Sleep Medicine Reviews, Vol. 5, No. 6, pp 491–506, 2001 doi:10.1053/smrv.2001.0164, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on SLEEP MEDICINE reviews PHYSIOLOGICAL REVIEW Sleep states and memory processes in humans: procedural versus declarative memory systems C. Smith Department of Psychology, Trent University, Peterborough, Ontario, Canada KEYWORDS humans, REM sleep, NREM sleep, learning, declarative memory, procedural memory Summary The present paper focuses on human studies attempting to relate sleep states to memory processes. These studies typically present learning material to participants and then examine their ability to recall this material after intervening posttraining sleep or sleep deprivation. Most experiments utilize either sleep recording or sleep deprivation following task acquisition to reach their conclusions, although cueing and position emission tomography (PET) scan studies have also been done. Results strongly suggest that REM sleep is involved with the efficient memory processing of cognitive procedural material but not declarative material. Although there are some data to suggest that stage 3/4 or NREM sleep is necessary for declarative memory consolidation, NREM may in fact simply be occurring at the same time as another factor that is actually involved in the memory processing. Preliminary results suggest that the length of the NREM–REM sleep cycle may be important for declarative memory. Preliminary data also suggest that stage 2 sleep may be involved with the memory for motor procedural but not cognitive procedural tasks. Sleep researchers would do well to capitalize on the latest advancements in memory research by choosing tasks that represent special memory systems and examining their relationships to sleep states. 2001 Harcourt Publishers Ltd INTRODUCTION It has been over 75 years since Jenkins and Dallenbach [1] claimed that recall in humans improved after an intervening night of sleep. Since this pioneering report, a substantial number of studies have been carried out to examine the possibility that sleep is related to the cognitive processes of learning and memory. Experiments have been done using both animal and human subjects. The present paper will focus on those studies using human Correspondence should be addressed to: Dr Carlyle Smith, Department of Psychology, Trent University, Peterborough, Ontario, Canada, K9J 7B8. E-mail: csmith@trentu.ca 1087–0792/01/060491+16 $35.00/0 participants and paradigms that examine the effects of sleep states on previously learned material. One of the earliest reviews of this area [2] examined the possible relationship between sleep states and learning/memory in both animals and humans. There was substantial evidence from animal studies to support the idea that post-training REM sleep was important for memory consolidation. The human studies, however, provided mixed results. The human studies cited as showing no relationship between sleep and memory utilized simple memorization tasks. Those studies that reported a positive sleep–memory effect utilized tasks requiring more complex cognitive activity such as anagrams or a test of story meaningfulness. Since then, research has shown that the types of memory tasks used in these studies are very important. 2001 Harcourt Publishers Ltd 492 THE EVOLUTION OF THE UNDERSTANDING OF MEMORY When the earlier sleep-learning studies were carried out, it was assumed that memory had three phases. These included a very short registration phase taking less than 1 s to occur followed by a period (minutes to hours) after training, during which learned material existed as relatively labile short-term memory that was vulnerable to various agents such as strong drugs or electroconvulsive therapy (ECT). As the minutes turned to hours and memory processing continued, the learned material was considered less vulnerable to disruption. After 24 h (or even less), memory formation was considered complete and, thus, no longer vulnerable to disruption. At this point a stable, permanent long-term memory was considered to have been formed. This process has been called consolidation and most workers still use the concept of a permanent “memory trace” existing in the brain [3–5]. In many of the early sleep–memory animal studies, it was assumed that the first few hours of sleep after training were sufficient to observe the conversion of learned material from short- to long-term memory [2, 6, 7]. In humans, one night of sleep was considered sufficient. However, as time passed, reports of vulnerability to such agents as drugs or ECT went from hours to days to weeks, months and years. For example, Squire et al. [8] reported that a series of five ECT treatments in human adults resulted in loss of memory for television shows seen in the last 3 years, but not before. Thus, although the phases leading to long-term memory have remained more or less theoretically intact, their timing and duration have been shown to be quite variable. Memory was traditionally thought to be a unitary system. However, there are now considered to be at least two kinds of memory in humans. Although a variety of terms are used to describe each, one of the most commonly utilized distinctions is between declarative and procedural knowledge. Declarative material has been described as “knowing that” and refers to memories accessible to conscious recollection. “Knowing how” describes procedural knowledge. These are memories of how to do some skill or how to solve a problem. These two memory systems are believed to have different functional systems and even different neuroanatomical C. SMITH substrates. Memories can also be explicitly learned or implicitly learned, meaning that they can be consciously acquired or acquired without conscious knowledge. Generally speaking, declarative material is usually explicitly (consciously) learned and procedural material is usually implicitly (unconsciously) learned [9, 10]. While this is a very rudimentary coverage of the nature of the memory systems now being studied, it will be sufficient for the purposes of this review. Since much of the knowledge about memory systems has become available in the last 15 years or so, earlier studies attempting to find a relationship between sleep and learning/memory naturally assumed that memory was a single monolithic process. More recent human studies have barely begun to relate sleep states specifically to the multiple memory systems model. APPROACHES TO THE STUDY OF SLEEP AND MEMORY There have been four main kinds of studies performed to study the relationship between sleep and memory in humans. One approach has been to train subjects on some task and then to record the changes in sleep parameters (or some other variables such as cerebral blood flow) on subsequent nights. These values are compared to the participants’ own baseline sleep activity as well as to the sleep activity of various non-learning and pseudolearning controls. The rationale for this method is to observe changes in sleep or other physiological activities correlated with learning progress. A second approach has been to deprive the subject of total sleep or of some specific phase or aspect of sleep following task acquisition in order to induce possible memory impairments. Memory for the task is then re-examined at some later post-test time and related to the type of sleep loss. A third approach has been to have the subject learn the task either just before bed or during the night at some particular point. Then the subjects are retested after only a partial night of sleep to observe the efficacy of some particular sleep phase. A fourth, and much less utilized method, has been to try to cue (usually auditory) subjects in the night during various sleep stages. The cue is related to the learning task and has sometimes resulted SLEEP STATES AND MEMORY PROCESSES IN HUMANS in memory enhancement. This is a method that attempts positively to influence the course of the memory formation during sleep, presumably while it is occurring. SLEEP AND MEMORY FOR DECLARATIVE MATERIAL Many of the earlier experiments [2] designed to study memory utilized the recording or deprivation approaches and their expectation was that the tasks would be involved with REM sleep. Several did not do polygraphic recording, but did compare learning following sleep versus learning following awake conditions [11, 12]. Many of these studies utilized learning tasks composed of declarative material. Historically, one of the most popular tasks has been the verbal learning task and virtually none of the studies using this type of task observed REM sleep parameter changes on the sleep nights following