AM-Club Towards a holistic conceptualisation of hedonic experiential consumption: The case of nightclubbing Introduction Drawing together a series of accepted experiential and consumption concepts this paper presents unified model explaining hedonic consumption. Where the previous literature supports a three stage consumption experience model (Arnould, Price, & Zinkhan, 2005; Caru & Cova, 2003) and decision-making research (Quintal, Lee, & Soutar, 2010), we explain the component constructs of each stage, and construct relationships across stages of hedonic experiential consumption (Hirschman & Holbrook, 1982; Holbrook & Hirschman, 1982). However, to our best knowledge, no single unifying conceptual model has emerged to explain these and it seems unlikely individual decision processes fit neatly into a single decision theory in hedonic experiential consumption situation. We also echo Goulding and Shankar (2011) call for advancing our knowledge in clubbing experience. Nightclubs are a good example of this type of hedonic experiential consumption as they consist of rituals such as mythology, formulism, sacredness, communitas and transformation, where “…the body becomes the locus of the experience” (Goulding & Shankar, 2011, p. 1449). Pleasure and hedonic consumption are important components of clubbing, where pleasure is “a complex biosocial phenomenon subject to competing sociohistoric discourses” (Goulding, Shankar, Elliott, & Canniford, 2009, p. 760) and hedonic consumption is “consumers' multisensory images, fantasies and emotional arousal in using products” (Hirschman & Holbrook, 1982, p. 93). Morover, consumers seek situations to escape the reality of everyday life and play an active role in the consumption of an experience (Fine, 2010; Firat & Dholakia, 1998). Therefore, marketers seek to engage consumers in a memorable and involved manner, offering extraordinary experiences (Arnould & Price, 1993; Caru & Cova, 2007; Schmitt, 2003). In other words, consumer experiences are at the heart of contemporary consumer behaviour and have a vital impact on what consumers play, enjoy (pleasurable experience), remember, and learn depending on their social and personal identities (Belk, 1988; Sherry, Kozinets, & Borghini, 2007). Bringing together literature from leisure, tourism and marketing, this paper combines different theories and concepts to create a unified model of hedonic consumption and apply it to the nightclub environment. This is in order to answer the underpinning research question: How do the differing stages of consumption affect the entire hedonic consumption experience? For example, how does the pre-stage influence the actual consumption stage and the final outcome? We divide the paper into three sections, the literature on consumption experience is reviewed and the theoretical constructs of the preconceived attributes (motivation, sociability and leisure entitlement), playful interaction (communitas, flow and ecstasy) and fulfilment (arousal and pleasure) as well as the proposed conceptual framework are introduced. The second section presents our methods and results, focusing on the questionnaire and the structural equation model which was analysed using Partial Least Squares (PLS). Finally, we discuss results, further research and implications. Literature Review and Conceptual Model Preconceived attributes (motivation to escape, sociability and leisure entitlement) AM-Club Lee, Lee, Bernhard, and Yoon (2006) identify three frameworks (including sociological, psychological and experiential) through which motivation has been studied in the context of gambling (an experiential consumption sharing many features of nightclubbing). Arguably, motivation to gamble is rooted in understanding of leisure behaviour and experiential consumption (Lee et al., 2006). Experiential rewards can be seen as “the temporary improvement in positive mood people experience when they are acting in goal-directed, purposeful ways”, and are an important motivation in examples of hedonic consumption such as night-clubbing (Csikszentmihalyi, 2008). Hosany and Witham (2010) have shown that escapism plays a strong motivating factor in their analysis of the experiential consumption of cruising, supporting wider tourism literature. However they identified that escapist intentions did not link strongly to overall satisfaction, or intention to recommend an experience to others, and call for further research using other scales to measure motivation and its importance in the subsequent experience consumption. Wohlfeil and Whelan (2006) highlight a link between arousal and the situational involvement that motivates people to an eventmarketing or experiential activity. As such a relationship between escapist motivations and arousal directly may be expected. Sociability is the tendency toward aligning and spending time with others rather than