AZTECHU1\IANSACRIFICE:CROSS-CULTURAL HYPOTHESIS1 OF THE ECOLOGICAL ASSESSMENTS 'm' A ; MichaelWinkelman ArizonaStateUniversity Ecological, religious, and social predictors of institutionalized human sacrifice are assessed through cross-cultural analysis. While human sacrifice has no signifcant correlations with measures of agricultural potential, protein, total food, food storage adequacy, and famine risk, there are significant positive correlations with population density, population pressure, and war for land and resources. Population pressure and war for land and resources have independently signiflcant correlations with human sacrifice, and together account for 38 per cent of its variance (multiple R = .62, p < .006). A measure of low hierarchical focus of religion provides signifslcantadditional explanation of variance (multiple R=.72, R2=.S1, pc.OOO), suggesting human sacrifice may play a cannibalism, religion, ethnology, ecology) role in ideological integration. (SacrifWlce, Concernwith the explanationof humansacrificeandcannibalismgainedheightened interestwith Harner's(1977a, 1977b)publicationson Aztec humansacrificeand cannibalism.Harnersuggests their causes were relatedto protein scarcity as a consequenceof high populationpressurein a distinctiveecologicalsituation.Ortiz de Montellano(1978, 1990) rejects Harner'sargumentsbased on Aztec dietary information.Price (1978), Hassig (1990), and Isaac (1983) suggest that human sacrifice was an epiphenomenonreflectinggeopoliticaldynamics, political and military instabilities, demographicconditions, and economic production and distributlonnetworks.But these rejectionsof the ecological hypothesisdo not convincinglysupportthe alternativesproposedas the causesof humansacrificeand cannibalism.Althoughsome reportsof cannibalismare doubtful institutionalized (e.g., see Arens 1979), evidencefor both humansacrificeand cannibalismis well (c.f. andclinicalliterature2 historical,bioarchaeological, documentedinethnographic, TurnerandTurner1995). Cannibalismin some nonhumanprimategroups(Goodall 1977) suggeststhatit may play a role in ecologicalandsocial adaptations. This articlereportscross-culturalanalyseson previouslypublisheddatasets of Sample(SCCS)(MurdockandWhite1969)to assessthe the StandardCross-Cultural role of ecologicalfactors,religiousconditions,and social complexityvariablesin predictinghumansacrifice.The focusis on legitimatehumansacrificecarriedoutby religiousleadersas normativesocialactivities.Thewide rangeof measuresexamined include:social complexityvariables;agriculturalpotential,meatprotein,domestic animals, and total foods; food storage adequacy;threat of famine; population (assessedthroughwarfarefor landand circumscription pressure;andenvironmental resources).The relationshipof religiousandsocial complexityconditionsto human sacrificeareassessedto illustratetherelevanceof othersocialfactorsto the incidence investigations. of humansacrifice,andto suggestdirectionsfor lFurther 285 ETHNOLOGYvol. 37 no. 3, Summer 1998, pp. 285-98. ETHNOLOGY, c/o Deparanentof Anthropology,The University of Pittsburgh,PittsburghPA 15260 USA Copyright 1998 The University of Pittsburgh.All rights reserved. This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 286 ETHNOLOGY HYPOTHESIS THEECOLOGICAL Harner (1977a, 1977b) rejects anthropologicaltheories that reflect Aztec explanationsfor humansacriElce;i.e., thatsacriflcewas requiredby their religion andgods. Basedon studiesof populationpressure,Harner(1970) suggeststhatthe unparalleledscope of Aztec human sacrifice and cannibalismresulted from factors which createdprotein shortagesand population demographic-ecological pressure:unfavorableagriculturalconditions,seasonalcrop failures, the lack of domesticatedherbivores,the depletionof wild game in the region, food scarcity, causedby limitationson theexpansionof circumscription famine,andenvironmental by poorfarmingland, agriculture.Thefertilecentralvalleyof Mexicois surrounded expansion.While precludingagricultural circumscription creatingan environmental lack of necessary their proteins, vegetable complementary provide beans and corn fattyacids andthe seasonalscarcityof thesefoods madethese sourcesinsufficient. This leads Harnerto hypothesizethatcannibalismprovideda significantsourceof protein.The Aztecsdidnot ordinarilyeatpeoplefromtheirownpolity,butpracticed warfare,calledflowery wars,"as ritualsto obtainsacrificialvictims.Theconquered territoriesnearbywere a convenientsourceof victims.Althoughthe consumptionof humanflesh was reservedfor the elite, theclasssystemallowedfor upwardmobility of greatwarriors,so warsto obtainvictimsfor sacrificeweresupportedby a hungry populationwhichdesiredbothprestigeandproteinin theformof humanflesh. Great warriorscouldalso receivetherightto consumehumanflesh, whichwas sharedwith lineagemembers. Harner'shypothesisof cannibalismbasedon chronic Price (1978) characterizes fromfamineandlack distinguished insuifficiently proteinshortagesas unsubstantiated, largelyconsumedby were sacriiSlces human that fact the by undermined