Presiding over a Capital Case • 137 CHAPTER 7 MENTAL RETARDATION Todd Elwyn, M.D., J.D. Melissa Piasecki, M.D. [7.1.] Introduction This chapter addresses the diagnosis of mental retardation in capital cases. First, we review the U.S. Supreme Court decision in Atkins v. Virginia,622 establishing the legal standard in capital cases for those diagnosed with mental retardation. Next, we describe the potential complexities facing a court when the defense argues that a defendant has mental retardation. These complexities include differing definitions of mental retardation and the numerous tools which can be used for measuring its component parts. Next, we discuss special issues in the diagnosis of mental retardation, such as the potential for malingering. We include an illustrative case to outline the perspectives of the parties and the potential contrasts in approaches to the defendant based on perspective. Last, we review cases following Atkins that offer additional perspectives on the questions relating to mental retardation in capital cases. [7.2.] Atkins v Virginia A Virginia jury convicted Atkins of capital murder and related crimes and he was sentenced to death. The Virginia Supreme Court affirmed relying on Penry v. Lynaugh,623 and rejected Atkins' contention that he could not be sentenced to death because he is mentally retarded. The U.S. Supreme Court reversed and remanded, holding that “death is not a suitable punishment for a mentally retarded criminal.”624 [7.3.] Facts of the Case On the night of August 16, 1996, after drinking alcohol and smoking marijuana, Daryl Renard Atkins and William Jones abducted Eric Nesbitt from a convenience store, robbed him, drove him to an automated teller machine to withdraw additional cash, then took him to a deserted area where, ignoring his pleas to leave him unharmed, shot him eight times and killed him.625 Jones and Atkins were indicted for capital murder, but Jones became ineligible to receive the death penalty after pleading guilty to first-degree murder in exchange for his testimony against Atkins.626 Although Atkins testified that Jones had actually 622 536 U.S. 304 (2002). 492 U.S. 302 (1989). 624 Atkins, 536 U.S. at 321. 625 Id. at 307. 626 Id. 623 138 • Presiding over a Capital Case shot and killed Nesbitt, Jones' testimony was deemed more coherent and credible than Atkins' testimony and Atkins was convicted of abduction, armed robbery, and capital murder.627 At the penalty phase of the trial, the state introduced victim impact evidence and proved two aggravating circumstances: future dangerousness and “vileness of the offense.”628 In the penalty phase, the defense relied on a forensic psychologist, Dr. Evan Nelson, who had concluded that Atkins was “mildly mentally retarded.”629 His opinion was based on interviews with Atkins, Atkins' family members, and deputies at the jail where he had been incarcerated for the preceding 18 months.630 Dr. Nelson reviewed Atkins' statements made to the police, the investigative reports concerning the case, and school and court records. He also administered a standard intelligence test (the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales test (WAIS-III)), which indicated that Atkins had a full scale IQ of 59. Dr. Nelson testified that, in his opinion, Atkins' limited intellect had been a consistent feature throughout his life, and that his IQ score of 59 was not an “aberration, malingered result, or invalid test score.”631 The jury sentenced Atkins to death.632 Because the trial court had used a misleading verdict form, the Virginia Supreme Court ordered a second sentencing hearing.633 Dr. Nelson again testified for the defense and an expert rebuttal witness, Dr. Stanton Samenow, testified for the state.634 Dr. Samenow opined that Atkins was not mentally retarded, but rather was of "average intelligence, at least," and diagnosable as having antisocial personality disorder.635 Dr. Samenow's opinion was based upon two interviews with Atkins, a review of his school records, and interviews with correctional staff.636 He did not administer an intelligence test, but did ask Atkins questions taken from the 1972 version of the Wechsler Memory Scale.637 Dr. Samenow attributed Atkins’ “terrible” academic performance to the fact that he “is a person who chose to pay attention sometimes, not to pay attention others, and did poorly because he did not want to do what he was required to do.”638 The jury again sentenced Atkins to death.639 627 Id. Id. at 308. 629 Id. 630 Id. 631 Id. 632 Id. at 309. 633 Id. 634 Id. 635 Id. 636 Id. at 309 n. 6. 637 Id. 638 Id. 639 Id. at 309. 628 Presiding over a Capital Case • 139 Atkins appealed to the Supreme Court of Virginia, arguing that he could not be sentenced to death because he is mentally retarded.640 In affirming the imposition of the death penalty, the majority of the state court rejected his contention.641 Relying on the U.S. Supreme Court’s holding in Penry v. Lynaugh,642 they indicated they were “not willing to commute Atkins' sentence of death to life imprisonment merely because of his IQ score.”643 The dissenting justices found Dr. Samenow's opinion that Atkins possessed average intelligence as “incredulous as a matter of law,” and concluded that “the imposition of the sentence of death upon a criminal defendant who has the mental age of a child between the ages of 9 and 12 is excessive.”644 [7.4.] Findings and Reasoning Justice Stevens, joined by Justices O'Connor, Kennedy, Souter, Ginsberg, and Breyer reviewed the Eighth Amendment prohibition on “excessive” sanctions: “Excessive bail shall not be required, nor excessive fines imposed, nor cruel and unusual punishments inflicted.”645 A punishment is “excessive” if it is not graduated and proportioned to the offense, as judged by currently prevailing standards of decency.646 Assessing proportionality should be informed by objective factors to the maximum possible extent, the clearest and most reliable of which is the legislation enacted by the country's legislatures.647 The U.S. Supreme Court noted, however, that on the question of the acceptability of the death penalty under the Eighth Amendment, the Constitution contemplates that in the end the U.S. Supreme Court must bring to bear its own judgment.648 The U.S. Supreme Court reviewed state legislation and noted that since 1986, 18 states and the federal government have enacted statutes prohibiting a person with mental retardation from the death penalty.649 The U.S. Supreme Court suggested this consistency in the direction of change provided powerful evidence that today society views mentally retarded offenders as less culpable than the average criminal.650 The execution of mentally retarded offenders is uncommon even in those states that allow it.651 The U.S. Supreme Court found that a national consensus has developed against it and that the only question of serious disagreement is in determining which offenders are, in fact, retarded.652 640 Id. at 310. Id. 642 492 U.S. 302 (1989). 643 Atkins, 536 U.S. at 310. 644 Id. 645 Id. at 311. 646 Id. 647 Id. at 312. 648 Id. at 313. 649 Id. at 314. 650 Id. at 315. 651 Id. at 316. 652 Id. at 316-317. 