A Tephritid Fly Mimics the Territorial Displays of its Jumping Spider Predators Erick Greene; Larry J. Orsak; Douglas W. Whitman Science, New Series, Vol. 236, No. 4799. (Apr. 17, 1987), pp. 310-312. Stable URL: http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0036-8075%2819870417%293%3A236%3A4799%3C310%3AATFMTT%3E2.0.CO%3B2-U Science is currently published by American Association for the Advancement of Science. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/journals/aaas.html. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academic journals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers, and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community take advantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. http://www.jstor.org Wed Jan 16 17:10:06 2008 wings with a black felt-ti ped marker. Flies were first immobiid by ch&g them at -10°C for about 30 seconds. 12. To determine that pen markine' on the wing do not directl cause increased m o d t y , we maintained 20 flies, of which we marked, within a Plexiglas cage. Mean age at death for each group did not Mer Mann-Whimey U test). Further, 0bse~ationSconed that flies did not behave diferently after ib 6, marking, nor was their ability to flee spiders hampered. an NSERC Canada 13. This spdy was sup rted o ranng grant to f?D.R.%e thank L. Dill, R. &berg, and R. Smith for reviewing the manuscript. We also thank L. Dill and B. Lalonde for advlce and B. Lalonde for photographs. 20 October 1986; accepted 11 F e b v 1987 A Tephritid Fly Mimics the Territorial Displays of Its Jumping Spider Predators The tephritid fly Zmmemuta vittigera (Cquillett) has a leg-like pattern on its wings and a wing-waving display that together mimic the agonistic territorial displays of jumping spiders (Salticidae). Zmmemuta flies initiate this display when stalked by jumping spiders, causing the spiders t o display back and retreat. Wing transplant experiments showed that both the wing pattern and wing-waving displays are necessary for effective mimicry: Zmsemuta flies with transplanted house fly wings and house flies with transplanted Zmsemuta wings were attacked by jumping spiders. Similar experiments showed that this mimicry does not protect Zotwsemuta against nonsalticid predators. This is a novel form of sign stimulus mimicry that may occur more generally. M OST FORMS OF MIMICRY, SUCH as cryptic coloration or Batesian and Miillerian systems, confer protection against a wide array of predators (1). We describe a novel form of mimicry in which an organism mimics its major predator and thereby reduces the risk of being eaten by it. A tephritid fly, by mimicking the stereotyped aggressive behavior of one family of spiders, can escape from spiders of this family but not from other predators. The fly Zmsemata v i e e r a (Diptera: Tephritidae) is purported to mimic jumping spiders (Araneae: Salticidae) (2, 3). Both sexes have dark wing bands, which resemble spider legs, and false eyespots on the end of the abdomen. When disturbed, these flies hold their wings perpendicular to the body and wave them up and down (Fig. 1A); this resembles the agonistic leg-waving behavior typical of the jumping spiders. However, there have been no experimental demonstrations that Zmsemata is a spider mimic. Many flies have dark wing markings and wing-flicking displays, so Zunosemata might fortuitously resemble jumping spiders, but not gain protection from predators by these features. If Zunosemata is in fact a jumping spider mimic, it is not clear what types of predators are deterred. Since salticids are quick and have a poisonous bite, it has been suggested that a salticid mimic may be shunned by many vertebrate and arthropod predators (3). Another possibility, which had not been suggested, is that Zmsemata displays may specificallymimic salticid territorial displays, and be effective only against salticid predators (4). Many salticids defend "privacy spheresyyaround themselves. Wheri two meet they usually initially perform agonistic displays (which may turn into courtship displays depending upon sex and species) (5). These displays can be performed by juveniles and adults of both sexes and occur within and between species. Although the Flg. 1. ( A ) A female Zmnnata vitt&cra beginning its wing-waving display toward a stalking jumping spider (Phidippw qacheanw).The jumping spider stopped stalking, waved its legs at the fly, and then retreated. ( B ) A Zaosnnata a'tt&cra fly with transplanted house fly wings. Such flies can display normally and fly. precise details of these stereotyped behavior- 2 displays vary intraspe~ifica"~,salticid agonistic displays generally commence with legwaving (6). To test the effect of the wing pattern and the wing-waving display on the behavior of jumping spiders and other potential predators, we transplanted wings between house flies (Mwca domestics) and Z m s m a flies (7). House fly wings are the same general size and shape as Zonaremata wings, but they lack pattern. After this operation, the flies retained complete movement of their wings, and could display and fly normally (Fig. 1B). Behavioral trials between jumping spiders and flies were