BioE 109 Lecture 14 Adaptation Adaptation What is adaptation? - the term “adaptation” has different meanings in different fields of biology. 1. physiologists use adaptation to describe acclimatization. - acclimatization refers to the physiological adjustment of individual organisms to different conditions (e.g., temperature, photoperiod). - note that there is no genetic change. 2. to evolutionary biologists, adaptation is used in two different ways. Adaptation 1. the process of becoming adapted. - the first definition of adaptation is as a process, i.e., the process of becoming adapted. - according to this definition, adaptation occurs by the process of natural selection that we considered in class. - for the process of adaptation to occur there must exist genetic variation among individuals in the population and among these different genotypes there must exist differences in relative fitness to drive the selective process. - according to this definition, adaptation is synonymous with the process of natural selection. Adaptation 2. the state of being adapted. - the second definition of adaptation is perhaps more widely used. - it refers to the end-point of this process, i.e., the state of being adapted. - according to this definition, adaptation is the end product of the process of natural selection. - individual characters of organisms are viewed as adaptations. - what is the common feature of all these traits? - the answer is that all are assumed to have evolved by the process of natural selection. - unlike the previous definition, however, we cannot observe this process directly but must infer its past action. Adaptation How do we recognize adaptations? Adaptations have two characteristics: 1. complexity - complexity can only evolve by natural selection. - many examples exist of traits being suspected of being adaptations prior to any evidence based on the criteria of complexity. - for example, the histological complexity of the lateral line in fishes and its uniformity in many clades suggested to physiologists that it was an adaptation. - this was eventually confirmed after extensive study showed that it proved to be sensitive to differences in water pressure. Complexity Complexity Adaptation 2. appearance of design - often we can infer the function of a structure from design principles that an engineer might apply to accomplish some goal like locomotion or the retention or dissipation of heat. - for example, in hot environments plants with large leaves often have leaflets or lobed forms that help to reduce the leaf’s temperature. - in many species of birds and mammals, those in colder environments tend to have larger bodies (Bergman’s rule) and have shorter ears and legs (Allen’s rule). - both factors tend to reduce the rate of body heat loss by acting to decrease the ratio of surface area to body mass. - these characteristics have the appearance of “design”- i.e., that they have been constructed or arranged to accomplish some function like growth, defense or dispersal. - in the non-living world we see nothing comparable to “complexity” or “design”. Spider silk The adaptationist program 1. Observe or describe some organism trait. 2. Formulate an adaptive hypothesis for the evolution of that trait. 3. Test hypothesis by experiment or by collecting additional data. Examples Batesian mimicry Muellerian mimicry Batesian mimicry (1861) Batesian mimicry involves a palatable, unprotected species (the mimic) that closely resembles an unpalatable or protected species (the model) Henry Walter Bates 1825-1892 Monarch Butterfly Viceroy Butterfly Blue Jay Muellerian mimicry (1890) Muellerian (Mullerian) mimicry refers to two unpalatable species that are mimics of each other with conspicuous warning coloration (also known as aposematic coloration). Fritz Mueller 1821-1897 Muellerian mimicry Two or more unpalatable species look like each other Two wasps species, both with stingers Muellerian mimicry Two or more unpalatable species look like each other Heliconius butterflies, all bad tasting Pitfalls - Giraffes - Polar Bears The neck of the Giraffe The neck of the Giraffe Foraging-competition hypothesis The neck of the Giraffe Most feeding is done below neck height. 7 7 Feeding height (meters) 6 6 5 Males 4 4 3 3 2 2 1 1 0 0 0 20 40 Females 5 0 20 Percentage of feeding bites 40 The neck of the Giraffe Male-male competition “Simmons and Schemers (1996)” Fighting Gerenuks (Giraffe necks) Leaf insects The common cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) Cuttlefish crypsis Cuttlefish crypsis How do we study adaptations? How do we study adaptations? 1. The experimental approach How do we study adaptations? 1. The experimental approach • hypotheses for the adaptive origins of traits are tested by experiments. How do we study adaptations? 