11.15 to see where these ideas are headed. Suggested Lecture Outlines DAY 1: We have found it effective to focus the first class day on learning to identify forces. The connection between force and motion then enters on Day 2. This is a chapter for which instructors are urged to have a wide variety of simple props available—springs, ropes, sticks, masses, blocks, rubber balls, etc. An effective tactic is to first apply a force to a student, asking him or her to verify from sensations that there is a push or a pull. Then apply the same force to an inanimate object, such as a block, and ask if the block also experiences the same force. If you pull on a student’s arm (gently!), he will agree that he feels a pulling force. If you now hand him one end of a rope, pull on the other end, and ask what he experiences, more than likely he’ll reply, “You’re pulling on me.” This is an opportunity to distinguish the immediate cause at a point of contact, namely the tension in the rope, from the ultimate cause of whatever pulls the other end of the rope. Hand a student one end of a spring and pull the other end. Have her agree that she feels a pulling force as the spring stretches. Then hang a mass from a spring. Because the spring stretches, most will now agree that the spring exerts a force on the mass. Then hang the same mass from a string. They don’t see the string stretch, so does it exert a force? You can ask students to imagine if they were hanging from a rope tied around their waist—would they feel a force from the rope? Once they agree that the string does exert a force on the block, you have a good opportunity to talk briefly about molecular bonds as atomic-level springs; the string does stretch ever so slightly as the molecular bonds stretch, and that’s what tension is. 4-5 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 These activities are good preparation for the critical demonstration of Day 1. Place a block in the center of the lecture table and ask students what force or forces are acting on the block. You’ll get lots of responses of “gravity.” If you inquire about other forces, a few will say “the normal force.” Reply, “OK, so you learned in high school about this thing called the normal force, but how many of you really believe that the table is exerting an upward force on the block?” My experience is that less than one-third of the students will raise their hands. While their high school physics books and teachers may have told them about normal forces, their doubts arise because they don’t see any mechanism by which the table exerts a force on the block. They need to be convinced, with evidence, that such a force is really there. To begin a series of demonstrations, hand out several compression springs (fairly stiff ones) and ask students to squeeze them. They’ll agree that the spring pushes back when it’s squeezed. Stand a fairly soft spring on the table and set the block on it, giving a very visible compression. (You’ll need some way to stabilize the block on top the spring.) They’ll agree that the spring exerts an upward force on the block. Then switch to a stiffer spring that barely compresses. This leads to the conclusion that the amount of compression is not the issue. Now place the block on a thin board (or meter stick) that is supported at the ends, causing the board to sag. They’ll agree that the board is also springy and exerts an upward force. (Ask them to imagine how their finger would feel if they pressed the board down and held it.) Finally, return to the block sitting on the table. No discernible sag or compression, so is there a force? The coup de grace is to place a mirror flat on the table, then reflect a laser beam from it at grazing incidence (laser beam almost parallel to table surface) with the reflected laser spot striking the side wall of the classroom. Ask a student to place a pencil tip next to the laser spot, then climb up and stand on the lecture table! Virtually all lecture tables will flex enough under the weight of the instructor to deflect the laser spot up a couple of millimeters. (It’s good to try this in advance to make sure it’s going to work. Note that many lecture tables have cross bracing underneath, and you don’t want to stand right over one of the stiffer braces.) Voila! The table really does compress, even if only a microscopic amount, and it exerts the same upward force as a spring. (Those molecular bonds again, as it’s good to note.) This sequence of demonstrations takes some time, but it’s well worth it. They make a memorable impression on students. We have received numerous unsolicited comments on evaluation forms that this demonstration was what really convinced students about the reality of 4-6 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 r r forces exerted by inanimate objects. (Note that appealing to Fnet 0 for static equilibrium to infer the existence of a normal force is not in the least convincing to most students. Many don’t yet r r accept that an object at rest must have Fnet 0. ) You can consolidate what they’ve seen about force to this point: forces are a push or pull, they occur at a point of contact between the object and some identifiable agent that exerts the force, and forces can be exerted by either animate or inanimate agents. This is a good opening to consider friction. Ask them to imagine dragging their hand across a very rough surface. Is there a force? What direction? Then what about a box sliding across the surface? Then turn to gravity. Drop a ball—why does it fall? This is a different type of force, a longrange force. You might want to have some magnets to demonstrate other long-range forces, but emphasize that gravity is the only long-range force that will be considered for quite some time. Every other force must be a contact force. Finally, toss a ball straight up into the air and inquire about the force or forces on the ball after it leaves your hand but before it reaches the top. If you had opened class with this question, a majority of the students would likely assign an upward “force of the throw” to the ball. Many may be doubting this answer after the sequence of demonstrations you’ve been through, but many others will still want an upward “force of the throw.” You can play devil’s advocate, first getting them to agree that there’s no contact with anything in the environment, then asking “But how can it go up unless there’s an upward force?” Don’t answer! This is where you really want them to be confronted by difficulties with their alternative conceptions of force and motion. Ask them to think about it, and promise to resolve the issue during the next class. With the time remaining, go over Tactics Box 4.2 for