conducted for 5 minutes in a glass-topped arena (8). Jumping spiders were collected on or around Zmsemata host plant (silver leaf nightshade, Solanum elaegnifolium). Twenty jumping spiders representing 11 species (9) were each presented with five treatments: normal Zonosemata, Zmsemata with other Zonusemata wings (sham operation), Zonusemata with house fly wings, house flies with Zonosemata wings, and normal house flies. Each spider was presented with these treatments in a random order. All jumping spiders were hungry when tested: they were given water but no food for 2 days before the trial. Individual spiders were never tested more than twice in one day. The wing pattern had a profound effect upon jumping spider behavior (Fig. 2). Normal Zonosemata and the sham-operated control flies were attacked or killed less fiequently than flies in the three remaining treatments (10). There was no smtisticallv significant diffeknce in the jumping spider;' responses to the normal Zonosemata flies and thi sham-operated control flies (homogeneity test, G = 3.28, P > 0.1), indicating that the operation itself did not affect spider responses. Jumping spiders began stalking these flies within seconds after the trial began. When the spider approached to within about 5 cm, Zonosma flies usually began a vigorous wing-waving display. response, the jumping spiders abruptly stopped stalking and waved their legs at the flies. The flies backed away in a zigzag fashion while waving their wings and flew off. Most jumping spiders made no further stalking attempts during the remaining 5 minutes. Jumping spiders were repelled from both the front and back of the flies. In E. Gmne, De amnent of Biology, Princeton University, Princeton, b 0 8 5 4 4 . L. J. Orsak, Institute of Ecology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602. and Population ManD. W.Whirman, Insect Biol agement Research LaboratoV~.s.Dqamnent of "culture, Agricultural R-ch Service, P.O. Box 7%; Tifton, GA 31793. SCIENCE, VOL. 236 and normal house flics). Of particular intcrest, Zonosernata with house fly wings displayed identically to normal Zonosenzata flies, but this never elicited the leg-waving displays from spiders, and all but one were attacked or killed. House flies with Zonosenzata wings held the wings flat over their bodies: the pattern was not visible to spiders, which never displayed toward these flies. T o determine if these displays also protected Zonosernata flics against other potcntial prcdators, we performed similar expcrimcnts using nonsalticid spiders (Oxyopessalticus), mantids (Mantis rel&iosa), assassin bugs (Pselliopus zebra), and whiptail lizards (Cnernidophorous uniparens; Arizona Research Permit 089). All predators wcre caught on or around the host plant of Zonosernata. These predators were presented Fig. 2. Behavioral responses of jumping spidcrs to with three fly treatments (A, C, and E of fly presentations. The response is the highest level Fig. 2). The same testing protocol was used of aggression attained during 5 minutes of behav- except that the test chamber sizes were ioral interaction in a test arena. The fly treatments varied to accommodate the different sizes of arc: A, normal Zonosemata; R, Zonosemata with %)nosenzatawings glued on (control for the opera- these predators (11). The Zonosenzata display is not effective tion); C, Zonosemata with housc fly wings; D, house fly with Zonosemata wings; and E, normal against these four types of prcdators (Table house fly. Sample sizes arc 20 for each fly treat- 1). None wcrc deterred by displaying flies, ment. The bars connect homogeneous groups (G tcsts, P's > 0.1). All other combinations are het- as occurred with jumping spiders. For each predator, there was 110 statistically signifierogeneous (all P's < 0.01). cant difference in capture times for the three six trials the jumping spiders stalked from treatment groups (pairwise Mann-Whitney behind, apparently ~ o 6 s e r v e dby the fly. U tests, all P's > 0.05). The flies gave spontaneous wing-flicks In summary, Zonosemata is a specialized (about one series in 10 seconds). The jump- mimic. Rather than conferring protection ing spiders stopped stalking, waved thEir against many types of visual prcdators, this legs, and backed away. jumping spider mimicry is effective only However, not all Zonosemata flies with the against jumping spidcrs. The neurological wing pattern were immune to jumping spi- mechanism of this mimicry is clear: jumping der attacks: of the 40 Zonosemata in treat- spidcrs possess feature detectors in the retina ments A and K, 2 wcre attacked, and 8 of their central anterior-median eyes which others were killed. This is partly a reflection arc excited by waving leg-like patterns (12). of the conservative bias used in classifying Model presentations to salticids have shown the behavioral responses: spiders were that leg-like patterns are potent releasers scored on their highest level of aggression that cause jumping spidcrs to abruptly stop during " the 5-minute ex~xriment.Of these what they are doing (usually stalking) and ten flics that were attacked, six probably display back (13). Thus, the Zonosemata would have escaped if