training. Changes in sleep parameters and memory following acquisition of declarative material: non-sleep deprivation studies Castaldo et al. [13] (Experiment 1) used both serially presented trigrams and verbal paired associates (PA; declarative memory tasks) but observed no postacquisition changes in any REM or NREM sleep parameters. Bertini and Torre [14] timed the recall of the task to occur following experimentally induced REM sleep rebound. They saw no facilitation of memory for their PA task. Yaroush et al. [15] compared the first and second half of the night of sleep using a verbal paired associates task. Some subjects learned the task at bedtime, slept 4 h (composed of a high percentage of stage 4 (NREM) and a low percentage of REM sleep) and were then awakened and retested. They showed superior memory to subjects awakened after 4 h sleep, trained and allowed another 4 h sleep (composed of a low percentage of NREM and high percentage of REM sleep) before being retested. There was also an “awake” group which was not allowed to sleep and did not do as well as the group getting large amounts of stage 4. The authors concluded that while stage 4 facilitated memory for the task, REM sleep did not. 493 Fowler et al. [16] did a very similar study. They refined the paradigm by allowing the first half (sleep during the first half of the night) subjects some sleep prior to original learning. Again they found that subjects trained on a PA task and allowed some stage 4 were superior to the awake subjects. However, the second half (sleep during the second half of the night and high levels of REM) group was also superior to the awake group on the PA task. They also used a PA task involving nonsense shapes. No group differences were found on this task. These last-mentioned studies [15, 16] confounded the time of day of learning and retesting with the sleep–wake cycle. Barrett and Ekstrand [17] controlled for time of day using the Fowler et al. [16] design. They showed that sleep composed predominantly of stage 4 was better for PA memory than REM sleep, but that when the prior sleep was composed of greater amounts of REM sleep (although not exclusively REM sleep), performance was also somewhat better than in the awake group. Benson and Feinberg [11] found that participants that had a night of sleep after learning a PA task showed superior memory to participants that stayed awake for an equal amount of time. Since no sleep recording was done, it is not possible to know which state of sleep was most beneficial. Grosvenor and Lack [12] also did a study using PA tasks. While they did not sleep record, they attempted to control for the amount of sleep between the first half and second half of the night. They had two groups that stayed awake until half the night was over and, after learning, either went to sleep (ALS) or stayed awake (ALA). Another two groups were allowed to sleep for the first half of the night and then after learning, either slept again for the second half of the night (SLS) or stayed awake (SLA). All groups learned equally well. The first recall test was 4 h later and the second 6 days later. Subjects that stayed awake prior to learning were superior at the 4 h recall to groups awakened to learn the task. Post-sleep groups remembered better than post-awake groups at the 4 h recall. However, the retest 6 days later showed no benefit of post-training sleep on the task. It was also concluded that there was no advantage of first half sleep over second half sleep when prior condition was controlled. Much more recently, Plihal and Born [18] used an early-night/late-night design to examine memory C. SMITH 494 for both declarative and procedural material. They found that subjects trained just before bed and awakened 3 h later (after predominantly stage 4 sleep) were superior to a corresponding “awake” group on the PA task, but not the procedural mirror trace task. On the other hand, subjects trained in the middle of the night, then allowed 3 h sleep (predominantly of the REM type) were superior on the procedural mirror trace task, but not the declarative PA task compared to a corresponding “awake” group when tested 3 h later. Effect of total or selective sleep deprivation on memory for declarative material A number of human studies have been done utilizing the second major paradigm to study sleep states and memory processes. As with the recording studies, REM sleep deprivation studies using verbal PA tasks reported no learning deficits as a result of selective REM sleep deprivation (REMD) following task acquisition (Castaldo et al. [13], Chernik [19], Ekstrand [20]). In a study by Ekstrand et al. [21], not only was the PA task not impaired by REMD, but performance was unaffected by stage 4 sleep deprivation as well. Lewin and Glaubman found no negative effect of overnight REMD on memory for serial verbal word lists or word clusters [22]. In more recent work by Smith [23], subjects were given a paired associate task and a complex logic task at the same training session. Groups were allowed normal sleep, were exposed to REMD, had NREM sleep interrupted or were totally sleep deprived (TSD). Retesting was done 1 week later. While all of the groups forgot a substantial amount of their PA task, there were no differences among the groups when retested 1 week later. In further studies by Smith [24] subjects were required to learn a word recognition task, a visual non-verbal figure task as well as several procedural tasks. Various groups were exposed to REMD (last two REM periods of the night), NREM interruption, TSD or normal rest. Groups did not differ on their post-training retest of the recognition and visual figure tasks 1 week later. Thus, there was no differential effect of TSD or REMD on memory for the declarative material. The results of these recording and deprivation studies provide very little support for a connection between REM sleep and simple declarative memory. SLEEP AND MEMORY FOR PROCEDURAL MATERIAL Many studies used training tasks that were either of the procedural type or at least had what appeared to be a procedural component. The tasks required participants to engage in cognitive activity that was more complex than simple recall or recognition of previously memorized material. These studies demonstrated a relationship between REM sleep and memory. Changes in recorded sleep parameters following acquisition of procedural tasks or tasks with a procedural component Lewin and Gombosh [25] observed an increase in REM sleep time in individuals exposed to tasks designed to induce curiosity, ego-involvement, emotionality, and divergent thinking prior to sleep onset. It is difficult to decide which of these tasks contributed the most toward the REM increases, since all were presented at the same time. Paul and Dittrichova [26] showed an increase in REM sleep over baseline in infants which learned a head turning response. No REM sleep increase was observed in infants that failed to learn the response. Several experiments were done examining subjects’ adaptation to a set of lenses that distorted the visual field. Two earlier studies concluded that there was no REM sleep involvement [27, 28]. The Allen et al. study [28], although it did provide a formal set of learning situations, only utilized three subjects. Further, the sampling method to estimate REM sleep density was not extensive enough to detect any changes in this measure. In the Zimmerman et al. [27] study, sleep variables were carefully examined, but results did not include any formal measures of adaptation to the prisms. A more recent and thorough study examining both sleep parameters [29] and dream content [30] found that subjects did show an increase in %REM sleep and decrease in REM density following adaptation to the inverted lenses as measured by a variety of formal learning tests. The dreams of these subjects revealed a gradient of adaptation. Significant increases in content with motor and visual problems, SLEEP STATES AND MEMORY PROCESSES IN HUMANS misfortune and dreamer confusion were observed in those subjects who had adapted the most completely. Those subjects that had not adapted so well, did not exhibit this type of mental content. Vershoor and Holdstock [31] presented a number of visual and auditory tasks to both learning and non-learning groups of undergraduate students. The visual and auditory learning groups exhibited REM sleep increases. Both the learning and non-learning visual groups also increased in REM burst percentage, while the two auditory groups (learning and non-learning) decreased their REM burst percentage. The authors concluded that excessive stimulation accounted for the increases