A research-based approach to improving student understanding of the vector nature of kinematical concepts Peter S. Shaffer and Lillian C. McDermott Department of Physics, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195-1560 共Received 6 April 2005; accepted 26 June 2005兲 In this paper we describe a long-term, large-scale investigation of the ability of university students to treat velocity and acceleration as vectors in one and two dimensions. Some serious conceptual and reasoning difficulties identified among introductory students also were common among pre-college teachers and physics graduate students. Insights gained from this research guided the development of instructional materials that help improve student learning at the introductory level and beyond. The results have strong implications for the teaching of undergraduate physics, the professional development of teachers, and the preparation of teaching assistants. © 2005 American Association of Physics Teachers. 关DOI: 10.1119/1.2000976兴 I. INTRODUCTION The Physics Education Group at the University of Washington 共UW兲 has been engaged in a long-term, large-scale investigation of student understanding of motion.1 In this project, we examined the ability of students to determine qualitatively the magnitude and direction of the instantaneous velocity and acceleration of an object from knowledge of its trajectory. Analysis of the results provided some of the underpinnings for the treatment of kinematics in our two curricula, Tutorials in Introductory Physics (TiIP)2 and Physics by Inquiry (PbI).3 Studies on student understanding of motion have been conducted at all levels of instruction.4,5 The findings have informed the treatment of kinematics in some innovative curricula.6 Of particular relevance to the research and research-based curriculum development discussed in this paper has been the work of Reif and his colleagues, who used kinematics as a context for studies on cognition.7 In an intensive small-scale study, university students were asked to find the acceleration of objects moving along various trajectories under different conditions. Drawing on insights gained from observations and in-depth interviews, Reif formulated some specific procedures to help students solve kinematics problems. From this experience, he proposed a set of instructional guidelines for teaching problem solving more generally. Like Reif, our group views teaching as a science. Student learning is our primary criterion for determining teaching effectiveness. We think of research, curriculum development, and instruction as an iterative cycle. In this paper, we describe how we used information obtained through this process to design and assess instructional strategies to improve student learning in kinematics. II. MOTIVATION FOR THE EMPHASIS ON VECTORS AND OPERATIONAL DEFINITIONS A series of events that occurred early in the present investigation greatly influenced the direction of our research. Between 1988 and 1994, we administered problems on the motion of a simple pendulum to four populations. One group consisted of about 125 undergraduates in introductory calculus-based physics at UW, in which lectures on kinematics had been completed. A second group of 18 students was 921 Am. J. Phys. 73 共10兲, October 2005 http://aapt.org/ajp enrolled in a UW physics course for preservice high school teachers, most of whom had studied kinematics in a previous course. There were 22 UW Teaching Assistants 共TAs兲 in the third group. Most were graduate students. In the fourth group were graduate students taking the Ph.D. qualifying examination at UW and Montana State University. The first three populations were asked for the direction of the velocity and the approximate direction of the acceleration of a pendulum bob at various points after it is released 关see Fig. 1共a兲兴.8 A similar problem on the Ph.D. qualifying examination was based on a child on a swing 关see Fig. 1共b兲兴. In addition to the questions on velocity and acceleration, the graduate students were asked to draw free-body diagrams for the child and seat at the bottom of the trajectory if the child was not trying to swing higher. Only about 30% of the introductory students gave a correct response for the velocity at all points and none did for the acceleration. The performance of the preservice teachers was better for the velocity 共70% correct兲, but similar for the acceleration 共⬍5% correct兲. Almost all of the TAs in the third group and the graduate students in the fourth group drew correct velocity vectors, but only 15% did so for the acceleration. In all four groups, many students tried to determine the acceleration from the forces. However, in these problems, the relative magnitudes of the forces are not given. To find the approximate direction of the acceleration, a kinematical analysis is necessary.9 On the Ph.D. qualifying examination, fewer than 15% of the students gave correct free-body diagrams at the bottom of the trajectory and also ranked the forces correctly. Most stated that the string tension is equal to the weight of the child and seat. Some explicitly stated that the net force is zero and thus the acceleration is zero, not realizing that neither quantity can be zero if the velocity is changing. Such errors indicate that difficulties with kinematics persist beyond introductory physics and are not adequately addressed in later courses. The results from the graduate students were a major motivation for focusing our research on the ability to apply the operational definitions of velocity and acceleration as vectors 共v = lim ⌬x / ⌬t and a = lim ⌬v / ⌬t兲 to the analysis ⌬t→0 ⌬t→0 of real-world motion.10 We wanted to deepen our understanding of the nature of student difficulties and also to determine their prevalence. © 2005 American Association of Physics Teachers 921 Fig. 1. Questions used early in the investigation. In each case, students were asked to draw velocity vectors and to indicate the approximate direction of the acceleration for various points in the motion. III. OVERVIEW OF THE INVESTIGATION A major goal of the project was to develop a set of tutorials in one- and two-dimensional kinematics for TiIP and to expand the treatment of kinematics in PbI. The primary environment for research and curriculum development was the introductory calculus-based mechanics course at UW, in which tutorials from TiIP2 supplement instruction by lectures, textbook, and the laboratory. Each tutorial is part of a sequence that consists of a pretest, worksheet, homework, and post-test. The pretests establish a baseline for student understanding before tutorial instruction 共usually after the relevant lectures兲 and provide insights into the nature of specific difficulties. The pretests also help students recognize what they are expected to understand. The 50-min tutorial sessions typically have about 20–25 