of food, and Montellano(1978) de Ortiz the elite classthatalreadyhadeasy accessto othermeat. presents nutritionalresourcedata and dietary informationchallengingHarner's hypothesis.By documentinga wide rangeof meatproteinsavailable,the enormous food tribute they received, the stores of food maintained,and the intensive agriculturaltechniquesemployed,he arguesfor an adequatediet for the Aztec population.Otherfactorsmitigatingagainstthe ecologicalhypothesisinclude: 1) of the total conqueringnew landsfor agricultureand tribute;2) the insigniElcance of humansacrifice humanproteinavailablefromsacrifice;and3) the preponderance duringthe annualperiods of food abundancefrom harvests,ratherthan during of periodsof proteinscarcity.Ortizde Montellanosuggeststhatthe preponderance and of thanks za gesture it was humansacrificeduringharvestperiodsindicatedthat reciprocityto the godst (1978:614)ratherthanfilling a needfor protein. RELIGIOUSAND SOCIALHYPOTHESES theAztecpracticeof humansacrifice Ortizde Montellano(1978, 1990)attributes to theirbelief thatthe gods requiredit. This cosmologicalhypothesisis obviously This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions AZTECHUMANSACRIFICE: THE ECOLOGICAL HYPOTHESIS 287 ttue but uninformative. Religious justifications for humarlsacriflce and cannibalism do not alone explain such beliefs arld behavior. The individual Aztec warrior's motivation for participation in warfare for sacrificial victims included the social benefits accrued. But such obvious linkages do not inform us of the reasons human sacrifice was adopted as a mechanism for allocating prestige and mobility. The motivationsfor self-sacrificewere undoubtedlyreinforcedby religious beliefs thatthe victims would go to heaven. But why such beliefs would be adopted remains unaddressed. Their beliefs may explain their behavior, but leave unanswered the question of why the beliefs and practices were adopted, and fail to identify the psychosocial functions of human sacrifice. A dominantperspective in social arlthropology,influenced by Emile Durkheim, is that religious beliefs and practices derive from and are reflective of social conditions (Bourguignon 1976; BourguignonandEvascu 1977; Davis 1971; Swanson 1960; Peregrine 1996; Winkelman 1986 1990, 1992)* If so, the determinantsof human sacrifice should be sought in the social conditions which struct:urereligious beliefs. The perspective that determinantsof human sacrifice are found in the social conditions which structurereligious beliefs is illustratedin assessments of the broader economic networks and political conditions associated with Aztec sacriElce and warfare (Price 1978; Hassig 1990; Isaac 1983). These reject human sacrifice as a motivatorfor Aztec flowery wars (presumptivelyfor sacrificial victims), and thereby dismiss the importance of human sacrifice within the broader context of state activities, warfare, economic systems, and class stratification. Price (1978) characterizescannibalismas a stylistic trait and an epiphenomenon which had little effect on economic intensification,warfare, political expaxlsion,and social stratification.Price suggests that the explanationof Aztec human sacrifice and cannibalism is to be found in economic networks of production and distribution, patterns of state redistribution,political and military instabilities, and pacifications of the nobility Her model postulates that human sacrifice reinforced existing stratificationand political power, but she acknowledgesthe lack of evidence for such effects in her work. Isaac (1983) hypothesizesthat humansacrifice served ideological functions in Aztec society by uniting the class interests of nobles and upwardly aspiring lower-class warriorssbut he fails to explain why human sacrifice would be adopted for such integrativepurposes. Hassig (1990) links Aztec humansacrifice to their precariouseconomic position and consequent social and political adaptation.Their flowery wars are analyzed as a strategyof empire-buildingwhich enabledgreaterexpansionby allowing dependent areasto be self-administeredyet pay tribute, ratherthan investing higher political and administrativecosts to directly administerthe conqueredareas. Hassig characterizes the flowery wars as a deliberatestrategyto wear down strongerenemies, ratherthan as an institutionalizedprocedurefor obtaining sacrificial captives. Price (1978) and Isaac (1983) similarly suggest that the flowery wars reflected the shifting balances of power in the geopolitical dynamics of the Aztec Triple Alliance and their inability to conquer the Tlaxcala-PueblanValley. Price suggests that the inconclusive natllre of This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 288 ETHNOLOGY militaryoperationsmadehumansacrificean ideologicalmechanismto explainaway the failure of militaryconquestunderthe guise of ritualpursuits.Moteuczoma's characterization of the wars as ritualsfor obtainingcaptivesandprovidingmilitary trainingfor soldiersis seen as a strategieruseto directthe Spaniards'attentionaway from Aztec politicalandmilitaryweaknesses(Isaac1983). While