or decreases in REM bursts while the REM sleep increases reflected learning. Mandai et al. [32] found an increase in REM sleep duration and number of REM episodes in subjects who underwent 90 min training in translation of Morse code. They also reported a high correlation between retention, number of REMs and REM density. A study by Smith and Lapp [33] assessed the effects of exams on sleep parameters. It was observed that 3–5 days after writing Christmas exams, senior undergraduate psychology students showed more eye movements and higher REM densities than they had shown in their own baselines the summer before and the summer after (when, by contrast, very little learning was occurring). They also exhibited higher values on these two measures compared to same age non-learning controls [33]. A study examining sleep parameter changes in individuals learning a second language in a total French immersion environment (DeKoninck et al. [34]) has reported a positive correlation between learning progress over the 6-week course and %REM sleep. Three subjects who did not improve their grades throughout the course did not show any %REM sleep increases. In an examination of the dream mentation [35] of these subjects, it was revealed that the students making the most progress in the course were the first to have French incorporated into their dreams and they communicated more often in French in their dreams. Scrima [36] presented narcoleptic patients with both an anagram and trigram task followed by a period of NREM sleep, REM sleep or no sleep. Subjects performed best on memory for the more complex anagram task after REM sleep and NREM sleep resulted in better performance than no sleep. 495 Performance on the simpler trigram task was not affected by either REM or NREM sleep. Buchegger et al. [37, 38] studied the effects of learning novel complex motor activity (trampolining) in novices over a period of 13 weeks and observed marked increases in REM sleep but not NREM sleep in successful participants. Control groups exerted an equal amount of energy but did not have to learn any novel motor movements (soccer, gymnastic dancing). The REM and NREM sleep of these latter groups was unchanged throughout the sessions. It was concluded that the important variable was the acquisition of the ability to correctly move the body in space, a task requiring complex and novel conceptualization. Fanjuad et al. [39] asked medical students to learn a variety of tasks including verbal and non-verbal material, a topographical map and a Chinese puzzle. Overall they observed a slight increase in REM sleep compared to the subjects own baselines. This was due to a larger amount of REM sleep in the second REM period. On retest, only performance on the topographical map task was improved, suggesting that this task was largely responsible for the observed REM sleep changes. Stickgold et al. [40] trained subjects on a visual search task. They found that improvement on the search task correlated highly with both the amount of NREM sleep in the first quadrant and REM sleep in the fourth quadrant of an 8 h night of sleep. The combined correlations for the two states of sleep accounted for 80% of the intersubject variance. REM sleep during the first 6 h was not related to task improvement. Interestingly, the study provides some evidence for memory processing during a specific time period during the night. There is substantial evidence for REM “windows” in animal studies, where REM sleep episodes at certain times after the end of training are the important times for memory consolidation to occur. REM at times outside of these windows is not apparently necessary for consolidation (see Smith [24, 41, 42] for reviews). The study by Stickgold et al. [40] is one of the two to suggest the existence of human REM windows within a single night of sleep. The study by Smith and Lapp [33], reviewed earlier, also suggested the existence of a REM window at the end of the night of sleep. This change in REM sleep manifested in an increased number of REMs in the fifth REM period as well an increased REM density in the fourth REM period 3–5 days following college examinations. 496 Effect of total or selective sleep deprivation on memory for tasks with a procedural component In a study by Smith [23] utilizing both a PA and a complex logic (procedural) task, groups experienced REMD, NREM awakening, TSD or were allowed normal sleep. Memory for the logic task was severely impaired in the REMD and TSD groups, strongly implicating REM sleep as important for the memory of procedural material. In a second experiment [23], subjects learned the complex logic task. Following this, one group experienced REMD for the first two REM periods of the night and then was allowed to sleep normally. A second group experienced REMD during the last two REM periods of the night after having slept normally for the first half of the night. Compared to a third, non-REMD group, both of the REMD groups were equally impaired on memory for the logic task when retested 1 week later. Thus REM during the first or last part of the night was equally valuable for memory efficiency. In yet another study [23], evaluating only the effect of TSD, it was observed that TSD was capable of inducing this same level of memory loss. Surprisingly, TSD was able to induce memory loss not only if experienced on the night of task acquisition, but also if experienced 2 nights (48–72 h) later. On the other hand, TSD one night after task acquisition or 3 nights after task acquisition (24–48 h or 72–96 h later) had no effect on memory for the logic task. These results demonstrate the vulnerability of some kinds of memory long after (2 days) the end of training. Empson and Clarke [43] showed that REMD after serial word list acquisition did not produce any memory deficits while these same subjects exhibited memory loss for more difficult material including meaningless sentences and anomalous prose passages. NREM-deprived controls got significantly less stage 4 than did REMD subjects, but were still superior on these latter tasks. In a study by Tilley and Empson [44], subjects were tested for retention of a story by means of free recall following either REMD or stage 4 deprivation. REMD subjects showed poorer retention for this material. In another report [45], they examined two sets of PA and two stories. While the PA tasks were unaffected, the most impaired group on story recall was the group with no REM sleep at all. Interestingly, a few subjects C. SMITH were inadvertently allowed a small amount of REM sleep. These subjects had much better story recall than those getting no REM sleep, suggesting that even partial REM sleep within a REM period may be important for memory consolidation. Karni et al. [46] had subjects learn a visual detection task. Participants showed improvement on this task after a normal night of sleep. REM sleep deprivation prevented improvement in the task, while NREM awakenings did not prevent this improvement from taking place. A variety of tasks [24] were presented to college students including word recognition, word fragment completion, visual memory, Corsi block tapping and Tower of Hanoi. Following acquisition of these tasks, the REMD group was deprived of the last two REM periods of the night. Control groups included a NREMD group, a TSD group, a normally rested sleep-recorded group and a normally rested home control group. Retest took place 1 week later. It was observed that the REMD and TSD groups were not impaired on the declarative word recognition or visual memorization task, but were impaired on the procedural word fragment completion, Corsi block tapping and Tower of Hanoi tasks compared to the other three experimental groups [24]. A few results do not apparently fit cleanly into either the declarative/procedural category [43, 44, 47]. For example, consistent with the theory, Empson and Clarke [43] reported no deficits in memory for a serial word list following REM sleep deprivation. However, these same experimenters also presented meaningless sentences and anomalous prose, material that would have to be considered declarative. Post-training REM sleep deprivation for these two tasks resulted in memory deficits. Likewise, Tilley and Empson [44, 47], consistent with the declarative/procedural dichotomy and REM sleep, showed that PA tasks were not vulnerable to REM sleep deprivation. On the other hand, they reported in the same studies that remembering the essence of stories was impaired by REM sleep loss. Thus, while simple declarative material (word lists, paired associates) was “immune” to REM sleep deprivation, memory for the more complex declarative