641 140 • Presiding over a Capital Case The task of developing appropriate ways to enforce the constitutional restriction upon the execution of sentences was left to the states.653 The U.S. Supreme Court asserted that the consensus against the execution of the mentally retarded reflected widespread judgment that mentally retarded offenders were less culpable and that the penological purposes of the death penalty are not served by executing the mentally retarded.654 The U.S. Supreme Court reviewed the common elements of the definition of mental retardation (subaverage intellectual functioning, and significant limitations in adaptive skills such as communication, self-care, and self-direction that manifest before age 18) and asserted that while the mentally retarded may know the difference between right and wrong and may be competent to stand trial, they have diminished capacities in several areas: understanding and processing information, communicating, abstracting from mistakes and learning from experience, engaging in logical reasoning, controlling impulses, and understanding the reactions of others.655 Also, they act on impulse rather than pursuant to a premeditated plan and in group settings are followers rather than leaders.656 Because of these deficiencies, the U.S. Supreme Court questioned whether the justifications recognized as the basis for the death penalty — retribution or deterrence — apply to mentally retarded offenders.657 If not, the U.S. Supreme Court asserted that the imposition of the death penalty on a mentally retarded person is the purposeless and needless imposition of pain and suffering, which is an unconstitutional punishment.658 Retribution -- the interest in seeing that the offender gets his "just deserts" — requires a severity of punishment depending upon the culpability of the offender such that the death penalty is reserved for the most serious of crimes.659 The U.S. Supreme Court held previously that the culpability of the average murderer is insufficient to justify the imposition of the death penalty, and thus the lesser culpability of the mentally retarded offender should not merit that form of retribution.660 Executing the mentally retarded will not measurably further the goal of deterrence.661 The theory of deterrence is predicated upon the notion that the increased severity of the punishment will inhibit criminal actors from carrying out murderous conduct.662 Capital punishment can serve as a deterrent only when murder is the result of premeditation and deliberation.663 The sort of cold calculus that precedes the decision is at the opposite end of the spectrum from behavior of mentally retarded offenders as they are less able to process the information of the 653 Id. at 317. Id. 655 Id. at 318. 656 Id. 657 Id. 658 Id. at 319. 659 Id. 660 Id. 661 Id. at 320. 662 Id. 663 Id. at 319. 654 Presiding over a Capital Case • 141 possibility of execution as a penalty and control their conduct based upon that information.664 The U.S. Supreme Court expressed concern that some of the characteristics of mental retardation undermine the procedural protections provided by capital jurisprudence and increase the risk that the death penalty will be imposed in spite of factors which may call for a less severe penalty.665 The U.S. Supreme Court cited to the possibility of false confessions and the lesser ability of mentally retarded defendants to make a persuasive showing of mitigation in the face of prosecutorial evidence of one or more aggravating factors.666 Mentally retarded defendants may also be less able to give meaningful assistance to their counsel, typically are poor witnesses, and their demeanor may create an impression of lack of remorse for their crimes.667 Reliance on mental retardation as a mitigating factor may enhance the likelihood that the aggravating factor of future dangerousness will be found by the jury.668 [7.5.] Dissenting Opinions (Chief Justice Rehnquist, joined by Justice Scalia, and Justice Thomas). Justice Rehnquist challenged the conclusion that a national consensus had developed against the death penalty for mentally retarded capital murder defendants who are competent to stand trial, aware of the punishment they are about to suffer and why, and whose mental retardation has been found an insufficiently compelling reason to lessen their individual responsibility for the crime.669 He noted that the laws of 19 other states besides Virginia continue to leave the question of proper punishment to sentencing judges or juries.670 Chief Justice Rehnquist agreed with Justice Scalia that the U.S. Supreme Court's assessment of the current legislative judgment regarding the execution of defendants like petitioner more resembled a post hoc rationalization for the majority's subjectively preferred result rather than any objective effort to ascertain the content of an evolving standard of decency.671 Justice Rehnquist criticized the majority’s decision to place weight on the views of professional and religious organizations, and opinion polls in reaching its conclusion as unsupported by precedents and antithetical to considerations of federalism.672 He argued they should not be accorded any weight on the Eighth Amendment scale when the elected representatives of a state's populace have not deemed them persuasive enough to prompt legislative action.673 He questioned whether the surveys upon which the U.S. Supreme Court relied were conducted in accordance 664 Id. Id. at 320. 666 Id. 667 Id. 668 Id. at 321. 669 Id. 670 Id. at 322. 671 Id. 672 Id. 673 Id. at 326. 665 142 • Presiding over a Capital Case with generally accepted scientific principles.674 He suggested that in order to be credited they should be offered as evidence at trial, where their sponsors can be examined and cross-examined about methodological matters.675 He argued that the work product of legislatures and sentencing jury determinations ought to be the sole indicators by which courts ascertain the contemporary American conceptions of decency for purposes of the Eighth Amendment.676 He concluded that they are the only objective indicia of contemporary values firmly supported by precedents and are better suited than courts to evaluating and giving effect to the complex societal and moral considerations that inform the selection of publicly acceptable criminal punishments.677 Additionally, he criticized the U.S. Supreme Court's reference to other countries' disapproval of imposing the death penalty for crimes committed by mentally retarded offenders because the viewpoints of other countries are irrelevant to the issue of national consensus.678 He asserted that there are strong reasons for limiting the inquiry into what constitutes an evolving standard of decency under the Eighth Amendment to the laws passed by legislatures and the practices of sentencing juries in America.679 (Justice Scalia, with whom Chief Justice and Justice Thomas join, dissenting.) Justice Scalia asserted that the majority's decision found no support in the text or history of the Eighth Amendment nor in current social attitudes but rested upon nothing but the personal views of its members.680 He began by reviewing the case: after convicting Atkins of capital murder, the jury at resentencing heard extensive evidence of petitioner's alleged mental retardation through the testimony of a defense and the evidence from family members.681 They also heard evidence contesting the evidence of retardation and presented testimony of a psychologist who found absolutely no evidence other than the IQ score indicating that Atkins was in the least bit mentally retarded and concluded that Atkins was of average intelligence, at least.682 The jury also heard testimony about petitioner's 16 prior felony convictions that provided graphic depictions of Atkins' violent tendencies.683 Atkins' mental retardation was a central issue at sentencing, but the jury, concluding it was not a compelling reason to spare him the death penalty in light of the other factors, sentenced him to death and the Supreme Court of Virginia affirmed.684 Justice Scalia criticized the U.S. Supreme Court's conclusion that no one who is even slightly mentally retarded can have sufficient moral responsibility to 674 Id. Id. at 327. 676 Id. at 328. 677 Id. 678 Id. 679 Id. 680 Id. at 337. 681 Id. at 338. 682 Id. 683 Id. at 339. 