identifying forces and work through several simple examples. You can start out by carefully working through the steps of the Tactics Box with the following example: Example: A block being dragged up a hill by a rope. 4-7 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 Then let them practice a few themselves. Two possibilities include: Example: A block A hangs from the ceiling by a rope. Another block B hangs from A. Identify the forces acting on A. Example: A note about notation: There is no happy solution to the issue of what symbols to use for forces. r r r T for tension, N for normal force, and W for weight run into conflicts with other uses of T, N, and r r r W. Fstring, Fgrav, and other subscripted uses of F are accurate, but they’re tedious and students r r have a tendency to forget the subscripts. We’ve elected to keep T for tension but use lower case n r and w for normal force and weight. Regardless of the choices made, remind students that the symbols are a language and that they can’t communicate with you or other students if they try to invent their own language. Insist that students learn and use the notation of the text and the classroom. DAY 2: The objective of Day 2 is to “discover” Newton’s first and second laws. What are the consequences of a force on an object? You’ll want to use whatever demonstrations you have available to show that an object continues to move at constant velocity in the absence of a net force and that force causes an object to accelerate. Convincing demonstrations here go a long way toward changing the common student beliefs that motion requires a force and that force is proportional to velocity. For demonstrating the first law, gliders on an air track are particularly effective here, as they can be made to move quite slowly, without much change in their speed. Also effective are dry ice pucks that slide down a long sheet of glass on a cushion of CO2 vapor. Other interesting demonstrations that illustrate the first law include: Demonstration: Show how to get ketchup out of a bottle, as in the Try it Yourself on page 107 of the text. The green gel used to ease sunburn pain works well here, and requires no refrigeration. First hit down on the bottom of the inverted bottle (which is pointing up), the “standard” way of 4-8 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 getting ketchup out. You’ll see that the ketchup tends to move toward the bottle’s bottom. But if you hit sharply up near the neck, the bottle will jerk upward while the ketchup remains stationary, leading to a net forward motion of the ketchup. Demonstration: A classic demonstration is to pull a tablecloth out from under some dishes. The key is to pull rapidly with a small downward component to the pull. You will need to justify a bit why you can neglect the force of the tablecloth on the dishes; it’s mostly because it acts for only a very short time. Demonstration: Show some test-dummy car crash videos, if available. Such videos vividly illustrating the role of the first law in “throwing” passengers from a car. Computer-based force probes and motion sensors are especially effective for demonstrating Newton’s second law. Attach a force probe to a low-friction cart on a track, then use a string and pulley to connect the force probe to a hanging weight. When the weight is released, the force probe measures the string tension while the motion detector measures the motion. This gives excellent results, showing that a constant force produces a linearly increasing velocity and a constant acceleration. Doubling the tension force doubles the acceleration, while doubling the cart mass halves the acceleration. These results can all be shown in just a few minutes with software tools. Priscilla Laws (1997) has developed an alternative approach that can be shown in a large lecture hall, using volunteers, though it is better suited to lab. One student sits on a cart with low-friction wheels (a Kinesthetics Cart is available from PASCO) and holds one end of a spring scale. Another students pulls the cart by pulling the other end of the spring scale, endeavoring to keep the reading as constant as possible. Nearly all students expect, before trying this, that they’re going to walk or run at a steady speed; they are very surprised to find that they must accelerate to keep the force constant. Once the cart is moving, they can let the spring scale reading drop to zero but the cart keeps moving! These demonstrations lead to the introduction of Newton’s second law in the operational form r r r r a Fnet / m. Although F ma looks more elegant, it conveys to many students the wrong 4-9 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 r impression that “ m a is a force.” The operational form conveys a better sense of cause and effect and helps students to develop better ability to reason with Newtonian concepts. The summary at the end of these demonstrations is that “motion does not need a cause.” The question is not “Why does an object move?” but “Why does it change its motion?” Remind students that this is easy to say, but it takes effort and practice to begin to think this way. They shouldn’t be surprised if it all seems confusing at first, but promise them that they will “get it” if they persevere. Several examples are worthwhile at this point, depending on how much time is left. Example 1: An elevator is going up at a steady speed. First have students identify tension and weight as the only two forces. Then ask: “Is T greater than, equal to, or less than w? Or is there not enough information to tell?” Many will answer “greater” because “motion requires a force.” Example 2: Push a block across the table at steady speed. Since you’re exerting a force on it, why isn’t it accelerating? Ask students to identify all the forces and to draw a free-body diagram. Finally, ask them to compare the size of the pushing force and the size of the friction force. Example 3: Push the same block fairly quickly, then release it so that it slides some distance before stopping. Have students analyze the forces and reach the conclusion that the acceleration vector points backward. Remind them of what they learned in kinematics about situations in which the acceleration vector is opposite the velocity vector. When done, congratulate them on having performed a Newtonian analysis to explain why a block coasts to a stop after you release it. Example 4: Return to the issue of the ball tossed straight up—where you left students hanging on Day 1 as to how it moves up without an upward force. Now you can complete the analysis, using Example 3 as a horizontal analogy. Remind them that no cause is needed for the ball to move upward—inertia takes care