material (anomalous prose, meaningless sentences, stories) was impaired by REM sleep deprivation in three studies [43, 44, 47]. One explanation might simply be the level of SLEEP STATES AND MEMORY PROCESSES IN HUMANS difficulty of the declarative material. Perhaps the PA tasks were too easily memorized and thus not vulnerable to sleep loss while the more complex material was not. This seems somewhat unlikely, since several studies presented PA tasks which were difficult enough that they were never completely learned [23, 24], yet, subsequent REM sleep deprivation and total sleep deprivation had no effect on memory for these tasks. A more likely possibility is that of familiarity of storage and/or response strategies. College students comprised the great majority of subjects in these studies and their familiarity with the words presented and their ability to give the simple response is based on many years of similar school activity. It would be reasonable to assume that these students have long since developed a strategy to memorize and to retrieve this type of material. On the other hand, perhaps they had less welldeveloped strategies to deal with the anomalous prose, meaningless sentences and stories. The idea that cognitive storage and/or retrieval strategies are helpful for remembering complex unfamiliar declarative material is well established. An obvious example is “chunking” or the memorizing of long strings of numbers by storing them in groups or chunks of three to four numbers. It seems reasonable that many real-life learning situations have both a declarative and a procedural component. For example, the words to a new language might be considered declarative material, but the use of these words would be procedural. Most students do not spend many hours per year memorizing meaningless sentences or anomalous prose. They would probably have greater trouble memorizing and/or retrieving this material than simple words and this suggests the possibility that they would have to develop new memory storage and/or retrieval strategies. While it could be argued that students would likely have had a more welldeveloped strategy for learning stories, in the studies mentioned [44, 47], the demand characteristics of the response were changed (subjects were asked for the essence of the stories rather than verbatim story recall ), possibly requiring new retrieval strategies. Thus subjects might have had to develop new strategies to handle the storage and/or retrieval of this less familiar material. REM sleep could be involved in the formation of these new cognitive procedural storage/retrieval strategies. While it is 497 not possible to decide this question based on the available data, the idea is quite testable and further, carefully designed studies can shed more light on this problem. The overview of the above recording and deprivation studies indicates that those studies using training tasks which were clearly of a declarative nature did not require REM sleep following acquisition for increased memory efficiency. On the other hand, those tasks that were clearly procedural or had a procedural component appeared to require REM sleep for maximum learning efficiency. INFLUENCING MEMORY PROCESSING DURING SLEEP BY CUEING It has been reported in animal studies by Hennevin and Hars [48, 49] that when an ear shock was used as the conditioned stimulus (CS) in a two-way shuttle shock avoidance task, enhanced memory for the task was induced by presenting sub-awakening threshold CS pulses to the rats during post-training REM sleep. These rats showed superior performance to control animals. It was concluded that the CS pulses acted as a “reminder” to the brain to process further the recently learned CS–UCS (unconditioned stimulus of foot shock) relationship. In humans [50], individuals learned to decipher Morse signals. During subsequent REM sleep one group was randomly presented with auditory “clicks”, such that most occurred when there were no actual rapid eye movements occurring. A second group received clicks that coincided with the actual rapid eye movements of their REM sleep episodes. Compared to a non-stimulated (“non-clicked”) group, enhanced memory for the group receiving REM-coincident clicks was reported. Smith and Weeden [51] had subjects learn a complex logic task either in the presence or absence of a clicking sound. There were four groups: 1) clicks during learning followed by REM-coincident clicks during subsequent REM sleep (clicks delivered to the ear at the maximum deflection of the horizontal eye movement); 2) no clicks during learning followed by REM-coincident clicks during subsequent REM sleep; 3) clicks during learning followed by clicks delivered during tonic REM sleep (quiet REM sleep with no REMs); 4) no clicks during learning and no clicks during the night. The group 498 C. SMITH getting clicks during training and then clicks coincident with the REMs of REM sleep were 23% better than all of the other groups. These studies indicate that REM sleep is related not only to procedural task memory but add to the possibility that the phasic component of REM sleep is an important component of memory processing. to sleep and memory studies and provides fertile ground for further research. FUNCTIONAL IMAGING STUDIES There are many animal and human studies that have examined the role of the medial temporal region, especially the hippocampus in memory processing. Although the functions of the many structures which provide input and output to this structure are not yet completely understood, it is becoming increasingly clear that the hippocampus is essential for normal declarative memory [9, 55–61]. From the analysis of the human sleep studies done to this point, it does not seem that REM sleep is involved with this type of learning. The data supporting the idea that NREM sleep is involved with declarative memory is mixed and more data will be required before this idea is confirmed. That the hippocampus is not involved with sleep states (especially REM) and memory is apparently at odds with recent data from animal studies [62–64]. However, understanding the role of the hippocampus and sleep in humans must await further research. While REM sleep appears to be involved with a variety of procedural tasks, the number of human studies attempting to pinpoint structures related to procedural memory are not nearly as numerous as for declarative memory. One group [61] demonstrated in normal humans using PET scans, that the visual association cortex was involved in a visual priming task, while the hippocampus was not. Another group [65, 66], examining patients with Parkinson’s disease, have evidence to suggest that the neostriatum and prefrontal cortex are involved with cognitive procedural, but not declarative memory. Maquet et al. [52] showed that a number of structures, including the extrastriate, left premotor, superior parietal, left primary sensorimotor, supplementary motor, precuneus, anterior cingulate and cerebellum were involved in acquisition of a procedural SRT task. Thus, while the brain structures involved with procedural memory appear to vary with type of task being presented, none of the tasks appear to involve the hippocampus. Memory researchers are now in the process of trying to integrate brain structures with different types of memory systems. To date, only one imaging study has been published which directly examined the effects of learning a task and the sleep patterns of the learner. Maquet et al. [52] presented two groups of subjects with a serial reaction time (SRT) task. One group had PET scans taken during the task acquisition, the second group had PET scans taken while they were in quiet waking, NREM and REM sleep. A third group was not trained but was scanned as was the second group in various stages of sleep. Those brain areas which were significantly more active during REM sleep of trained subjects compared to the nontrained subjects, and involved in the execution of the task during waking included the extrastriate cortex (bilaterally), the left premotor cortex (all subjects were right handed) and the mesencephalon. The results strongly suggest that further processing of the task was occurring during REM sleep. There was no such comparable activity in quiet waking, stage 2 or stage 4 sleep. IS THE NREM–REM CYCLE IMPORTANT? There are few studies that have adequately controlled for or reported lengthening or shortening of the NREM–REM sleep cycle length. Two recent reports suggest that it may be an important factor. Mazzoni et al. [53] presented word pairs to elderly subjects, and after an intervening night of sleep, tested their cued recall. The level