684 Id. 675 Presiding over a Capital Case • 143 be subjected to capital punishment for any crime as an implausible sociological and moral conclusion and an even more implausible interpretation of the Constitution.685 A punishment is "cruel and unusual" under the Eighth Amendment if it is one of those modes or acts of punishment that 1) was considered cruel and unusual at the time that the Bill of Rights was adopted, or 2) is inconsistent with modern standards of decency.686 Because the execution of the mildly mentally retarded would not have been considered "cruel and unusual" in 1791, the majority argues that execution of the mildly retarded is inconsistent with the "evolving standards of decency that mark the progress of a maturing society.”687 Eighth Amendment judgments regarding the existence of social "standards" are to be informed by objective factors, first among which are the statutes passed by society's elected representatives as they will have a better sense of the evolution in views of the American people.688 Justice Scalia berated the majority’s finding of a national consensus forbidding execution of the mentally retarded when only 18 states--less than half (47%) of the 38 states that permit capital punishment (for whom the issue exists)--have very recently enacted legislation barring it.689 He further pointed out that only 7 of the 18 states have legislation prohibiting the execution of all mentally retarded defendants since several states allow it under some circumstances (e.g., for those already on death row).690 Justice Scalia concluded the prohibition against executing the mentally retarded was not a statement of absolute moral repugnance, but one of current preference between two tolerable approaches.691 Justice Scalia asserted that the "fudged 47%" figure resembled evidence the U.S. Supreme Court found inadequate to establish consensus in earlier cases.692 He worried that the legislation of all 18 states is still in its infancy such that no state knows whether these laws are sensible in the long term.693 He panned the U.S. Supreme Court's argument regarding the "consistency of the direction of change" because there was no other direction in which change could occur, and pointed out that merely 14 years earlier all death penalty statutes included the mentally retarded.694 He warned that reliance upon trends of even much longer duration than 14 years is a perilous basis for constitutional adjudication.695 He also criticized the majority's reliance upon the margins by which state legislatures have enacted bans on execution of the retarded as one could then argue that the number of people represented by the legislators voting 685 Id. Id. 687 Id. at 340. 688 Id. at 341. 689 Id. at 342. 690 Id. 691 Id. at 343. 692 Id. 693 Id. at 344. 694 Id. 695 Id. at 345. 686 144 • Presiding over a Capital Case for each bill would be significant in determining consensus.696 Consensus, he argued, is a consensus of the sovereign states that form the Union, not a nose count of Americans for and against.697 Justice Scalia criticized the argument that evidence of a "national consensus" could be found in the infrequency with which retarded persons are executed in states that allow their execution. He found it overwhelmingly probable that those considerations that induced the majority to believe that death should never be imposed on mentally retarded offenders cause prosecutors and juries to believe that it should rarely be imposed.698 Justice Scalia agreed with the Chief Justice's dissent, that the views of professional and religious organizations, the results of opinion polls, and the practices of the world community are irrelevant.699 Justice Scalia challenged the two assumptions underlying the U.S. Supreme Court's analysis: that the Eighth Amendment prohibits excessive punishments, and that sentencing juries or judges are unable to account properly for the "diminished capacities" of the retarded.700 The Eighth Amendment, he asserted, is addressed to always-and-everywhere "cruel" punishments, such as the rack and the thumbscrew.701 The assumed inability of judges or juries to take proper account of mental retardation, he declared, was unsubstantiated and contradicted the immemorial belief that they play an indispensable role in such matters.702 Justice Scalia disagreed with the two reasons the U.S. Supreme Court gave as to why the death penalty was an excessive punishment for all mentally retarded offenders.703 The claim that the "diminished capacities" of the mentally retarded raise a "serious question" whether their execution contributes to the "social purposes" of the death penalty, (retribution and deterrence) ignores a third "social purpose" of the death penalty--the incapacitation of dangerous criminals and the consequent prevention of crimes that they may otherwise commit in the future.704 Moreover, Justice Scalia challenged the claim that retribution is not advanced because the mentally retarded are no more culpable than the average murderer, asserting that there is no established correlation between mental acuity and the ability to conform one's conduct to the law in such a rudimentary matter as murder.705 Even if there were a direct connection, Justice Scalia questioned whether there is a scientific analysis that shows that a mildly retarded individual who commits an exquisite torture-killing is "no more culpable" than the "average" murderer in a holdup-gone-wrong or a domestic dispute; or a 696 Id. at 346. Id. 698 Id. 699 Id. at 347. 700 Id. at 348. 701 Id. at 349. 702 Id. 703 Id. 704 Id. at 350. 705 Id. 697 Presiding over a Capital Case • 145 moderately retarded individual who commits a series of 20 exquisite torturekillings.706 Justice Scalia asserted that culpability, and deservedness of the most severe retribution, depends not merely upon the mental capacity of the criminal but also upon the depravity of the crime--the sort of question traditionally answerable by the sentencer's weighing of the circumstances (both degree of retardation and depravity of crime) in the particular case.707 He reasoned that the fact that juries continue to sentence mentally retarded offenders to death for extreme crimes shows that society's moral outrage sometimes demands execution of retarded offenders.708 By admitting that mental retardation does not render the offender morally blameless, the majority cannot then say that the death penalty is never appropriate retribution, no matter how heinous the crime.709 As long as a mentally retarded offender knows "the difference between right and wrong," only the sentencer can assess whether his retardation reduces his culpability enough to exempt him from the death penalty for the particular murder in question.710 Regarding the argument that the deterrent effect of the death penalty is not advanced, Justice Scalia reiterated that this claim would lead to the same erroneous conclusion discussed earlier--that the mentally retarded (because they are less deterred) are more likely to kill.711 Even if the deterrent effect for mentally retarded individuals is less for others, the deterrent effect of a penalty is adequately vindicated if it successfully deters many, but not all, of the target class.712 Moreover, Justice Scalia argued, it is not clear that a murderer is somehow less blameworthy if he knew his act was wrong but he did not fully appreciate that he could die for it.713 But if so, he should be treated like an offender who may be "less likely" to respond to the death penalty because he was abused as a child such that his background should be considered by the sentencer as a mitigating factor.714 Justice Scalia reasoned that if mentally retarded offenders face a special risk of wrongful execution due to such things as being less able to be an effective witness or provide assistance to counsel it might support a due process claim but has nothing to do with an Eighth Amendment claim that execution of the mentally retarded is cruel and unusual.715 Finally, Justice Scalia predicted that the process of a capital trial would be turned into a game since the symptoms of mental retardation can readily be feigned, noting that the mere pendency of this case brought petitions by death row inmates claiming for the first time, after multiple habeas petitions, that they are retarded.716 706 Id. Id. at 351. 