there are many economic, social, and politicalexplanationsfor Aztec humansacrifice,no convincingevidenceis offeredas to why they shouldhave had a role in theseprocesses.Whyshouldthesesocialconditionsleadto humansacrifice? Theflowerywarsdidserveas a population-control mechanism-reducingpopulation pressuresin the Valleyof Mexicoby cullingyoungmalesthroughmilitaryfatalities (Prlce1978)-and undoubtedly relievedpressureon the landtenuresystem.Butwhy use humansacrificeas a justificationandmechanism? If populationor otherecological,economic,class, or politicalfactorsareto be demonstrated as causesof religiouspractices(e.g. humansacrifice),they shouldbe shown to have systematicrelationsin a cross-culturalsample. Argumentsabout systemic causes of humansacrificeand cannibalismbased on case materialsare unconvincingon methodologicalgrounds.The failureto establishrelationshipsof humansacriflcewith specificecologicalor socialvariablesleavesexplanationof the behavioropento characterization as socialpathology.Butif humansacrificeis to be explainedas an ecological,economicssocial, or politicalphenomenon,it mustbe demonstrated throughsystematiccross-cultural analysis. METHODS This studyused a subsample3(Winkelman1986, I990, 1992; Winkelmanand White1987)fromthe StandardCross-Cultural Sample(SCCS)(MurdockandWhite 1969) and its datasets for a systematiccross-cultural assessmentof ecologicaland social predictorsof humansacrifice.The subsampleincludedsocietiesof the major regionsof the worldandcovereda timespanfrom 1750B C. to the presentcentury. Assessmentsof humansacriflcewere derivedfrom a studywhich utilizedformal analysisof codedvariablesbasedon descriptivedataderivedfromethnographies (see Winkelman1992; Winkelmanand White 1987 for methodology). Culturally recognizedmagico-religiouspractitionerstatuseshad been previouslyassessedon social,political,religious,medical,cosmologicalandritualcharacteristics, including formsof sacrifice.The presentstudycompileddatafromthe individualmagicoreligiouspractitionerstatusesto determinethe societalincidenceof humansacrifice. HumanSacrzfce Variable The present researchassesses ritual humansacriE1ceratherthan sacrificial cannibalism.As therearedifferentformsof humansacrifice(e.g* socialcontrolvs. socialpathology)(TurnerandTurner1995),thepresentstudydistinguisheslegitimate human sacriElce(a form of normativebehavior, involving propitiation)from This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions AZTECHUMANSACRIFICE: THE ECOLOGICAL HYPOTHESIS 289 malevolenthumansacrifice(immoralpracticesattributedto sorcerersandwitches). These malevolentbeliefs (e.g., consumingbody parts, or the soul or spirit of the victim) apparentlyreflect attributionsratherthan actualbehaviorand are not the concern of this study. Legitimatehuman sacrifice is typically associatedwith magico-religious practitioners calledpriests.Priestsareelitesociopoliticalfunctionaries who also have centralroles in communalreligious activitiesassociatedwith propitiationandcalendricalagricultural rituals. Seven societies of the sample had humansacrifice (Ovimbundu,Ibo, Kafa, Roman,Marquesans,Atayal,andAztec). The Romanswere the only case in which the practicesof humansacrificewere not carriedout by priests. In Rome, human sacrificewas carriedout by a practitionercalled by terms translatedas witch, sorcerer,and necromancer.In formalcross-culturalanalysis,this practitionerwas classifiedas a shaman/healer, not a witch/sorcerer(Winkelman1986, 1992). This practitioner'suse of humansacrificeinvolvedeffortsto contactand influencethe spiritworld, makingit a formof propitiation.AnalysesdroppingRomansfromthe sampleproducedslightlyweakercorrelations,suggestingits appropriateinclusion here. Ecologicaland Social Variables The ecologicalhypothesisof humansacrifice,whichpostulateshumansto be a food resource utilized under adverse conditions, requiresassessing the factors affectingfood availability(agricultural potential,rainfall,naturaldisasters,risks of famine, adequacyof storage, trade networksfor food etc.). These ecological predictorswere derivedfrom previouslypublishedSCCS data sets on agriculture suitability(landslope suitabilityof soil, climate,andannualrainfall)(Pryor1986); subsistenceeconomyandsupportivepractices(includingintercommunity tradeas a food source,foodpreservation andstorage,food surpluses,andsubsistencevariables on contributionsof agriculture,hunting,fishing,gathering,anddomesticanimalsto the food supply)(Murdockand Morrow 1970);politicalorganization(Tudenand Marshall1972);politicalparticipation andpeace,includingdecision-making processes andpoliticalfission (Ember,Russett and Ember1993);measuresof war for land and resourcesand unpredictableresourceproblems,includingthreat of famine, weatheror pest disasters,andchronicresourceproblems(EmberandEmber1992); culturalcomplexity(MurdockandProvost1973);settlemetltpatternsandcommunity organization(Murdockand Wilson 1972); and religious variables(Whyte 1978; Murdock1967;WinkelmanandWhite 1987;Winkelman1992). RESULTS Datawere