of recall was positively correlated with the average duration of the NREM–REM sleep cycles. This same laboratory [54], in a more detailed study, again using word pairs as their memory task, demonstrated that it was disturbed sleep cycle activity rather than just loss of either NREM or REM sleep that contributed to declarative memory loss. While more data are required, these studies point to a manipulation that has not been systematically explored with respect HOW DO THE RESULTS FROM HUMAN SLEEP STUDIES RELATE TO BRAIN STRUCTURES? SLEEP STATES AND MEMORY PROCESSES IN HUMANS When these relationships are more clearly understood, we may be able to understand how they interact with sleep state mechanisms. BRAIN PATHOLOGY INDUCED SLEEP LOSS AND MEMORY A number of authors [67–69] have examined patients with bilateral pontine lesions who were completely paralyzed (“locked-in” syndrome) and were almost completely lacking in REM sleep. It might be expected that such individuals would show memory impairments. However, the assessment of memory was not very sophisticated in these studies. The authors noted that these individuals demonstrated memory for events and people (declarative material) [67–69]. In a few cases, patients with bilateral pontine damage who were conscious, mobile and verbally communicative have been observed [69–72]. For example, Lavie et al. [71] reported recording from a male with a pontine lesion. He showed virtually no REM sleep (approximately 2%), but was otherwise normal. He recovered from his wound and lived a fairly normal life, becoming a lawyer. Perret et al. [72] studied the sleep of a male adult with extensive nigro-striatal damage. He showed no sign of REM or stage 4 sleep. He was presented with an array of tasks and it was concluded that there was no memory loss despite this abnormal situation. A close examination of the tasks used in these studies indicates that they were all of a declarative nature. While these reports do indicate that the individuals have coped well with their problems and appear to lead normal lives, they do not necessarily demonstrate memory normality. It is more likely that the rate of learning REM sensitive material is slower than for normal individuals, although still able to occur at this somewhat slower, less efficient rate. This is certainly the case in REM sleep deprivation studies [23, 24, 43–46, 73]. Material not sensitive to REM sleep would be unaffected. These reports simply do not address such a possibility. MEMORY IN PATIENTS TAKING REM-SUPPRESSING DRUGS It is widely known that virtually all major antidepressant drugs, including the monoamine oxidase 499 inhibitors (MAOIs) [74], the tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs) [75] and the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) [76] profoundly reduce the amount of REM sleep. The REM-suppressing effect of these drugs may be the reason for their success [77, 78]. It might be expected that individuals taking such drugs would be at a disadvantage in terms of their efficiency in learning cognitive procedural material and there have been reports of some of these drugs inducing memory loss. The most potent substance is the drug amitriptyline [79, 80], although the memory loss observed after this drug is apparently due to its strong anticholinergic effects rather than to its REM sleep suppressing properties. Another drug reported to induce memory loss is trazodone, a TCA that reportedly has a sedating effect. However, the many antidepressant REM-suppressing drugs without either of these properties have not been associated with memory loss [79–81]. While there have been many reports that cognitive function is not impaired as a result of REM-suppressing drugs, the tests were mostly of the declarative type – (MAOIs) [82–84], (TCAs) [81, 85–88] and SSRIs [80, 81, 86, 89]. Furthermore, the design of the studies was invariably to assess individuals at various times after drug administration. There were no tests which re-examined patients on material learned several days previously (after intervening sleep). Thus, it is not surprising that authors found their patients to be living relatively normal lives. These individuals would behave quite normally in terms of remembering names, places and facts required of any declarative task. Further, they would also be able to learn cognitive procedural material. They simply would not be expected to learn this kind of material as efficiently as would an individual getting a normal amount of REM sleep each night. The difference would only become obvious over the long term of days and weeks. THE CASE FOR REM SLEEP AND MEMORY FOR PROCEDURAL BUT NOT DECLARATIVE MATERIAL The examination of studies using declarative and/ or procedural tasks suggests that REM sleep is not involved with consolidation of declarative material. Six of the seven non-deprivation studies [12–16, 18] indicated no specific REM sleep effect. The 500 seventh study [16] reported that the group receiving the last half of the night of sleep (high in REM and low in NREM or stage 4) performed better than subjects who had stayed awake. On the other hand, 14 of 16 non-deprivation studies that utilized tasks of a procedural nature or tasks that had a procedural component [25, 26, 29–40] all reported increases in some component of REM sleep. Some of the studies reported increases in the number of minutes of REM sleep or %REM sleep, while others observed increases in number of actual REMs and REM densities. The two studies that did not report REM sleep changes were both studies in which the subjects were exposed to visual distortion using special prisms [27, 28]. As mentioned above, both had some methodological problems, although a third, more recent study did report REM sleep changes as a result of this manipulation [29]. Two of the non-deprivation recording studies examined dream content as well [30, 35] and the dream content reflected learning progress. For the REMD studies, all seven experiments using declarative material reported lack of effect on memory [13, 19–21, 23, 24]. By contrast, seven REMD studies reported impairment of memory for procedural material [23] (two experiments), [24, 43–46]. The brain imaging study [52] also supports the idea of REM sleep being involved with procedural memory. Results of the two cueing studies [50, 51] indicated not only that REM sleep is important for procedural memory, but that the actual phasic component of REM sleep is an important indicator and possible mechanism for memory processing. While much remains to be done, there is clearly a strong relationship between procedural memory and REM sleep. It is probably premature to decide which component of REM sleep is most clearly involved with memory processing. In fact, the facet of REM sleep that changes following learning may depend on a number of factors, of which the most obvious is type of task. By contrast, there would appear to be no relationship between memory of simple declarative material and REM sleep. THE CASE FOR NREM SLEEP AND MEMORY Several recording (non-deprivation) studies have suggested a relationship between NREM sleep or C. SMITH stage 3/4 and memory for declarative tasks [16–18, 53]. On the other hand, six studies [13, 19–21, 23, 24] found no effect of NREM deprivation on memory for declarative tasks. For procedural tasks, Scrima [36] reported that NREM sleep was better than staying awake for memory of difficult trigrams. Stickgold et al. [40] have shown that the amount of NREM in the first quarter of the night is highly correlated with the efficient learning of a visual detection task. However, Karni et al. [46] have reported, using the same visual detection task, that NREM deprivation did not impair memory. These results seem contradictory. If amount of NREM sleep is highly correlated with the ability to do this task, it seems logical that NREM deprivation would induce memory deficits but it does not appear to do so. This paradoxical situation can be seen in other studies as well. If NREM sleep is important for memory processing, it might be expected that subjects exposed to TSD (where both REM and NREM sleep are deprived) would exhibit even worse performance than selectively REM deprived subjects. That is, one might expect that the NREM sleep deprivation would provide a component of the deficit. In two studies that examined both declarative and procedural material, this was not the case. In one study [23], individuals learned a complex logic task. For the logic task, the groups exposed to TSD and to REMD showed inferior memory to the NREM awakened and normal sleep groups and the last two groups did not differ. Thus, not only was the NREM