Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 2445–2453 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev Review Chocolate and the brain: Neurobiological impact of cocoa flavanols on cognition and behavior Alexander N. Sokolov a,∗ , Marina A. Pavlova b , Sibylle Klosterhalfen a , Paul Enck a a Department of Internal Medicine VI: Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Research Division, University of Tübingen Medical School, D 72076 Tübingen, Germany b Developmental Cognitive and Social Neuroscience Unit, Department of Pediatric Neurology and Developmental Medicine, Children’s Hospital, University of Tübingen Medical School, D 72076 Tübingen, Germany a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 18 February 2013 Received in revised form 17 June 2013 Accepted 18 June 2013 Keywords: Cocoa flavanols Chocolate Antioxidant Anti-inflammatory Neurogenesis Angiogenesis Age- and disease-related decline Neurocognition Neuromodulation Neuroprotection a b s t r a c t Cocoa products and chocolate have recently been recognized as a rich source of flavonoids, mainly flavanols, potent antioxidant and anti-inflammatory agents with established benefits for cardiovascular health but largely unproven effects on neurocognition and behavior. In this review, we focus on neuromodulatory and neuroprotective actions of cocoa flavanols in humans. The absorbed flavonoids penetrate and accumulate in the brain regions involved in learning and memory, especially the hippocampus. The neurobiological actions of flavanols are believed to occur in two major ways: (i) via direct interactions with cellular cascades yielding expression of neuroprotective and neuromodulatory proteins that promote neurogenesis, neuronal function and brain connectivity, and (ii) via blood-flow improvement and angiogenesis in the brain and sensory systems. Protective effects of long-term flavanol consumption on neurocognition and behavior, including age- and disease-related cognitive decline, were shown in animal models of normal aging, dementia, and stroke. A few human observational and intervention studies appear to corroborate these findings. Evidence on more immediate action of cocoa flavanols remains limited and inconclusive, but warrants further research. As an outline for future research on cocoa flavanol impact on human cognition, mood, and behavior, we underscore combination of functional neuroimaging with cognitive and behavioral measures of performance. © 2013 Published by Elsevier Ltd. Contents 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Cocoa flavanols in the brain signaling cascades . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Neuroprotective action of cocoa flavanols in aging and neurological disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.1. Animal studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.2. Human population studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.3. Clinical studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Neuromodulation of cognition, mood, learning, and memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.1. Animal studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4.2. Human studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Concluding remarks and future directions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2445 2446 2447 2447 2448 2448 2449 2449 2449 2450 2452 2452 1. Introduction ∗ Corresponding author at: Department of Internal Medicine VI: Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Research Division, University of Tübingen Medical School, Osianderstr. 5, D 72076 Tübingen, Germany. Tel.: +49 7071 29 89118; fax: +49 7071 29 4382. E-mail address: alexander.sokolov@uni-tuebingen.de (A.N. Sokolov). 0149-7634/$ – see front matter © 2013 Published by Elsevier Ltd. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.013 Cocoa products and especially, chocolate has taken a special place in our daily life and culture. This food of the gods as tells its Latin name Theobroma cacao given by the noted Swedish nosologist Carl Linnaeus in 1753, has been ennobled in many countries around the globe as a curative drug, a culinary delight, and even a currency for commodity trading, retaining its appeal over the centuries. No 2446 A.N. Sokolov et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 2445–2453 other natural product but chocolate has ever been viewed as having a positive effect on a wide variety of health conditions ranging from intestinal and female complaints, fever, and cardiovascular diseases to promotion of strength before military and sexual conquests (Wilson, 2010; Wolfe and Shazzie, 2005). Reports on chocolate’s health benefits are dated back as far as Aztec and Maya medical practice (e.g., Hurst et al., 2002) and ever since, anecdotal evidence has been abundant on chocolate effects on health. Only by the end of the 20th century, however, claims on supposed health benefits of chocolate have increasingly drawn a scientific interest in cocoa products and chocolate, which eventually resulted in an approval by the European Food Safety Agency (EFSA, 2012) of a health claim for dark chocolate with high flavanol content as to its impact on “maintenance of normal endothelium-dependent vasodilation”. Most studies so far have been conducted on the effects of chocolate intake on the cardiovascular system, skin, cholesterol concentrations, and the release of neurotransmitters anandamide and serotonin, and on the health-related properties of highquality dark chocolate, containing the stimulants theobromine and caffeine (Lamuela-Raventós et al., 2005; Katz et al., 2011). Dark chocolate also comprises high concentrations of flavanols (a flavonoid subgroup, mainly epicatechin; Whiting, 2001) known as potent antioxidative agents. While some work has been done on the influence of theobromine and caffeine on mood and cognition (e.g., Smit and Rogers, 2000; Smit et al., 2004; Smit and Blackburn, 2005; Nehlig, 2010), the impact of cocoa flavanols on human cognitive and affective function, executive control and behavior has yet to be determined. In accord with accumulating evidence for enhancing effects of chocolate consumption on cognitive function, Messerli (2012) reports, as an occasional note, a strong positive correlation between chocolate intake per capita and the number of Nobel laureates in various countries. In contrast to potential effects on cognition and behavior, evidence-based benefits of cocoa and chocolate consumption for cardiovascular system are well established and include endothelium-dependent vasodilation recently found to contribute to normal blood flow (Engler et al., 2004; Hooper et al., 2012; Kay et al., 2006; Grassi et al., 2005, 2008). Cardiovascular health has been closely linked to cognitive performance (e.g., DeCarli, 2012). Animal studies have shown that the absorbed flavonoids directly interact with a number of cellular and molecular targets in the brain, exerting pronounced antioxidative effects and improving brain tissue and function in the regions mainly implicated in learning, memory, and cognition (Andrés-Lacueva et al., 2005; Passamonti et al., 2005; Vauzour et al., 2008). This suggests a potential neuromodulatory and neuroprotective role for cocoa flavanols and their significance for cognitive and affective function, executive control and behavior. However, only few human studies so far have specifically addressed neurobiological, cognitive, affective and behavioral effects of flavanol-rich cocoa products. The present review focuses on analysis of the existing evidence on potential neuromodulatory and neuroprotective actions of cocoa flavanols in humans. Our analysis highlights further ways in investigation of the impact of flavanol-rich cocoa products on neurocognition and behavior. 