1. The experimental approach • hypotheses for the adaptive origins of traits are tested by experiments. 2. The comparative approach How do we study adaptations? 1. The experimental approach • hypotheses for the adaptive origins of traits are tested by experiments. 2. The comparative approach • hypotheses for the adaptive origins or traits are tested by: How do we study adaptations? 1. The experimental approach • hypotheses for the adaptive origins of traits are tested by experiments. 2. The comparative approach • hypotheses for the adaptive origins or traits are tested by: (a) Performing comparisons among species How do we study adaptations? 1. The experimental approach • hypotheses for the adaptive origins of traits are tested by experiments. 2. The comparative approach • hypotheses for the adaptive origins or traits are tested by: (a) Performing comparisons among species (b) Making observations within species The experimental approach Example: the tephritid fly, Zonosemata vittigera The experimental approach Example: the tephritid fly, Zonosemata vittigera Initial observations: The experimental approach Example: the tephritid fly, Zonosemata vittigera Initial observations: 1. distinctive dark wing bands The experimental approach Example: the tephritid fly, Zonosemata vittigera Initial observations: 1. distinctive dark wing bands 2. wing-waving behavior The experimental approach Example: the tephritid fly, Zonosemata vittigera Initial observations: 1. distinctive dark wing bands 2. wing-waving behavior Initial hypothesis: • markings and behavior mimics jumping spiders thus deterring other predators. The experimental approach Example: the tephritid fly, Zonosemata vittigera Initial observations: 1. distinctive dark wing bands 2. wing-waving behavior Initial hypothesis: • markings and behavior mimics jumping spiders thus deterring other predators. Alternative hypothesis: • markings and behavior mimics jumping spiders to deter predation by jumping spiders. Staring down your enemy… The experimental approach Question: The experimental approach Question: Do the traits actually mimic the threat display of the jumping spider thereby allowing the fly to escape predation? The experimental approach Question: Do the traits actually mimic the threat display of the jumping spider thereby allowing the fly to escape predation? Hypotheses: The experimental approach Question: Do the traits actually mimic the threat display of the jumping spider thereby allowing the fly to escape predation? Hypotheses: HO: Flies do not mimic jumping spiders (null hypothesis). The experimental approach Question: Do the traits actually mimic the threat display of the jumping spider thereby allowing the fly to escape predation? Hypotheses: HO: Flies do not mimic jumping spiders (null hypothesis). H1: Flies mimic jumping spiders to avoid other predators. The experimental approach Question: Do the traits actually mimic the threat display of the jumping spider thereby allowing the fly to escape predation? Hypotheses: HO: Flies do not mimic jumping spiders (null hypothesis). H1: Flies mimic jumping spiders to avoid other predators. H2: Flies mimic jumping spiders to avoid predation by jumping spiders. The experimental design Experimental results The comparative approach (a) Performing comparisons among species The comparative approach: testes size in fruit bats & flying foxes The comparative approach (a) Performing comparisons among species Observation: Testes size is highly variable among species. The comparative approach (a) Performing comparisons among species Observation: Testes size is highly variable among species. Hypothesis: Males have evolved large testes in some taxa due to sperm competition. The comparative approach (a) Performing comparisons among species Observation: Testes size is highly variable among species. Hypothesis: Males have evolved large testes in some taxa due to sperm competition. Prediction: A positive relationship should exist between testes size and social group size. The comparative approach (a) Performing comparisons among species Observation: Testes size is highly variable among species. Hypothesis: Males have evolved large testes in some taxa due to sperm competition. Prediction: A positive relationship should exist between testes size and social group size. (Assuming that sperm competition is more intense in larger social groups.) Relationship between testes size and social group size A potential problem – lack of independence Performing phylogenetically independent contrasts After correcting for the phylogeny… After correcting for the phylogeny… YES! Polar Bears “Sterling 1974” 1 “sneak and pounce” Polar Bears 54 “jump and crush” 233 “sit and wait” Polar Bears UV Hypothesis