of that. The proper question to ask is not “Why does it move upward?” but “Why does it slow down and eventually fall?” As you work through these examples, point out that you are giving an explanation of an event in terms of physical principles (Newton’s laws) and logical inference. They’ll be asked to give similar explanations in some end-of-chapter problems (especially problems such as 64 and 65). Students find this kind of reasoning very hard to do, so suggest that they use the just-worked examples as a template. Free-body diagrams have been skipped over thus far in class, although you likely will have drawn some simple ones in the process of explaining things. If students understand how to identify 4-10 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 forces, they’ve already overcome the largest hurdle to drawing free-body diagrams. They can practice on their own, following examples in the text, and you can give more explicit examples in the next chapter. One issue to watch for, and perhaps note in simple examples, is that free-body diagrams show only the forces acting on the object. Many students will try to include forces exerted by the object on other systems. If you have a few minutes left, working through the free-body diagram for an object sliding on an inclined plane, with friction, is an excellent example. DAY 3: On Day 3 you can turn to interacting objects and Newton’s third law. Most students readily accept that if A pushes/pulls B, then B pushes/pulls back on A. If you ask a student to stretch a spring, she can “feel” that the spring pulls on her hand at the same time she pulls on the spring. Long-range forces are more troublesome because students don’t yet understand the role of mass in the “outcome” of an interaction. The earth clearly pulls down on a ball that is dropped, but there’s little evidence of the ball exerting a force on the earth. You can make this idea plausible by demonstrations with magnets. If a student holds two fairly strong magnets, he can feel that each is pulling (or pushing) on the other. If you attach magnets in the repulsive orientation to two gliders on an air track, both gliders move apart when released. More importantly, if you now weight the gliders differently, students can see that there is a mass effect and that the lighter glider does most of the motion. Now it’s a much smaller step to accepting that the ball really does exert a reaction force on the earth. With this as a starting point, it’s good to spend an entire class asking students to follow the steps in Tactics Box 4.4 for identifying and labeling action/reaction pairs and for drawing free-body diagrams. It’s worth starting with a simple example, such as a block sitting at rest on a table. Have students identify the objects that make up the system—the block and the table—and identify all forces acting on the objects. They should be able to identify the external forces, which act from r r outside the system, and give these labels like nT and wB . Then they should identify the forces between objects that make up the system. The latter are the forces that make up the action/reaction pairs of Newton’s third law, and should be labeled using the r FA on B notation, and connected with dotted lines. Even this simple situation will rapidly lead to r r conflict for many students who want w and n to be an action/reaction pair. Then place a second block on top of the first. The lower block now experiences two normal forces, one from above and one from below. Again, this apparently trivial situation is initially difficult for many students. Fortunately, most will “get it” after just a few such examples. 4-11 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 The important idea of propulsion forces is often difficult for students to grasp. You can convey the idea by asking them to imagine what would happen on a frictionless surface. Also ask them to imagine what direction loose gravel would be “kicked” as they sprint forward or a car accelerates forward. The force of the tire on the gravel must be directed backward, so the force of the gravel on the tire—the propulsion force—must be directed forward. These exercises show that the members of an action/reaction pair act in opposite directions. But what about the magnitudes of these forces? Unfortunately, it isn’t easy to provide a demonstration of the equal magnitudes. By far the most convincing demonstration we know of is the collidingforce-probe experiment comes from Thornton, Sokoloff, and Law’s RealTime Physics. Two lowfriction carts of very different masses are pushed toward each other such that the collision occurs between the tips of their respective force probes. This gives instant and dramatic confirmation that the forces between two colliding carts are always equal in magnitude, regardless of the masses or the initial velocities of the carts. A simpler demonstration is to have two students of different size push against each other with bathroom scales, each calling out the reading on “his/her” scale as they move forward or backward. An interesting variant is for one student to stand on a skateboard. Even though this student begins to accelerate backward, “losing” the battle, both students will still call out the same forces. Clicker Question: 10-year-old Sarah stands on a skateboard. Her older brother Jack starts pushing her backward and she starts speeding up. The force of Jack on Sarah is A. Greater than the force of Sarah on Jack. B. Equal to the force of Sarah on Jack. C. Less than the force of Sarah on Jack. Other Resources In addition to the specific suggestions made above in the daily lecture outlines, here are some other suggestions for demonstrations and questions you could weave into your class time. Suggested Demonstrations Tug of War. Select two teams—the boys vs. the girls—for a tug of war. Choose them so that the boys’ team is the stronger. The have a gentle tug of war, which the boys will presumably win. Ask which team pulled harder. Now make the boys wear plastic grocery bags on their feet. The girls will 4-12 Knight/Jones/Field Instructor Guide Chapter 4 now win. You can then point out the important role of friction in determining who wins; it is net force that determines acceleration. Sample Reading Quiz Questions 1. What is a “net force?” 2. List at least three of the steps used to identify the forces acting on an object. 3. Which of these is not a force discussed in this chapter? A. The tension force. C. The orthogonal force. B. The normal force. D. The thrust force. 4. An action/reaction pair of forces A. points in the same direction. B. acts on the same object. C. are always long-range forces. D. acts on two different objects. 4-13