2. Cocoa flavanols in the brain signaling cascades The flavanol monoisomers epicatechin and catechin are the predominant flavonoid compounds in cocoa, with the 2-phenyl-3,4-dihydro-2H-chromen-3-ol as underlying skeleton. These monomers represent the base molecules for concatenated oligomers, the proanthocyanidins. Antioxidant properties of flavanols are chemically mediated through oxidation of two aromatic hydroxyl groups to a quinone (Bors and Michel, 2002). In addition, flavanols foster antioxidant system through modulation of enzymatic activity (Stevenson and Hurst, 2007; Mann et al., 2009). Flavanols occur in high concentrations in beverages such as green tea and red wine, fruits and berries (e.g., apple skin, grapes, pears, blueberry), vegetables (tomatoes, soy, and olives), and, especially, cocoa (Manach et al., 2004; Neveu et al., 2010; Scalbert et al., 2011; Sies et al., 2012). The flavonoid contents in cocoa products and chocolate differ substantially depending on the cocoa variety (in some beans, amounting up to 20%), geographic origin, cultivating, agricultural and postharvest practices, and manufacturing (Wollgast and Anklam, 2000; Niemenak et al., 2006). In the Dutch population, chocolate contributes to about 20% of the total flavonoid intake in adults, with an even higher percentage in children (Arts et al., 1999). In American diet, chocolate represents the third top source of antioxidants after fruits and vegetables (255, 233, and 104 mg/day, respectively; Vinson et al., 2006). Also in the French adult population with the total daily dietary polyphenol intake of 1.2 g, including 99 mg catechins, cocoa products account for the third major source of epicatechin (17%) after green tea (28%) and apples (24%; Pérez-Jiménez et al., 2011). Yet human and animal studies on neuroprotective properties of flavonoids, especially in preventing cognitive decline, have mostly examined plant-derived substances other than cocoa flavanols (e.g., Macready et al., 2009). Flavonoid-rich pine extracts such as Gingko biloba are reported to delay the onset of memory loss, dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease (Weinmann et al., 2010), but the evidence remains controversial (Birks and Grimley Evans, 2009). Animal studies show that flavanols and their metabolites can cross the blood–brain barrier, inducing beneficial effects on the brain tissue and function (angio- and neurogenesis, changes in neuron morphology) and stimulating widespread blood circulation in the brain (Vauzour et al., 2008). The most common flavanol found in cocoa, epicatechin (Whiting, 2001), is rapidly absorbed in humans and is detectable in blood plasma already 30 min after intake. The epicatechin levels peak 2–3 h after intake, exhibiting a strong positive correlation with the dose of ingested chocolate (Richelle et al., 1999), and return to baseline by 6–8 h after consumption. The possibility of flavanols and metabolites to penetrate and accumulate in the brain regions mainly related to learning and memory, suggests they may exert a direct positive impact on the brain, including cognition and neuroprotection (Nehlig, 2013). Neurobiological impact of flavanols on the brain, learning, memory, and cognition are believed to occur in two major ways (Fig. 1). First, flavonoids can specifically interact within a number of cellular signaling pathways, primarily with mitogen-activated protein (MAPK), extracellular-signal-regulated (ERK) and phosphoinositide 3-kinase (PI3-kinase/Akt) signaling cascades. These cascades trigger gene expression and protein synthesis for maintaining long-term potentiation (LTP) and establishing long-term memories (Kelleher et al., 2004). Flavonoids modulate the transcription factors engaged in signal transduction through protein-kinase inhibition (Goyarzu et al., 2004), and promote the expression of brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) that is critical for neurogenesis, also in adult animals, synaptic growth and neuron survival, especially in the learning- and memory-related brain regions such as the hippocampus and subventricular zone (Kim et al., 2006; Valente et al., 2009). Second, flavonoids facilitate production of the signaling molecule nitric oxide, which inhibits the incidence of atheromatous plaque adhesion molecules causing inflammation (Gonzalez-Gallego et al., 2007), and importantly, improves vascular endothelial function by relaxing the smooth muscle tissue of blood vessels (e.g., Heiss et al., 2003; Schroeter et al., 2006). In this way, flavanol-rich cocoa can impose vasodilation in a nitric oxidedependent way both at the cardiovascular and peripheral levels. This in turn results in enhanced cerebral blood flow and blood perfusion throughout the central and peripheral nervous system (Fisher et al., 2003, 2006; Hollenberg et al., 2009), affording better A.N. Sokolov et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 2445–2453 2447 flavonoids may effectively protect against neural and oxidative damage eventually giving rise to neurological disease, cognitive and functional decline. Cocoa flavanols 3.1. Animal studies MAPK, ERK, PI3 signaling gene expression, protein synthesis expression of BDNF neurogenesis, synapc growth, neuron survival nitric oxide synthesis inflammaon vascular endothelian funcon, angiogenesis vasodilaon (heart, CNS, sensory systems) LTP, memory formaon oxygen, glucose supply neurocognive funcons neuroprotecon Fig. 1. Schematic of putative neurobiological actions of cocoa flavanols; small upand downward inclined arrows show up and down regulation of function, respectively. The absorbed flavanols either directly interact with cellular cascades (left, green), yielding expression of neuroprotective and neuromodulatory proteins that promote neurogenesis, neuronal function and brain connectivity, or act by improving central and peripheral blood flow and angiogenesis in the brain and sensory systems (right, yellow). Note the two action pathways may cross-talk, contributing to more than one function. For example, improved neurogenesis, synaptic growth, and neuron survival may promote neuroprotection, while neurocognition may benefit from increased oxygen and glucose supply due to vasodilation. LTP, long-term potentiation; mitogen-activated protein (MAPK), extracellular-signalregulated (ERK), and phosphoinositide 3-kinase (PI3-kinase/Akt) cascades; BDNF, brain derived neurotophic factor; CNS, central nervous system. (For interpretation of the references to color in the figures throughout the article, please consult the web version of this article.) supply of oxygen and glucose to the neurons and removal of waste metabolites in the brain and sensory systems (e.g., blood delivery to the retina; Huber et al., 2006; Kalt et al., 2010). In addition, animal models indicate that cocoa flavonoid administration stimulates angiogenesis in the hippocampus (van Praag et al., 2007). In a nutshell, the multiple neurobiological actions of cocoa flavonoids in enhancing cognitive function and behavior may be attributed to the expression of neuroprotective and neuromodulatory proteins that increase the number and connectivity of neurons, and improve neuronal function. The other class of putative mechanisms is related to the effects on vascular function that through enhanced blood perfusion both in the brain and peripheral nervous system including sensory systems, may improve neuro- and angiogenesis (Williams and Spencer, 2012), cognitive function, and executive control. However, while the long-term actions of cocoa flavanols in counteracting oxidative stress, neuroinflammation and neurodegeneration appear to be better understood, the mechanisms of more immediate neuromodulatory effects on cognition and behavior remain unclear. 3. Neuroprotective action of cocoa flavanols in aging and neurological disease A number of health conditions in typical and atypical aging (Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, Parkinson’s disease) and acute neurological conditions such as stroke have been associated with disturbances of cerebral blood flow and oxidative stress (McGeer and McGeer, 2003; Hirsch et al., 2005). Potent antioxidant action and endothelium-dependent vasodilation capacity of Several animal models of normal and pathological aging examined the effects of high-flavanol cocoa on the onset of age-related cognitive deficits. Rozan et al. (2007) assessed the preventive effects of Acticoa powder, a cocoa polyphenolic extract, on free radical production by leucocytes in rats following heat exposure and protective effects of the cocoa powder on subsequent cognitive performance. Either high-flavanol cocoa powder (22.9 mg/kg/day) or vitamin E, as the antioxidant reference, was administered orally to rats for 14 days prior to heat exposure. The day after heat exposure, free radical production in rats treated with cocoa powder or vitamin E was significantly reduced compared to controls. Unlike controls, cocoa powder and vitamin E-treated rats discriminated between active and inactive levers in a light extinction paradigm. Throughout testing, the treated rats also exhibited decreased escape latencies for reaching the hidden platform in the Morris water maze. The results suggest that the daily oral administration of Acticoa powder or vitamin E counteract the overproduction of free radicals, and thereby protect rats from cognitive impairments caused by heat exposure. Bisson et al. (2008) administered orally Acticoa powder to rats (as they grew from 15 to 27 months of age) at the dose of 24 mg/kg/day. In aged rats, high-flavanol cocoa improved cognitive performance in light extinction and water maze paradigms, increased lifespan and preserved high urinary free dopamine levels. Acticoa powder appears to retard age-related impairments, including cognitive decline in normal aging and neurodegenerative diseases. Converging evidence comes from animal