708 Id. 709 Id. 710 Id. 711 Id. 712 Id. 713 Id. at 352. 714 Id. 715 Id. 716 Id. at 353. 707 146 • Presiding over a Capital Case [7.6.] Mental Retardation Several factors complicate making the diagnosis of mental retardation. First, different organizations define mental retardation slightly differently. Second, psychologists and psychiatrists can select from a variety of assessment tools to detect mental retardation and all of the tools have some degree of potential error.717 Third, while all definitions of mental retardation require both below average intelligence and functional limitations, individuals may differ greatly in their social, academic and occupational abilities. Fourth, mental retardation has many potential causes but the diagnosis is independent of causal factors. Even the term mental retardation is subject to dispute—The American Association on Mental Retardation718 recently changed its terminology for the term to “intellectually and developmentally disabled.” For the purposes of this chapter, we use the more commonly understood term “mental retardation.” Conditions that cause the mental retardation may be present at birth but the diagnosis may not be established until many years later, especially for mildly mentally retarded people. Some of the congenital causes of mental retardation include exposure to infections or toxins such as alcohol during fetal development, chromosomal abnormalities and oxygen deprivation during delivery. After birth, environmental problems that can lead to mental retardation include lead exposure or extreme deprivation. In the majority of mental retardation diagnoses, doctors are unable to identify the underlying cause. The etiology in these cases is idiopathic, or unknown. [7.7.] Diagnostic Criteria Both the American Psychiatric Association (APA) and the American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (AAIDD) offer definitions for mental retardation. The APA publishes the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 4th Edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR).719 The DSM-IV-TR, the accepted reference manual for psychiatric diagnosis used by psychiatrists, 717 For an in depth analysis of the challenges facing courts implementing Atkins and a review of Virginia’s relevant statutes, see Richard J. Bonnie and Katherine Gustafson, The Challenge of Implementing Atkins v. Virginia: How legislatures and courts can promote accurate assessments and adjudications of mental retardation in death penalty cases 41 U. RICH L. REV. 811 (April 26, 2007) [hereinafter Challenges of Implementing Atkins]. 718 The American Association on Mental Retardation changed its name on January 1, 2007 to the American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. Press Release, American Association on Mental Retardation, World’s Oldest Organization on Intellectual Disability Has a Progressive New Name (Nov. 2, 2006), http://www.aamr.org/About_AAID/name_change_PRdreen.htm. We will use “AAIDD” except when referencing citations from publications that were published under the earlier designation of “AAMR.” 719 AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION, DIAGNOSTIC AND STATISTICAL MANUAL 4TH EDITION, TEXT REVISION (American Psychiatric Press Inc., 2000) (1952) [hereinafter DSM-IV-TR]. Presiding over a Capital Case • 147 psychologists and other mental health professionals, defines mental retardation using specific criteria described in the next section. The AAIDD has defined mental retardation since 1921720 and describes it not as a mental disorder but in terms of a functional state.721 Rarely would a psychiatrist or psychologist select a diagnostic definition from a source other than these two organizations. Their definitions are sufficiently different to merit a comparative review in the following section. The two organizations require, however, the same three prongs for a diagnosis of mental retardation: (1) significant limitations in intelligence (operationalized by IQ scores); (2) multiple deficits in adaptive behavior; and (3) onset during the developmental period before 18. Regardless of the definition used, mental retardation is a clinical diagnosis that requires interpretation of the diagnostic criteria by a trained mental health professional. [7.8.] DSM-IV-TR Criteria The DSM-IV-TR definition of mental retardation requires that the patient display both of the following: “[s]ignificantly subaverage intellectual functioning: an IQ of approximately 70 or below” and “[c]oncurrent deficits or impairments in present adaptive functioning . . . in at least two of the following areas: communication, self-care, home living, social/interpersonal skills, use of community resources, self-direction, functional academic skills, work, leisure, health and safety.”722 The definition also requires onset of these characteristics before age 18.723 The age requirement establishes that mental retardation represents a delay or interruption of normal development rather than a loss of functioning due to an injury or dementing illness. While the diagnosis requires evidence of impairment before age 18, there is no cut-off age (in any diagnostic scheme) for making the diagnosis of mental retardation. The DSM-IV-TR is a clinical tool and does not give specific rationale for making the diagnosis definition of mental retardation or any other diagnosis. The DSM-IV-TR also includes a cautionary statement that the inclusion of a diagnosis in the manual “does not imply the diagnosis meets legal or other medical criteria for what constitutes a mental disease, mental disorder or mental disability.”724 The DSM-IV-TR defines several levels of mental retardation. Mild mental retardation is defined as an IQ level of 50-55 to approximately 70. The range in definitional IQ scores reflects the margin of error of all IQ tests, which will be addressed in a later section. The vast majority of people with mental 720 AMERICAN ASSOCIATION ON MENTAL RETARDATION, MENTAL RETARDATION DEFINITIONS, CLASSIFICATION, AND SYSTEMS OF SUPPORTS (Luckasson et al. eds., 10th edition, 2002) (1921) [hereinafter AAMR]. 721 In addition to the book and manual on mental retardation, the AAID’s website includes information on the diagnosis at http://www.aamr.org/Policies/faq_mental_retardation.shtml. 722 DSM-IV-TR at 41. 723 Id. 724 Id. at xxxvii and at xxxi; The DSM-IV-TR also includes a section on the limitations of the categorical approach to psychiatric diagnoses. 148 • Presiding over a Capital Case retardation are mildly mentally retarded. People with mild mental retardation have functional impairments that require additional community support and special education classes at school. As adults, they may live independently with support from family, “trainers” or in group homes with minimal supervision. Some people with mild mental retardation maintain employment, especially if an employer is motivated to provide additional support and to modify the job to fit the skills of the individual.725 Individuals with IQ scores in the 71-84 range may be diagnosed with “borderline intellectual functioning” which the DSM-IV-TR describes as a condition distinct from mental retardation.726 The capital cases which involve defendants with a differential diagnosis of mild mental retardation versus borderline intellectual functioning are those most likely to focus the court’s attention to the known limitations of the current measurement tools. According to the DSM-IV-TR, people with moderate mental retardation have IQ scores of 35-40 to 50-55. They are able to learn vocational skills and manage their daily self-care with support and supervision. People in this category may live in a community group home or with family.727 Defendants with this level of intellectual deficit are less likely to generate controversy about the diagnosis of mental retardation since their deficits would be more obvious. Severe mental retardation is associated with IQ levels of 20-25 to 35-40. People in this range have very limited verbal communication skills and require higher levels of supervision and support in community settings than people with less disability.728 People with an IQ level less than 20-25 are diagnosed with profound mental retardation. This diagnosis implies global deficits in functioning and a need for constant supervision and care.729 IQ SCORE 71-84 55-70 40-55 25-40 <25 DIAGNOSIS Borderline Intellectual Functioning Mild Mental Retardation Moderate Mental Retardation Severe Mental Retardation Profound Mental Retardation FUNCTIONING Some impairment May live independently Group home living Limited communications skills Needs constant care At times, an evaluator is unable to administer an IQ test to an individual who appears functionally limited. This may happen with very young children or with uncooperative examinees. The diagnosis of Mental Retardation, Severity Unspecified communicates the evaluator’s judgment that the individual would meet the diagnosis of mental retardation if a valid IQ measure were obtained.730 725 Id. at 42-43. Id. at 740. 