students. Structure is provided by worksheets with sequenced questions designed to guide students, working collaboratively in groups of 3–4, through the reasoning required to develop a functional understanding. Teaching assistants help students by asking additional questions. The TAs prepare for this role in a required weekly seminar in which they take the pretests and work through the tutorials in the same way as the introductory students. Tutorial homework reinforces and extends what students are expected to learn in the group sessions. The effectiveness of the tutorials is assessed by post-test questions on course examinations. More than 20 000 students took part in the present investigation. Of these, about 11 000 were students in more than 60 different sections of introductory calculus-based mechanics at UW. Additional data came from calculus- and algebrabased courses at eight colleges and universities that were pilot-sites for our curricula: Georgetown University, Grand Valley State University, Harvard University, Pierce College 共CA兲, Purdue University, Syracuse University, the University of Cincinnati, and the University of Colorado.11 TAs 共mostly graduate students兲 at UW, Montana State, and Purdue also participated. Another 200 participants were enrolled in special UW physics courses for K–12 teachers. Information obtained from the analysis of pretests and post-tests was supplemented by our observations and by our interactions with students as they worked through the curriculum. In this way we became acutely aware of their many struggles with the concepts, vector representations, and limiting procedures of one- and two-dimensional 共1D and 2D兲 kinematics. 922 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 Fig. 2. Questions used to examine the relation between vector skills and conceptual understanding: 共a兲 1D pretest on collision of two carts, 共b兲 1D matched pretests on manipulation of vectors, and 共c兲 2D post-test on manipulation of vectors. IV. PROBLEMS USED FOR DISTINGUISHING VECTOR SKILLS FROM CONCEPTUAL UNDERSTANDING Most previous studies on kinematics have involved the interpretation of observed or simulated motions and their algebraic or graphical representations.12 The ability to use and interpret vectors has been examined in several studies and found to be weaker than many instructors realize.13 To gauge the extent to which difficulties with the kinematical concepts could be attributed to lack of skill with vectors, we gave three tests at various times during the calculus-based mechanics course and one at the end. All four tests required the same vector operations, but two involved a physical context and two did not. A. Test on 1D problem: Colliding carts The velocity vectors of two carts are shown before and after a collision 关see Fig. 2共a兲兴. Students are asked to determine the direction of the average acceleration of each cart and to compare the magnitudes for the indicated time interval. No knowledge of dynamics is required, nor is formal P. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 922 Table I. Results from 1D pretest on the colliding carts 关Fig. 2共a兲兴 and 1D post-test on the colliding pucks 关Fig. 4共a兲兴. Pretest a Post-test Undergraduates N ⬃ 5040 TAs N ⬃ 170 Undergraduates N ⬃ 1845 50% 90% 70% 40% … 5% … 25% 30% 65% 75% 40% 25% 5% 20% 10% 10% 20% 60% 55% Directions of accelerations Correct 共aA to left and aB to right兲 Most common incorrect aA to right 共pretest兲 aB to left 共post-test兲 Relative magnitudes of accelerations Correct 共aA ⬎ aB兲 Incorrect aA = aB aA ⬍ aB Directions and relative magnitudes Correct on both a Includes results from most of the Ph.D. granting universities in the study, which had very similar results. About 35% of the students 关N ⬃ 200兴 at Harvard University and the UW honors section of the calculus-based course answered both pretest questions correctly. About 5% of the students 关N ⬃ 95兴 from the two-year college and the four-year, non-Ph.D. granting university answered both pretests questions correctly. These data are not included. knowledge of vectors necessary. The changes in the velocities can be found by subtracting the initial from the final velocity for each cart. Because the time interval is the same, the ratio of the magnitudes of the average accelerations is the same as for the changes in velocity. Thus, cart A has the larger acceleration, which is opposite to the direction of the acceleration of cart B. This problem was given to more than 5000 students in introductory calculus-based physics at several universities at various times during the first three weeks of class. The timing did not matter. Overall, about 20% of the students gave both correct directions and relative magnitudes for the accelerations 共see Table I兲. The success rate for TAs on this problem was 60%. It seemed unlikely that this result and the poorer performance of the graduate students on the pendulum and swing problems were primarily due to lack of basic vector skills. We wondered to what extent difficulties with vectors were responsible for the trouble the introductory students had with the colliding carts problem. B. Tests on 1D matched problems: Concept application and vector manipulation We gave two matched, multiple-choice questions in several lecture sections 关see Fig. 2共b兲兴. In one version of the question, a cart strikes and rebounds from a wall. The velocity vectors are shown and the students are asked to find the direction of the average acceleration 关N ⬃ 360兴. In the other version of this question, students are asked to find the difference between the same two vectors with no physical context 关N ⬃ 115兴. The problems were similar, but students did better 共65%兲 on the one involving only vector subtraction than on the other 共45%兲. On other questions from the same test that were identical, performance in the various lecture sections was the same. 923 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 C. Test on 2D problem: Vector manipulation We gave a question, previously used by Nguyen and Meltzer,14 on a final examination in a section of the calculusbased course 关N = 100兴 关see Fig. 2共c兲兴. Only the manipulation of vectors is required. About 95% of the students gave a correct response. D. Commentary A comparison of the results from the four tests indicates that many more students were able to subtract vectors when there was no physical context than when a collision was involved. After completing introductory mechanics, students were much more able to manipulate vectors than to apply them in real-world problems. Clearly, the difficulties that the introductory students had with kinematics extended beyond vector formalism. V. PROBLEMS USED FOR PROBING CONCEPTUAL UNDERSTANDING To extend our knowledge of student difficulties in kinematics, we administered qualitative problems 共similar to those used by Reif7兲 to many students. Explanations were required. We gave the pretests described in this section to both the introductory students and TAs before they worked through the tutorials described later in the paper. Before taking the 2D pretests, the students had worked through the 1D tutorial. Within equivalent groups, responses on similar pretests were similar and have been combined. A. 1D pretest: Ball moving up and down an inclined ramp One of the pretests on 1D motion involves a ball rolling up and then down a ramp. Students are given two strobe diaP. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 923 for the velocity, but only about 20% did so for the acceleration15 共see Table II兲. When a demonstration or simulation accompanied the pretest, the results were the same. B. 2D pretest: Object moving along a closed, horizontal track Fig. 3. Examples of pretests administered to large numbers of students. Students were asked to draw velocity and acceleration vectors at various points during each motion. 共a兲 1D pretest on ball moving up and down a ramp. 共b兲 2D pretest on object moving at constant speed along a closed, horizontal track. Some students were also asked about the case that the object speeds up from rest. grams, each showing half of the motion. The turnaround point is on both diagrams 关see Fig. 3共a兲兴. Students are asked to draw vectors at specified points to indicate the magnitude and direction of the velocity and acceleration of the ball. 共Sometimes only acceleration vectors were required.兲 The questions about the direction of the velocity and acceleration can be answered from knowledge of the motion. Dynamics is not required. The displacement and hence the velocity are up the ramp as the ball moves upward. The reverse holds for the downward motion. The velocity decreases in magnitude as the ball rolls up, is zero instantaneously at the turnaround, and increases as the ball rolls down. The change in velocity 共and hence the acceleration兲 is always directed down the ramp. Students usually know from the lectures, textbook, or laboratory experiments that the magnitude of the acceleration is constant. Versions of this pretest were given to more than 20 000 undergraduates, at times ranging from the first to the third week of class. About 80% of the students answered correctly We decided to probe student understanding of 2D motion in physical contexts that are conceptually easier than the pendulum. We used curved, closed and open trajectories with shapes chosen to elicit specific difficulties. The students were asked to draw velocity and acceleration vectors at several points. Various versions were given to almost 7000 students. One version involves an object moving along a closed, horizontal oval track with constant speed and, in some cases, also with increasing speed 关see Fig. 3共b兲兴. To find the direction of the velocity, students could draw the displacement for a small time interval about each point and let the interval approach zero. They could then reason that the velocity is tangent to the trajectory at each point and that the lengths represent the relative speeds. They also could simply state that the velocity is tangent to the trajectory. The question about the acceleration also could be answered in various ways. Students might state that the acceleration has a tangential component if the speed is changing and a perpendicular component if the direction is changing. Thus, the acceleration has only a perpendicular component for constant speed. If the speed is increasing, the acceleration has both tangential and perpendicular components except at the instant at which the object starts from rest, when the acceleration is purely tangential. Students also could draw velocity vectors at two instants near a point and estimate the change in velocity. Correct diagrams, with or without explanations, were Table II. Results from 1D pretest on the ball on ramp 关Fig. 3共a兲兴 and 1D post-test on the motion of two blocks 关Fig. 4共b兲兴. Not all students were asked about both the velocity and the acceleration. Pretest Velocity Correct 共up along ramp, zero, down along ramp兲 Incorrect Nonzero vector drawn at point where v = 0 Acceleration Correct 共down along ramp at all points兲 Incorrectb acceleration mimics velocity acceleration straight down 共at one or more points兲 acceleration zero at top Post-test Undergraduatesa TAs Undergraduatesa N ⬃ 715 … … 80% … … 15% … … N ⬃ 20 110 N ⬃ 285 N ⬃ 575 20% 75% 75% 共top only兲 20% 20% 5% 10% … 10% 50% 15% 10% a Includes results from most of the Ph.D. granting universities, which had very similar results. About 35% of the students 关N ⬃ 500兴 at Harvard University and in the UW honors section of calculus-based physics answered the question about acceleration correctly. These data are not included. b Categories not mutually exclusive. 924 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 P. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 924 Table III. Results from 2D pretests 关Fig. 3共b兲兴 and post-tests 共Fig. 5兲 involving motion at constant speed in a horizontal plane. Pretest a Velocity Undergraduates TAs Undergraduatesa N ⬃ 450 … N ⬃ 390 90% … 95% 5% … ⬍5% N ⬃ 6900 N ⬃ 75 N ⬃ 390 20% 65% 80% 15% 10% 20% 25% … 5% 5% ⬍5% ⬍5% Correct 共tangent to trajectory, equal length兲 Incorrect Vectors curved or extend from point to point Acceleration 共direction only兲 Post-test Correct 共a perpendicular to trajectory at all points兲 Incorrect acceleration toward “foci” or “center” of track acceleration tangent to trajectory acceleration zero everywhere a The results are from undergraduates at Ph.D. granting universities. treated as correct answers. As can be seen in Table III, when the speed was constant, about 90% of the students gave correct responses for the velocity, but only 20% did so for the acceleration. The results in Table IV for increasing speed are much poorer. Before taking the 2D pretest, some students had attended lectures on 2D motion, including uniform circular motion. Because results were similar with and without lectures, the data have been combined. VI. ANALYSIS OF INCORRECT RESPONSES ON 1D AND 2D PRETESTS Most students were able to draw vectors representing the velocity of the ball rolling up and down the ramp, as well as along the oval when the speed was constant. On both pretests, performance was much weaker on questions about the acceleration. There were many errors at points at which the velocity changes direction. The instant at which motion begins caused much difficulty for students at all levels. It was the most challenging point on the 2D pretest and the pendulum problem. We organized the incorrect responses on the pretests into four overlapping categories based on the incorrect reasoning used by students. Many of the errors on the pendulum and swing problems also fall into the same categories. The first deals with difficulties at arbitrary points along the trajectory; the second and