models of neurodegenerative diseases (such as Alzheimer’s disease), which make use of polyphenol-enriched diets. Fernández-Fernández et al. (2012) examined an ability of a diet rich in polyunsaturated fatty acids and polyphenols from dry fruits and cocoa (called LMN diet, a patented product known to induce hippocampal neurogenesis in adult mice) for counteracting the age-related impairment and neuropathology in wild type and transgenic mice (Tg2576 genotype with over-expression of the human APP gene carrying the Swedish mutation, K670N:M671L), an animal model of Alzheimer’s disease. At the age of 13 months, once the amyloid plaques (A) were formed, both mice types received LMN diet for further five months. In the last two months, they performed on a behavioral test battery. Overall, both typically aging wild and, to a greater degree, Tg mice exhibited reduction in sensorimotor reflexes, exploratory behavior in the hole board, activity in the elevated plus-maze, ambulation in the home cage during the dark phase, and in spatial learning in the Morris water maze. The diet did not impact the detrimental effects observed in sensorimotor reflexes, but did clearly reverse the behavioral effects of both aging and Tg genotype. This behavioral improvement correlated with a 70% increase in cellular proliferation in the subventricular zone of the brain, rather than with a decrease of amyloid plaques. In contrast, LMN diet administered at an age of 10 months (before the plaques occurred) led to a decreasing tendency of soluble and fibrillar A levels in the hippocampus along with a decrease in A plasma content, suggesting a putative role of the diet in delaying plaque formation. This is the first evidence that LMN diet can prevent behavioral deterioration caused by aging and Tg genotype, and delay the A plaque formation. The results also highlight the increasing importance of polyphenols from dry fruits and cocoa as human dietary supplements in amelioration of the cognitive and functional impairment during aging and neurological disease. Arendash et al. 2448 A.N. Sokolov et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 2445–2453 (2007), however, administered a polyphenol-rich omega-3 fatty acid diet of Greenland Eskimos to another type of Tg mice (another animal model of Alzheimer’s disease), and did not find any diet benefits, except for several behavioral measures such as open field activity and maze entries. In both studies, Tg mice were similar, with Arendash et al. (2007) using a second-generation cross of the Tg2576 and the 6.2 lines that carried an additional mutation in the presenilin gene 1, PS1. The distinct outcomes were likely due to the difference in either diets or study protocols to which the mice were exposed. In an animal model of stroke, Shah et al. (2010) administered orally mice with 5, 15, or 30 mg/kg epicatechin 90 min before middle cerebral artery occlusion. Epicatechin-treated mice, compared to the control group, exhibited significantly smaller lesion volumes and improved neurological scores. Mice that were posttreated with 30 mg/kg of epicatechin at 3.5 h after the occlusion had also significantly smaller infarct volumes and improved neurological scores. Villarreal-Calderon et al. (2010) reported that a treatment with dark chocolate prevents the inflammation of the vagus nerve resulting from a 16-month exposure of mice to the polluted air of Mexico City. Mice exposed to polluted air had a significant imbalance in genes coding for antioxidant defenses, apoptosis and neurodegeneration at the level of the dorsal vagal complex and this imbalance was mitigated by chocolate administration. 3.2. Human population studies Observational human population studies on neuroprotective and neuromodulatory action of flavonoids, including high-flavanol cocoa, have often been poorly controlled, whereas prospective longitudinal studies remain laborious and costly. To date, therefore, evidence for beneficial neuroprotective and anti-inflammatory effects of cocoa flavanols on cognitive and behavioral decline in aging and neurological disease is rather limited. In a prospective study of a large sample of men aged 69–89 years (Kalmijn et al., 1997), risk of cognitive decline (assessed by the Mini-Mental State Examination) tended to inversely relate to flavonoid intake, though no association was found between the risk of cognitive decline and vitamin C- or E-intake served as the antioxidant reference to flavonoids. In a cross-sectional study of the elderly Norwegian population, Nurk et al. (2009) investigated the association between cognitive performance and flavonoid intake from chocolate, wine, and tea. Participants aged 70–74 years (n = 2031; 55% females) completed a comprehensive cognitive test battery consisting of the Kendrick Object Learning Test, Trail Making Test (part A), versions of the Digit Symbol Test, Block Design, Mini-Mental State Examination, and Controlled Oral Word Association Test. Habitual food intake was assessed by a self-reported food frequency questionnaire. Chocolate, wine, or tea consumers yielded significantly better mean test scores and lower prevalence of poor cognitive performance. Those consuming all three items had the best test scores and the lowest risk for poor test performance. The association between intake of the foods and cognition were dose-dependent, with sharp improvements at intakes of ∼10 g/day chocolate and ∼75–100 ml/day wine, and a linear improvement for tea intake. The effect was most pronounced for wine and modestly weaker for chocolate intake. It appears that in the elderly, a diet high in some flavonoid-rich foods is associated with better performance in several cognitive abilities in a dose-dependent manner. Participants of a prospective 10-year Personnes Agées Quid (PAQUID) study (n = 1640; aged ≥65 years, free from dementia) underwent testing of cognitive function at four consecutive time points (Letenneur et al., 2007). At each visit, the cognitive functions were assessed by test battery including Mini-Mental State Examination, Benton’s Visual Retention Test, and “Isaacs” Set. Flavonoid intake from multiple food items including chocolate (assessed once as the study begun) was associated with both better cognitive performance at baseline and better evolution of performance over time. The most positive evolution was found in participants in the two highest quartiles of flavonoid intake compared to those in the lowest quartile. After 10-year follow-up, participants with the lowest flavonoid intake had lost on average 2.1 points on the Mini-Mental State Examination, compared to the 1.2-point loss in participants of the highest quartile. The study raises the possibility of an association between dietary flavonoid intake and cognitive aging. 3.3. Clinical studies The known benefits of flavonoids for vascular health (Ried et al., 2012) may represent a promising approach in treating cerebrovascular disorders and protecting cognitive and functional behavior in the elderly. Age- and disease-related disturbances in cerebral blood flow are thought to be commonly accompanied by cognitive and behavioral decline. Dietary high-flavanol cocoa intake is associated with an increased cerebral blood flow velocity in the middle cerebral artery, suggesting a promising role for high-flavanol cocoa consumption in the treatment of cerebrovascular ischemic syndromes such as dementia and stroke. For instance, Sorond et al. (2008) report that following two weeks of high-flavanol cocoa intake, in 34 healthy elderly volunteers (aged 72 ± 6 years; 16 males), mean blood flow velocity in the middle cerebral artery measured by transcranial Doppler ultrasound significantly increases by 8% at one week and 10% at two weeks. Potent antioxidative and anti-inflammatory properties of flavonoids have been proposed to play a role in preventing mild cognitive impairment, a precursor of dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease. In Alzheimer’s disease, an increased production and accretion of A-peptides activates microglia, resulting in release of inflammatory mediators that further enhance A production, giving rise to neuronal dysfunction and cellular death. While and ␥-secretase facilitate A production, ␣-secretase inhibits it. Recent work in cultured human neuroblastoma cells shows that low concentrations of nitric oxide up-regulate the expression of ␣secretase and down-regulate that of -secretase. This suggests the cerebrovascular nitric oxide might inhibit A production (McCarty, 2006; Pak et al., 2005). Cocoa flavanols, especially epicatechin, act directly on the endothelium of brain vessels, stimulating activity of the endothelial nitric oxide synthase that in turn induces vasodilation and improves cerebrovascular perfusion (Fisher et al., 2006; Schroeter et al., 2006; Patel et al., 2008). So far, there does not seem