awakened group not different from normal controls, the TSD group, exposed to both REM and NREM sleep deprivation, did not exhibit a worse performance than participants exposed to REMD alone. In another study [24], subjects were asked to learn a variety of declarative and procedural tasks. Groups included normally rested controls, REMD (last two REM periods of the night), NREM awakening and TSD. Again, testing occurred 1 week later. The three procedural tasks were poorly remembered by the REMD and TSD subjects compared to the other three groups. Yet they were not different from each other, even though TSD included deprivation of NREM as well as REM sleep. As previously noted, the PA and word recognition tasks were not impaired by TSD (which includes NREM deprivation) or by the NREM awakenings compared to the normally rested controls. SLEEP STATES AND MEMORY PROCESSES IN HUMANS Stickgold et al. [40] have proposed a two-stage process based on the visual search task which requires NREM sleep in the first quadrant and REM sleep in the last quadrant of the night for learning efficiency to be maximized. It seems likely that some factor that accompanies NREM sleep but which is not disturbed by NREM deprivation, such as levels of glucocorticoids, is at work Plihal et al. [90]. The NREM data do not indicate clear differences according to a declarative–procedural dichotomy as is found with REM studies. One important variable seems to be the retest time. Retesting done shortly (24 h or less) after training in non-deprivation studies [16–18, 53] provides data suggesting a positive relationship between NREM sleep and memory while studies with retest several days to 1 week later do not [12, 23, 24]. Thus a role for NREM sleep in memory processing is possible, but much more research needs to be done before that role is understood. Interestingly, there is evidence for stage 2 being involved in the consolidation of a motor procedural task. Smith and MacNeill [91] have shown that memory for a pursuit rotor task 1 week after initial acquisition can be impaired by either stage 2 deprivation or interruption. In very recent work [73], it has been found that while a motor task requiring a complex cognitive adjustment (mirror trace task) is sensitive to REMD, direct tracing (simple motor procedural) task is sensitive to stage 2 sleep interruption and not to REMD. Although more work must be done, it appears that there is a different mode of processing for cognitive motor procedural than for simple motor procedural tasks that may involve different sleep stages. 501 types of memory). Many of the concepts now being used by memory theorists have not been examined using sleep parameters as variables. As an example, Tulving and Schacter [92] have argued for some years that priming requires a separate memory system, yet only one study has ever examined this type of task in connection with sleep deprivation [24]. As the nature of new memory systems becomes clear, their interaction with sleep mechanisms can be more precisely studied. Experiments on memory systems using sleep as a variable will undoubtedly add to the understanding of these systems. There are already preliminary data (as mentioned above) that procedural motor memory may not be a unitary system but can be dissociated depending on the amount of cognitive involvement [91]. Future sleep–memory research in humans should focus on learning tasks that are considered by memory theorists to utilize functionally distinct systems. Then, using the combined techniques of sleep recording or sleep deprivation following task acquisition, it will be possible to decide if the sleep state is necessary for memory or simply correlated with memory processing. As well as sleep recording and sleep deprivation, the use of other techniques, such as the PET scan, will provide new insights into the nature of the sleep–memory relationship. The study of sleep states in conjunction with memory processes will allow us to understand further some of the functions of sleep. On the other hand, the comprehension of memory systems will not be complete until its relationship to the states of sleep is understood. Practice Points FUTURE DIRECTIONS In this paper the assumption has been made that all of the learning tasks utilized in the sleep–memory studies were either declarative (and likely explicit) or procedural (and probably implicit) in nature. In future, as we learn more about memory systems, these assumptions will undoubtedly turn out to be too general. In particular, the term procedural may not be broad enough to include all the types of learning that are not declarative (many authors prefer the term “non-declarative” to describe these 1. Adult patients attempting to do a lot of learning should be warned that the rate at which they learn procedural material will be reduced if they are not sleeping well or if their sleep onset times are frequently changed (e.g. shiftwork). 2. Children often do not sleep long enough or suffer from poor sleep for various reasons. Parents should be warned that progress in school, sports and other activities such as music may suffer as a result. Improving sleep quality will improve learning ability. C. SMITH 502 3. It may be necessary to give patients drugs such as MAOIs, TCAs or SSRIs which are known to suppress REM sleep. The patient should be informed of this REM sleep suppressing effect and how it will likely impact on them. They should realize that the rate at which they can acquire complex cognitive procedural material will be reduced. 4. Patients attempting to refine fine motor movements (sports, musical instruments, etc.) should be made aware that stage 2 loss or interruption can lead to slower progress. If the patient suffers many awakenings in the night or must get up at such times as to miss the last hour or so of sleep, progress in learning skills will be reduced and more training will be required. 5. 6. 7. Research Agenda 1. The role of partial REM sleep deprivation following acquisition of procedural material should be examined more thoroughly. Are there REM windows? 2. Memory for procedural tasks may be vulnerable to REM sleep loss several days after the end of acquisition, not just the same day as acquisition. Thus, real world tasks like learning a second language would be remembered best when a good sleep night follows the day of acquisition and also follows 2 days after the day of acquisition. 3. The degree to which school children learn procedural material should be examined more closely. Some topics, like mathematics and physics would have high cognitive procedural content. Second language learning also has an important procedural component. Timing of presentation of this material with respect to expected subsequent sleep could result in increased learning efficiency. For example, offering math on Friday, when it is likely that many young children will stay up beyond normal bedtimes, should be avoided. 4. The study of the REM dreams of subjects who have recently learned procedural material may lead to a better understanding of 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. sleep–memory systems and possibly dream formation mechanisms. Cueing is a novel new method of examining the sleep-memory relationship and many questions remain, including: 1) how many cues have to be presented? 2) Why is the time of presentation at the REM deflection so important? 3) Is one particular time of the night important? This research paradigm could provide interesting information about the nature of the REM sleep–memory relationship. Although it is expensive and time consuming, PET scan technology, combined with sleep recording could provide invaluable information about the neural mechanisms of the sleep–memory relationship. Manipulation of the NREM–REM cycle may provide us with information about the relationship between sleep parameters and declarative memory and should be further explored. An adequate study, including a set of procedural tasks with retesting at least several days after acquisition has yet to be done comparing patients on REM-suppressing medication with normal matched controls. Cognitive procedural tasks (not motor procedural) should be used to examine the relationship between REM sleep and memory. The nature of the phasic component of REM sleep (REM sleep densities and number of REMs) and its relationship to cognitive procedural memory is not clear and should be systematically examined Stage 2 sleep loss may well limit the rate at which people learn and refine motor skills. This possibility should be examined in real life situations. This could be particularly important to pre-school and grade-school children, since they are particularly busy with this type of learning. Retesting of individuals should take place at least several days after the end of acquisition and/or sleep deprivation in order to see that these effects are of such an enduring nature that they can be generalized to practical situations SLEEP STATES AND MEMORY PROCESSES IN HUMANS 13. Virtually no systematic studies have examined the role of REM sleep displacement such as occurs in individuals involved in shift work. Attempts to train individuals on procedural tasks while they are exposed to changing sleep times may be a very inefficient method of teaching. If so, such groups as nurses, interns, police and pilots should not be asked to learn crucial cognitive procedural material while doing shift work. 503 15. 16. 17. 18. REFERENCES 1. Jenkins JG, Dallenbach KM. Oblivescence during sleep and waking. Amer J Psychol 1924; 35: 605–612. 2. McGrath MJ, Cohen DB. REM sleep facilitation of adaptive waking behavior: a review of the literature. Psych Bull 1978; 85: 24–57. 3. Graham RB. Physiological Psychology. 1 ed. Belmont: Wadsworth, 1990. 4. McGaugh HL, Herz MJ. Memory Consolidation. San Francisco: Albion Publication Co., 1972. 5. Best JB. Cognitive Psychology. 4 ed. St. Paul, MN: West Publishing Company, 1995. 6. Bloch V, Hennevin E, Leconte P. Relationship between paradoxical sleep and memory processes. In: Brazier MAB, (ed.): Brain Mechanisms in Memory and Learning: From the Single Neuron to Man. New York: Raven Press, 1979: 329–343. 7. Pearlman C. REM sleep and information processing: evidence from animal studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1979; 3: 57–68. 8. Squire LR, Slater PC, Chace PM. Retrograde amnesia: Temporal gradient in very long term memory following electroconvulsive therapy. Science 1975; 187: 77–79. 9. Squire LR. Mechanisms of memory. Science 1986; 232: 1612–1619. 10. Squire RL, Zola-Morgan S. Memory: Brain systems and behavior. Trends in Neurosci 1988; 11: 170–175. 11. Benson K, Feinberg I. The beneficial effect of sleep in an extended Jenkins and Dallenbach paradigm. Psychophysiol 1977; 14: 375–384. 12. Grosvenor A, Lack LC. The effect of sleep before or after learning on memory. Sleep 1984; 7: 155–167. 13. Castaldo V, Krynicki V, Goldstein J. Sleep stages and verbal memory. Percept Mot Skills 1974; 39: 1023–1030. 14. Bertini M, Torre A. REM sleep and memory consolidation. In: Levin P, Koella WP, (eds.): Sleep: 19. 20. 21. 22. ∗23. ∗24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. ∗ The most important references are denoted by an asterisk. Physiology, biochemistry, psychology, pharmacology, clinical implications. Basel, Switzerland: Karger, 1973; 453–457. Yaroush R, Sullivan MJ, Ekstrand BR. Effect of sleep on memory. II: Differential effect of first and second half of the night. J Exp Psych 1971; 88: 361–366. Fowler MJ, Sullivan MJ, Ekstrand BR. Sleep and memory. Science 1973; 179: 302–304. Barrett TR, Ekstrand BR. Effect of sleep on memory. III. Controlling for time of day effects. J Exp Psych 1972; 96: 321–327. Plihal W, Born J. Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. J Cog Neurosci 1997; 9: 534–547. Chernik DA. Effect of REM sleep deprivation on learning and recall by humans. Percept Mot Skills 1972; 34: 283–294. Ekstrand BR. To sleep, perchance to dream (about why we forget). In: Duncan CP, Sechrest L, (eds.): Human memory: Festschrift in honor of Benton J. Underwood. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972: 59–82. Ekstrand BR, Sullivan MJ, Parker DF, West JN. Spontaneous recovery and sleep. J Exp Psych 1971; 88: 142–144. Lewin I, Glaubman H. The effect of REM deprivation: Is it detrimental, beneficial or neutral? Psychophysiol 1975; 12: 349–353. Smith C. REM sleep and learning: Some recent findings. In: Moffitt A, Kramer M, Hoffmann R, (eds.): The Functions of Dreaming. New York: SUNY Press, 1993: 341–361. Smith C. Sleep states and memory processes. Behav Brain Res 1995; 69: 137–145. Lewin I, Gombosh D. Increase in REM time as a function of the need for divergent thinking. In: Koella WP, Levin P, (eds.): Sleep: Physiology, Biochemistry, Psychology, Pharmacology, Clinical Implications. 1st European Congress of Sleep Research. Basel: Karger, 1973: 399–403. Paul K, Dittrichova J. Sleep patterns following learning in infants. In: Levin P, Koella WP, (eds.): Sleep 1974. Basel, Switzerland: Karger, 1975; 388–390. Zimmerman JT, Stoyva JM, Reite ML. Spatially rearranged vision and REM sleep: A lack of effect. Biol Psychiat 1978; 13: 301–316. Allen SR, Oswald I, Lewis S, Tagney J. The effects of distorted visual input on sleep. Psychophysiol 1972; 9: 498–504. DeKoninck J, Prevost F. Le sommeil paradoxal et le traitment de l’information: une exploration par l’inversion du champ visuel. Can J Psych 1991; 45: 125–139. De Koninck J, Prevost F, Lortie-Lussier M. Vertical inversion of the visual field and REM sleep mentation. J Sleep Res 1996; 5: 16–20. 504 31. Verschoor GJ, Hodstock TL. REM bursts and REM sleep following visual and auditory learning. South African J Psych 1984; 14: 69–74. 32. Mandai O, Guerrien A, Sockeel P, Dujardin K, Leconte P. Rem sleep modifications following a morse code learning session in humans. Physiol Behav 1989; 46: 639–642. ∗33. Smith C, Lapp L. Increases in number of REMs and REM density in humans following an intensive learning period. Sleep 1991; 14: 325–330. ∗34. DeKoninck J, Lorrain D, Christ G, Proulx G, Coulombe D. Intensive language learning and increases in rapid eye movement sleep: evidence of a performance factor. Internat J Psychophysiol 1989; 8: 43–47. 35. DeKoninck J, Christ G, Hebert G, Rinfret N. Language learning efficiency, dreams and REM sleep. Psychiatr J Univ Ottawa 1990; 15: 91–92. 36. Scrima L. Isolated REM sleep facilitates recall of complex associative information. Psychophysiol 1982; 19 (3): 252–258. 37. Buchegger J, Meier-Koll A. Motor learning and ultradian sleep cycle: an electroencephalographic study of trampoliners. Percept Mot Skills 1988; 67: 635–645. 38. Buchegger J, Fritsch R, Meier-Koll A, Riehle H. Does trampolining and anaerobic, physical fitness affect sleep? Percept Mot Skills 1991; 73: 243–252. 39. Fanjaud G, Calvet U, De Feneyrols RA, Barrere M, Bes A, Arbus L. Role du sommeil paradoxal dans l’apprentissage chez l’homme. Rev EEG Neurophysiol 1982; 12: 337–343. ∗40. Stickgold R, Whidbee D, Schirmer B, Patel V, Hobson JA. Visual discrimination task improvement: a multistep process occurring during sleep. J Cog Neurosci 2000; 12: 246–254. 41. Smith CT. Sleep states and learning: a review of the animal literature. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1985; 9: 157–168. ∗42. Smith C. Sleep states, memory processes and synaptic plasticity. Behavioural Brain Research 1996; 78: 49–56. 43. Empson JAC, Clarke PRF. Rapid eye movements and remembering. Nature 1970; 227: 287–288. 44. Tilley AJ, Empson JAC. REM sleep and memory consolidation. Biol Psych 1978; 6: 293–300. 45. Empson JAC, Hearne KMT, Tilley AJ. REM sleep and remeniscence. In: Koella WP, (ed.): Sleep 1980. Basel: Karger, 1981: 364–366. 46. Karni A, Tanne D, Rubenstein BS, Askenasy JJM, Sagi D. Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science 1994; 265: 679–682. 47. O’Keefe J, Nadel L. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. New York: Oxford University Press, 1978. C. SMITH 48. Hars B, Hennevin E, Pasques P. Improvement of learning by cueing during postlearning paradoxical sleep. Behav Brain Res 1985; 18: 241–250. ∗49. Hennevin E, Hars B. Is increase in post-learning paradoxical sleep modified by cueing? Behav Brain Res 1987; 24: 243–249. 50. Guerrien A, Dujardin K, Mandai O, Sockeel P, Leconte P. Enhancement of memory by auditory stimulation during postlearning REM sleep in humans. Physiol Behav 1989; 45: 947–950. 51. Smith C, Weeden K. Post training REMs coincident auditory stimulation enhances memory in humans. Psychiatr J Univ Ottawa 1990; 15: 85–90. ∗52. Maquet P, Laureys S, Peigneux P, Fuchs S, Petiau C, Phillips C, Aerts J, Delfiore G, Degueldre C, Meulemans T, Luxen A, Franck G, Van Der Linden M, Smith C, Cleeremans A. Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep. Nat Neurosci 2000; 3: 831–836. 53. Mazzoni G, Gori S, Formicola G, Gneri C, Massetani R, Murri L, Salzarulo P. Word recall correlates with sleep cycles in elderly subjects. J Sleep Res 1999; 8: 185–188. 54. Ficca G, Lombardo P, Rossi L, Salzarulo P. Morning recall of verbal material depends on prior sleep organization. Behav Brain Res 2000; 112: 159–163. 55. Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. 3 ed. New York: W.H. Freeman, 1990. 56. Squire LR. The neuropsychology of human memory. Ann Rev Neurosci 1982; 5: 241–273. 57. Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Amaral DG. Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: Enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 1986; 6: 2950–2967. 58. Zola-Morgan S, Squire LR, Mishkin M. The neuroanatomy of amnesia amygdala-hippocampus versus temporal stem. Science 1982; 218: 1337–1339. 59. Squire LR, Cohen NJ, Zouzounis JA. Preserved memory in retrograde amnesia: sparing of a recently acquired skill. Neuropsychologia 1984; 22: 145–152. 60. Kartsounis LD, Rudge P, Stevens JM. Bilateral lesions of CA1 and CA2 fields of the hippocampus are sufficient to cause a severe amnesic syndrome in humans. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat 1995; 59: 95–98. 61. Squire LR, Ojemann JG, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, Videen TO, Raichle ME. Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: A functional anatomical study of memory. Proc Nat Acad Sci 1992; 89: 1837–1841. 62. Smith C, Rose G. Posttrainng paradoxical sleep in rats is increased after spatial learning in the Morris Water Maze. Behav Neurosci 1997; 111: 1197–1204. SLEEP STATES AND MEMORY PROCESSES IN HUMANS 63. Poe GR, Nitz DA, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA. Experience-dependent phase reversal of hippocampal neuron firing during REM sleep. Brain Res 2000; 855: 176–180. 64. Louie K, Wilson M. Temporally structured REM sleep replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity. Neuron 2001; 29: 145–156. 65. Saint-Cyr JA, Taylor AE, Lang AE. Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunctioning in man. Brain 1988; 111: 941–959. 66. Saint-Cyr JA, Taylor AE. The mobilization of procedural learning: the “key signature” of the basal ganglia. In: Squire LR, Butters N, (eds.): Neuropsychology of Memory. New York: The Guilford Press, 1993: 188–202. 67. Chase TN, Moretti L, Prensky AL. Clinical and electroencephalographic manifestations of vascular lesions of the pons. Neurol 1968; 18: 357–368. 68. Markand ON, Dyken ML. Sleep abnormalities in patients with brain stem lesions. Neurol 1976; 26: 769–776. 69. Cummings JL, Greenberg R. Sleep patterns in the “locked in” syndrome. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol 1977; 43: 270–271. 70. Osorio I, Daroff RB. Absence of REM and altered NREM sleep in patients with spinocerebellar degeneration and slow saccades. Ann Neurol 1980; 7: 277–280. 71. Lavie P, Pratt H, Scharf B, Peled R, Brown J. Localized pontine lesion: Nearly total absence of REM sleep. Neurol 1984; 34: 118–120. 72. Perret JL, Tapissier J, Jouvet M. Insomnie et memoire. A propos d’une observation de degenerescence striato-nigrique. Electroenceph clin Neurophysiol 1979; 47: 499–502. 73. Aubrey J, Smith C, Tweed S, Nader R. Cognitive and motor procedural tasks are dissociated in REM and Stage 2 sleep. Sleep Res Online 1999; 2 (Suppl 1): 220. 74. Wyatt RJ, Fram DH, Kupfer DJ, Snyder F. Total prolonged drug-induced REM sleep suppression in anxious-depressed patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1971; 24: 145–155. 75. Thase ME. Depression, sleep and antidepressants. J Clin Psychiatry 1998; 59: 55–65. 76. Sharpley AL, Williamson DJ, Attenburrow MEJ, Pearson G, Sargent P, Cowen PJ. The effects of paroxetine and nefazodone on sleep: a placebo controlled study. Psychopharmacology 1996; 126: 50–54. 77. Vogel GW. A review of REM sleep deprivation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1975; 32: 749–761. 78. Vogel GW. Evidence for REM sleep deprivation as the mechanism of action of antidepressant drugs. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiat 1983; 7: 343–349. 505 79. Lamping DL, Spring B, Gelenberg AJ. Effects of two antidepressants on memory performance in depressed patients. Psychopharmacology 1984; 84: 254–261. 80. Spring B, Gelenberg AJ, Garvin R, Thompson S. Amitriptyline, clovoxamine, and cognitive function: a placebo-controlled comparison in depressed patients. Psychopharmacology 1992; 108: 327–332. 81. Thompson PJ. Antidepressnats and memory: a review. Human Pharm 1991; 6: 79–90. 82. Raskin A, Friedman AS, DiMascio A. Effects of chlopromazine, imipramine, diazepam and phenelzine on psychomotor and cognitive, skills of depressed patients. Psychopharmacol Bull 1983; 19: 649–652. 83. Georgotas A, Reisbeg B, Ferris S. First results on the effects of MAO inhibition on cognitive functioning in elderly depressed patients. Arch Gerontol Geriat 1983; 2: 249–254. 84. Georgotas A, McCue RE, Reisberg B, Ferris SH, Nagachandran N, Chang I, Mir P. The effects of mood changes and antidepressants on the cognitive capacity of elderly depressed patients. Internat Psychogeriat 1989; 1: 135–143. 85. Knegtering H, Eijck M, Huijsman A. Effects of antidepressants on cognitive functioning of elderly patients: A review. Drugs and Aging 1994; 5: 192– 199. 86. Amado-Boccara I, Gougoulis N, Poirier Littre MF, Galinowski A, Loo H. Effects of antidepressants on cognitive functions: A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1995; 19: 479–493. 87. Curran HV, Sakulsriprong M, Lader M. Antidepressants and human memory: an investigation of four drugs with different sedative and anticholinergic properties. Psychopharmacology 1988; 95: 520–527. 88. Peselow ED, Corwin J, Fieve RR, Rotrosen J, Cooper TB. Disappearance of memory deficits in outpatient depressives responding to imipramine. J Affective Dis 1991; 21: 173–183. 89. Kerr JS, Fairweather DB, Mahendran R, Hindmarch I. The effects of paroxetine, alone and in combination with alcohol on psychomotor performance and cognitive function in the elderly. Internat Clin Pharmacol 1992; 7: 101–108. ∗90. Plihal W, Pietrowsky R, Born J. Dexamethasone blocks sleep induced improvement of declarative memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1999; 24: 313– 331. ∗91. Smith C, MacNeill C. Impaired motor memory for a pursuit rotor task following Stage 2 sleep loss in college students. J Sleep Res 1994; 3: 206–213. 92. Tulving E, Schacter DL. Priming and human memory systems. Science 1990; 247: 301–305. 506 APPENDIX 1. GLOSSARY OF TERMS Declarative memory: Memories accessible to conscious recollection. Declarative memories are usually explicit in nature. Procedural memory: Memory for certain ways of doing things, solving problems or how to make certain motor movements. Procedural memories are usually implicit in nature. Cognitive procedural memory: Memory for certain ways of doing things or solving problems. Mirror trace task: Requires the participant to trace between the lines of a double lined figure with a pencil by watching their hand in a mirror. Participants cannot see their hand directly and must try to draw a continuous line around the figure, staying between the lines. Considered to be a cognitive procedural task. Motor procedural memory: Memory of how to make specific motor movements. Skill memory: Same as motor procedural. Explicit learning: The individual is consciously aware of learning the material required. Implicit Learning: The individual is not consciously aware that s/he is learning, but at a later time can demonstrate that the information was acquired. Paired associates (PA): A task in which a number of word pairs are presented, one pair at a time C. SMITH during acquisition. The number of training trials can vary, but the test involves presenting one of the two words alone. The participant must respond by giving the other member of the pair from memory. Priming: A type of memory in which exposure to a particular stimulus facilitates perception of that stimulus or related stimuli. Tasks take the form of simple figures or words which are initially presented. On retest, fragments or parts of these figures or words are presented and the participant is able to recall the material despite its incompleteness. Anagram: A series of letters that may form a word. Subjects are asked to rearrange the letters to form a new word. Trigram: Three letter consonant groupings that do not spell actual words. Lists of these trigrams are presented to participants and after several presentations, the participant is asked to predict the next trigram s/he would see in the list, based on the one they are presently observing. Verbal learning: Tasks requiring subjects to learn lists of words, or pairs of words (PA) are all considered to be types of verbal learning. Word fragment completion task: Special case of priming. Words are presented at training. Then at the retest only certain letters are presented with spaces where the other letters should go. Participants are able to “guess” the correct words (as opposed to other words) because they have recently been exposed to them during training.