remaining alone (Spake & Megehee, 2010b). Levels of sociability show a strong link to character traits; high sociability people tend to be extroverted and crave excitement, need people talk to and laugh with, and are more prone to engage in social activities (Hills & Argyle, 2001). Spake and Megehee (2010) have argued that sociability has an impact on commitment to service providers in high involvement settings whilst outlining the need for consumer sociability to be further studied in experiential situations. This is not a new discovery, however. Sparks and Tucker (1971, p. 70) have shown that "sociability combined with emotional stability and irresponsibility is oriented toward one sort of action while sociability combined with emotional instability and cautiousness is oriented toward its opposite". Jafari, Taheri, and vom Lehn (2013) have cautioned against oversimplification of sociability due to the different levels of interaction people are willing to partake in and the importance of personal characteristics, such as motivation. The literature shows indirect links between the sociability construct and the desire to enter a variety of relationships and groups (Spake & Megehee, 2010b). Unger and Kernan (1983b) have shown how leisure has subsequently been understood as disinterested and hedonistic, seen to involve an emotional element of pleasure, as personally rewarding, and to involve intrinsic satisfaction (Iso-Ahola, 1982). McGinnis and Gentry (2004b) and McGinnis, Gentry, and Gao (2012) presents a useful framework – 'leisure entitlement' – in which to understand the construct in the context of consumer research. Leisure entitlement is conceptually defined here as the extent to which people believe they have the rights to and time for leisure. It measures whether leisure takes priority in a person's life and whether a person sets aside time specifically for activities that are independent of work and one's obligations to others." Further work highlights that leisure entitlement is constrained by intrapersonal (such as lack of knowledge, interest or time); interpersonal (difficulty finding someone to join in leisure with, restrictive societal expectations such as perceptions of gender appropriate activities); and structural (scheduling problems, seasonal cycles, family life cycles) constraints (McGinnis & Gentry, 2006b). Playful interaction (Communitas, Flow and Ecstasy) AM-Club McGinnis and Gentry (2004) have explored the concept of “play” and demonstrated how golf can be seen to involve this element of playfulness; here we use the concept for our analysis of nightclub consumption. Their definition of play contends that it is made up of communitas, flow and ecstasy; it is derived from definitions given by Deegan (1988), who describes play as liberation from modern societal structures, and Holt (1995), who describes it as autotelic consumption with no ulterior end. Play is similar to fun, differentiated in that fun does not carry the same liberating quality. The constructs which make up play are explored individually below. Turner (1956) used the term communitas to describe the unstructured, or – more applicable in a clubbing study – rudimentarily structured collective of equal individuals. During a “moment in and out of time” these individuals share “a generalized social bond that has ceased to be and has simultaneously yet to be fragmented into a multiplicity of structural ties” (Turner, 1969 p.96). Communitas plays a central role in the conceptualization of ‘play’ in ritual experience, leisure activity and hedonistic consumption where it produces a transcendent group camaraderie (Celsi, Rose, & Leigh, 1993). It can be seen as a removal of status and social categorisation, a period in which “people of the same condition develop fleeting feelings of comradeship and egalitarianism” (McGinnis & Gentry, 2004 p.405) and “wherein each member’s essential humanity is recognized and shared” (Deegan, 1998 p.26). “Communitas, although highly enjoyable and transcending, does not necessarily relate to the leisure pursuit itself as much it does to the participation and comradeship of other participants.” (McGinnis et al, 2008) As such we expect the relationship between leisure and communitas to be less powerful than between leisure and flow. Another element of the ‘play’ conceptualization is flow, defined by its architect Csikszentmihlayi (1988, 1990) as an autotelic experience involving the harmonious ordering of consciousness. According to Belk, Wallendorf, and Sherry (1989, p. 8) it involves “a centring of attention, a loss of self, a feeling of being in control of self and the environment”. Flow is often described as stand out moments, memorable yet effortless, a loss of selfconsciousness and appreciation of ordinary reality during which senses become heightened and the individual more serene (Deegan, 1989; McGinnis & Gentry, 2004). As such flow presents an important tool in experiential and hedonic consumption environments. McGinnis et al (2008) highlight that leisure is an intrinsically motivated pursuit, and that flow – through its autotelic capacity – closely represents intrinsically motivated behaviour; as such, a strong relationship is expected. Sacred, ritual like processes have been analysed as being able to produce ecstatic experiences so strong that individuals feel as though they are standing outside of themselves (Belk et al, 1989). It is related to the other components of this “During” phase of consumption in that, as with flow and communitas, someone must feel involved in order to experience ecstasy. It is a momentary experience which cannot be achieved if alienation prevents the required mental capacity required to feel positive, almost transcendental, emotions (Belk, 1989; McGinnis & Gentry, 2004). Belk et al (1989) show that, though not essential, group experiences can strengthen ecstasy. Fulfilment stage (Arousal and Pleasure) Pleasure is a concept which has emerged in many contexts and descriptions of affect, dating back as far as the fourth century B.C. when it was described as “the beginning and root of all good” by Epicurus (Russell, Weiss, & Mendhelson, 1989). It has come to be thought of as an AM-Club essential emotion, the maximization of which forms a basic human motive (Russell, 2003). Bigné, Andreu, and Gnoth (2005) have defined pleasure as referring to someone feeling good, feelings of joyfulness and happiness. The origins of arousal are much more contemporary than those of pleasure though they can be seen in Wundt’s (1912/1924, cited in Russell et al 1989) proposal of tension-relaxation. Arousal, activity and activation have been studied in a number of context and arousal is defined by Russell et al (1989) as, though involving or being based on physiological activity, representing a reported subjective feeling. Bigné et al (2005) have defined arousal as representing the degree an individual feels stimulated or active. Arousal is controlled by the brain’s emotional centre, the amygdale, and it is possible to differentiate empirically between low (base) and high (elevated) levels of arousal; “elevated arousal diminishes cognitive capacity and results in heuristic or superficial processing” (Fredorikhin & Patrick, 2010). Individuals in a high-arousal state have been seen to be more perceptive to peripheral cues and a disrupting effect on information processing has been seen (ibid). Figure 1 shows an overview of hypothesized effects. Preconceived attributes (motivation to escape, sociability and leisure behaviour) influence playful interaction (Communitas, Flow and ecstasy) and fulfilment stages of the nightclub experience. Preconceived attributes also influence fulfilment; this is illustrated in Figure. 1. Figure 1: Conceptual Model Method and Results To analyse the data, we used Partial Least Squares (PLS). Unlike co-variance based structural equation modelling (e.g., AMOS), which use the structure of latent variables, PLS is a AM-Club component based approach suitable for both predictive applications and theory building (Gotz, Kerstin, & Krafft, 2010) and appropriate for different levels of data (Henseler, Ringle, & Sinkovics, 2009). It is also used to estimate the structural paths coefficients, R2 using 500 randomly generated sub-samples (Gotz, et al., 2010). Moreover, Alexander, MacLaren, O’Gorman, and Taheri (2012) and Chin (2010) applied the geometric mean of the average communality and R2, within a range of values from zero to one as overall goodness of fit (GoF) measures for PLS. To ensure content validity of our survey we initially conducted a literature review to identify appropriate measures, employing existing scales for the main constructs (Bigné et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2006; McGinnis & Gentry, 2004b, 2006b; Spake & Megehee, 2010b). Scales were measured using 7 point Likert scales with “disagree stronglyagree strongly” as anchors. 54.7% of participants are male (total sample size: 1045). In the case of the scales (Table 1), Composite reliability (ρcr) scores range from .80 to .91 above the recommended cut off of .7 (Hair et al., 2010). Convergent validity was assessed using average variance extracted (AVE) and our factors scored .50 and .63 once again meeting the .5 threshold suggested (Chin, 2011; Hair et al., 2010). Finally, discriminate validity of the scales was measured by comparing the square root of AVE (represented by the diagonal with inter-construct correlations in (Table 2). All appear to support the reliability and validity of the scales. To examine the hypotheses, the structural model (Table 3) was simultaneously tested within SmartPLS (Ringle, Wende, & Becker, 2005). The modelled constructs explain 20% of the variance in communitas, 