to be any proven association between intake of antioxidants and vitamins and Alzheimer’s disease (see Luchsinger and Mayeux, 2004 for review), however, several studies did report a diminished cerebral blood flow in dementia patients (Ruitenberg et al., 2005; Nagahama et al., 2003). Cerebrovascular atrophy is also known to lead to mild cognitive impairment and subsequently to Alzheimer’s disease. It is therefore conceivable that beneficial properties of cocoa flavanols may slow down the transition from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease (Nagahama et al., 2003). Commenges et al. (2000) conducted a clinical trial with 1367 participants aged over 65 years, 66 from which developed dementia. The relative risk of developing dementia adjusted for age for the two highest consumptions of flavonoids was 0.55 (95% confidence interval, 0.34–0.90). After further adjustment for sex, education level, weight and vitamin C intake, the relative risk decreased to 0.49 (95% confidence interval, 0.26–0.92). Thus, it appears that antioxidant flavonoid intake is inversely related to the risk of dementia. However, in this study flavonoids came mainly from fruits, vegetables, wine and tea rather than cocoa. A.N. Sokolov et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 2445–2453 Most recently, Desideri et al. (2012) studied 90 elderly individuals with mild cognitive impairment (mean age, 71 years; 43 males) who consumed once daily for eight weeks a drink containing either ∼990 mg (high), ∼520 mg (moderate), or ∼45 mg (low-flavanol) of cocoa flavanols. Cognitive function was assessed by the MiniMental State Examination, Trail Making Test A and B, and verbal fluency test. At the end of the follow-up period, Mini-Mental State Examination scores were similar in the treatment groups. In the high- and moderate-flavanol compared to the low-flavanol groups, time required to complete both Trail Making Tests was significantly lower. Verbal fluency score was significantly better in the highcompared to low-flavanol group. The high- and moderate-flavanol groups also exhibited decreased insulin resistance, blood pressure, and lipid peroxidation. This appears to be the first dietary intervention study to demonstrate the efficacy of regular consumption of cocoa flavanols for improving cognitive function in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment. The ability of flavonoids to improve and maintain vascular function offers a further possibility to investigate the relationship between cocoa flavanol intake and neuronal and functional loss after stroke (for recent animal data, see, e.g., Shah et al., 2010; Section 3.1). In the meta-analysis of three human studies comprising 114,009 participants, Buitrago-Lopez et al. (2011) reported a 29% reduction of stroke risk in high chocolate consumers compared to low consumers. Buijsse et al. (2010) found an even stronger inverse correlation between chocolate consumption and stroke risk than for myocardial infarction. Rautiainen et al. (2012) examined the association between the total antioxidant capacity (including fruits, vegetables, tea, coffee, and chocolate) and stroke risk in women, aged 49 to 83 years, from the Swedish Mammography cohort. The study included 5680 women with and 31,035 women without a history of cardiovascular disease. Diet was assessed with a self-reported food frequency questionnaire, and dietary total antioxidant capacity calculated using oxygen radical absorbance capacity values. Stroke cases were subdivided into cerebral infarctions, hemorrhagic and unspecified strokes. Using multivariate analyses with hazard ratios, the dietary antioxidant capacity was found to be inversely associated with total stroke in disease-free women (17% risk reduction) and hemorrhagic stroke in women with disease history (45% risk reduction). In Parkinson’s disease, abnormal action of the neuromodulator adenosine, which fails to suppress unwanted motor activity in the basal ganglia via striatopallidal neurons, has been linked to impaired motor function (Jankovic, 2008). Adenosine antagonists, including caffeine and chocolate, have therefore been considered for ameliorating parkinsonian motor dysfunction. Parkinson’s patients do report an increased chocolate consumption, independent of concomitant depressive symptoms (Wolz et al., 2009). However, in an investigator-blinded, placebo-controlled, crossover trial in 26 patients with moderate Parkinson’s disease (Wolz et al., 2012), a single acute dose of dark chocolate failed to improve motor function (assessed by Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale motor score) over white flavanol-free white chocolate. The outcome might likely be due to lacking patient blindfolding, the dose of chocolate used, its flavanol content, and the time frame of treatment. To the most part, the neuroprotective potential described above is attributable to cocoa and chocolate flavanols rather than other ingredients such as caffeine that has been widely implicated in counteracting age- and disease-related cognitive decline such as in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease (Costa et al., 2010; Santos et al., 2010). Unlike in coffee, tea and soft drinks, the caffeine (and theobromine) concentration in chocolate and cocoa products is much lower than that of flavanols to account for potential effects of chocolate (Benton, 2004). 2449 4. Neuromodulation of cognition, mood, learning, and memory 4.1. Animal studies Flavonoids are believed to trigger expression of neuromodulatory proteins in the brain regions implicated in learning, memory, and cognition, suggesting cocoa flavanols can exhibit immediate and short-term action on neurocognition, mood, and behavior. Surprisingly, only a few animal studies have addressed these issues. Mice treated with one of the major chocolate flavanols, epicatechin, at the dose of 500 g/g (daily supply of 2.5 mg) showed pronounced angiogenesis in the hippocampus (van Praag et al., 2007). Epicatechin treatment combined with exercise (running a wheel) improved retention of spatial memory and increased dendritic spine density in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus. Moreover, epicatechin treatment facilitated gene expression associated with learning in the hippocampus but did not affect hippocampal adult neurogenesis. Yamada et al. (2009) compared the effects of short-term versus long-term (two-week) oral administration of cocoa mass in large amounts (100 mg/100 g body weight) in the rat elevated T-maze test, an animal model of anxiety. Shortterm administration significantly abolished avoidance behavior during immediate test performance, suggesting a reduced fear conditioning. Long-term administration enhanced brain concentration of emotion-related neurotransmitter serotonin and its turnover. The findings indicate short-term cocoa intake shows an anxiolytic effect, whereas long-term intake affects brain monoamine metabolism. This suggests flavanol impacts on the amygdala underpinning regulation of anxiety and encoding of affective valence (e.g., Morrison and Salzman, 2010). Flavanol action therefore may occur in the brain regions outside the hippocampus and subventricular zone, in which it has already been established. 4.2. Human studies In humans, several studies have aimed to identify immediate and short-term action of cocoa flavanols on mood and cognitive performance with as yet inconclusive outcome. Crews et al. (2008) had healthy older adults (n = 101; 41 males; age, ≥60 years) to consume daily for 6 weeks either a 37-g bar of dark chocolate and 8 ounces (237 ml) of artificially sweetened cocoa beverage or similar placebo products. Participants underwent hematological, blood pressure, and pulse rate measurements, and accomplished several cognitive tests: Selective Reminding, Wechsler Memory Scale-III Faces I and Faces II subtests, Trail Making Test, Stroop Test, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Digit Symbol-Coding subtest, and General Activation subscale of the Activation-Deactivation Adjective Check List (A-DACL). The only effect observed was a significantly higher, compared to the placebo group, pulse rate, with no effects found on blood pressure, hematological, and cognitive variables. In a randomized, double-blind, controlled, balanced, three period crossover study, 30 healthy young adults (mean age, 22 years; 13 males) consumed high-flavanol (520 mg and 994 mg) cocoa drinks and a matched control drink, with a three-day washout between drinks (Scholey et al., 2010). Over a 1 h testing period, participants repeatedly performed 10-min cycles of a Cognitive Demand Battery (two serial subtraction tasks, Serial Threes and Serial Sevens), a Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVIP) task, and a “mental fatigue” scale. High-flavanol cocoa intake improved Serial Threes performance. The 994-mg beverage yielded speeded RVIP responses, but also more errors during Serial Sevens. Only the 520-mg beverage attenuated self-reported “mental fatigue”. This is the first report on immediate improvements of cognitive function following high-flavanol cocoa consumption in healthy young adults. 