unconfined since they display mimics a sign stimulus recognized repeatedly displayed and repelled attacks (a by salticids but not by other predators. This mean of 2.1 effective displays). Also, even accounts for the extreme specificity of the though they were eventually attacked, the protection: other examples of sign stimulus flies displaying with patterned wings were mimicry arc cffcctive against a similarly naraEorded some ~rotection:the latencv to the row range of signal receivers (14). first attack was longer for these six flies How could this mimicry syndrome have than for Zonosenzata flies with house fly evolved? Wing markings and wing-flicking wings (median latency was 44 seconds ver- displays arc common among acalyptrate sus l l seconds; Mann-Whitney U = 61, P flies, and are especially prominent in the < 0.005). The remaining four flies were courtship displays of tephritid flics (15). attacked so quickly they had no chance to Salticids can occur at very high densities, display. and they can exert strong predation presIn contrast, the spiders responded much sures on many insects, such as tephritid flies, more aggressively &ward the three other that spend time on exposed vegetation (16). types of flies (Zonosemata with house fly It is possible that the defensive display of wings, house flies with Zonosenzata wings, Zonosernata derived from courtship behav- A B C D E Table 1. Capture times for three fly treatments by potential predators of Zonosemata. The fly treatment symbols arc the same as for Fig. 2. For each predator there is no statistically significant difference between treatment medians (painvise MannWhitney U tcsts, all P s > 0.05). Fly treatment A 6: E ple (n) Median capture time (seconds) Range (scconds) Nonsalticid spzder Oxyopes salt~cus 10 80 16-207 10 77 32-149 10 56 32-292 Assassin bufi Pselliopus zebra 8 234 39-632 8 309 52-889 8 162 34-736 A C E Mantis Mantis 12 12 12 A C E rcligiosa 88 64 94 29-435 11-215 41-341 Wh$tail liza~dCnemidophorous uniparens 10 101 47-355 A C 10 81 24-370 E 10 92 9-1 19 ior, since refinements of displays that deterred jumping spider attacks could confer large survivorship advantages. A cursory glance at a museum drawer of flics reveals many with leg-like wing patterns. Thus, jumping spider mimicry may be a widespread phenomenon among many species of flies. Analogous defenses might be expected in other insects that are also highly apparent to a specific class of prcdators. REFERENCES A N D NOTES 1. M. Edmunds, 1)efenc~in Animals: A Sun~cyoj'AntiI'redntorlIefEnces (Longman, Essex, 1974). 2. T. Eisncr, Nat. Hist. 112, 103 (1984). It has bccn suggested that other tephritids in the Hhn~oletis pommella s ccies group reseniblc jumping spidcrs [A. C. Hofson, Ext. Polde~Minn. A ~ t i c Ext. . SEW. 122 (1944); L. G. Monteith, Can. Entomol. 104, 257 (1972); G. L. Rush, cited in T. Eisncr (3, p. 1 n4)1 observed that jumping spidcrs sce~nedto avoid t h i tephritid fly Rha&~tis pomonella, but he did not propose a mechanisn~. 5. R. R. Jackson, in Spider (:ommunicatwn:Mechanisms and Ecolu&al S&n$cance, P. N . Witt and J. S. Rovner, Eds. (Princcton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ, 1982), PI) 213-247. 6. L. Forster, in Spider Communication: Mechanisms and Ecolo~ical S&nzficance, P. N. Witt and J. S. Rovner, Eds. (Princeton Univ. Press, Princcton, NJ, 1982), PI) 161-212. 7. Zonosemata flics wcrc obtained by gently swcepnetting the host plant, silver leaf ni hnhade (Solanum elaegn$dium). Flies were coltckd between 15 June and 5 August 1986, along State Line Road near Rodeo, NM, and briefly maintained in a 2 by 1 by 1 nl terrarium that contiined potted nightshade and water, d q powdered milk, and a honey and watcr solution. Flies wcre cooled in a refrigerator until inactive, thal held immobile on an operating board with paper strips and insect pins. Their wings were cut off distal to a line connecting the nicdial cell and & , (Cmnland the intersection of veins KLti stock terminology). Wings from donor flies wcre http://www.jstor.org LINKED CITATIONS - Page 1 of 1 - You have printed the following article: A Tephritid Fly Mimics the Territorial Displays of its Jumping Spider Predators Erick Greene; Larry J. Orsak; Douglas W. Whitman Science, New Series, Vol. 236, No. 4799. (Apr. 17, 1987), pp. 310-312. Stable URL: http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0036-8075%2819870417%293%3A236%3A4799%3C310%3AATFMTT%3E2.0.CO%3B2-U This article references the following linked citations. If you are trying to access articles from an off-campus location, you may be required to first logon via your library web site to access JSTOR. Please visit your library's website or contact a librarian to learn about options for remote access to JSTOR. References and Notes 14 Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris Fireflies: Signal Repertoires by Femmes Fatales James E. Lloyd Science, New Series, Vol. 187, No. 4175. (Feb. 7, 1975), pp. 452-453. Stable URL: http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0036-8075%2819750207%293%3A187%3A4175%3C452%3AAMIPFS%3E2.0.CO%3B2-T NOTE: The reference numbering from the original has been maintained in this citation list.