35% of the variance in flow, 19% of the variance in ecstasy, 45% of variance in arousal and 90% of variance in pleasure; the overall GoF is .62. To simplify the model only significant relationships are displayed. Conclusions The present study contributes to a better knowledge of the interplay among the stages of hedonic experiential consumption in the clubbing context. The majority of proposed relationships have been supported by the data which is also supported by previous studies (Belk, Wallendorf, & Sherry, 1989; Lee et al., 2006; McGinnis & Gentry, 2004b; McGinnis et al., 2012; Russell et al., 1989; Spake & Megehee, 2010b) (Table 3). This study highlights the need for further research into the influence of preconceived motivational factors and playful experience on arousal and pleasure. Our study adequately demonstrates that motivation to escape; sociability and leisure behaviour can positively influence clubbers playful experience, something widely explore in the extant literature (Cheek & Buss, 1981; Hills & Argyle, 2001; McGinnis & Gentry, 2004a, 2006a; McGinnis, Gentry, & Gao, 2008; McGinnis et al., 2012; Pine II & Gilmore, 1998; Reynolds & Beatty, 1999; Spake & Megehee, 2010a). Motivation to escape and leisure behaviour directly influence arousal with leisure also sharing a direct relationship with pleasure; these links supporting assertions from previous literature (Berlyne, 1969; Dawson, Bloch, & Ridgway, 1990; McGoldrick & Pieros, 1998; Unger & Kernan, 1983a). Given the lack of emphasis on the relationship in the literature explored here it is perhaps not surprising that sociability does not influence arousal and pleasure. Communitas and ecstasy affect arousal, but do not affect pleasure. Flow influences pleasure and does not influence arousal. These relationships have no been empirically analysed elsewhere though some suggestions of playful interaction upon the pleasure and arousal constructs has been suggested (Belk, Wallendorf, & Sherry Jr., 1989; Canniford & Shankar, 2013; McGinnis & Gentry, 2004a) In this exploratory study, the authors aimed to explore the consumption experience model in a hedonic situation based on previous studies. We have produced a putative model AM-Club highlighting the importance of the relationships between accepting constructs. Further, the model outlines the importance of relationships along the three-stages of the consumption process. By establishing a distinct multidimensional approach to measuring hedonic experiential consumption, this study lays the groundwork for future research examining different established constructs in hedonic experiential consumption model which plays important role for decision-making process in a hedonic environment. As such, further study of hedonic experience should employ mixed-method approaches in order to explore concepts further. In terms of managerial implications, the results are primarily relevant for the management and marketing of night clubs. AM-Club Appendix: Table 1: Assessment of the Measurement model Scales Motivation M1: To escape from routine life M2 : To alleviate boredom M3 : To escape from responsibility M4 : To release tension Sociability S1: I like to be with other people S2 : I prefer being with others than being alone S3 : I enjoy social gatherings just to be with people S4 : I value having relationships with other people S5 : I generally view myself as a person who is interested in establishing relationships with others Leisure entitlement L3 : I usually have several hours for leisure each week L4 : Nobody sets restrictions upon my leisure time L5 : Leisure time is something to which I believe I am entitled L6 : My leisure time is closely monitored L7 : I believe that I am deserving of my leisure time Communitas C3 : When I go out to nightclubs, I feel a sense of belonging with others at the club C4 : When I go out to nightclubs, I feel a sense of harmony with the others C5 : When I go out to nightclubs, I feel a sense of sharing with the people there C6 : Clubbing really allows me to get to know my fellow clubbers Flow F2 : Clubbing is the best way to relieve my stress F4 : When I go out to nightclubs, it receives my total concentration F5 : When I go out to nightclubs, I am surprised to find I have lost track of time F6 : When I go out to nightclubs, I can become totally involved in what I am doing F7 : When I go out to nightclubs, time seems to rush by quickly Ecstasy E1: I find clubbing to be a very liberating experience E3 : I often lose control when I club due to extreme excitement E4 : When I go out to nightclubs, I often become spiritually uplifted E5 : When I go out to nightclubs, I often lose my mind in rapture E6 : When go out to nightclubs , I get caught in a state of euphoria E7 : When I go out to nightclubs, I easily become ecstatic Pleasure P1: Unhappy-Happy P2 : Annoyed- Pleased P3 : Unsatisfied- Satisfied