2450 A.N. Sokolov et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 2445–2453 In healthy young adults (n = 30, eight males; age range, 18–25 years) who in a crossover, order counterbalanced design consumed once either 35 g dark chocolate (with 720 mg of high-flavanol cocoa) or a matched quantity of flavanol-free white chocolate, high-flavanol cocoa improved visual contrast sensitivity (assessed by reading numbers varying in their luminance relative to background), working memory for location, choice reaction time, and the time required to detect direction of coherent motion (Field et al., 2011). This outcome extends the range of cognitive functions affected by high-flavanol cocoa consumption and provides the first evidence on the immediate effects of high-flavanol cocoa on visual functions. In 72 healthy participants aged 40–65 years, Pase et al. (2013) examined immediate and sub-chronic effects of cocoa flavanols on mood and cognition. Three separate groups were assigned an Acticoa dark chocolate drink mix containing 500, 250, or 0 mg (placebo) of polyphenols once daily for 30 days. At baseline, at 1, 2.5, and 4 h after a single acute dose, and again after 30-day treatment, performance on cognitive tasks (Simple and Choice Reaction Time, Digit Vigilance, Tracking, Spatial and Numeric Working Memory, Immediate and Delayed Word Recall, Delayed Word and Picture Recognition) was assessed with the Cognitive Drug Research battery (Kennedy et al., 2002) and self-reported mood with the Bond-Lader Visual Analogue Scale (Bond and Lader, 1974). While mood remained unaffected by the acute treatment, no effects on cognition occurred at all time points. At 30 days, the high-dose treatment significantly improved self-rated calmness and contentedness compared to placebo. This is the first evidence on the effects of cocoa polyphenols on mood in healthy participants. The outcome suggests a possibility for cocoa polyphenols for ameliorating the symptoms associated with clinical anxiety and depression. Although some recent studies do indicate a relationship between chocolate consumption and depressive symptoms (e.g., Rose et al., 2010), the nature of the relationship remains to be clarified. Latest developments in modern non-invasive neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), electroencephalography, and magnetoencephalography (EEG/MEG), have made it possible to explore modulatory effects of food constituents on neural activity in the human brain and its association with behavior. Yet, with the exception of some work done on the modulation of brain reward system by chocolate intake (e.g., Small et al., 2001; Stice et al., 2008; Valentin et al., 2007), only a few neuroimaging studies have examined the neural networks selectively activated by cocoa flavanols. Francis et al. (2006) used a paired letter-digit task in healthy young female participants (n = 16; age range, 18–30 years) while recording the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) responses in an fMRI protocol following 5-day ingestion of 150 mg of cocoa flavanols. Participants had to press a key, making either one judgment in the “no switch” condition (“is the letter a vowel or consonant”, “is the digit even or odd”) or two judgments in the “switch” condition (responding both to the letter and digit). Although an overall BOLD signal increased during the cognitive task, no effects were found in response time, switch cost (the difference in response time between “switch” and “no-switch” conditions), and heart rate after consumption of this moderate dose of cocoa flavanols. Ingestion of a single acute dose (450 mg of cocoa flavanols) yielded an increased cerebral blood flow (Fig. 2), confirming a potential of cocoa flavanols for treatment of vascular impairment such as dementia and stroke (see Section 3). Over a 30-day period, Camfield et al. (2012) administered a daily chocolate drink (250 mg or 500 mg cocoa flavanols versus lowflavanol cocoa as placebo) to 63 volunteers (aged 40–65 years). Neurocognitive changes associated with flavanol supplementation during performance of a spatial working memory task at baseline Fig. 2. Increased blood flow response (±SEM, standard error of mean) in the gray matter of four female participants following ingestion of an acute dose of high compared to low flavanol drink. From Francis et al., 2006. The effect of flavanol-rich cocoa on the fMRI response to a cognitive task in healthy young people. Journal of Cardiovascular Pharmacology 47, Suppl. 2, S215–20. Copyright © 2006 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins with permission of Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. and at the end of the treatment were assessed using EEG (steady state visually evoked potentials, SSVP). Behavioral measures of accuracy and response time did not differ in a dose-dependent manner, whereas average amplitude and phase of the evoked potentials at a number of posterior parietal and centro-frontal sites significantly differed between the groups during memory encoding, the memory hold period, and retrieval (Fig. 3). The authors assume the differences in brain activation, even in the absence of behavioral effects, point to an increased neural efficiency in spatial working memory associated with chronic cocoa flavanol consumption. Table 1 summarizes the results of research on the neuroprotective action of flavanols in aging and neurological diseases on the one hand, and the neuromodulatory effects on cognition, mood, learning, and memory on the other hand. As seen from the table, basic animal intervention studies on mechanisms of action of flavanols still outnumber human and clinical studies demonstrating that these effects are of physiological and clinical relevance. In summary, recent research has yielded encouraging albeit as yet inconclusive outcome. For exploring possible effects of highflavanol cocoa on human behavior, cognitive and brain functions, standardized psychometric tasks and neuroimaging protocols are required along with administering proper high-flavanol cocoa dosages. To date, there is still limited evidence for high-flavanol cocoa effects on human cognitive and affective function, and behavior. Moreover, it remains unclear how high-flavanol cocoa modulates the brain networks underlying neural cognitive processing. 5. Concluding remarks and future directions Flavonoids, potent antioxidant and anti-inflammatory agents, represent up to 20% of compounds found in cocoa beans. Flavanols, and especially epicatechin, are the most common cocoa flavonoids. The flavanol contents in cocoa products and chocolate vary greatly depending on the bean variety and origin, agricultural and processing practices. In part, the variability of flavanol contents in cocoa and chocolate may be responsible for the mixed outcomes presently observed in research on the effects of cocoa flavanols on neurocognitive and affective function, executive control, and behavior. This might be due to the fact that to date, only few – especially, human – intervention studies have directly examined exposure to cocoa flavonoids, while the most data comes from A.N. Sokolov et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 2445–2453 2451 Fig. 3. Topographic differences in the average steady state visually evoked potentials SSVEP amplitudes at baseline and retest for a spatial working memory task (encoding, hold interval, and retrieval) in the low, medium and high cocoa flavanol (CF) groups. Warm and cool colors show SSVP amplitude decreases and increases, respectively, post-treatment compared to baseline. (For interpretation of the references to color in the figures throughout the article, please consult the web version of this article.) From Camfield et al., 2012. Steady state visually evoked potential (SSVEP) topography changes associated with cocoa flavanol consumption. Physiology and Behavior 105, 948–57. Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. with permission of Elsevier Ltd. Table 1 Cocoa flavonoid effects on the brain, cognition, and behavior – overview. Brain region/function Animal models Human studies 15 8, 9, 10, 19, 24, 25 Hippocampus Neurogenesis Improved neuronal connectivity Angiogenesis 15, 26 27 27 28 Subventricular zone of the brain Neurogenesis 6, 26 Whole brain Improved cerebral blood flow (general improvement of neural function due to enhanced oxygen and glucose delivery) Vision Improved blood delivery to the retina Rapid visual processing Visual contrast sensitivity Coherent motion direction 14 Performance Choice reaction time 7 Cognition, learning, and memory; mood General 22 Mental calculation Working memory for location 29 Fear conditioning (anxiety) Calmness, contentedness Aging Reduced cognitive decline Neurological disease Alzheimer (reduced cognitive decline) Stroke (reduced risk) 11 23 7 7 1, 6 16 23 7 20 13, 16, 18, 25 4, 5 2, 3, 12, 17, 21 Key to references: 1: Bisson et al. (2008), 2: Buijsse et al. (2010), 3: Buitrago-Lopez et al. (2011), 4: Commenges et al. (2000), 5: Desideri et al. (2012), 6: FernándezFernández et al. (2012), 7: Field et al. (2011), 8: Fisher et al. (2003), 9: Fisher et al. (2006), 10: Francis et al. (2006), 11: Huber et al. (2006), 12: Janszky et al. (2009), 13: Kalmijn et al. (1997), 14: Kalt et al. (2010), 15: Kim et al. (2006), 16: Letenneur et al. (2007), 17: Mink et al. (2007), 18: Nurk et al. (2009), 19: Patel et al. (2008), 20: Pase et al. (2013), 21: Rautiainen et al. (2012), 22: Rozan et al. (2007), 23: Scholey et al. (2010), 24: Schroeter et al. (2006), 25: Sorond et al. (2008), 26: Valente et al. (2009), 27: van Praag et al. (2007), 28: Vauzour et al. (2008), 29: Yamada et al. (2009). research using dark chocolate as flavanol supply. Despite multiplicity of flavanol effects in the brain, neurobiological actions of flavanols are believed to occur in two major ways: (i) via direct interactions with cellular and molecular signaling cascades, especially in the brain regions dedicated to learning and memory, and (ii) via central and peripheral blood-flow improvement and angiogenesis, including the brain and sensory systems. Overall, evidence on the persistence of neuroprotective and neuromodulatory actions of cocoa flavanols, albeit as yet limited and inconclusive, suggests that cocoa flavanols may exert both long lasting and immediate effects on neurocognition and behavior. The immediate effects can be attained with a single acute or subchronic (for several weeks) administration of cocoa flavanols in appropriate dosages. The lasting effects likely require chronic intake of flavanolrich cocoa products over an extended time frame. In both instances, more controlled follow-up studies should be in place to determine the effects’ duration. While long lasting neuroprotective properties of flavonoid intake for neurodegenerative and neuroinflammatory diseases of the nervous system have been relatively well documented (Katz et al., 2011; although more research specifically on cocoa flavanols and greater, e.g. population-based, participant samples is required), more immediate action on cognitive and affective function, executive control, and behavior remains largely unknown, as are sex differences in both long-term and immediate actions (see Sokolov et al., 2013 for a companion review). Sex-related variability in data is likely to account for the lack of significant effects of cocoa flavanols on the brain responses and cognitive performance observed in some of the studies. In females, distinct phases of the menstrual cycle and the perimenopause may also add to data variability. The main issues to resolve in the studies to come include (i) blindfolding both the experimenter and participants, which is important for reducing the well-known reward value of chocolate intake, (ii) determining an appropriate (immediate vs. short-term vs. long-term) time frame and dose of flavanol administration, (iii) choice of simple tests of cognitive, executive and affective function and performance that possess sufficient sensitivity and specificity (Macready et al., 2009), and (iv) for examining neuroprotective properties of flavanol consumption, the onset and duration of consumption. Another important issue to consider is a so called 2452 A.N. Sokolov et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 2445–2453 food matrix, or food composition, in which cocoa flavonoids appear in food (e.g., Lamuela-Raventós et al., 2005; Manach et al., 2004). For example, ingestion of 100 g dark chocolate along with 200 ml milk results in a substantial reduction of both total antioxidant capacity and (-)epicatechin content of human plasma, compared to ingestion of 100 g pure dark chocolate, and the reduction is even greater after ingestion of 200 g milk chocolate (Serafini et al., 2003). Finally, other constituents of cocoa and plants rich in flavanols, such as their relatively high content of tryptophan, a precursor of neurotransmitter serotonin (e.g., Bertazzo et al., 2011), may either contribute to the neurobiological effects of flavanols or, depending on the constituents’ bioavailability (e.g., due to the food matrix; Smit, 2011), exert different actions through different pathways. Forthcoming studies should explore whether these actions are complementary, antagonistic or synergistic. In conclusion, future research has to combine functional neuroimaging techniques such as fMRI, EEG and MEG with neurocognitive and behavioral correlates to uncover long lasting and immediate effects of chocolate consumption on human cognition, mood, and behavior. Acknowledgements We thank Otmar and Edelgard Knoll and the Willy Robert Pitzer Foundation for support. MAP was supported by the Else Kröner Fresenius Foundation (Grants P2010 92 and P2013 127), the Reinhold Beitlich Foundation, the Berthold Leibinger Foundation, and by the Heidehof Foundation. References Andrés-Lacueva, C., Shukitt-Hale, B., Galli, R.L., Jauregui, O., Lamuela-Raventós, R.M., Joseph, J.A., 2005. Anthocyanins in aged blueberry-fed rats are found centrally and may enhance memory. Nutr. Neurosci. 8, 111–120. Arendash, G.W., Jensen, M.T., Salem Jr., N., Hussein, N., Cracchiolo, J., Dickson, A., Leighty, R., Potter, H., 2007. A diet high in omega-3 fatty acids does not improve or protect cognitive performance in Alzheimer’s transgenic mice. Neuroscience 149, 286–302. Arts, I.C.W., Hollman, P.C.H., Kromhout, D., 1999. Chocolate as a source of tea flavonoids. Lancet 354, 488. Benton, D., 2004. The biology and psychology of chocolate craving. In: Nehlig, A. (Ed.), Coffee, Tea, Chocolate and the Brain. CRC Press, Strasbourg, pp. 205–218. Bertazzo, A., Comai, S., Brunato, I., Zancato, M., Costa, C.V.L., 2011. The content of protein and non-protein (free and protein-bound) tryptophan in Theobroma cacao beans. Food Chem. 124, 93–96. Birks, J., Grimley Evans, J., 2009. Ginkgo biloba for cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 1, CD003120. Bisson, J.F., Nejdi, A., Rozan, P., Hidalgo, S., Lalonde, R., Messaoudi, M., 2008. Effects of long-term administration of a cocoa polyphenolic extract (Acticoa powder) on cognitive performances in aged rats. Br. J. Nutr. 100, 94–101. Bond, A., Lader, M., 1974. The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 47, 211–218. Bors, W., Michel, C., 2002. Chemistry of the antioxidant effect of polyphenols. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 957, 57–69. Buijsse, B., Weikert, C., Drogan, D., Bergmann, M., Boeing, H., 2010. Chocolate consumption in relation to blood pressure and risk of cardiovascular disease in German adults. Eur. Heart J. 31, 1616–1623. Buitrago-Lopez, A., Sanderson, J., Johnson, L., Warnakula, S., Wood, A., Di Angelantonio, E., Franco, O.H., 2011. Chocolate consumption and cardiometabolic disorders: systematic review and metaanalysis. Br. Med. J. 343, d4488. Camfield, D.A., Scholey, A., Pipingas, A., Silberstein, R., Kras, M., Nolidin, K., Wesnes, K., Pase, M., Stough, C., 2012. Steady state visually evoked potential (SSVEP) topography changes associated with cocoa flavanol consumption. Physiol. Behav. 105, 948–957. Commenges, D., Scotet, V., Renaud, S., Jacqmin-Gadda, H., Barberger-Gateau, P., Dartigues, J.F., 2000. Intake of flavonoids and risk of dementia. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 16, 357–363. Costa, J., Lunet, N., Santos, C., Santos, J., Vaz-Carneiro, A., 2010. Caffeine exposure and the Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Alzheimers Dis. 20 (Suppl. 1), S221–S238. Crews Jr., W.D., Harrison, D.W., Wright, J.W., 2008. A double-blind, placebocontrolled, randomized trial of the effects of dark chocolate and cocoa on variables associated with neuropsychological functioning and cardiovascular health: clinical findings from a sample of healthy, cognitively intact older adults. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 87, 872–880. DeCarli, C., 2012. Cerebrovascular disease: assessing the brain as an end-organ of vascular disease. Nat. Rev. Cardiol. 