third, with special points. The difficulties in the fourth category stem from dynamics. Table IV. Results from 2D pretests 关Fig. 3共b兲兴 and post-tests 共Fig. 5兲 involving motion with increasing speed in a horizontal plane. Pretest Velocity Correct 共v = 0 at start, then tangent with increasing magnitude兲 Incorrect Nonzero velocity at starting point Acceleration Correct 共a tangent at start; perpendicular and tangential components elsewhere兲 Incorrect acceleration tangent to trajectory everywhere 共excluding point A兲 acceleration incorrect for point A 共starting point兲 acceleration zero at start Post-test Undergraduatesa TAs Undergraduatesa N ⬃ 3375 N ⬃ 75 N ⬃ 685 30% 70% 65% 55% 30% 30% 共N ⬃ 3375兲 共N ⬃ 50兲 共N ⬃ 685兲 ⬍5% 20% 35% 共60%兲b 45% 10% 15% 85% 70% 45% 20% 20% 30% a The results are from undergraduates at Ph.D. granting universities. The second number represents the percentage of students who drew correct acceleration vectors if the point at which the object starts from rest is ignored. b 925 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 P. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 925 A. Incorrect reasoning about kinematics at arbitrary points along a trajectory 1. Not recognizing that instantaneous velocity is tangent to the trajectory Some students drew velocity vectors that were not tangent to the curve on the 2D pretest. Most followed the curvature. Other vectors were straight with both tangential and radial components, possibly indicating the direction in which the object was going to move. Often they started at the point of interest and ended at the next labeled point. Such responses could represent confusion between average and instantaneous velocity and, in some cases, confusion between the concepts of displacement and velocity. In some cases, students commented that the velocity is the slope of the curve, indicating possible confusion between a trajectory and a position versus time graph. Such statements were more common on pretests involving motion on open curves. 2. Not distinguishing between velocity and acceleration and sometimes using identical vectors for both Many of the errors made by the students suggested confusion between the concepts of velocity and acceleration.16 On the 1D pretest, some students drew acceleration vectors that seemed to mimic the velocity. They often stated that “the acceleration would be in the direction that the ball is moving.” Sometimes they showed the magnitude of the acceleration as decreasing on the way up and increasing on the way down. Even students who drew acceleration vectors in the correct direction often made this error. Overall, more than half of the students indicated that the magnitude of the acceleration changed as the ball moved up or down the ramp. Other errors also suggested confusion between velocity and acceleration. A common response on the 2D pretest was to draw both velocity and acceleration vectors tangent to the oval. Confusion between velocity and acceleration also was evident on the colliding carts pretest 关see Fig. 2共a兲兴. Many students claimed that the acceleration of cart A is to the right. Some seemed to be thinking of an average or “overall” velocity and reasoned that because the car’s initial velocity 共to the right兲 is larger than the final velocity 共to the left兲, the acceleration is to the right. Even some students who answered correctly seemed to relate the acceleration to the direction of the final velocity, not to the change in velocity. 3. Mistakenly assuming that the acceleration is zero because the speed is constant On the 2D pretest, about 20% of the students stated that the acceleration is zero for an object moving with constant speed along the oval track. They treated the motion as if it were one-dimensional, not realizing that a change in direction of the velocity means a change in the velocity vector and so corresponds to a nonzero acceleration. 共This was the most common error made by the graduate students on the pendulum problem.兲 4. Mistakenly assuming that the acceleration is directed toward special points For motion with constant speed around the oval, with constant speed, about 15% of the students drew acceleration 926 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 vectors that were clearly directed toward the center of the oval or toward points that some students labeled as “foci.” Some students seemed to be over-generalizing what they had learned about uniform motion along a circle or motion along an elliptical orbit.17 B. Incorrect reasoning for a turnaround point 1. Mistakenly using a nonzero vector for the velocity at the turnaround point The most common error 共15%兲 on the velocity portion of the 1D pretest was to draw a velocity vector for the ball at the top of the ramp. Some did so even though they stated that the velocity is zero. Often they showed a velocity vector up the ramp at the turnaround on the top diagram 共which shows the ball during its ascent兲 and/or a velocity vector down the ramp on the bottom diagram 共which shows the ball during its descent兲. Students also incorrectly drew velocity vectors at the turnaround point for the pendulum. 2. Mistakenly assuming that the acceleration is zero at a turnaround point About half of the students stated that the acceleration is zero at the turnaround point for the 1D pretest involving the ball on the ramp. Similar errors were made at the end points on the pendulum problem. Often students reasoned that because the velocity is zero, the acceleration is zero. This widely-recognized conceptual error is closely related to the tendency to confuse velocity and acceleration. C. Incorrect reasoning for the point at which an object starts from rest 1. Not treating the instantaneous velocity as zero for an object starting from rest Some students seemed to believe that an object about to move must have nonzero velocity. Many drew nonzero vectors at the starting point for the oval and pendulum. 2. Mistakenly assuming that the instantaneous acceleration is zero for an object starting from rest On the 2D pretest, about 85% of the students gave incorrect answers for the acceleration at the starting point. About 20% claimed that it is zero.18 The undergraduate and graduate students made a similar error on the pendulum and swing problems. 3. Mistakenly assuming that the instantaneous acceleration has a radial component for an object starting from rest A very common error on the pendulum and 2D pretests was to indicate a radial component to the acceleration at the starting point. In some cases, students drew only a radial component; in others, they drew both radial and tangential components. In the former case, this answer often seemed to be a memorized response based on uniform circular motion. In other cases, they seemed to be thinking that the acceleration should have a radial component because the object was starting to move along a curve. P. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 926 D. Incorrect (or incomplete) reasoning about application of dynamics to kinematics Many students tried to use incorrect 共or incomplete兲 dynamical arguments to find the acceleration from insufficient information about the net force. They did not recognize that the direction of the net force cannot be determined from dynamics when one of the forces is “passive,” 共for example, the normal force by a sloping surface or tension in a rope兲. The direction must be inferred from kinematics. 1. Not associating the direction of the acceleration with that of the net force On the pretest involving the ball on the ramp, about 20% of the introductory students drew acceleration vectors straight downward. They often stated that the acceleration is down because gravity is the “cause” of the motion or that gravity is the force “responsible.”19 Some said explicitly that the magnitude is 9.8 m / s2, as in free-fall. They did not associate the difference in direction of the motion 共along the incline versus straight down兲 with a corresponding difference in the acceleration. They neglected to consider the normal force exerted on the ball by the ramp. Instead of relating the direction of the acceleration to that of the change in velocity, some students simply assumed that the force was down the ramp, and thus so was the acceleration. On the oval pretest, a number of students justified the direction of their acceleration vectors by statements about the direction of the force “causing the motion.” A similar error was made by graduate students on the pendulum problem, for which about 20% said the acceleration is straight downward. 2. Confusing net force and acceleration About 40% of the introductory students stated that the accelerations of the two carts on the colliding carts pretest are equal. Often they based their explanation on Newton’s third law. These students did not seem to distinguish between acceleration and net force. Although the magnitude of the force exerted by each cart on the other is the same, the accelerations depend on the relative masses, which are not given. Confusion between the net force and acceleration also might have contributed to poor performance on the problem in which the cart strikes a wall and rebounds. E. Commentary on results from 1D and 2D pretests On the 1D pretest, very few students gave a correct answer and explanation for their choice of direction of either the velocity or the acceleration vectors. The errors and explanations often indicated that students did not have a functional understanding of 共1兲 the operational definitions of velocity and acceleration as rates of change of displacement and velocity, respectively, or 共2兲 the vector nature of these quantities. Many students seemed to be recalling memorized answers, apparently not understanding how these had been obtained or the conditions under which they held. Reif has made similar observations.7 The difficulties that students encountered on the 2D oval pretest indicated a general failure to apply the operational definitions of velocity and acceleration. Students generally failed to transfer what they had learned about motion in one dimension to two dimensions. They often drew on their memorized knowledge of specific cases.7 Some tried to use 927 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 their knowledge of the radial acceleration for an object moving with uniform speed in a circle, the derivation of which appears in almost every introductory course and textbook. Because in this case the radial acceleration is constant and can be expressed by a simple formula, the reasoning involved in the limiting process often is ignored by students. Therefore, they cannot transfer to situations in which the velocity is not constant in magnitude and the motion is not circular. VII. DEVELOPMENT OF TUTORIAL CURRICULUM The results discussed in the previous sections suggest that students need targeted help to develop an understanding of the kinematical concepts that extends beyond the ability to apply formulas. We found that an effective instructional approach is to help them develop facility in applying the operational definitions for velocity and acceleration as vectors. In this section, we illustrate the implementation of this strategy in the design of a curriculum. We hope the discussion of how we have used research to guide instruction will be helpful to others in their teaching of kinematics. To help students construct a coherent conceptual framework for a given topic, we begin with the identification and analysis of the intellectual hurdles that they commonly encounter. This detailed knowledge helps guide the design and assessment of instructional strategies to address specific difficulties. However, a list of errors cannot drive effective instruction. Many difficulties are interrelated and interdependent and therefore must be treated together. Moreover, as we have noted, the context is critically important. A brief description of the development and assessment of the 1D and 2D tutorials follows. The design of both embodies certain generalizations that we have drawn from our experience and an instructional strategy that we have often found effective.20 A. Kinematics in one dimension It seemed likely that students would benefit from direct experience in determining a direction for the change in velocity and in relating this quantity to the direction of the acceleration. We therefore designed a tutorial to guide students through the process of applying the operational definition of acceleration to a motion in one dimension. The context for the tutorial worksheet is essentially that of the ball on the ramp 1D pretest. Students are asked a sequence of questions that not only help them identify the steps needed to determine the direction of the acceleration, but also serve to elicit incorrect ideas. The students first consider the motion of the ball up the incline and draw vectors that represent the velocity at several instants separated by equal time intervals. They then are asked to find the change in the velocity between two specific instants. On the basis of a velocity versus time graph that they are given for the motion, they decide how the change in velocity would differ for a time interval that is half as large. The students are led to recognize that the change in velocity for each unit of time is the same throughout the motion. They then consider how the result of dividing the change in velocity by the corresponding time interval would change as the interval becomes smaller and are led to associate this quantity with the instantaneous acceleration. Our observations of students as they work through the tutorial indicate that many have not thought about a limiting process before and are surprised that P. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 927 the acceleration is not zero. The students repeat the same steps for motion down the ramp. Even after this experience, many still think that the acceleration is zero at the top. This error is a common conceptual difficulty that we elicit through the pretest. By guiding students in applying the operational definition for acceleration at the turnaround point the tutorial helps them confront and resolve this error. In the tutorial homework, the students apply and reflect on what they have learned. A major focus is on relating vector representations for motion to verbal, graphical, and other diagrammatical representations, as well as to real-world motions. The homework also provides practice in determining the acceleration from initial and final velocities. The students generalize their findings by articulating how an object moves if its velocity and acceleration are in the same, or in opposite, directions. One homework problem is based on the pretest situation involving two colliding carts. We have given several post-test problems related to this tutorial after students also have worked through the 2D tutorial. B. Kinematics in two dimensions The 2D tutorial helps students apply the operational definitions of the kinematical concepts to two-dimensional motion and also helps them develop skill in adding and subtracting vectors. The context is the horizontal oval track on which the pretest is based. As is the case for 1D motion, the tutorial includes questions to elicit student thinking and to help them confront and resolve incorrect ideas. By engaging students in the reasoning, we try to help them deepen their understanding. The students are guided through the process of finding the acceleration through the use of vectors for an object moving along the oval at constant speed, increasing speed, and decreasing speed. They select an arbitrary origin, draw position vectors for two points on the oval, draw the displacement vector 共⌬s兲 between the two points, and note that it is independent of the choice of origin. The students then apply a limiting procedure to find the direction of ⌬s as the second point approaches the first. They observe that ⌬s becomes tangent to the curve in the limit. They are led to associate the limiting direction with that of the instantaneous velocity at that point. The students begin the process of determining the acceleration by drawing the instantaneous velocity vectors at two nearby points. They find the change in the velocity vector 共⌬v兲 and consider how it changes as the second point approaches the first along the oval. They observe that the triangle formed by the velocity vectors and the change-invelocity vector is isosceles. They reason that, when the speed is constant, the angle between each velocity vector and the change-in-velocity vector approaches 90° in the limit of small time intervals. They draw acceleration vectors at various points on the oval and consider how the curvature affects the magnitude. They repeat the procedure for the case when the speed of the object is increasing including the case for which the first point is the starting point. The slowing-down case is completed as homework. In the tutorial homework, students generalize their results. They note that the angle between the velocity and acceleration vectors 共drawn tail-to-tail兲 is 90° for constant speed, less than 90° for speeding up, and greater than 90° when the object is slowing down. They recognize that for the speed to 928 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 Fig. 4. Examples of 1D post-tests. 共a兲 Two pucks collide on a frictionless table. Students were asked to give the directions of the accelerations and the relative magnitudes. 共b兲 Two blocks of different mass 共mA = mB / 2兲 move on a frictionless incline. Students were asked to indicate the direction of the acceleration of block B at the earlier instant and to draw the change in velocity vector for block A. increase 共decrease兲, there must be a component of the acceleration in the same 共opposite兲 direction as the velocity. VIII. ASSESSMENT OF 1D AND 2D TUTORIALS Both the 1D and 2D post-tests were given after students had worked through the 2D tutorial. Tables I–IV indicate the combined effect of both tutorials. There are fewer post-test than pretest results because not all the tutorials were assessed every quarter. A. 1D post-tests Two examples of post-tests on velocity and acceleration in one dimension are shown in Fig. 4. Each has been given in the form depicted and in several variations. 1. Collision of two pucks on frictionless table This post-test 关see Fig. 4共a兲兴 is similar to the question on the colliding carts discussed earlier, on which about 20% of the students 关N ⬃ 5040兴 gave correct answers for the acceleration 共direction and magnitude兲. On the post-test, about 55% 关N ⬃ 1845兴 answered correctly 共see Table I兲. 2. Motion of two blocks up and down an inclined ramp Two blocks 共A and B兲 of different mass move independently on a frictionless incline 关see Fig. 4共b兲兴. The initial velocities of both blocks and the final velocity of block A are shown. The first part of the post-test checks whether students realize that the acceleration is not zero when an object turns around 共block B兲. The second part asks students to find the change in velocity for an object that turns around 共block A兲. The first part of the post-test can be compared to the 1D pretest on the ball and the ramp. On the post-test, which is more difficult than the pretest, about 75% of the students 关N ⬃ 575兴 found the correct acceleration at the top 共see Table II兲. Only 20% had done so on the pretest. On the second part of the post-test, students were shown initial and final velocity vectors and asked about either the change in velocity or acP. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 928 2. Pendulum Fig. 5. Example of 2D post-test. Students were asked to sketch velocity and acceleration vectors at the labeled points for the case that the object moves at constant speed and to draw acceleration vectors for the case that the object moves with increasing speed. celeration. This part can be compared to the colliding carts question. On the post-test, 55% answered correctly, in contrast to 20% on the pretest. 3. Commentary Although there were no formal assessments of the 1D tutorial alone, results from other problems showed that many students did learn to distinguish the kinematical concepts clearly from one another and to recognize that the acceleration is not zero at a starting or turnaround point. As we discussed, the 1D and 2D tutorials together led to improved performance on both 1D post-tests when compared with the 1D pretests. The percentage making errors that the tutorials try to address decreased. As on the pretests, the most common errors involved inappropriate dynamical arguments. The introductory students did about as well on both 1D post-tests as the TAs on the 1D pretests. However, it is clear from the 2D pretest that few of the introductory students were able to transfer to two dimensions much of what they had learned in the 1D tutorial. B. 2D post-tests A variety of post-tests on 2D motion were given after students had worked through the 1D and 2D tutorials. Here, we discuss the results from post-tests based on closed horizontal curves and on the pendulum. 