727 Id. at 43. 728 Id. at 43-44. 729 Id. at 44; For a critique on the severity subcategories see AAMR at 26-27 and 35-36. 730 Id. at 52. 726 Presiding over a Capital Case • 149 [7.9.] AAMR Definition In contrast to the American Psychiatric Association and the DSM-IV-TR definition to mental retardation, the definition for mental retardation put forth by the American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities does not specify a numerical value for IQ. In the Atkins case, the definition of mental retardation used by Justice Stevens was similar to the 1992 American Association on Mental Retardation (“AAMR”) definition which was: Mental retardation refers to substantial limitations in present functioning. It is characterized by significantly subaverage intellectual functioning, existing concurrently with related limitations in two or more of the following applicable adaptive skills areas: communication, self-care, home living, social skills, community use, self-direction, health and safety, functional academics, leisure, and work. Mental retardation manifests itself before age 18.731 The current 2002 AAID definition is “Mental retardation is a disability characterized by significant limitations both in intellectual and adaptive behavior as expressed in conceptual, social and practical adaptive skills. This disability originates before age 18.” 732 The AAMR states that the diagnosis of mental retardation serves the functions of establishing eligibility for services, benefits and legal protection.733 Although the AAMR definition itself does not specify what IQ test results represent “significant limitations in intellectual behavior,” the current AAMR workbook and textbook state that the IQ criterion is “approximately two standard deviations below the mean.”734 Rather than describe levels of severity in mental retardation, the AAMR identifies levels of necessary community support for mentally retarded people.735 [7.10.] Tools for Assessing Mental Retardation In order to accurately diagnose mental retardation, psychologists and psychiatrists require tools that are reliable and accurate. The tools must directly assess the elements specified in the definitions and they must validly measure IQ 731 Atkins, 536 U.S. at 308, n 3 (the court references both the 1991 AAMR definition and the APA definitions of mental retardation but did not endorse a single definition. The court describes mental retardation as clinically defined by “subaverage intellectual functioning but also significant limitations in adaptive skills such as communication, selfcare, and self-direction that become manifest before age 18.”). 732 Id. at 1. 733 Id. at 22; the AAMR Workbook is a manual that includes case studies and worksheets for use in diagnosis, classification and identifying systems of supports. 734 Id. at 58. 735 P. J. Accardo and A. J. Capute, Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Review, 4(1) MENTAL RETARDATION 2-5 (1998); AAMR at 28. 150 • Presiding over a Capital Case and adaptive functioning in order to meet a standard of scientific certainty about the diagnosis. Bonnie and Gustafson stated “At the very least, the Court must have assumed that expert differences would rest on articulable differences in scientific or clinical judgment, rather than on hidden disagreements about whether the offender deserves a death sentence.”736 [7.11.] Measurement of IQ For over 100 years, psychologists have measured intelligence with standardized tests.737 A individual’s test results are compared to a reference population and converted into a number referred to as an Intelligence Quotient (IQ). IQ tests have evolved over time. A small number of tests tend to be very commonly used but many tests are available.738 The expanded options in IQ tests allow examiners to select a test that best matches the cultural and linguist characteristics of the test taker.739 Although they are standardized, IQ tests do not perfectly measure intelligence. The potential error in IQ tests can impact an individual’s score and sometimes lead to discrepant conclusions about the diagnosis of mental retardation. If a psychologist tested the IQ of 1,000 randomly selected people, the 1,000 IQ scores would symmetrically spread around the average score (or mean) arbitrarily set at 100. IQ scores in a population have a predictable spread (or distribution) around the mean and predictable variability between scores. This allows a description of the variance of IQ scores known as the “standard deviation” or “SD.” Most IQ tests have a standard deviation of 15 points. The great majority of people in a random sample have IQ scores within two standard deviations (thirty points) of the mean. This encompasses the range of 100 +/- 30, or all scores in the range of 70 to 130. Below 70 is the extreme low, or mental retardation range.740 Above 130 is the very superior or genius range. Only a small percentage of the population has IQ scores that are more than two SD’s from the mean. The lowest two or three percent of the population meet the IQ criterion for mental retardation with a score at or below 70.741 736 Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 3. GALE H. ROID AND R. ANDREW BARRAM, ESSENTIALS OF STANFORD-BINET INELLIGENCE SCALES (SB5) ASSESSMENT 3-9 (John Wiley and Sons 2004). 738 DSM-IV-TR at 41; In addition to the Wechsler and the Stanford-Binet scales, other IQ tests in common use include the Cognitive Assessment System and the Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test. See Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 7 as well as AAMR at 59-66. 739 Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 6. 740 See Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 2 in which they state “[i]n short, the diagnosis of mental retardation is in large part a statistical construct.” 741 D. WEDDING and M. L. STUBER, BEHAVIOR & MEDICINE 236-237 (Hogrefe & Huber Publishers 2006). 737 Presiding over a Capital Case • 151 [7.12.] Commonly Used IQ Tests The psychologist David Wechsler developed tests to measure IQ for several different age ranges. These tests, like other IQ tests, use subtests to measure verbal and nonverbal or performance abilities. For example, verbal and memory abilities are measured through vocabulary tasks, verbal reasoning skills and short term recall tasks. Nonverbal tasks include logical completion of diagrams and copying block designs. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, currently in its fourth revision (the WISC-IV), is for children from six to seventeen years old. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, currently in a third revision (the WAIS-III) is used for testing people in the 16 to 89 age range. It contains six verbal subtests and five perceptual/ motor subtests.742 The Stanford-Binet IQ test, currently in its fifth revision (the StanfordBinet-V or SB5), is used for ages from two and a half years to young adult. The Stanford-Binet-V may not be able to accurately measure IQ below 40 (moderate and severe mental retardation ranges).743 These and all IQ tests must be administered in a standard fashion to be valid. Administration must follow the test guidelines such as reading the instructions and timing tests in a standardized way. Departing from the clinical standards may lead to invalid results. [7.13.] Screening IQ Tests Some shorter IQ tests, which take less time to administer, are used for screening. When an individual scores outside the normal IQ range, they are selected out for closer evaluation. Screening IQ tests, also described as “short form” tests, are more prone to error than standard IQ tests. In situations where much is at stake, such as a capital case, screening tests are not adequate.744 742 AAMR at 60-61. AAMR at 62. 744 See Rivera v. Quarterman, 505 F.3d 349 (5th Cir. 2007); J. Widroff and C. Watson, Mental Retardation and the Death Penalty: Addressing Various Questions Regarding an Atkins Claim 36 J. AM. ACAD. PSYCHIATRY L. 413-415 (2008). 743 152 • Presiding over a Capital Case [7.14.] Errors in Measurement Psychological tests, like all measurement tools, have a potential for error. Because they often rely on subjective ratings, psychological tests may be more error-prone than other tools.745 The amount of possible error in IQ tests is quantified and is expressed as “standard error of measurement” or SEM. Common IQ tests have different SEMs which vary from one to five points and depend, in part, on the age of the test-taker. For a test with an SEM of three points, a score of 100 might be expressed as 100 +/- 3. This means that the true score very likely lies between the values of 97 and 103. For an individual with a score near average, these ranges are not very significant. For the capital defendant with a borderline IQ score of 71, the standard error of measurement is a critical consideration, since the true score is likely in the range of 68 to 74.746 The SEM is the reason that the DSM-IV-TR includes a five point spread in the descriptions of different categories of mental retardation and why some state statutes include a range in the definition. The AAID states that the IQ criterion in their definition reflects “the SEM for the specific assessment instruments used and the instrument’s strengths and weaknesses.” 747 The SEM for the WISC-IV is 3.2. The SEM for the WAIS varies slightly with age but averages around 2.3. The Stanford-Binet has a SEM 1.6 for adults and 3.58 for children, reflecting the greater known error in tests with children. Another area of potential error in IQ tests is a phenomenon called the Flynn Effect, which describes how a population’s mean score “creeps” up over time up to three IQ points per decade. In order to keep the average score 100, IQ tests need to be recalibrated (renormed) every ten to twenty years. If an individual takes an IQ test at the end of a test’s cycle when it is due for recalibration, that individual may test up to six points higher than he would on the newer version. The Flynn Effect is a potential confound for defendants who score on the borderline of mental retardation. They may argue that an older version of the test led to an incorrectly high IQ score and that downward adjustment is necessary to reflect the true IQ score.748 A third possible source of error in IQ tests is the “test-retest” or practice effect. If a person is retested on IQ within six to twelve months he or she is likely to score higher since he has familiarity and practice with the type of tasks 745 Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 10. Sometimes a broader range of IQ score is used to increase the confidence that the true score is captured. Psychologists often seek a high level of confidence by using a broader score range. A 95% confidence interval is one in which one may be 95% confident that the true score lies within the interval. This level of confidence requires an interval of 2 SEMS, or +/- ten points. 747 AAMR at 58. 748 Tomoe Kanaya, Matthew Scullin and Stephen Ceci, The Flynn Effect and U.S. Policies: The Impact of Rising I.Q. Scores on American Society via Mental Retardation Diagnosis, 58 AM. PSYCHOL. 778, 786-87 (2003); Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 11. 746 Presiding over a Capital Case • 153 required by the test.749 The practice effect typically affects the performance tasks more than verbal tasks.750 A fourth possible source of error is transient or state dependent symptoms that may negatively affect an IQ score. A person with poorly controlled seizures may test lower on an IQ test during a period when the seizures occur three times a week. The same person may test higher on IQ during a period when the seizures are well controlled. Artificially low scores can also occur when a person has active symptoms of mental illness, such as depression or psychosis that affect concentration and motivation.751 Once an IQ measurement enters the legal setting, there is the potential for forensic bias. This is the possible unintentional bias of an examiner who is aligned with the attorneys who retained him or her. Since the examiner who tests IQ is responsible for scoring and interpreting the test results, the bias may express itself in the form of an elevated or lowered IQ score or in the findings of adaptive behavior tests. Some might argue that this potential for forensic bias means that IQ test results preceding an individual’s arrest are more likely to be accurate than those obtained after an individual becomes a defendant. However, well meaning clinicians providing community services prior to legal entanglements may liberalize a diagnosis of mental retardation in order for their clients to receive benefits and services eligible only to people with the diagnosis. Thus, even the pre-arrest clinical record has the potential to distort diagnostic criteria. [7.15.] Tests of Adaptive Behavior Since 1959, the AAID has required deficits in three domains of functional or adaptive behavior to meet the diagnosis of mental retardation. This reflects the awareness that IQ scores have limitations and should not be the sole determinant of a diagnosis of mental retardation.752 The DSM-IV-TR definition also requires functional deficits. In general, the behaviors that are targeted by these criteria are those that impact daily independent living. If an individual requires supports for managing everyday life, this should translate into measurable adaptive limitations. The field of psychology offers an abundance of tools for assessing functional abilities but the tests are only scientifically valid if the tools are administered in a standardized fashion.753 Like IQ tests, the mean score on adaptive behavior tests is set at 100 and the standard deviation is generally around 15. Significant deficits of adaptive behavior are noted in individuals scoring two or more standard deviations below the mean score.754 749 R. J. McCaffrey and H. J. Westervelt, Issues Associated with Repeated Neuropsychological Assessments 5(3) NEUROPSYCHOLOGY REV. 203-221 (1995). 750 Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 11-12. 751 Id. at 12. 752 AAMR at 56-57. 753 Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 15. 