9, 435–436. Desideri, G., Kwik-Uribe, C., Grassi, D., Necozione, S., Ghiadoni, L., Mastroiacovo, D., Raffaele, A., Ferri, L., Bocale, R., Lechiara, M.C., Marini, C., Ferri, C., 2012. Benefits in cognitive function, blood pressure, and insulin resistance through cocoa flavanol consumption in elderly subjects with mild cognitive impairment: the Cocoa, Cognition, and Aging (CoCoA) study. Hypertension 60, 794–801. EFSA Panel on Dietetic Products, Nutrition and Allergies (NDA), 2012. Scientific Opinion on the substantiation of a health claim related to cocoa flavanols and maintenance of normal endothelium-dependent vasodilation pursuant to Article 13(5) of Regulation (EC) No 1924/2006. EFSA J. 10, 2809. Engler, M.B., Engler, M.M., Chen, C.Y., Malloy, M.J., Browne, A., Chiu, E.Y., Kwak, H.K., Milbury, P., Paul, S.M., Blumberg, J., Mietus-Snyder, M.L., 2004. Flavonoid-rich dark chocolate improves endothelial function and increases plasma epicatechin concentrations in healthy adults. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 23, 197–204. Fernández-Fernández, L., Comes, G., Bolea, I., Valente, T., Ruiz, J., Murtra, P., Ramirez, B., Anglés, N., Reguant, J., Morelló, J.R., Boada, M., Hidalgo, J., Escorihuela, R.M., Unzeta, M., 2012. LMN diet, rich in polyphenols and polyunsaturated fatty acids, improves mouse cognitive decline associated with aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Behav. Brain Res. 228, 261–271. Field, D.T., Williams, C.M., Butler, L.T., 2011. Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in an acute improvement in visual and cognitive functions. Physiol. Behav. 103, 255–260. Fisher, N.D., Hughes, M., Gerhard-Herman, M., Hollenberg, N.K., 2003. Flavanolrich cocoa induces nitric oxide-dependent vasodilation in healthy humans. J. Hypertens. 21, 2281–2286. Fisher, N.D., Sorond, F.A., Hollenberg, N.K., 2006. Cocoa flavanols and brain perfusion. J. Cardiovasc. Pharmacol. 47 (Suppl. 2), S210–S214. Francis, S.T., Head, K., Morris, P.G., Macdonald, I.A., 2006. The effect of flavanolrich cocoa on the fMRI response to a cognitive task in healthy young people. J. Cardiovasc. Pharmacol. 47 (Suppl 2), S215–S220. Gonzalez-Gallego, J., Sanchez-Campos, S., Tunon, M.J., 2007. Antiinflammatory properties of dietary flavonoids. Nutr. Hospital. 22, 287–293. Goyarzu, P., Malin, D.H., Lau, F.C., Taglialatela, G., Moon, W.D., Jennings, R., Moy, E., Moy, D., Lippold, S., Shukitt-Hale, B., Joseph, J.A., 2004. Blueberry supplemented diet: effects on object recognition memory and nuclear factor-kappa B levels in aged rats. Nutr. Neurosci. 7, 75–83. Grassi, D., Desideri, G., Necozione, S., Lippi, C., Casale, R., Properzi, G., Blumberg, J.B., Ferri, C., 2008. Blood pressure is reduced and insulin sensitivity increased in glucose-intolerant, hypertensive subjects after 15 days of consuming highpolyphenol dark chocolate. J. Nutr. 138, 1671–1676. Grassi, D., Necozione, S., Lippi, C., Croce, G., Valeri, L., Pasqualetti, P., Desideri, G., Blumberg, J.B., Ferri, C., 2005. Cocoa reduces blood pressure and insulin resistance and improves endothelium-dependent vasodilation in hypertensives. Hypertension 46, 398–405. Heiss, C., Dejam, A., Kleinbongard, P., Schewe, T., Sies, H., Kelm, M., 2003. Vascular effects of cocoa rich in flavan-3-ols. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 290, 1030–1031. Hirsch, E.C., Hunot, S., Hartmann, A., 2005. Neuroinflammatory processes in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinson. Relat. Disord. 11 (Suppl. 1), S9–S15. Hollenberg, N.K., Fisher, N.D., McCullough, M.L., 2009. Flavanols, the Kuna, cocoa consumption, and nitric oxide. J. Am. Soc. Hypertens. 3, 105–112. Hooper, L., Kay, C., Abdelhamid, A., Kroon, P.A., Cohn, J.S., Rimm, E.B., Cassidy, A., 2012. Effects of chocolate, cocoa, and flavan-3-ols on cardiovascular health: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized trials. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 95, 740–751. Huber, K.K., Adams, H., Remky, A., Arend, K.O., 2006. Retrobulbar haemodynamics and contrast sensitivity improvements after CO2 breathing. Acta Ophthalmol. Scand. 84, 481–487. Hurst, W.J., Tarka Jr., S.M., Powis, T.G., Valdez Jr., F., Hester, T.R., 2002. Cacao usage by the earliest Maya civilization. Nature 418, 289–290. Jankovic, J., 2008. Are adenosine antagonists, such as istradefylline, caffeine, and chocolate, useful in the treatment of Parkinson’s disease? Ann. Neurol. 63, 267–269. Janszky, I., Mukamal, K.J., Ljung, R., Ahnve, S., Ahlbom, A., Hallqvist, J., 2009. Chocolate consumption and mortality following a first acute myocardial infarction: the Stockholm Heart Epidemiology Program. J. Intern. Med. 266, 248–257. Kalmijn, S., Feskens, E.J., Launer, L.J., Kromhout, D., 1997. Polyunsaturated fatty acids, antioxidants, and cognitive function in very old men. Am. J. Epidemiol. 145, 33–41. Kalt, W., Hanneken, A., Milbury, P., Tremblay, F., 2010. Recent research on polyphenolics in vision and eye health. J. Agric. Food Chem. 58, 4001–4007. Katz, D.L., Doughty, K., Ali, A., 2011. Cocoa and chocolate in human health and disease. Antioxid. Redox Signal. 15, 2779–2811. Kay, C.D., Kris-Etherton, P.M., West, S.G., 2006. Effects of antioxidant-rich foods on vascular reactivity: review of the clinical evidence. Curr. Atheroscler. Rep. 8, 510–522. Kelleher III, R.J., Govindarajan, A., Jung, H.Y., Kang, H., Tonegawa, S., 2004. Translational control by MAPK signaling in long-term synaptic plasticity and memory. Cell 116, 467–479. Kennedy, D.O., Scholey, A.B., Wesnes, K.A., 2002. Modulation of cognition and mood following administration of single doses of Ginkgo biloba, ginseng, and a ginkgo/ginseng combination to healthy young adults. Physiol. Behav. 75, 739–751. Kim, D.H., Jeon, S.J., Son, K.H., Jung, J.W., Lee, S., Yoon, B.H., Choi, J.W., Cheong, J.H., Ko, K.H., Ryu, J.H., 2006. Effect of the flavonoid, oroxylin A, on transient A.N. Sokolov et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (2013) 2445–2453 cerebral hypoperfusion-induced memory impairment in mice. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 85, 658–668. Lamuela-Raventós, R.M., Romero-Pérez, A.I., Andrés-Lacueva, C., Tornero, A., 2005. Review: health effects of cocoa flavonoids. Food Sci. Technol. Int. 11, 159–176. Letenneur, L., Proust-Lima, C., Le Gouge, A., Dartigues, J.F., Barberger-Gateau, P., 2007. Flavonoid intake and cognitive decline over a 10-year period. Am. J. Epidemiol. 165, 1364–1371. Luchsinger, J., Mayeux, R., 2004. Dietary factors and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 3, 579–587. Macready, A.L., Kennedy, O.B., Ellis, J.A., Williams, C.M., Spencer, J.P., Butler, L.T., 2009. Flavonoids and cognitive function: a review of human randomized controlled trial studies and recommendations for future studies. Genes Nutr. 4, 227–242. Manach, C., Scalbert, A., Morand, C., Rémésy, C., Jiménez, L., 2004. Polyphenols: food sources and bioavailability. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 79, 727–747. Mann, G.E., Bonacasa, B., Ishii, T., Siow, R.C., 2009. Targeting the redox sensitive Nrf2-Keap1 defense pathway in cardiovascular disease: protection afforded by dietary isoflavones. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 9, 139–145. McCarty, M.F., 2006. Toward prevention of Alzheimer’s disease – potential nutraceutical strategies for suppressing the production of amyloid beta peptides. Med. Hypotheses 67, 682–697. McGeer, E.G., McGeer, P.L., 2003. Inflammatory processes in Alzheimer’s disease. Progress Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 27, 741–749. Messerli, F.H., 2012. Chocolate consumption, cognitive function, and Nobel laureates. N. Engl. J. Med. 367, 1562–1564. Mink, P.J., Scrafford, C.G., Barraj, L.M., Harnack, L., Hong, C.P., Nettleton, J.A., Jacobs Jr., D.R., 2007. Flavonoid intake and cardiovascular disease mortality: a prospective study in postmenopausal women. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 85, 895–909. Morrison, S.E., Salzman, C.D., 2010. Re-valuing the amygdala. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 221–230. Nagahama, Y., Nabatame, H., Okina, T., Yamauchi, H., Narita, M., Fujimoto, N., Murakami, M., Fukuyama, H., Matsuda, M., 2003. Cerebral correlates of the progression rate of the cognitive decline in probable Alzheimer’s disease. Eur. Neurol. 50, 1–9. Nehlig, A., 2010. Is caffeine a cognitive enhancer? J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 20 (Suppl. 1), S85–S94. Nehlig, A., 2013. The neuroprotective effects of cocoa flavanol and its influence on cognitive performance. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 75, 716–727. Neveu, V., Pérez-Jiménez, J., Vos, F., Crespy, V., du Chaffaut, L., Mennen, L., Knox, C., Eisner, R., Cruz, J., Wishart, D., Scalbert, A., 2010. Phenol-Explorer: an online comprehensive database on polyphenol contents in foods. Database (Oxf.), http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/database/bap024. Niemenak, N., Rohsius, C., Elwers, S., Ndoumou, D.O., Lieberei, R., 2006. Comparative study of different cocoa (Theobroma cacao L.) clones in terms of their phenolic and anthocyanins contents. J. Food Compos. Anal. 19, 612–619. Nurk, E., Refsum, H., Drevon, C.A., Tell, G.S., Nygaard, H.A., Engedal, K., Smith, A.D., 2009. Intake of flavonoid-rich wine, tea, and chocolate by elderly men and women is associated with better cognitive test performance. Nutrition 139, 120–127. Pak, T., Cadet, P., Mantione, K.J., Stefano, G.B., 2005. Morphine via nitric oxide modulates beta-amyloid metabolism: a novel protective mechanism for Alzheimer’s disease. Med. Sci. Monitor 11, BR357–BR366. Pase, M.P., Scholey, A.B., Pipingas, A., Kras, M., Nolidin, K., Gibbs, A., Wesnes, K., Stough, C., 2013. Cocoa polyphenols enhance positive mood states but not cognitive performance: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. J. Psychopharmacol. 