analyzedwithSYSTAT(1992) utilizingthe TablessCorrelation,and Multiple Regressionprograms.Measuresof culturalcomplexity (Murdockand Provost1973)wereusedto identifygeneralsocietalconditionsassociatedwithhuman This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 290 ETHNOLOGY sacrif1ce.These societiesare characterized by a varietyof socialconditionsandare typicallyat the mid-rangeof levels of culturalcomplexity.None of the original culturalcomplexitymeasures4(five-pointordinalscales) is signiElcantly correlated with humansacrifice;the measuresdo not linearlydistinguishsocietieswith human sacriE1ce fromthosewithout.All sevencasesof humansacrif1cearefoundin societies witha highrelianceuponagriculture andwithsedentaryandrelativelypermanent and fixedresidencepatterns.All of thesocietiesin thesamplewithhumansacrificerelied upon domesticatedanimals(bovines or smaller domesticatedanimals)for food sources,but they were not pastoralsocietiesanddid not heavilyuse milk products. Six of the cases were at the highestlevels of populationdensity. AgricultaralPotential Societieswith humansacrificehavea majorrelianceuponagricultureandin all but one agriculturecontributedmore than any other food source. Gathering contributedless than 10 per cent of the food supplyin all cases exceptone, but all reliedupon huntingandgatheringto some extent.Morethanhalf of the cases had good or betterlandslope, and all had fair to good land;five of seven cases were ratedas havingexcellentclimate,and all had high levels of averagerainfall(600 mm, most over 1,000 mm). NeitherPryor's(1986) agricultural potentialmeasures, nor recodesfocusedon poor climate,land, or agricultural potential,nor summary measuresof agriculturalsuitabilityhadsignificantcorrelationswith humansacrifice (Table 1). All of the societies with humansacrifice, however, had low or fair agriculturepotential(as opposedto good or excellent),andall werein the mid-range of measuresof agricultural potential. Table 1: Correlationof Agricultural Suitabilitywith HumanSacrifice IndependentVariables Land Slope Suitability of Soil Climate and Rainfall Total Suitability Lowest Sliitability Spearman'srho= p -.09 .11 .13 .04 -.07 (one-tailed) ns ns ns ns ns Food Supply and Famine Threat Measuresof the availabilityof a rangeof food suppliesandresources(Murdock andMorrow1970;EmberandEmber1992)were not significantlycorrelatedwith humansacrifice(Table2). All of the societieswith humansacrificehad meansof food storage,but flve of the seven experiencedseasonalor annualvariationin the food supply. Threeof the seven importedfood throughintercommunity trade,but four had inadequatefood suppliesto lastthroughdifficulttimes. The majorityof This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions AZTECHUMANSACRIFICE: THE ECOLOGICAL HYPOTHESIS 291 Table 2: Correlationof Pamine and Food Supply Variables with Human Sacrifice Independent Vanables Spearman's rho= p< (one-tailed) Huntingand Fishing DomesticAnimalFood Food Surplusvia Storage StorageAdequacy Season/Annual Variation FamineThreat WeatherThreat ChronicResourceProblems -.22 .07 .03 .00 .10 -.02 -.25 -.15 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ComputedVariables: Sllmma ry Food Threat Risk of Famine(CompositeMeasure) TotalMeatProtein TotalFood Food Problems -. 18 -.26 .06 -.07 -.10 ns ns ns ns ns societies with humansacriflcehad low threatsof famine, food problemsdue to weatheror pests, or chronicresourceproblems(one exceptionon each variable), with only one case of high risk of famine(Aztec).The food shortagedata(Ember andEmber1992) has limitedusefulnessbecauseof numerouscases of missingdata or unreliablecodings for the societieswith humansacrifice. Over half of all the ratingsfor these societies were unlcnownor lacked sufficientrater reliabilityto report.Giventheuncertainty aboutfoodreliabilityindicatedby the missingdata,this informationwas used as a partof an indexon food uncertainty.Caseswith missing data were placed on a scale (SummaryFood Threat)between no/low resource problemsand moderateresourceproblems.Humansacrifice had no significant relationshipto this recodenorto a compositefood resourcescale basedon the sum of the recodesof resourcevariables(Table2). A similarscale was computedon faminerisk, utilizingMurdockandMorrow's(1970) variablesfor food supplyand storageadequacyin combination withtheEmbers'variables.Thiscompositemeasure of famine risk was nonsigniElcantly associatedwith humansacrifice(Spearman's rho=-.26), but in the oppositedirectionpredictedby the ecologicalhypothesis. SammaryFood Ecology Measures Themeasuresreportedabovewerereducedin categories,weighted,andsummed intothreemajorscales assessingfood resources:totalmeatprotein(contributions of hunting,fishing, domesticanimals,largeherbivores);total food (meatproteinplus food import, total agriculturalpotential,and food storage adequacy);and food problems(threatof famine,weather,resourceproblems,faminerisk,poorland,poor This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 292 ETHNOLOGY climate, and poor agriculture potential). None of these summary scales