754 AAMR at 79; Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 17. Although definitions for mental retardation include the requirement of adaptive deficits in three domains, the 154 • Presiding over a Capital Case Tests of adaptive behavior, although well-established for use in diagnosing mental retardation, are less developed than IQ tests and carry a greater risk of error.755 Adaptive skills tests are scored according to age. Adaptive skills tests sample behavior in three or more domains. The usual areas are conceptual (communication and money skills), social (following rules, avoiding victimization and socializing) and practical (self-care, housekeeping and occupational skills). Many tools for adaptive behavior use a third party respondent—a person who knows the individual well and can describe his or her level of functioning in a number of domains. Respondents may be parents, spouses, counselors, work supervisors, or anyone who is familiar with the individual’s abilities.756 The rationale for using a respondent instead of a direct interview is the tendency of people with mental retardation to be poor historians of their abilities. The Vineland Adaptive Behavioral Scales, Second Edition (Vineland-II) is a commonly used test of adaptive functioning. The Vineland-II has three versions, all of which require a respondent. The preferred version is a semistructured interview with the respondent. Information from the interview is coded on a survey interview form. Another version of the Vineland-II offers a questionnaire with rating scales that the respondent completes. The rating scale version may risk more potential bias than the interview version.757 The VinelandII is standardized and an individual’s score can be reported in terms of standard deviations above or below the mean similar to IQ tests.758 Among the many other tests of adaptive behavior are the AAMR Adaptive Behavioral Scales, the Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised and the Adaptive Behavior Assessment Scale, Second Edition (ABAS-2).759 [7.16.] Potential Errors of Measurement of Adaptive Behavior Error may be introduced into the results of adaptive behavior tests in several ways. First, the term “functional deficit” may be interpreted in different ways. The AAID emphasizes deficit in performance rather than capacity. That is, the individual may have the ability to function at a non-deficit level but he or she may not know when to use certain skills or may lack motivation to use them.760 Second, the adaptive behavior of criminal defendants must often be assessed in the correctional setting. None of the adaptive behavior tests are specifically validated for use in prisons, much less on death row. The restricted range of performance required to function in correctional environments sets a low criteria is met when a person scores two standard deviations below the mean in one domain and some limitations in two other domains. 755 Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 6. 756 SARA SPARROW, PH.D., VINELAND II, SECOND EDITION SURVEY FORMS MANUAL 12 (AGS Publishing 2005) [hereinafter Vineland II Survey Forms Manual]. 757 Vineland II Survey Forms Manual at 9-11. 758 AAMR at 88. 759 AAMR at 88-90. 760 Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 15; AAMR at 74. Presiding over a Capital Case • 155 standard for adaptive behavior and there are few opportunities for third party respondents to observe an inmate’s leisure or community skills. The AAID’s textbook on mental retardation cautions that “[o]bservations made outside the context of community environments typical of the individual’s age peers and culture warrant significantly reduced weight.”761 Outside record review and interviews with people who knew the defendant well before incarceration should better inform the examiner in an overall determination of adaptive deficits.762 A third source for error is the respondent. A close friend or relative of the defendant who understands the protective implications of a diagnosis of mental retardation may exaggerate observed deficits. On the other hand, using correctional staff as the respondent may yield the opposite result. The validity of the test’s results decreases when respondents have only limited knowledge of the defendant. [7.17.] Statutory Variations in the Definition of Mental Retardation States that address mental retardation in their statutes related to capital punishment vary significantly in their definitions. Most states use language similar to the AAIS’s criterion of “significantly subaverage general intellectual functioning.” Some states require a cutoff of IQ at 70; others do not specify an IQ. Some states require the onset of disability by 18, for Utah it is age 22763 and others state “during developmental period.” Nebraska’s statute does not include any age criterion.764 In Kansas, the definition for mental retardation uses diminished capacity language: “[s]ignificantly subaverage general intellectual functioning, . . . to an extent which substantially impairs one's capacity to appreciate the criminality of one's conduct or to conform one's conduct to the requirements of law.”765 State statutes may require specific functional deficits and may delineate the necessary qualifications of the examiner if appointed by the court.766 State statutes also vary in the process by which mental retardation is determined. [7.18.] Special Issues in the Diagnosis of Mental Retardation Mental retardation diagnoses differ from other psychiatric diagnoses and raise special concerns about malingering. Large discrepancies appear in retests. This section addresses some of the special issues in mental retardation. 761 AAMR at 85. Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 15. 763 UTAH CODE ANN. § 77-15a-101 (2003). 764 NEB. REV. STAT. § 28-105.01 (2002). 765 KAN. STAT. ANN. § 21-4623 (2004). 766 VA. CODE ANN. §§ 19.2-264.3:1.1-19.2-264.3:1.2 (2003). 762 156 • Presiding over a Capital Case [7.19.] Categorical Diagnosis Receiving a psychiatric diagnosis does not typically confer a categorical legal protection, although it is often a requirement for potential protections. For example, people with schizophrenia are not categorically acquitted of crimes in states that allow for acquittal by reason of insanity nor are they automatically deemed incompetent to stand trial. Rather, the clinical diagnosis suggests the presence of impairments that may impact the individual’s understanding of their actions or ability to assist in their own defense. Some authors have wondered why the U.S. Supreme Court did not adopt a diminished capacity/responsibility approach for capital punishment in mentally retarded defendants, since this would allow for evaluation of meaningful functional abilities relevant to each case.767 In capital cases where mental retardation is at issue, the diagnosis itself confers a categorical legal protection, regardless of the individual’s cognitive and functional capacities to plan and understand his or her actions. The placement of an important legal protection on a clinical diagnosis highlights the fallibilities of the diagnostic definitions and tools. [7.20.] Malingering As noted earlier in this chapter, Justice Scalia referred to the potential for malingered mental retardation in his dissent.768 Malingering is likely to figure prominently in contested mental retardation diagnoses. In the DSM-IV-TR, malingering is defined as “intentional production or false or grossly exaggerated . . . symptoms motivated by external incentives.”769 There is no definitive test for malingering, and mental health professionals have no claim to superior detection of deception.770 The potential for malingered intellectual and functional deficits in the correctional setting can be offset by adequate collateral documentation of pre-existing deficits. Records from health care providers (including pediatric records), schools, employers, military, mental health, and even prior incarcerations can identify the presence of functional deficits. For example, a written request for medical services while in a jail will often be preserved in the medical record and can serve as an example of an inmate’s reading and writing abilities. Malingering, even when identified with confidence, does not rule out a diagnosis of mental retardation since both may occur in a defendant. It is possible that an individual who has a valid diagnosis of mental retardation will exaggerate his or her deficits to achieve some type of gain, such as avoiding trial or obtaining a desired housing unit. 767 Challenges of Implementing Atkins at 2. Atkins, 536 U.S. at 353. 