1. Motion with changing speed along a closed horizontal trajectory Many of the post-test questions involved motion at changing speed along a closed horizontal trajectory 共see Fig. 5兲. Typically, the object moves at constant speed or starts from rest, speeds up, and eventually moves with constant speed. Students are asked to indicate the approximate direction of the acceleration at various points. For points that correspond to constant speed, about 80% of the students recognized that the acceleration is perpendicular to the motion 共see Table III兲. Only 20% had done so on the pretest. For points corresponding to increasing speed, about 35% answered correctly, whereas fewer than 5% did so on the pretest 共see Table IV兲. The point corresponding to acceleration from rest was the most difficult for students. When that point is ignored in the analysis, about 60% of the students gave correct answers. 929 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 Students performed less well on post-test questions that involved vertical motion, such as the pendulum. We have posed a variety of questions in which students are asked to draw velocity vectors at several points of the motion and to indicate the approximate direction of the acceleration. The success rate increases from essentially 0% on the pretest to only about 15% on the post-test. Many of the same conceptual and reasoning difficulties appear on the post-test as on the pretest. In the case of the pendulum, many students still try to relate the acceleration of the bob to the forces exerted on it. Early in the investigation, we recognized that the ability to determine the direction of the acceleration often is adversely affected by a tendency to reason on the basis of forces.21 Although such an analysis yields a range of possible directions, the information is insufficient to determine the direction. Kinematical analysis is necessary. 3. Commentary As on the 1D post-tests, student performance on both types of 2D post-tests approximately matches that of physics graduate students on the corresponding questions. We generally consider a tutorial to be successful when the performance of introductory students on a post-test matches, or surpasses, that of the TAs on the corresponding pretest. Although the kinematics tutorials meet this criterion, we are continuing to strive for greater improvement by probing more deeply into how difficulties with dynamics affect the ability of students to determine acceleration.22 IX. APPLICATION TO PRECOLLEGE TEACHER PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT Some of the participants in the study were enrolled in special preservice and inservice physics courses that our group conducts for the preparation of teachers in physics and physical science.23 The curriculum, Physics by Inquiry, consists of a set of laboratory-based modules expressly designed to help teachers do the reasoning required to construct a coherent conceptual framework. The published version of PbI includes Kinematics, a module that treats motion in one dimension only. We have recently developed a module on 2D kinematics that incorporates many of the strategies in the tutorials but is more rigorous. The post-test performance of teachers who have worked through these modules often has surpassed that of students enrolled in tutorials in the introductory calculus-based course. We attribute this accomplishment to the treatment in PbI, which requires a greater degree of intellectual engagement and more time than is available for the tutorials. X. CONCLUSION Evidence from this study and other research by our group documents the difficulties that kinematics presents not only to introductory students, but also to many precollege teachers and even graduate students. After standard instruction, students often cannot apply the formalism that they have been taught to determine the velocity and acceleration of a motion that they have observed or that has been described. In some cases, inability to perform basic vector operations is the obstacle. However, in many instances, the difficulties are primarily conceptual, rather than mathematical. Certain conP. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 929 texts seem to lead students to try to reason on the basis of dynamics rather than kinematics, even when this approach does not yield sufficient information about the acceleration. The tendency to try to infer the magnitude and direction of the acceleration from the forces involved, rather than from the velocity, is especially common in the presence of gravitational interactions 共for example, a ball on an incline and a swinging pendulum兲. The findings from our research, as well as our experience in teaching kinematics, have led to the emphasis in our curriculum on operational definitions of velocity and acceleration in terms of their vector representations. Because difficulties with kinematics in one dimension become even more pronounced in two, we explicitly try to help students learn how to apply vectors in the analysis of motion in one dimension and then help them transfer this skill to two dimensions. Tutorials in Introductory Physics has not only helped the introductory students for whom it was expressly designed, but also has helped undergraduate and graduate TAs both deepen their own understanding and recognize the difficulties inherent in this topic. The corresponding modules in Physics by Inquiry have served the same purpose for precollege teachers. Thus, the results from this investigation have implications for instruction from the introductory to the graduate level and provide an incentive for ongoing research and curriculum development in kinematics. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors gratefully acknowledge the collaboration of many members of the Physics Education Group. Costas Constantinou, Gregory F. Francis, Mark D. Somers, and Stamatis Vokos participated in the development of curriculum. Contributions to the research were made by Bradley S. Ambrose, Sean M. Courtney, Paula R. L. Heron, MacKenzie R. Stetzer, and John R. Thompson. Special thanks are due to the instructors whose classes were included in this investigation. We also deeply appreciate support from the Division of Undergraduate Education and the Division of Physics of the National Science Foundation. 