had a significant correlation with human sacriflce, thus providing little evidence in favor of the ecological hypothesis. The nonsignificantnegative correlations with weather threat, resource problems, and risk of famine suggest the opposite relationship between food supply and human sacrifice of that predicted by the ecological hypothesis. Popalation Density and Population Pressure Murdock and Provost (1973) provide a measureof populationdensity rankedat five levels, from less than one person per squaremile (psm) through 100 psm. The societies with human sacrifice all have higher levels of population density, with all but one society with human sacrifice at 26+ psm. There was a significant Pearson correlationof r= .40 (p < .01) with a binary recode (population < /= vs. > 26 psm). Population pressure measures were derived by dividing population density by food resource measures and the inverse of famine risk and food problem variables. These assessments emphasize the pressure created by large populations with serious food resource limitations or high famine risks. Human sacrifice was significantly and positively associated with the population pressure measures based upon total food supplies (r-.49, p<.OO1), adequacy of food storage (r=.S1, p<.OO1), and meat protein (r= .29, p < .05), while the populationpressuremeasuresbased on a summary of food problems and resourcesSand famine risk were positively but nonsignificantly correlated with human sacrifice (Table 3). Table3: Correlationof PopulationPressurewith HumanSacnfice IndependentVanables Population Density Recode Population Pressure Based on Storage Adequacy Population Pressure Based on Meat Protein Population Pressure Based on Total Food Popiation Pressure Based on Famine Risk Population Pressure Based on Food Problems Environmental Pearson's r= .40 .51 29 49 20 .22 p < (one-tailed) .01 .000 05 .001 ns ns Circamscription One cause of Aztec cannibalismpostulatedby Harner is environmental circumscription.The lack of availabilityof good agricultllralland in nearbyareas furtherexacerbatedfood shortages The measuresof Ember and Ember (1992) provideproxy assessmentsof environmental circumscription in their measuresof warfarefor the seizureof resourcesandthe use of landobtainedin warfare.These originalvariableswere recodedto binaryvariablesreflectingthe absenee/presence This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions AZTECHUMANSACRIFICE: THE ECOLOGICAL HYPOTHESIS 293 Table4: Correlationsof Environmental Circumscription with HumanSacnfice Independent Vanables InternalWarfor Land ExternalWarfor Land OverallWarfor Land InternalWarfor Resources ExternalWarfor Resources OverallWarfor Resources OverallWarfor LandandResources Spearmansrho- p .44 .20 .30 .37 .17 .30 .35 (one-tailed) .007 ns .04 .02 ns .05 .02 of such conditions, and the individualsummarymeasuresfor overall land and resourceseizurethroughwarwere summedinto an overallmeasureof war for land and resources.The individualsummarymeasuresof internalwarfarefor land and resourcesas well as combinedmeasures,werepositivelyandsignificantlyassociated with humansacrifice(Table4). The measureof localpoliticalfissionwas also used as an assessmentof (a lack of) environmental circumscription. Whiletherewas no significantassociationwith local politicalfission, six of the seven societies with humansacrificewerewithoutlocalpoliticalfission,consistentwiththe hypothesisof environmental circumscription. Bothpopulation pressure(basedon storageadequacy) andthe overallmeasureof warfor landandresourceshaveindependently significant contributionsto the predictionof humansacrifice (populationpressurer=.51, p<.001; overall war for land and resourcesr=.35, p<.02; multiple R=.62, p < .006). Butlimitedexplainedvariance(38 percent)indicatesthatotherfactorsare also responsiblefor humansacrifice. Thehypothesisof generalsocialexplanations of humansacrif1ceis not, however, supportedby the present research.Inclusionin the regressionequationof ten measuresof culturalcomplexity(MurdockandProvost1973;see note 4), including variablesfoundpredictingmagico-religious practicesin previousstudies(Winkelman 1986, 1992) (fixity of residence, agricultureSpolitical integration,and social stratification),providedno significantincreasesin explainedvariancein human sacrificebeyondthataccountedfor by the ecologicalvariables(populationpressure andenvironmental circumscription). This rejectsthe notionthathumansacrificeis a generaladaptation to conditionsof increasingsocietalcomplexity.Butthe relatively limitedexplanationof varianceby theecologicalmeasures(38 percent)indicatesthat thereare still substantialdeterminants to be identif1ed. The presentstudy'sf1ndingsillustratethe relationof sacriElcein general,and human sacrifice in particular to socioeconomicconditions:the absence of all sacriiiceonly in huntingandgatheringsocieties;humansacrificefoundin complex ratherthansimpleagriculxralsocietiessbutnot in pastoralsocieties;andthe lackof a linearassociationof humansacrificewith measuresof complexity,with human sacrificeoccurringin