769 DSM-IV-TR at 739-740. 770 Several malingering tests do exist and may be helpful in detecting gross exaggerated deficits. An example is the Test of Malingered Memory ("TOMM"). 768 Presiding over a Capital Case • 157 [7.21.] Discrepant Findings Courts will hear evidence that reflects discrepant findings related to the diagnosis of mental retardation in a capital defendant. Below is a list of some of the reasons for discrepancies. [7.22.] Examiners use different tests to measure IQ and adaptive functioning. Examiners do not adhere to standardized administration, scoring or reporting of test results. Examiners use different versions of the same test (Flynn Effect). Examiners use different collateral sources or give different weight to collateral reports. Examiners use different respondents for adaptive skills assessment. Examinee was experiencing a time-limited disturbance in thinking and performance. There is a test-retest practice effect on IQ tests. Examiners use different interpretations of mental retardation definition. Forensic bias on the part of the examiners. Co-morbidity The diagnosis of mental retardation does not rule out or exclude other diagnoses. For example, a diagnosis of another psychiatric disorder, such as schizophrenia, can co-exist with mental retardation. People with mental retardation may have personality disorders, substance use disorders or other developmental disorders such as autism. In general, people with mental retardation have three to four times the number of co-morbid mental disorders when compared to the general population.771 Some people with mental retardation have behavioral disturbances that do not meet criteria for another disorder but require behavioral or medication treatment. Although there is no specific medical treatment for mental retardation, psychiatric medications are sometimes prescribed to decrease symptoms from behavioral and emotional problems. [7.23.] Illustrative Case This is a fictional case that illustrates some of the issues in the diagnosis of mental retardation. John Smith is a 21 year old white man who is arrested on charges of killing three people during a botched armed robbery. There is evidence that he 771 DSM-IV-TR at 45. 158 • Presiding over a Capital Case killed the victims because he thought they would later identify him. The coroner reported that each victim was shot point blank in the head, execution style. Mr. Smith has a history of three previous felonies, all related to burglaries and thefts. Mr. Smith asserts that he is not eligible for capital punishment because he is mentally retarded. In the jurisdiction of this case, the diagnosis of mental retardation is determined by jury prior to the sentencing phase of the trial and the burden of proof is on the defense. [7.24.] Defense Perspective Mr. Smith’s attorney presents evidence that his client has spent his life attempting to minimize and mask his deficits. In grade school he felt humiliated by his status as a special education student and had fantasies of moving to another school where he could start over as a normal kid. He stated to the forensic examiner that his brothers told him to “Never let anyone know you are retarded—no girl will ever want to go out with you.” Mr. Smith dropped out of school in 10th grade. Although he never learned to read fluently, Mr. Smith learned social skills and distracted others from his academic limitations. Still, his intellectual disability interfered with his ability to maintain jobs as a dishwasher or busboy because he was unable to learn the tasks and meet the demands of the employer. He lived with his parents until he dropped out of high school and then lived with a girlfriend because he lacked independent living skills. When he was arrested, he used his well-developed social skills to try and navigate his new situation in the jail. He was aware that if anyone knew about his disability, he would be seen as a weak person and at risk for abuse. Mr. Smith confessed in the police car even before booking not because he was guilty but because he felt the officers wanted him to say those things. Mr. Smith’s mother was the respondent for the adaptive behavior scale and noted multiple deficiencies relative to his peers. Mr. Smith’s IQ test score of 71 reflects mental retardation because the standard error of measurement of the test places his score as low as 68 (range of 68 to 74). The second IQ test was artificially inflated due to the test-retest effect. The examiner was not qualified to test IQ and erred in using the same test within a three month time period. Adaptive testing done by the prosecution in the jail setting is invalid because the test reflected only the level of functioning required in a highly-structured institution. The respondents for the test, correctional officers, were highly biased against Mr. Smith. Even severely mentally retarded people can adapt to the diminished expectations of the regimented schedule requiring only showing up for meals, showering twice a week and keeping quiet. Mr. Smith shows adaptive deficits in educational, occupational, self-care and social functioning. [7.25.] Prosecution Perspective Mr. Smith is malingering—he is exaggerating his deficits on IQ testing to escape the consequences of his behavior. The collateral information is from Presiding over a Capital Case • 159 family and friends who are vested in the court finding the diagnosis of mental retardation. Mr. Smith’s school records provide little support for the claim of mental retardation. Many of his classes were in "regular" education. He did not require special education classes until he was in high school. By then, he was engaged in using drugs, hanging out with delinquent peers, and had stopped trying. Mr. Smith may have a learning disability that affected his reading skills but it does not rise to the level of mental retardation. He simply was not a motivated student and so did not learn in school. Mr. Smith’s work record suggests he left jobs because he was impulsive and had a poor sense of responsibility. His impulsive behaviors and parasitic lifestyle, living off of his parents and girlfriends, are simply more evidence of the obvious antisocial personality that is highlighted by his criminal record. Mr. Smith confessed because he was guilty. There was no coercion. He waived his Miranda rights voluntarily and knowingly. He did not minimize or mask mental retardation to his friends and employers simply because he did not have mental retardation. The first IQ test was low because he “played dumb” at his family’s urging. The second IQ test showed a normal intelligence score of 78 which is in the normal range. The functional assessment scale completed in the prison showed he was normal. He is no different from the other inmates. [7.26.] Summary and Future Directions The challenges of reliably diagnosing mental retardation relate to the limitations of measurement tools and the potential for malingering and bias. Nonetheless, courts can sort through some of these confounds when hearing cases in which capital defendants assert a diagnosis of mental retardation. The use of qualified examiners using validated tools in standardized fashion and heavy reliance on collateral records that predate the criminal case will assist the trier of fact in untangling the issues. Psychological tests will evolve and it is likely tests of adaptive functioning within correctional institutions will be developed and validated in response to Atkins. The American Bar Association adopted recommendations in 2006 that extended the legal protections from Atkins to defendants with brain injury or dementia.772 States are certain to see attempts at similar extensions to other groups of defendants. 772 ABA RECOMMENDATION 122A (2006) Submitted by Paul Igasaki, Chair, Section of Individual Rights and Responsibilities.