1 Results from these investigations are in D. E. Trowbridge and L. C. McDermott, “Investigation of student understanding of the concept of velocity in one dimension,” Am. J. Phys. 48 共12兲, 1020–1028 共1980兲; “Investigation of student understanding of the concept of acceleration in one dimension,” 49 共3兲, 242–253 共1981兲; and L. C. McDermott, M. L. Rosenquist, and E. H. van Zee, “Student difficulties in connecting graphs and physics: Examples from kinematics,” ibid. 55 共6兲, 503–515 共1987兲. Documentation of similar findings in the honors section of the introductory course are in P. C. Peters, “Even honors students have conceptual difficulties with physics,” Am. J. Phys. 50 共6兲, 501–508 共1982兲. 2 L. C. McDermott, P. S. Shaffer, and the Physics Education Group at the University of Washington, Tutorials in Introductory Physics 共Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 2002兲. 3 L. C. McDermott and the Physics Education Group at the University of Washington, Physics by Inquiry 共Wiley, New York, 1996兲. 4 For a list of references to studies at the precollege level, see R. Duit, “Students’ and teachers’ conceptions and science education, Bibliography,” Leibniz Institute for Science Education at the University of Kiel, Olshausenstr. 62 24098 Kiel, Germany, or 具http://www.ipn.uni-kiel.de/ aktuell/stcse/stcse.html典. 5 For a list of references to studies at the university level, see L. C. McDermott and E. F. Redish, “Resource letter: PER-1: Physics education research,” Am. J. Phys. 67 共9兲, 755–767 共1999兲. 930 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 6 For example, Laws, Thornton, and Sokoloff have applied findings from research to the development of real-time microcomputer-based laboratory tools to help students make connections between real motions and their graphical representations. A discussion of the instructional approach appears in P. W. Laws, “Millikan lecture 1996: Promoting active learning based on physics education research in introductory physics courses,” Am. J. Phys. 65 共1兲, 14–21 共1997兲; and R. K. Thornton and D. R. Sokoloff, “Learning motion concepts using real-time microcomputerbased laboratory tools,” ibid. 58 共9兲, 858–867 共1990兲. 7 P. Labudde, F. Reif, and L. Quinn, “Facilitation of scientific concept learning by interpretation procedures and analysis,” Int. J. Sci. Educ. 10 共1兲, 81–98 共1988兲; F. Reif and S. Allen, “Cognition for interpreting scientific concepts: A study of acceleration,” Cogn. Instruct. 9 共1兲, 1–44 共1992兲. 8 This problem was used by Reif in the studies described in Ref. 7. 9 There are many problems of this type that students should be able to solve. Another example of the need to infer information about the acceleration from kinematical constraints is given in L. C. McDermott, P. S. Shaffer, and M. D. Somers, “Research as a guide for teaching introductory mechanics: An illustration in the context of the Atwood’s machine,” Am. J. Phys. 62 共1兲, 46–55 共1994兲. Students were presented with a problem involving two blocks of different mass connected by an inextensible string passing over a pulley. About 15% failed to recognize that the accelerations of the blocks were equal. 10 For a discussion on the importance of emphasizing operational definitions in teaching physics and physical science, see A. Arons, Teaching Introductory Physics 共Wiley, New York, 1997兲. 11 Most of the Ph.D. granting universities had similar results on the pretests and post-tests. The results have been combined and are presented in the tables and text. Footnotes to the tables give results from non-Ph.D. granting institutions, Harvard University, and a self-selected honors section of UW calculus-based physics, which had a higher percentage of physics and science majors than other sections. 12 For examples of research related to graphical interpretations of motion, see R. J. Beichner, “Testing student understanding of motion graphs,” Am. J. Phys. 62 共8兲, 750–762 共1994兲; F. M. Goldberg and J. H. Anderson, “Student difficulties with graphical representations of negative values of velocity,” Phys. Teach. 27 共4兲, 254–260 共1989兲; and the third paper in Ref. 1. 13 See, for example, S. Flores, S. E. Kanim, and C. H. Kautz, “Student use of vectors in introductory mechanics,” Am. J. Phys. 72 共4兲, 460–468 共2004兲; N. L. Nguyen and D. E. Meltzer, “Initial understanding of vector concepts among students in introductory physics courses,” ibid. 71 共6兲, 630–638 共2003兲; R. D. Knight, “The vector knowledge of beginning physics students,” Phys. Teach. 33 共2兲, 74–78 共1995兲; J. M. Aguirre, “Student preconceptions about vector kinematics,” ibid. 26 共4兲, 212–216 共1988兲; and J. M. Aguirre and G. Erickson, “Students’ conceptions about the vector characteristics of three physics concepts,” J. Res. Sci. Teach. 21, 437–457 共1984兲. 14 See the second paper in Ref. 13. 15 This result is similar to that obtained by Reif and Allen on a corresponding question in the small-scale study 共N = 5兲 described in the second article in Ref. 7. 16 Confusion between velocity and acceleration in one dimension has been documented previously. See, for example, the first two papers in Ref. 1. 17 We also have administered pretest questions involving open, curved trajectories. Most students do not claim that the acceleration is directed toward the center or the “foci”; however, the percentage of correct answers is similar to that on the oval pretest. 18 An object that starts to move from rest at t = t0 is usually treated as having an acceleration that changes discontinuously at t0. Some students might have had difficulty with this approximation. 19 There is a common tendency to associate causes of motion only with active forces and not with passive forces. See, for example, J. Minstrell, “Explaining the ‘at rest’ condition of an object,” Phys. Teach. 20 共1兲, 10–14 共1982兲; L. C. McDermott, “Research on conceptual understanding in mechanics,” Phys. Today 37 共7兲, 24–32 共1984兲. 20 Some of the generalizations, their basis, and an instructional strategy that can be summarized as “elicit, confront, and resolve” are discussed in greater detail in L. C. McDermott, “Oersted medal lecture 2001: Physics education research—The key to student learning,” Am. J. Phys. 69 共11兲, P. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 930 1127–1137 共2001兲 and L. C. McDermott, “Millikan lecture 1990: What we teach and what is learned—Closing the gap,” Am. J. Phys. 59 共4兲, 301–315 共1991兲. 21 Reif had observed a similar tendency. See, for example, Ref. 7. 22 Research on student understanding of 2D vertical motion has been con- 931 Am. J. Phys., Vol. 73, No. 10, October 2005 ducted at the University of Washington, at the University of Maine 共J. R. Thompson兲, and at Grand Valley State University 共B. S. Ambrose兲. 23 For a description of these courses, see L. C. McDermott, “A perspective on teacher preparation in physics and other sciences: The need for special science courses for teachers,” Am. J. Phys. 58 共8兲, 734–752 共2000兲. P. S. Shaffer and L. C. McDermott 931