27, 451–458. Passamonti, S., Vrhovsek, U., Vanzo, A., Mattivi, F., 2005. Fast access of some grape pigments to the brain. J. Agric. Food Chem. 53, 7029–7034. Patel, A.K., Rogers, J.T., Huang, X., 2008. Flavanols, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s dementia. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Med. 1, 181–191. Pérez-Jiménez, J., Fezeu, L., Touvier, M., Arnault, N., Manach, C., Hercberg, S., Galan, P., Scalbert, A., 2011. Dietary intake of 337 polyphenols in French adults. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 93, 1220–1228. Rautiainen, S., Larsson, S., Virtamo, J., Wolk, A., 2012. Total antioxidant capacity of diet and risk of stroke: a population-based prospective cohort of women. Stroke 43, 335–340. Richelle, M., Tavazzi, I., Enslen, M., Offord, E.A., 1999. Plasma kinetics in man of epicatechin from black chocolate. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 53, 22–26. Ried, K., Sullivan, T.R., Fakler, P., Frank, O.R., Stocks, N.P., 2012. Effect of cocoa on blood pressure. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 8, CD008893. Rose, N., Koperski, S., Golomb, B.A., 2010. Mood food: chocolate and depressive symptoms in a cross-sectional analysis. Arch. Intern. Med. 170, 699–703. Rozan, P., Hidalgo, S., Nejdi, A., Bisson, J.F., Lalonde, R., Messaoudi, M., 2007. Preventive antioxidant effects of cocoa polyphenolic extract on free radical production and cognitive performances after heat exposure in Wistar rats. J. Food Sci. 72, S203–S206. Ruitenberg, A., den Heijer, T., Bakker, S.L., van Swieten, J.C., Koudstaal, P.J., Hofman, A., Breteler, M.M., 2005. Cerebral hypoperfusion and clinical onset of dementia: the Rotterdam Study. Ann. Neurol. 57, 789–794. Santos, C., Costa, J., Santos, J., Vaz-Carneiro, A., Lunet, N., 2010. Caffeine intake and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis,. J. Alzheimer Dis. 20 (Suppl. 1), S187–S204. Scalbert, A., Andres-Lacueva, C., Arita, M., Kroon, P., Manach, C., Urpi-Sarda, M., Wishart, D., 2011. Databases on food phytochemicals and their healthpromoting effects. J. Agric. Food Chem. 59, 4331–4348. Scholey, A.B., French, S.J., Morris, P.J., Kennedy, D.O., Milne, A.L., Haskell, C.F., 2010. Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvements in mood and 2453 cognitive performance during sustained mental effort. J. Psychopharmacol. (Oxf.) 24, 1505–1514. Schroeter, H., Heiss, C., Balzer, J., Kleinbongard, P., Keen, C.L., Hollenberg, N.K., Sies, H., Kwik-Uribe, C., Schmitz, H.H., Kelm, M., 2006. (−)-Epicatechin mediates beneficial effects of flavanol-rich cocoa on vascular function in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 1024–1029. Serafini, M., Bugianesi, R., Maiani, G., Valtuena, S., De Santis, S., Crozier, A., 2003. Plasma antioxidants from chocolate. Nature 424, 1013. Shah, Z.A., Li, R.C., Ahmad, A.S., Kensler, T.W., Yamamoto, M., Biswal, S., Doré, S., 2010. The flavanol (−)-epicatechin prevents stroke damage through the Nrf2/HO1 pathway. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 30, 1951–1961. Sies, H., Hollman, P.C., Grune, T., Stahl, W., Biesalski, H.K., Williamson, G., 2012. Protection by flavanol-rich foods against vascular dysfunction and oxidative damage: 27th Hohenheim Consensus Conference. Adv. Nutr. 3, 217–221. Small, D.M., Zatorre, R.J., Dagher, A., Evans, A.C., Jones-Gotman, M., 2001. Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain 124, 1720–1733. Smit, H.J., Blackburn, R.J., 2005. Reinforcing effects of caffeine and theobromine as found in chocolate. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 181, 101–106. Smit, H.J., Gaffan, E.A., Rogers, P.J., 2004. Methylxanthines are the psychopharmacologically active constituents of chocolate. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 176, 412–419. Smit, H.J., Rogers, P.J., 2000. Effects of low doses of caffeine on cognitive performance, mood and thirst in low and higher caffeine consumers. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 152, 167–173. Smit, H.J., 2011. Theobromine and the pharmacology of cocoa. In: Fredholm, B.B. (Ed.), Methylxanthines, Handbook of Experimental Pharmacology 200. Springer, Berlin/Heidelberg, pp. 201–234. Sokolov, A.N., Pavlova, M.A., Klosterhalfen, S., Enck, P., 2013. Sex differences in chocolate action and consumption (in preparation). Sorond, F.A., Lipsitz, L.A., Hollenberg, N.K., Fisher, N.D.L., 2008. Cerebral blood flow response to flavanol-rich cocoa in healthy elderly humans. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treatment 4, 433–440. Stevenson, D.E., Hurst, R.D., 2007. Polyphenolic phytochemicals – just antioxidants or much more? Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 64, 2900–2916. Stice, E., Spoor, S., Bohon, C., Veldhuizen, M.G., Small, D.M., 2008. Relation of reward from food intake and anticipated food intake to obesity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study, Journal of. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 924–935. Valente, T., Hidalgo, J., Bolea, I., Ramirez, B., Anglés, N., Reguant, J., Morelló, J.R., Gutiérrez, C., Boada, M., Unzeta, M., 2009. A diet enriched in polyphenols and polyunsaturated fatty acids, LMN diet, induces neurogenesis in the subventricular zone and hippocampus of adult mouse brain. J. Alzheimer Dis. 18, 849–865. Valentin, V.V., Dickinson, A., O’Doherty, J.P., 2007. Determining the neural substrates of goal-directed learning in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 27, 4019–4026. van Praag, H., Lucero, M.J., Yeo, G.W., Stecker, K., Heivand, N., Zhao, C., Yip, E., Afanador, M., Schroeter, H., Hammerstone, J., Gage, F.H., 2007. Plant-derived flavanol (−)epicatechin enhances angiogenesis and retention of spatial memory in mice. J. Neurosci. 27, 5869–5878. Vauzour, D., Vafeiadou, K., Rodriguez-Mateos, A., Rendeiro, C., Spencer, J.P., 2008. The neuroprotective potential of flavonoids: a multiplicity of effects. Genes Nutr. 3, 115–126. Villarreal-Calderon, R., Torres-Jardón, R., Palacios-Moreno, J., Osnaya, N., PérezGuillé, B., Maronpot, R.R., Reed, W., Zhu, H., Calderón-Garcidueñas, L., 2010. Urban air pollution targets the dorsal vagal complex and dark chocolate offers neuroprotection. Int. J. Toxicol. 29, 604–615. Vinson, J.A., Proch, J., Bose, P., Muchler, S., Taffera, P., Shuta, D., Samman, N., Agbor, G.A., 2006. Chocolate is a powerful ex vivo and in vivo antioxidant, an anti-atherosclerotic agent in an animal model, and significant contributor to antioxidants in European and American diets. J. Agric. Food Chem. 54, 8071–8076. Weinmann, S., Roll, S., Schwarzbachv, C., Vauth, C., Willich, S.N., 2010. Effects of Ginkgo biloba in dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Geriatr. 10, 14. Whiting, D., 2001. Natural phenolic compounds 1900–2000: a bird’s eye view of a centuries chemistry. Nat. Prod. Rep. 18, 583–606. Williams, R.J., Spencer, J.P., 2012. Flavonoids, cognition, and dementia: actions, mechanisms, and potential therapeutic utility for Alzheimer disease. Free Rad. Biol. Med. 52, 35–45. Wilson, P.K., 2010. Centuries of seeking chocolate’s medicinal benefits. Lancet 376, 158–159. Wolfe, D., Shazzie, 2005. Naked Chocolate: The Astonishing Truth About the World’s Greatest Food. North Atlantic Books, Berkeley, CA. Wollgast, J., Anklam, E., 2000. Review on polyphenols in Theobroma cacao: changes in composition during the manufacture of chocolate and methodology for identification and quantification. Food Res. Int. 33, 423–447. Wolz, M., Schleiffer, C., Klingelhöfer, L., Schneider, C., Proft, F., Schwanebeck, U., Reichmann, H., Riederer, P., Storch, A., 2012. Comparison of chocolate to cacaofree white chocolate in Parkinson’s disease: a single-dose, investigator-blinded, placebo-controlled, crossover trial. J. Neurol. 259, 2447–2451. Wolz, M., Kaminsky, A., Löhle, M., Koch, R., Storch, A., Reichmann, H., 2009. Chocolate consumption is increased in Parkinson’s disease. Results from a selfquestionnaire study. J. Neurol. 256, 488–492. Yamada, T., Yamada, Y., Okano, Y., Terashima, T., Yokogoshi, H., 2009. Anxiolytic effects of short- and long-term administration of cacao mass on rat elevated T-maze test. J. Nutr. Biochem. 20, 948–955.