the mid-rangeof culturalcomplexity.Societieswith human sacrificealwayshad priests,but neverhad shamans(as determinedin Winkelman This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 294 ETHNOLOGY 1986 and 1992). HumansacriElceas a religiousactivityis relatedto otherreligious andsocial conditionsO Furtherassessmentof the relationshipbetweenhumansacrificeand religious beliefs was based on Murdock's(1967) variableon high gods being presentand important,and Whyte's(1978) dataon the relativestatusof males and femalesin religiousbeliefs. Manyof the societieswith humansacrificehad religiousbeliefs characterized by the relativeequalityof maleandfemaleroles, andsix of sevenhad high gods absentor unimportant. The unimportance of high gods in these complex societies with humansacrificeis inconsistentwith generalfindingsof high gods associatedwithsocialcomplexity(Swanson1960;Davis 1971;Peregrine1996).This suggeststhatsincehumansacrificewas associatedwithsocietieswithouthierarchically integratedreligioussystems,humansacrificemaycreatereligiousintegration,as suggestedby Isaac(1983)andPrice(1978). MurdockandWhyte'soriginalvariables wererecodedintobinaryvariablesrepresenting the lackof highgods andthe relative equalityof male andfemalereligiousfigures(gods and spirits,mythicalfounders, and witches). Some of these individualmeasureswere positivelyand significantly correlatedwith humansacrifice(Table5). A summarymeasureof low religious hierarchy(computedby summationof the binary recodes) had the strongest correlation(Spearman'srho=.49, p<.001). Inclusionof this summaryreligious measureof low religious hierarchyin multipleregressionalong with the two ecologicalmeasures(population pressureandenvironmental circumscription [overall war for landandresources])accountedfor significantadditionalvariancein human sacrifice(multipleR=.72, R2=.S1, F ratio=14.4, 3, 41 df, p<.000, 13 per cent increase; 51 per cent total varianceexplainedwith inclusion of the religious measure).The low religious hierarchymeasureenteredthe equationbefore the environmental circumscription measure,suggestingits strongerpredictivepower. Table 5: Correlationof Religious Variableswith Human Sacrifice IndependentVariables High Gods (present vs. unimportant/absent) Gods and Spiritss Both Male and Female Mythical Founders, Both Male and Female Witches, Both Male and Female Low Religious Cohesion (computed) Spearman'srho= .16 .42 .30 .32 .49 p< (one-tailed) ns .004 .05 .04 .001 AZTECHUMANSACRIFICEIN CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE The cross-culturalanalyses support the general hypothesis of ecological contributions to theexplanation of humansacrifice.Someof theecologicalconditions Harnerhypothesizesas leadingto Aztec cannibalismare correlatedwith human sacrificecross-culturally.In contrastto Harner'shypothesis,however,all societies with humansacriElcehaddomesticated food animals,butthe reductionof wild game This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions AZTECHUMANSACRIFICE: THE ECOLOGICAL HYPOTHESIS 295 is suggested by these agriculturalsocieties with high populationdensity still dependinguponhuntingandfishingin theoveralldiet.Measurements of cropfailures andfaminesarenotsignificantlyassociatedwithhumansacrifice,andthe nonsignificant correlationswith resourceproblemsand faminerisks are the oppositeof the ecologicalprediction.The Aztecs,however,werehigh on manyof thesemeasures. The signiElcantcorrelationsof humansacrificewith populationpressureand environmental circumscription assessments(warfarefor larldandresources)support the ecological hypothesis. Prices argumentthat Harner's hypothesis is not sufficientlydistinguishedfrom famineor lack of food is addressedin this crossculturalanalysis.The lack of correlationof humansacrificewith famine, resource adequacy,and food threatsindicatesthat the causes of humansacrificeare not a directfunctionof food availabilityper se. Societieswith humansacrificeappearto havethe oppositerelationshipto food supply,takingstepsto assurefood adequacy throughfood storageand importation.The significantpredictionof humansacrifice by specific ecologicalvariablesclarifiesthe generalecologicalconditionsHarner hypothesizesas leadingto Aztec cannibalism.The ecological effects on human sacrificeare not directlyfromthe availabilityof resources(food scarcity),but from populationpressure.The significantlystrongerpredictionof humansacrificeby populationpressure,ratherthanpopulationdensity,indicatesthatthe causesarenot the sheer numberof people per se, but ratherthe stressfulconditionsthat density createsunderresourcescarcityandunreliability. But ecologicalfactorsalone are insufficient,as well over half of the variance remainsto be explained.Inadditionto theseecologicalinfluences,measuresof a lack of religioushierarchyare also significantlyassociatedwith the incidenceof human sacrifice. Religiousbeliefs do reflect societalconditionswhich apparentlyhave a significanteffect in motivatingthe adoptionof humansacrifice.The correlations alonedo notrevealthepsychosocialfunctionsof the