P4 : Melancholic- Contented P6 : Bored- Relaxed Arousal A1: Calm- Excited A2 : Slugglish- Frenzied A3 : Relaxed- Stimulated Item loading Composite reliability 0.80 AVE 0.89 0.63 0.86 0.50 0.86 0.52 0.86 0.56 0.91 0.60 0.90 0.58 0.84 0.57 0.50 .75 .70 .79 .70 .80 .73 .82 .82 .80 .73 .80 .73 .77 .70 .74 .82 .79 .73 .70 .71 .70 .74 .71 .74 .77 .81 .81 .78 .80 .76 .81 .80 .80 .71 .80 .78 .73 Note: Only the significant items used in Table2. Non-standardized coefficients; p< 0.5 AM-Club Table 2: Latent variables correlation matrix (discriminate validity) Arousal Communitas Ecstasy Flow Arousal .75 Communitas .36 .72 Ecstasy .46 .44 .77 Flow .39 .39 .49 .74 Leisure .64 .43 .55 .57 Leisure Motivation Pleasure .70 Motivation .32 .33 .28 .34 .29 .70 Pleasure .63 .37 .32 .51 .63 .26 .76 Sociability .36 .23 .26 .38 .32 .40 .31 n.a. Not applicable. Table 3: The results of hypothesis testing Structural relations M M M M S S S L L L L C F E A C F E C F E C F A P A P A Standardised path coefficients .124* .196** .176* .383*** .181** .155** .150* .197** .493*** .513*** .722*** .162* -.138* .140* ***p<0.001; **p<0.01; *p<0.05 Sociability .79 AM-Club References Alexander, M., MacLaren, A., O’Gorman, K., & Taheri, B. (2012). “He just didn’t seem to understand the banter”: Bullying or simply establishing social cohesion? Tourism Management, 33(5), 1245-1255. Arnould, E. J., & Price, L. L. (1993). River magic: Extraordinary experience and the extended service encounter. The Journal of Consumer Research, 20(1), 24-45. Arnould, E. J., Price, L. L., & Zinkhan, G. M. (2005). Consumers. New York: McGraw-Hill. Belk, R., Wallendorf, M., & Sherry Jr., J. (1989). The Sacred and Profane in Consumer Behavior: Theodicy on the Odyssey. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(1), 1-38. Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the Extended Self. The Journal of Consumer Research, 15(2), 139-168. Belk, R. W., Wallendorf, M., & Sherry, J. F., Jr. (1989). The Sacred and the Profane in Consumer Behavior: Theodicy on the Odyssey. The Journal of Consumer Research, 16(1), 1-38. Berlyne, D. (1969). Laughter, Humour and Play. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The Handbook of Social Psychology. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Bigné, J. E., Andreu, L., & Gnoth, J. (2005). The theme park experience: An analysis of pleasure, arousal and satisfaction. Tourism Management, 26(6), 833-844. Canniford, R., & Shankar, A. (2013). Purifying Practices: How Consumers Assemble Romantic Experiences of Nature. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(5), 1051-1069. Caru, A., & Cova, B. (2003). Revisiting consumption experience: A more humble but complete view of the concept. Marketing Theory, 3(2), 267-286. Caru, A., & Cova, B. (2007). Consuming experiences: An introduction. In A. Caru & B. Cova (Eds.), Consuming Experience (pp. 3-16). UK: Routledge. Celsi, R. L., Rose, P. L., & Leigh, T. W. (1993). An exploration of high-risk leisure consumption through skydiving. Journal of Consumer Research, 20(June), 1-23. Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(2), 330-339. Chin, W. W. (2010). How to write up and report PLS analyses. In V. Esposito Vinzi, W. W. Chin, J. Henseler & H. Wang (Eds.), Handbook of Partial Least Squares: Concepts, Methods and Applications (pp. 655-690). Berlin: Springer. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2008). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York: Harper Perennial Modern Classics. Dawson, S., Bloch, H., & Ridgway, N. (1990). Shopping Motives, Emotional States, and Retail Outcomes Journal of Retailing, 66(4), 408-427. Deegan, M. J. (1988). The American Ritual Tapestry: Social Rules and Cultural Meanings: Westport: Greenwood Press. Fine, L. M. (2010). Altruism and hedonism: A review and discussion of recent findings in the marketing and consumer behavior literature. Business Horizons, 53, 241-246. Firat, F. A., & Dholakia, N. (1998). Consuming People: From Political Economy to Theaters of Consumption. London: Routledge. Fredorikhin, A., & Patrick, V. M. (2010). Positive Mood and Resistance to Temptation: The interfering Influence of Elevated Arousal. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(4), 698711. Gotz, O., Kerstin, L.-G., & Krafft, M. (2010). Evaluation of structural equation models using the Partial Least Squares (PLS) approach. In V. Esposito Vinzi, W. W. Chin, J. Henseler AM-Club & H. Wang (Eds.), Handbook of Partial Least Squares: Concepts, Methods and Applications (pp. 691-711). Berlin: Springer. Goulding, C., & Shankar, A. (2011). Club culture, neotribalism and ritualised behaviour. Annals of Tourism Research, 38(4), 1435-1453. Goulding, C., Shankar, A., Elliott, R., & Canniford, R. (2009). The Marketplace Management of Illicit Pleasure. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(5), 759-771. Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., & Sinkovics, R. R. (2009). The use of partial least squares path modeling in international marketing. Advances in International Marketing, 20, 277319. Hills, P., & Argyle, M. (2001). Happiness, introversion-extraversion and happy introverts. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(4), 595-608. Hirschman, E. C., & Holbrook, M. B. (1982). Hedonic consumption: Emerging concepts, methods and propositions. The Journal of Marketing, 46(3), 92-101. Holbrook, M. B., & Hirschman, E. C. (1982). The experiential aspects of consumption: Consumer fantasies, feelings and fun. Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 132-140. Holt, D. B. (1995). How consumers consume: A typology of consumption practices. Journal of Consumer Research, 22(June), 1-16. Hosany, S., & Witham, M. (2010). Dimensions of Cruisers’ Experiences, Satisfaction and Intention to Recommend. Journal of Travel Research, 49(3), 351-364. Iso-Ahola, S. E. (1982). Toward a social psychological theory of tourism motivation: A rejoinder. Annals of Tourism Research, 9(2), 256-262. Jafari, A., Taheri, B., & vom Lehn, D. (2013). Cultural consumption, interactive sociality, and the museum. Journal of Marketing Management, 29(15-16), 1729-1752. Lee, C.-K., Lee, Y.-K., Bernhard, B. J., & Yoon, Y. S. (2006). Segmenting casino gamblers by motivation: A cluster analysis of Korean gamblers. Tourism Management, 27(5), 856866. McGinnis, L., & Gentry, J. (2004a). Examining the Mediating Relationship of "Play" on Ritual Enduring Involvement. Advances in Consumer Research, 31(1), 405-411. McGinnis, L., & Gentry, J. (2006a). Getting Past the Red Tees: Constraining Women Face in Golf and Strategies to Help Them Stay. Journal of Sport Management, 20(2), 218247. McGinnis, L., Gentry, J., & Gao, T. (2008). The Impact of Flow and Communitas on Enduring Involvement in Extended Service Encounters. Journal of Service Research, 11(1), 7490. McGinnis, L. P., & Gentry, J. W. (2004b). Examining the Mediating Relationship of “Play” on Ritual Enduring Involvement. Advances in Consumer Research, 31(1), 405-411. McGinnis, L. P., & Gentry, J. W. (2006b). Getting Past the Red Tees: Constraints Women Face in Golf and Strategies to Help Them Stay. Journal of Sport Management, 20(2), 218247. McGinnis, L. P., Gentry, J. W., & Gao, T. (2012). Antecedents to Consumer Perceptions of Sacredness in Extended Service Experiences: The Case of Golf. Journal of Service Research, 15(4), 474-488. McGoldrick, P., & Pieros, C. (1998). Atmospherics, Pleasure And Arousal: The Influence Of Response Moderators Journal of Marketing Management, 14(1-3), 173-197. Pine II, B., & Gilmore, J. (1998). Welcome to the Experience Economy. Harvard Business Review, 76(4), 96-105. AM-Club Quintal, V. A., Lee, J. A., & Soutar, G. N. (2010). Risk, uncertainty and the theory of planned behavior: A tourism example. Tourism Management, 31, 797–805. Reynolds, K., & Beatty, S. (1999). Customer benefits and company consequences of customer salesperson relationships in retailing. Journal of Retailing, 75(1), 11-32. Ringle, C. M., Wende, S., & Becker, J.-M. (2005). SmartPLS 2.0, from http://www.smartpls.de/forum/ Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., & Mendhelson, G. A. (1989). Affect Grid: A Single-Item Scale of Pleasure and Arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 493-502. Schmitt, B. H. (2003). Customer Experience Management. NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Sherry, J. F., Jr., Kozinets, R. V., & Borghini, S. (2007). Agents in paradise: Experiential cocreation through emplacement, ritualisation, and community. In A. Caru & B. Cova (Eds.), Consuming Experience (pp. 17-32). UK: Routledge. Spake, D., & Megehee, C. (2010a). Consumer sociability and service provider expertise influence on service relationship success. Journal of Services Marketing, 24(4), 314324. Spake, D. F., & Megehee, C. M. (2010b). Consumer sociability and service provider expertise influence on service relationship success. Journal of Services Marketing, 24(4), 314324. Sparks, D. L., & Tucker, W. T. (1971). A Multivariate Analysis of Personality and Product Use. Journal of Marketing Research, 8(1), 67-70. Turner, R. H. (1956). Role-Taking, Role Standpoint, and Reference-Group Behavior. American Journal of Sociology, 61(4), 316-328. Unger, L., & Kernan, J. (1983a). On the Meaning of Leisure: An Investigation of Some Determinants of the Subjective Experience. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(4), 381392. Unger, L. S., & Kernan, J. B. (1983b). On the Meaning of Leisure: An Investigation of Some Determinants of the Subjective Experience. The Journal of Consumer Research, 9(4), 381-392. Wohlfeil, M., & Whelan, S. (2006). Consumer motivations to participate in event-marketing strategies. Journal of Marketing Management, 22, 643-669.