religiousactivities.Theysuggest thathumansacrificeis a mechanismfor achievinga formof religiousintegrationin societieslackingintegrativehierarchical systemsof belief.Themotivationsfor human sacrificeandfor cannibalismareillustratedby the broaderpsychocultural dynamics of sacrificewith respectto intergroupsocial relations.The psychosocialeffects of cannibalismare suggestedby the Aztecs' dynamicsof in-groupand out-group relationshipswith respectto consumptionfor sacrifice- whetherone is consumed (victim)or co-consumer.Since the Aztecsdid not consumemembersof their own group,unitingwith them in wars for sacrificialvictims and cannibalisticpractices was a mechanismfor establishingin-groupmembership.The functionsof sacriflce in creatingan integrationof peopleare illustratedby its servingas a mechanismfor permittingclass mobilityof the warriorandhis relatives.This extensionof the right to consumehumanflesh to family memberssignals a very basic level of social * fi nc uslveness. These systematiccorrelationsof humansacrificewith ecologicaland religious conditionsillustratethatsuchpracticesshouldbe understoodin the contextof social determinantsof behavior,ratherthanas forms of social pathologyand aberrance. This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 296 ETHNOLOGY in theAmericanSouthwestled TurnerandTurner's(1995)assessmentof cannibalism them to reject explanationsbased on social pathology.They (Turnerand Turner 1995:13) point insteadto violent social control, possibly initiatedby socially of humansacrificewithpopulationpressure t Thecorrelation pathologicalindividuals. indicates that populationdynamics are importantcontributoryfactors to the developmentof thesepractices.Thesesocialdynamicsproducinghumansacrificeare not, however,a generalfunctionof increasingsocialcomplexity. CONCLUSIONS Harner'soriginalhypothesisof ecologicalcausesof Aztechumansacrificeand cannibalismreceivespartialbut significantsupportfrom the presentcross-cultural of humansacrificethatremain analysis.Thereare, however,additionaldeterminants social, andpoliticalfactors economic of systemic to be identified.The hypotheses involvedin the complexof Aztecwarfare,humansacriElce,andcannibalismsuggest directionsfor futureworkto identifyadditionalfactorsthatpredisposesocietiesto humansacrifice.While discussionsof Aztec humansacrificeandcannibalismhave typicallyemphasizedthese as uniquefeaturesof Aztec society, the cross-cultural analysespresentedhere suggestthatthe Aztecs typify patternsof humansacrifice found cross-culturally.But the Aztecs might have been particularlymotivatedto consumehumanflesh as a consequeneeof ecologicaland populationpressures.In eomparisonto other societies with humansacrifice,the Aztecs were extremein severalmeasures:the only humansacrificesocietyin this samplewith a highrisk of famine; the highest on several measuresof populationpressure;in the highest categoryof populationdensity(over 500 personsper squaremile); andthe highest levels of overallwarfarefor land and resources.So while Aztec humansacrifice conformsto typical cross-culturalpatterns,the magnitudeof their sacrifice and cannibalismmay reflecttheirextremeconditionson manyecologicalvariables. NOTES 1. I tharlkDavid Jacobs for his encouragementto pursue this research project. I also thank him and Doug White, Michael Harner, ChristyTurner, Carol Ember, and Rob Le Veille for helpful suggestions, and Cindy Winkelman for her assistance with data analysis. 2. Acosta Saignes 1950; Benson and Boone 1984; Bergmann 1992; Boal 1982; Brown 1991; Gohain 1977; Gonzalez Torres 1985; Green 1975; Hamerton-Kelly1987; Hogg 1966; Hughes 1991; Levenson 1993; Lewis 1986; Loeb 1964; Macoby 1982; Roman Berrelleza and Alberto 1990; Sagan 1974; Sugiyama 1995; Taahill 1975; Tierney 1989. 3. A 25 per cent stratiEledrandom45-society subsample of the SCCS was used in the present study. Societies in the subsampleinclude: Africa: Nama Hottentot,SKungBushmen, Ovimbundu,Ibo; CircumMediterranean:Wolof, Fulani, Pur, KafaflAmhara, Tuareg, Babylonians, Romans, Kurd; Eurasia: Samoyed, Toda, Kazak, Garo, Vietnamese, Semang, Tanala,Japanese,Chukohee;InsularPaciElc:Iban, Alor, Kirnam, Lesu, Pentecost, Marquesans, Trukese, Atayal; North America: Montagnias, Kaska, Twana, Paiute, Hidatsa, Creek, Zuni, Aztec; South America: Bribri, Callinago, Saramacca Jivaro, Tupinamba, Cayua, Mapuche. Societies with legitimate human sacrifice are: Ovimbundu, Ibo, Kafa, Romaxl,Marquesans, Atayal, and Aztec. This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions AZTECHUMANSACRIFICE: THE ECOLOGICAL HYPOTHESIS 297 4. Variablesincludedare: writing and records, fixity of residence, agriculture,urbanization, technologicalspecialization,landtransport, money,populationdensity,politicalintegration,andsocial stratification . BIBLIOGRAPHY AcostaSaignes,M. 1950. Tlacaxipeualiztli; un complejomesoamericano entrelos caribes.Caracas. Arens,W. 1979. The Man-Eating Myth:Anthropology andAnthropophagy. New York. Benson,E., andE. H. Boone(eds.). 1984. RitualHumanSacrificein Mesoamerica.WashingtonDC. Bergmann,M. S. 1992. Inthe Shadowof Moloch:TheSacrificeof ChildrenandIts Impacton Western Religions.New York. Boal, B. M. 1982. The Konds:HumanSacrificeand ReligiousChange.Wilts. Bourguignon,E. 1976. Possession.San Francisco. Bourguignon, E., andT. Evascu.1977. AlteredStatesof Consciousness withina GeneralEvolutionary Perspective:A Holocultural Analysis.BehaviorScienceResearch12(3):197-216. Brown,S. S. 1991. Late Carthaginian ChildSacrificeand SacrificialMonumentsin TheirMediterraneanContext.Sheffield. Davis, W. 1971. Societal Complexityand the Nature of PrimitiveMan's Conceptionof the Supernatural. Ph.D. dissertation,Universityof NorthCarolina.ChapelHill. Ember,C., andM. Ember.1992.Warfare,Aggression,andResourceProblems:Cross-Cultural Codes. BehaviorScienceResearch26(1-4):169-226. Ember,C., B. Russett,andM. Ember.1993. PoliticalParticipation andPeace:Cross-Cultural Codes. Cross-Cultural Research27(1&2):97-145. Gohain,B. C. 1977. HumanSacnficeandHead-Hunting in North-Eastern lndia. Gauhati. GonzalezTorres,Y. 1985. E1sacriElcio humanoentrelos mexicas.Mexico. Goodall,J. 1977. InfantKillingand Cannibalism in Free-LivingChimpanzeesof the GombeStream Reserve.AnimalBehaviorMonographs1:161-311. Green,A. R. W. 1975. The Role of HumanSacrificein the AncientNearEast. Missoula. Hamerton-Kelly, R. (ed.) 1987. ViolentOrigins:WalterBurkert,ReneGirardandJonathanZ. Smith on RitualKillingand CulturalFormation.Stanford. Harner,M. 1970. PopulationPressureandthe SocialEvolutionof Agriculturalists. SouthwestJournal of Anthropology26(1):67-86. 1977a. The EcologicalBasisfor AztecSacrifice.AmericanEthnologist4:117-35. 1977b.The Enigmaof Aztec Sacrifice.NaturalHistory86(4):47-51. Hassig, R. 1990. AztecWarfare.HistoryToday40:17-24. Hogg, G. 1966. Cannibalism andHumanSacrifice.New York. Hughes,D. D. 1991. HumanSacrificein AncientGreece.New York. Isaac, B. 1983. The Aztec "PloweryWar":A GeopoliticalExplanation.Journalof Anthropological Research39:415-32. Levenson,J. 1993. The Deathand Resurrectionof the BelovedSon: The Transformation of Child Sacnfilcein Judaismand Christianity. New Haven. Lewis, I. M. 1986. Religionin Context:Cultsand Charisma.New York. Loeb, E. M. 1964. The BloodSacrificeComplex.New York. Maccoby,H. 1982. The SacredExecutioner:HumanSacrificeandthe Legacyof Guilt.New York. Murdock,G. P. 1967. Ethnographic Atlas.Pittsburgh. Murdock,G. P., andD. Morrow.1970.SubsistenceEconomyandSupportivePractices:Cross-Cultural Codes 1. Ethnology9:302-30. Murdock,G. P., and C. Provost.1973. Measurement of CulturalComplexity.Ethnology12:379-92. Murdock,G. P., and D. White. 1969. StandardCross-Cultural Sample.Ethnology8:329-69. Murdock,G. P., andS. Wilson.1972. SettlementPatternsandCommunityOrganization: Cross-Cultural Codes 3. Ethnology11:254-95. This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 298 ETHNOLOGY Ortiz de Montellano, B. 1978. Aztec Cannibalism:An Ecological Necessity? Science 200:611-17. 1990. Aztec Medicine, Health, and Nutrition. New Brunswick. Peregrine, P. 1996. The Birth of the Gods Revisited: A PartialReplication of Guy Swanson's (1960) Cross-CulturalStudy of Religion. Cross-CulturalResearch 30(1):84-112. Price, B. 1978. Demystification, Enriddlement,and Aztec Cannibalism:A Materialist Rejoinder to Harner. American Ethrlologist5(1):98-115. Pryor, P. 1986. The Adoption of Agriculture: Some Theoretical and Empirical Evidence. American Anthropologist 88:879-97. Roman Berrelleza, R., and J. Alberto. 1990. Sacrificio de ninos en el Templo Mayor. Mexico DP. Sagan, E. 1974. Camibalism: Human Aggression and CulturalForm. New York. Sugiyama, S. 1995. Mass HulmanSacrifice and Symbolism of the Feathered Serpent Pyramid in Teotihuacan, Mexico. Tempe. Swanson, G. 1960. Birth of the Gods. Ann Arbor. SYSTAT for Windows. Version 5 Edition. 1992. Evanston, IL. Tamahill, R. 1975. Flesh and Blood: A History of the Cannibal Complex. New York. Tierney, P. 1989. The Highest Altar: The Story of Human Sacrifice. New York. Tuden, A., and C. Marshall. 1972. Political Organization:Cross-CulturalCodes. Ethnology 11:436-64. Turner, C., and J. Turner. 1995. Camibalism in the Prehistoric American Southwest: Occurrence, Taphonomy, Explanationand Suggestions for StandardizedWorld Definitions. Anthropological Science 103(1):1-22. Winkelman, M. 1986. Magico-Religious PractitionerTypes and Socioeconomic Conditions. Behavioral Science Research 20(1-4):17-46. 1990. Shaman and Other "Magico-Religious" Healers: A Cross-CulturalStudy of Their Origins, Nature, and Social Transformations.Ethos 18:308-52. 1992. Shamans, Priests and Witches. A Cross-Cultural Study of Magico-Religious Practitioners. Tempe. Winkelman, M., and D. White. 1987. A Cross-CulturalStudy of Magico-Religious Practitionersand ASC: Data Base. HRAP Research Series in QuantitativeCross-culturalData, Vol. 3, eds. D. Levinson and R. Wagner. New York. Whyte, M. K. 1978. Cross-CulturalCodes Dealing with the Relative Status of Women. Ethnology 17:21 1-37. This content downloaded from 129.252.86.83 on Mon, 10 Mar 2014 16:32:37 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions