Decision Making in Criminal Justice Revisited: Toward a General Theory of Criminal Justice A dissertation submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Cincinnati in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the School of Criminal Justice of the College of Education, Criminal Justice, and Human Services by Daniel J Lytle B.A. Marquette University, 2005 M.S. University of Cincinnati, 2007 Committee Members Lawrence F. Travis, III, Ph.D. (Chair) James Frank, Ph.D. Paula Smith, Ph.D. William King, Ph.D. ABSTRACT The study of correlates of decision-making has been an important and integral part of criminal justice research. While this important research has been studied largely since the conception of criminal justice research, the attempts to take stock and determine what the body of research collectively “knows” has been limited. These limitations have included both the method of synthesis and also the focus of the synthesis. Traditionally, criminal justice scholars have taken stock of correlate research by using narrative literature review techniques. While this research does make some contribution to the field, it is flawed because of the inherent problems in all narrative reviews, namely, double counting studies, and the subjective nature of narrative reviews. The flaw has been that reviews have focused on a few important variables, race and gender, and not on a more comprehensive view. This dissertation seeks to expand upon the current state of correlates of decision-making research by conducting a meta-analysis to examine decision making across the system, specifically, arrest, sentencing, and parole revocation. Results indicated that several variables are important at multiple stages of the criminal justice system. These predictors are both legal and extra-legal variables, legal predictors of decision-making included seriousness of the offense, and the offender’s prior criminal record, while extra-legal predictors of decision-making included race, gender, and ethnicity. These findings were not mitigated by moderating factors, but instead persisted across moderator categories. In addition to system-wide correlates, there were several factors, which were unique to a specific decision point, these included, at arrest, suspect demeanor, and, at sentencing, mode of conviction. ii iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS There are many people that I would like to thank that made this process possible. First, I would like to thank my dissertation chair and mentor Dr. Lawrence F. Travis, III. You have mentored me since I walked through the doors at the University of Cincinnati and I am eternally grateful for your guidance, mentorship, and the occasional figurative swift kick in the pants. You have helped to mold me into a scholar. Second, I would like to thank Dr. James Frank; thank you for your guidance on this project and your mentorship as I progressed through the doctoral program at the University of Cincinnati. Third, I would like to thank Dr. Paula Smith for your guidance and assistance with the technical aspects of conducting this meta-analysis. Fourth, I would like to thank Dr. William King for serving as my outside reader on this project. Thank you all for your service, helpful comments, and assistance in this dissertation process. I would also like to thank the entire criminal justice faculty at the University of Cincinnati. I would especially like to thank Dr. Robin Engel for first exposing me to criminal justice theory and for writing the piece that served as an inspiration for this project. There are two other professors that I have had over the course of my academic career that deserve special mention, Dr. Carol Archbold and Dr. Amy Stichman. Thank you to you both for setting me on the path of graduate school at the University of Cincinnati. Finally, I would like to thank my friends and family. First and foremost, I would like to thank my parents Joseph and Juanita Lytle. Without your encouragement and support this process would never have been possible. Second, there are several friends that deserve special mention. I would like to thank Cody Stoddard, Ryan Randa, Rich Lemke and Jackee Rohrbach. I would also like to thank Jim McCafferty for assisting with my reliability coding, and Kevan Galyean and Cheryl Jonson for answering those random questions. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS ABSTRACT ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS iv LIST OF TABLES viii LIST OF FIGURES ix CHAPTER 1: PROBLEM STATMENT 1 What is Criminal Justice? Criminal Justice Dependent Variable Criminal Justice Theory Duality of criminal justice Meta-analysis, criminal justice and criminal justice theory Criminal Justice Knowledge Synthesis Chapter Summary A Look Ahead: The Plan for this Dissertation 1 3 4 6 7 9 10 11 CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW 13 Situational Theories Gottfredson and Gottfredson Focal concerns theory Actor Theories Workgroup/Organizational Theories Courtroom workgroup Police workgroup Community Theories Black Duffee Wilson Conflict Theory Summary Research Synthesis in Criminal Justice Narrative Reviews Arrest Summary Sentencing Parole revocation Meta-Analysis Gender Race Victim characteristics 14 14 15 16 16 17 19 21 21 23 26 29 30 31 32 33 37 37 38 39 40 40 42 v Chapter Summary 42 CHAPTER 3: META-ANALYSIS: BENEFITS AND CRITIQUES 46 Traditional Narrative Literature Reviews Vote-counting Meta-Analysis as an Alternative Advantages of Meta-Analysis Effect size Methodological adjustments Replication Dynamic Critiques of Meta-Analysis File drawer problem Apples-and-oranges problem Independence of effect sizes Chapter Summary 47 48 49 50 50 51 51 52 52 52 54 55 56 CHAPTER 4: METHODS 59 Research Questions Sample of Studies Inclusion Criteria Selection Criteria Dependent Variable Independent Variables System-wide variables Arrest Sentencing Parole revocation Moderating Variables Study characteristics Methodological quality Reliability of Coding Analytic Strategy Effect size measure Heterogeneity of measures Fail-safe N statistic Conclusions Limitations 59 60 62 62 63 65 65 66 66 66 67 67 68 69 70 70 72 74 74 76 CHAPTER 5: RESULTS 77 Publication Characteristics Arrest Sample characteristics 77 81 81 vi Effect sizes Fail-safe N analysis Moderating variable analysis Sentencing Sample characteristics Effect sizes Fail-safe N analysis Moderating variable analysis Parole Revocation Sample characteristics Effect sizes Fail-safe N analysis Moderating variable analysis Chapter Summary 84 86 87 113 113 116 118 119 138 138 141 142 143 149 CHAPTER 6: DISCUSSION 151 Summary of Findings Missing variables Criminal Justice Theory Organization Toward a General Theory of Criminal Justice Future Research Limitations 152 156 157 159 160 161 REFERENCES 164 APPENDIX A 180 Arrest Coding Guide Sentencing Coding Guide Parole Revocation Coding Guide 180 193 205 vii LIST OF TABLES Table 5.1: Publication Characteristics Table 5.2: Arrest Sample Characteristics Table 5.3: Mean Effect Sizes Arrest Table 5.4: Suspect Race Moderator Analysis Table 5.5: Suspect Gender Moderator Analysis Table 5.6: Suspect Ethnicity Moderator Analysis Table 5.7: Suspect Intoxication Moderator Analysis Table 5.8: Suspect Weapon Use Moderator Analysis Table 5.9: Suspect Demeanor Moderator Analysis Table 5.10: Sentencing Sample Characteristics Table 5.11: Mean Effect Sizes Sentencing Table 5.12: Defendant Race Moderator Analysis Table 5.13: Defendant Gender Moderator Analysis Table 5.14: Defendant Ethnicity Moderator Analysis Table 5.15: Prior Record Moderator Analysis Table 5.16: Attorney Type Moderator Analysis Table 5.17: Mode of Conviction Moderator Analysis Table 5.18: Parole Revocation Sample Characteristics Table 5.19: Mean Effect Sizes Parole Revocation Table 5.20: Offender Race Moderator Analysis Table 5.21: Risk Moderator Analysis viii 80 83 85 91 95 99 102 105 109 115 117 122 125 128 131 134 136 140 141 145 147 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 2.1: Diagram of Gottfredson and Gottfredson’s Theory of Criminal Justice Figure 2.2: Diagram of Focal Concerns Theory Figure 2.3: Diagram of Eisenstein and Jacob’s Courtroom Workgroup Theory Figure 2.4: Diagram of Klinger’s Ecological Theory of Police Behavior Figure 2.5: Diagram of Black’s Theory of the Behavior of Law Figure 2.6: Diagram of Duffee’s Theory of Criminal Justice Figure 2.7: Diagram of Wilson’s Theory of Police Behavior Figure 2.8: Diagram of Scheingold’s Theory of Policy Creation ix 14 15 19 20 22 25 27 29 CHAPTER 1 PROBLEM STATEMENT According to Marenin and Worrall (1998), “criminal justice is an academic discipline in practice but not yet in theory” (p. 465). The quote by Marenin and Worrall sums up the major problem with the field of criminal justice science research. There is a perception that criminal justice science as a field of study is atheoretical. However, in order for any scientific field to become a full-fledged discipline, it must create theory. The field of criminal justice is developing toward a strong scientific discipline. In order to achieve this status and be seen as a scientific discipline in its own right, criminal justice must continue the trend of recent scholarship that has put a direct emphasis on examining this critical issue (see Bernard and Engel, 2001; Crank and Bowman, 2008 and Duffee and Maguire, 2007). The goal of this dissertation is to continue this recent trend of criminal justice theory development by performing a comprehensive review of correlates of decision-making. Through this analysis I will seek to determine the most appropriate way to categorize and organize the field of criminal justice theory. What is Criminal Justice? The concept of criminal justice is complex. Packer (1968) argues that there are two ideal types of criminal justice systems, the due process and crime control models. The due process model is described as an obstacle course, where blockades are constantly put up to prevent any innocent individual from being processed through the system and punished. Conversely, the crime control model is described as an assembly line. Justice must be meted out with consistency to have the impact on criminal conduct. Duffee and Allan (2007) attempt to clarify the study of criminal justice by defining criminal justice science as “the study of governmental 1 social control premised on punishment or blameworthiness” (p. 8). In contrast, Duffee and Allan (2007) define criminology as “the study of crime and criminal behavior” (p. 8). Using these definitions, a study that examines the factors that influence arrest would be a study of criminal justice and a study that looks at the effective crime reduction of a policing intervention would be a study of criminology. The critical difference between the two fields is that criminal justice science seeks to understand the process of the criminal justice system and criminology is concerned with the effectiveness of criminal justice interventions. Thus, criminology is the study of crime and criminal justice is the study of discretion. Discretion is the ability to choose, and within the context of the criminal justice process the decision of interest is whether to process an individual through the criminal justice system (Davis, 1969). Since discretion is the ability to make a choice, it is necessary to consider the definition of a decision. According to Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989), decisions have 3 components: goals, alternatives, and information. By goals, they mean that a decision must have some objective. For example, the decision to arrest may have a goal of taking a suspect away from the person he or she is victimizing. The second component to a decision is alternatives, which means that there must be multiple options. If there was only one option in a “decision,” one could not make a choice. Instead, that person would be mandated to do some act. According to the authors, the last component of a decision is information. Information is the data that are used to make the best possible decision. The best decision is one in which there is little “uncertainty about the consequences of the decision” (Gottfredson and Gottfredson, 1989, p. 3). 2 Criminal Justice Dependent Variable Criminological theories center around one dependent variable, crime. While crime may be operationalized in a variety of ways, criminological research centers on the study of crime and delinquency. The logical organization of criminological theory, since it has one dependent variable, is to organize theories around primary predictive factors. Since criminal justice is a related discipline it would appear to be appropriate to categorize and organize criminal justice theory in a similar manner. However, scholars do not agree that organizing criminal justice theory by key independent variables is possible for criminal justice theory. The disagreement revolves around the number of dependent variables that criminal justice studies. Due to the relatively broad definition above, criminal justice covers a greater number of discrete topics than criminology. Bernard and Engel (2001) subsequently argue that since there are so many dependent variables, criminal justice theories should be categorized around each these dependent variables. This viewpoint creates a problem for criminal justice research. That problem is confusion on the number of dependent variables. Since criminal justice is the study of the process by which formal social control is executed, criminal justice research is chiefly concerned with discretionary application of that social control. Contrary to Bernard and Engel, it is possible to think of all the discrete variables studied in criminal justice as different ways to operationalize discretion. For example, Lundman and Kaufman (2003) and Ulmer, Kurlychek and Kramer (2007) both conducted studies of criminal justice decision making and both used a conflict theory framework. Lundman and Kaufman are studying traffic citations and Ulmer and colleagues are studying judicial discretion in sentencing. Both studies are arguing essentially the same ideas, yet, they operationalize the dependent variable differently. Since they are examining 3 the same idea, these studies should be grouped together. Thus, there are two competing organizational frameworks. One recommends that theories be grouped based on dependent variable then by primary independent variables. The other argues that there is one dependent variable, discretion, and as a result theories should be organized around primary independent variables. Criminal Justice Theory According to Kuhn (1996), for a discipline to be scientific, it must have theory. More importantly, a true scientific discipline requires a paradigm. Kuhn (1996) defines a paradigm as, 1) “an achievement [that] was sufficiently unprecedented to attract an enduring group of adherents away from competing modes of scientific activity” and 2) “it was sufficiently openended to leave all sorts of problems for the redefined group of practitioners to resolve” (p. 10). This means that a paradigm frames a discipline and dictates the important questions. When new information creates a conflict with the current paradigm a process of scientific revolution occurs (Kuhn, 1996). This state of flux is the current state of criminal justice theory. There are many different perspectives operating at multiple levels and the field is waiting for a dominant paradigm to emerge. It may be that criminal justice has created theories but no one theory has supplanted the others to emerge as the paradigm of the field. An equally important and related issue to the paradigm of criminal justice is how criminal justice theory should be organized. However, because of the issue with dependent variables, there appears to be a divide in how to categorize and organize criminal justice theory. There have been two previous significant attempts to categorize, and organize the various perspectives of criminal justice theory. First, Hagan (1989) attempted to categorize criminal justice theory breaking down criminal justice theories into two perspectives: consensus and conflict. He 4 argued that all theories that place the greatest emphasis on legal variables are consensus theories, and those theories that place the greatest emphasis on extra-legal variables would be conflict theories. Some scholars rejected this method as too simple. Second, Bernard and Engel (2001) argued that criminal justice theories should be organized by the dependent variable and then by key independent variables. They argued for grouping theories around three broad categories of dependent variables: individual criminal justice agents, criminal justice organizations, and overall system and its components. Under this scheme, conflict theory could be split across all three variable types when the theory is applying the same concepts to explaining the process of criminal justice, just on different dependent variables. These two perspectives create a fundamental problem, should criminal justice theories focus only on the system as a whole or should criminal justice theories be decision-specific? If criminal justice theories are grander theoretical approaches focusing on the system as a whole, then predictors in criminal justice theory will be consistent across the system as a whole. Under this grand theory perspective, predictors of decision-making will be the same at each stage because the same theoretical concepts are impacting decision makers in the same way. For example, Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989) argue that three factors: seriousness of the offense, victim-offender relationship and criminal history predict the actions of decision makers across the system. From this perspective, these three factors are the primary predictors of criminal justice actor behavior for all criminal justice actors. Duffee and Allan (2007) argue that criminal justice theories need not address all parts of the system to be a theory of criminal justice. Under this perspective, criminal justice theories may be decision-specific and factors may vary across decision points. For example, Eisenstein and Jacob (1997) argue that dynamics of the courtroom workgroup are the key predictors of 5 court-case decision making. Conversely, Wilson (1968) argues that the local political culture and the effectiveness of the chief of police in controlling his or her officers are the key to predicting the type of police department, which in turn influences the behavior of officers. Under the grand theory perspective, correlates of decision-making will be consistent across the system and various decision points within each component. If the decision-specific perspective is correct, then correlates of decision-making will be different across the system and each decision point. Duality of criminal justice Another issue to consider, as part of the conception of criminal justice decision-making research, is the issue of the duality of criminal justice. Several scholars have commented on the duality of the American criminal justice system, for example, Packer (see above). More recently, Gottfredson and Gottfredson addressed this issue of the duality of criminal justice as a function of seriousness of the offense Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989) argue that there are two criminal justice systems, one for serious offenses and a second for less serious offenses. They noticed that decision-making research indicated that criminal justice actors exhibited less variation in the exercise of their discretion in certain situations. Low-variation discretionary situations were characterized by serious criminal incidents, those committed by offenders with long criminal histories, and/or those cases where the victim and offender are strangers to each other. In these cases, there was “very little discretion exhibited at any of the major criminal justice decisions” (Gottfredson and Gottfredson, 1989, p. 260). It is near certainty that these serious situations will be processed through the criminal justice system, provided there is sufficient evidence. 6 The second criminal justice system is one that involves the opposite type of incidents from the first system. Cases in this second criminal justice system involve less serious crimes, those committed by individuals with short or no criminal history, and/or crimes between those with a closer relational distance. Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989) argue that this second system is where extralegal factors become more prominent because of greater amounts of discretion afforded to the criminal justice actor. For example, in this second criminal justice system demeanor of the suspect may play a larger role in the decision to make an arrest. Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989) assert that this second system is created because 1) “the desire by some to pursue alternatives to full processing”, 2) “the inconsistency arising from a lack of explicit guidelines”, and/or 3) “the result from the use of invidious decision criteria” (p. 261). It is in this second system that there is less certainty that a case will be processed fully through the criminal justice system and greater variation in the exercise of discretion. It appears that criminal justice decision making may be a function of offense seriousness. This means that to fully examine the effect of predictors, other than offense seriousness, offense seriousness must first be controlled. After offense seriousness is controlled, the effect of other predictors, such as suspect characteristics, criminal justice agent characteristics, organizational characteristics or community characteristics may explain the remaining variation in decision making. Meta-analysis, criminal justice and criminal justice theory In order to evaluate these organization methods, and this idea of the duality of criminal justice, it is necessary to take stock of existing research and evaluate the research on correlates of decision-making. One approach is to use narrative literature reviews. While this approach has been a good first step, a newer and potentially superior technique for research synthesis is meta- 7 analysis. Meta-analysis statistically examines the effect of independent variables on the dependent variable across studies. According to Hunt (1997), meta-analysis combines the effects of variables across studies and creates a single measure of strength for that predictor on the dependent variable. This technique has been used to evaluate criminological theories and other anti-crime interventions (see Hubbard and Pratt, 2002; Makarios and Pratt, 2012; Pratt, Cullen, Blevins, Daigle, and Madensen 2006; and Pratt and Cullen, 2000, 2005). This technique can be applied to evaluate the issue of a grand theory approach, or a decision-specific approach. Meta-analysis can be used to evaluate the correlates of decision-making across the system. If a majority of the same correlates are significant predictors across the system, then the grand theory approach would appear to be the most appropriate organization method. However, if each decision yields a number of unique correlates, then criminal justice theories should be organized around specific decisions. Moreover, for the decision-specific model to be the preferred model, unique factors would also have to play a larger role than system-wide factor. This requirement demonstrates that there is some quality about each decision that is a stronger factor than system-wide factors. It is likely not possible to fully explore the idea of the duality of criminal justice using meta-analysis. None of the studies separated their samples based on the seriousness of their cases. However, the idea may be partially supported if several conditions are met. First, seriousness of the offense must be paramount. The duality idea proposed by Gottfredson and Gottfredson hinges on the idea that there is low variation in the exercise of discretion for serious incidents and higher variation in the exercise of discretion in less serious incidents. This means that the mean effect sizes for measures of seriousness must be stronger than the mean effect sizes for other predictors. Second, the mean effect size for seriousness measures must indicate that as 8 seriousness increases, the likelihood of processing also increases. There must be a positive relationship between the dependent variable, arrest, sentencing or parole revocation, and the independent variable. Third, mean effect sizes of seriousness measures must be significant across decisions. Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989) indicated that their finding regarding the effect of seriousness was consistent at any decision point. Criminal Justice Knowledge Synthesis A second issue with criminal justice research is the lack of a comprehensive synthesis of criminal justice correlates across the system. Over thirty years ago, Gottfredson and Gottfredson undertook an attempt to synthesize knowledge of correlates of criminal justice decision-making. No study since has attempted to replicate their work on a similar scale. Instead, research synthesis studies have focused on one branch of the criminal justice system or on one decision point in the criminal justice system. Synthesis research has also at times focused too myopically on one particular variable, e.g., race, rather than take stock of the entire field of criminal justice decision making. When the results from these individual branch/decision points are combined a picture begins to emerge. That picture indicates that similar correlates of decision-making have been examined at multiple stages of criminal justice processing. For example, race, gender, and age are constantly examined in numerous studies. Yet, often the effects of these variables are conflicting from study to study. This conflict creates problems of interpretation in research synthesis. The relationship of suspect demeanor to arrest is a more specific example of research conflict. Generally, the effect of suspect demeanor as a predictor of arrest was that disrespectful suspects were more likely to be arrested than those that deferred to the officer’s authority (see Smith, Visher, and Davidson, 1984; Worden, 1989; and Worden and Shepard, 1996). However, 9 Klinger (1994) argued that suspects that were disrespectful were not more likely to be arrested. Instead these supposedly disrespectful suspects were actually committing new crimes during the interaction phase of the officer-suspect encounter. This interaction phase crime was responsible for the arrest, not demeanor. Klinger (1994) found that demeanor was not a statistically significant predictor when controlling for interaction-phase crime. Since Klinger’s study, Mastrofski, Snipes, Parks and Maxwell (2000) found that police were not significantly influenced by a suspect’s disrespect. Meta-analysis can provide a solution to the problem of conflicting information. If the effect size for a particular variable is significant then that variable would be seen as critical to the understanding of that decision. For example, if measures of suspect demeanor have an overall significant effect, then Klinger (1994) is incorrect and suspect demeanor plays a role in arrest decisions. If demeanor does not produce a significant effect size, then Klinger is correct and demeanor does not influence officer decision making and instead it is some other mechanism. Meta-analysis has had limited implementation in criminal justice research, but is an analytical technique that is expanding in popularity. The potential benefits of the technique have had even less use in criminal justice decision-making research outside of sentencing. Chapter Summary There are several problems with the current state of criminal justice research. First, there is a perception that the discipline of criminal justice is atheoretical. This problem leads to the discipline of criminal justice being seen as a secondary or applied science. This lack of theory development leads to disagreement as to appropriate scope of theories within the discipline, namely, a system-wide approach versus a decision-specific approach. One goal of this dissertation is to examine this issue and see if correlates of decision-making are similar across 10 the system or if they are unique to particular decision points. If correlates are consistent across the system, a system-wide approach would appear to be more applicable. However, if correlates vary from decision point to decision point, then decision-specific explanations of criminal justice behavior would be more appropriate. In other words, the goal is to determine if there is a possibility to create a general theory criminal justice or should the field continue on its current trajectory which focuses more on decision-specific theorizing. Second, there is a lack of updated knowledge as to correlates of criminal justice processing. There are a number of predictors that may influence decision making across the system. However, past efforts at synthesis research were simple literature reviews. These literature reviews led researchers to treat variables with mixed results as non-significant. One way to combat this problem is through meta-analysis, which allows for synthesis of statistical information across studies. The potential solution to both of these issues involves examination of the correlates of criminal justice decision making using meta-analysis to make more definitive rejections or acceptance of various influences on decision making. A Look Ahead: The Plan for this Dissertation The primary goal of this dissertation is to continue what Gottfredson and Gottfredson began over thirty years ago. They examined the correlates of criminal justice decision-making across the system. This dissertation will also examine correlates of decision-making across the system. Specifically I examine correlates of arrest, sentencing, and parole revocation. Each of these decisions was chosen because 1) each of these decision points are completely within a particular branch of the criminal justice system and, 2) each decision point represents one of the main functions of that branch of the criminal justice system. Unlike Gottfredson and Gottfredson, this dissertation will use meta-analysis to fully examine the effects of various 11 predictors of arrest, sentencing, and parole revocation. The purpose of this examination is to attempt to determine the best way to organize criminal justice theory and to determine what correlates should be included in any theory of criminal justice. In the chapters ahead, Chapter 2 presents a review of current criminal justice theories and current knowledge of correlates of decision-making based, primarily on the narrative literature reviews. Chapter 3 presents a critique of narrative literature review methods and the benefits of meta-analysis. Chapter 4 outlines the research questions, sample generation procedures, and statistical procedures of the analyses of this dissertation. Chapter 5 discusses the results of the meta-analysis. Finally, Chapter 6 discusses conclusions and provides a general summary of this dissertation. 12 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW While criminal justice theory is still developing and organizing, there are distinct and established theoretical perspectives. These theoretical perspectives vary on two key criteria. First, variation occurs because of the theory’s primary predictive variables. Many scholars have suggested that certain factors may influence the behavior of criminal justice agents. Each scholar that has theorized a perspective has stipulated that there are certain key predictors. For example community theories emphasize certain aspects of the macro-social community. The theoretical categories are: situational theories, actor theories, workgroup/organizational theories, community theories, and conflict theories. The second point of variation is the scope of the theory, total-system versus decision-specific theories. Total-system theories seek to predict the use of discretion across the criminal justice system, while decision-specific theories are directed at one particular decision. First, there is a discussion of each theoretical perspective in criminal justice. Following each theory explanation, there is a discussion of the current evidence for each theory. Second, this chapter explores the current state of knowledge regarding each decision point examined in this dissertation. There is a discussion of arrest, sentencing, and parole revocation. The section reviewing decision making is divided into two subsections. The first subsection contains information based on narrative literature reviews. The second subsection contains information based on meta-analyses. 13 Situational Theories Situational theories are those theories of criminal justice where the main predictive elements are aspects of the context in which discretion is exercised. Situational characteristics are often studied in decision-making research and as such it is natural that theories emerge emphasizing these factors. These theories promote the idea that the criminal justice system does not operate in a vacuum and aspects of the criminal event can and will influence, at least in part, the decisions of the criminal justice actors and the system. Two current situational theories are Gottfredson and Gottfredson’s theory of criminal justice and focal concerns theory. Gottfredson and Gottfredson Gottfredson and Gottfredson’s theory states that situational factors about the criminal event dictate the decisions made by criminal justice actors (see the discussion in Chapter 1). These situational factors influence the amount of the exercise discretion for a particular decision, which ultimately leads to the particular processing decision. Through their analysis, they find that these factors continually inform all decision points in the criminal justice system, from the decision of the victim to contact the police through the parole release decision. Numerous empirical assessments since Gottfredson and Gottfredson discussed their theory have confirmed the impact of seriousness of the offense, prior record, and victim-offender relationship. See the discussion below on narrative reviews of decision making for more information. Figure 2.1: Diagram of Gottfredson and Gottfredson’s theory of criminal justice Prior record Seriousness of the offense Discretion Victim-offender relationship 14 Processing decision Focal concerns theory Focal concerns theory is a theory of the exercise of discretion. The theory stipulates that criminal justice decision making is based on three focal concerns (Steffensmeier, Kramer, and Ulmer, 1998). According to Steffensmeier et al. (1998), these focal concerns are blameworthiness of the offender, protection of the community, and practical constraints and consequences. Blameworthiness deals with the culpability of the offender in the criminal act and the degree of injury. Protection of the community pertains to the potential for future harm from a particular offender, and an element of deterrence of other offenders. Finally, Steffensmeier and his colleagues explain that practical constraints and consequences are other miscellaneous considerations like the offender’s ability to survive the punishment, and organizational issues such as the availability of resources and space. These focal concerns shape the exercise of a criminal justice actor’s discretion and determine their response in a particular criminal event. Figure 2.2: Diagram of focal concerns theory Blameworthiness Protection of the community Sentence Practical constraints Empirical assessments of focal concerns theory have produced findings consistent with the theory. In addition to Steffensmeier’s and his colleague’s initial assessment of focal concerns theory, many other studies have found results consistent with the theory (Johnson, 2003; Johnson, 2005; Johnson 2006; Kramer and Ulmer, 2002; Steffensmeier and Britt, 2001; Ulmer and Johnson, 2004; Ulmer, Kurlychek, and Kramer, 2007). These studies have consistently shown that male individuals, convicted of more serious offenses, with longer criminal histories are more likely to not only be incarcerated but also receive longer prison 15 sentences. These offenders are considered more blameworthy and able to survive the prison environment. Additionally, these studies found that those that committed more serious violent crime were more likely to receive longer prison terms. Actor Theories The critical component that sets actor theories of criminal justice apart from other theories is that actor theories place a greater emphasis on the individual actors in the criminal justice system. While the individual is part of the situation, situational theories place a greater emphasis on the totality of circumstances. Actor theories place a greater emphasis on one aspect of the situation, the individual criminal justice actor. While there is not a specific theory of criminal justice based on actor characteristics, it is a studied area of decision-making research and is a plausible explanation of criminal justice behavior. If criminal justice actor characteristics emerge as a significant correlate of decision-making, then a theory should be developed focusing on this particular set of predictors. Workgroup/Organizational Theories Criminal justice actors act within the context of a situation, but there are also collective aspects of their working environment and organization that influence their behavior and the exercise of their discretion. This idea is the central theme of workgroup and organizational theories. Workgroup theories argue that the collective norms that develop between coworkers within an organization are the key factors in the exercise of discretion. Two examples of these types of theories are Eisenstein and Jacob’s courtroom workgroup theory and Klinger’s ecological theory of police behavior. 16 Courtroom workgroup Eisenstein and Jacob (1977) postulate that it is the courtroom workgroup that heavily influences discretionary decisions made in the criminal justice system and the criminal courts specifically. By workgroup, Eisenstein and Jacob are referring to the main three courtroom actors, the judge, prosecutor, and defense attorney. In their original conceptualization of their workgroup theory, they argue that the political environment influences both the sponsoring organization characteristics and the workgroup characteristics. These sponsoring organization characteristics and workgroup characteristics then directly influence the outcomes of a case. The political environment also directly impacts the outcomes of a case, and the sponsoring organizations also impact the workgroup characteristics. Each of the above variables includes multiple components. The political environment incorporates the larger contextual environment. The police are seen as gatekeepers of the system; if they do not make arrests then the courts have no cases to process. Therefore, Eisenstein and Jacob (1977) argue that police are part of the political environment and context. Specifically, they argue that police influence cases via the quality of evidence they collect. The next component of the political context is the legislature and appellate courts. Each of these entities creates rules that the workgroup must follow; the legislature via creation of the court system and establishment of the law and appellate courts by creating procedural rules that the workgroup must follow. According to Eisenstein and Jacob (1977), the next component is prison authorities. This entity influences case processing because they regulate sentencing, judges may only sentence defendants to the available space. The final influential member of the environment is the media. The authors explain that the sponsoring agencies and the media have 17 a reciprocal relationship. The sponsoring agencies want to be portrayed positively so they use the media for that end and the media uses the sponsoring agencies for news content. Workgroups are complex entities. There is more to the workgroup than simply who is a member. Many factors influence workgroups. Eisenstein and Jacob (1977) explain that the degree of workgroup stability and the degree of familiarity that members have with one another influence the workgroup. They argue that workgroups that are stable and familiar with one another are able to process cases more informally and rely less on the adversarial process and more on a negotiation style of case disposition. Additionally, each workgroup works toward certain goals. The authors explain that goals are shaped by two factors. The first is the origin of the goal. Origin refers to where the goal originates, either external or internal to the workgroup. For example, a goal that results from pressures from one of the entities of the political environment is an external goal. The second goal-shaping component is the function of the goal. The authors explain that a goal may be expressive or symbolic in nature, or a goal may be instrumental, or focused on improving efficiency. Each combination of these two categories produces a different goal that in turn influences the workgroup. The final independent variable in the workgroup theory is the supporting agency characteristics. The supporting agencies are those agencies that have control over the personnel of the workgroup. Eisenstein and Jacob (1977) argue that since these agencies have control over the workgroup personnel, recruitment and assignment procedures impact the workgroup because of their effects on stability and familiarity. Assignment procedures that constantly change the workgroup create instability in the workgroup and the workgroup begins to rely more on the adversarial process versus negotiations. Eisenstein and Jacob (1977) also argue that the amount of control a sponsoring agency has over it’s members influences the workgroup. For example, if 18 a prosecutor’s office creates a policy that restricts plea-bargaining in felony cases, then prosecutors must turn toward a more adversarial case processing because their ability to negotiate has been reduced by the supporting agency Figure 2.3: Diagram of Eisenstein and Jacob’s courtroom workgroup theory Sponsoring Organization Characteristics Informal Norms Political Environment Outcomes Workgroup Characteristics Police workgroup Klinger provides another workgroup theory that is aimed at explaining the exercise of police discretion. Klinger (1997) argues that the amount of deviance in a district leads to an overall district workload, but also impacts officers’ perceptions of deviance in that area and the seriousness of the immediate offense. As the district workload also increases, resource constraints increase along with perceptions of deviance among officers. He argues that an officer’s understanding of district deviance impacts what crimes they perceive as normal, the deservedness of victims and levels of police cynicism. As perceptions of deviance increase, more serious crimes become “normal”, victims become less deserving, and police officers become more cynical. According to Klinger (1997), resource constraints, police cynicism, deservedness of victims, and perceptions of normal crime all impact workgroup rules about discretion. These workgroup rules about police discretion then directly impact the exercise of discretion. Finally, the seriousness of the immediate offense also directly impacts police action. 19 Figure 2.4: Diagram of Klinger’s ecological theory of police behavior Level of district deviance Seriousness of the immediate offense Normal crimes Officer’s understanding of district deviance Deserving victims Group rules Vigor Police cynicism District workload Resource constraints Recently scholars have begun to test Klinger’s theory. Sobol (2010) attempted a partial test of Klinger’s theory examining measures of cynicism and district workload on police vigor. He found that neither of these two variables were significantly associated with vigor in the full model. Workload was significantly related to vigor at the bivariate level but the relationship was rendered non-significant in the full model, which lead Sobol (2010) to conclude that the relationship was spurious. While not a complete test, Philips and Sobol (2011) were able to support the workload hypothesis of Klinger’s theory. They found that officers that worked in districts with higher workloads were less likely to view force as unnecessary. None of the above studies were able to completely test Klinger’s theory. Both studies were unable to tap into victim deservedness. Another limitation of Philips and Sobol’s (2011) study is that they used vignettes and were not able to examine police cynicism. 20 Community Theories Community theories argue that the larger community is the driving factor in criminal justice decision making. In other words, the collective norms and attitudes of the community dictate the type of criminal justice organizations and thus, the actions of the individual criminal justice actors. Three examples of these types of theories are Black’s theory of the behavior of law, Duffee’s community theory of criminal justice, and Wilson’s community theory of police. Black (1976) argues that the law, which is defined as “governmental social control” (p.2) and can be interpreted as the discretionary action of a criminal justice system, is the product of five macro-social constructs: stratification, morphology, culture, organization, and social-control. Duffee (1990) argues that criminal justice action is the result of a type of community. That community is the product of two characteristics vertical relations and horizontal articulation. Each of these constructs leads to a particular type of community, which leads to a particular type of criminal justice organization. These organizations will have differing approaches to criminal justice. Finally, Wilson (1968) argues that the local political culture of the community is critical in determining the type of police agency in the community. The critical component in all three of these theories is the role of the community in shaping the type of criminal justice exercised within that community. Black Black argues that five predictor variables: stratification, morphology, culture, organization, and social control, influence the behavior of law. According to Black (1976), stratification is a measure of the wealth of a society, and law varies directly with stratification. Black (1976) also states that stratification is “the vertical distance between the people of a social setting” (p. 13). In other words, stratification refers to a measure of social status. Morphology is 21 a person’s relational distance to another individual; it is a person’s level of interconnectedness. He explains that if stratification is the vertical distance of society, then morphology is the horizontal distance of society. This concept is similar to collective efficacy. According to Sampson, Raudenbush and Earls (1997), collective efficacy is a community’s level of integration. When communities have a greater degree of interconnection they are able to police themselves and have less reliance on formal social control. Black (1976) explains that the relationship between law and morphology is curvilinear. Black’s final three variables are culture, organization, social control. According to Black (1976), “culture is the symbolic aspect of social life, including expressions of what is true, good, and beautiful” (p. 61). Culture incorporates a society’s level of education, religion, and technology. Black (1976) states that societies with more culture will have more law. The fourth variable is organization. Organization is a society’s ability to use collective action. He argues that societies with greater amounts of organization will have greater amounts of law. The final variable is social control. Black (1976) explains that social control is society’s informal social control. Communities that have greater amounts of social control will have less law. Figure 2.5: Diagram of Black’s theory of the behavior of law Stratification Morphology Culture Law Organization Social Control Empirical assessments of Black’s theory have been partially supportive. Gottfredson and Hindelang (1979) and Braithwaite and Biles (1980) tested the theory and found that some of 22 Black’s constructs did not hold up empirically. Gottfredson and Hindelang (1979) found support for organization; they found that businesses were more likely to report their victimization to the police. There was also support for culture, measured as education, but the effect of culture was small. The finding for the culture construct was not as large as Black indicated it should have been. Braithwaite and Biles (1980) also found that there was limited evidence in support of Black’s theory. More recent examinations of Black’s theory have found similar results to previous examinations. Kuo, Longmire, Cuvelier, and Chang (2010) found that measures of culture and organization significantly impacted prosecutorial decision making in Taiwan in the directions predicted by Black. Clay-Warner and McMahon-Howard (2009) found that one measure of stratification, household income, significantly predicted rape reporting. Additionally, they found that as relational distance increased odds of reporting increased. There were no significant results for the other constructs: organization, culture, or social control. Duffee Duffee (1980) argues that two factors, a community’s vertical relations and a community’s horizontal articulation, shape the community’s criminal justice system. Vertical relations refer to a community’s reliance on other non-local organizations. Communities that have a high level of vertical relations are reliant on private or the state and Federal Government to provide goods and services. Duffee (1980) describes horizontal articulation in similar terms to Black’s concept of morphology; it is the degree of cohesiveness of the community. Communities with strong horizontal articulation “interface and cooperate with each other” (Duffee, 1980, p. 153). Populations that live in communities with high levels of horizontal articulation are homogeneous and share similar norms and values. 23 Duffee (1980) describes each of the four types of communities based on the level amount of vertical relations and horizontal articulation. The first community, disorganized, has both low vertical relations and low horizontal articulation. Norms in these communities are not well defined. Duffee (1980) explains that “there will be no group boundaries against which to define deviant action, and on the other hand there will be no processes by which groups inculcate mutual standards in individuals” (p. 155). Attempts at formal control have little legitimacy. When criminal justice action does occur it is limited to major issues and crises. Criminal justice agents are seen as outsiders and any criminal justice action is seen as coercive. The second type of community, solidary, has low vertical relations and high horizontal articulation. Duffee (1980) argues that the norms of this community are so well established that there is little need for formal sanctions. He states that enforcement is often based on peer pressure. This community is self-contained and is able to control actions informally rather than formally. Formal sanctions that take place will need to conform to local goals and norms. The third type of community has high vertical relations but low horizontal articulation. This community is described as a fragmented community (Duffee, 1980). According to Duffee, (1980) this community uses organizations that focus on special individual clients “without regard for the community” (p. 157). This community type has unclear norms. They also have a high reliance on external entities. The combination of these two situations creates an environment where multiple organizations have very specific functions. These specifically focused organizations often have turf wars. The focus of these agencies tends to be client centered. In these communities criminal justice is concerned with “reduction of future crime[…]rather than a concern for maintaining an existing normative pattern” (Duffee, 1980, p. 157). 24 The final community has high vertical relations and high horizontal articulation. According to Duffee (1980), this community is interdependent; it wants to rely on outsiders to provide services, but at the same time it is interconnected to be self-sufficient. This community is somewhat odd and not likely to exist. Like the solidary community type they can police themselves because they have good integration amongst members, but yet they rely on an outside authority due to the high vertical relations. Duffee explains that “administration of formal organizations involved in social control would seem to be a difficult activity here” (Duffee, 1980, p. 157). There is a reliance on complex outside funded entities, but community members will likely be dissatisfied with the services provided when services are not directed toward the community as a whole. Figure 2.6: Diagram of Duffee’s theory of criminal justice Vertical relations Community type Execution of criminal justice Horizontal articulation While not explicitly testing Duffee’s theory, Liederbach and Travis (2008) found that police agency style, a proxy for criminal justice system type, was dependent upon community size. Specifically, they found that larger communities tended toward legalistic type department. Smaller communities tended toward order maintenance policing. Legalistic departments tend toward the law enforcement function and rely on the formal sanction to deal with problems (Wilson, 1968). As the community grows larger it cannot maintain that high level of horizontal articulation and begins to rely on outsiders for criminal justice services. They are simply too big to maintain close interpersonal relationships within the community as a whole. Smaller 25 communities can move away from formal law enforcement function because they may be more likely to have higher levels of horizontal articulation and thus, can resolve matters informally. Wilson Criminal justice systems and specifically policing systems in a community may be influenced by the local political culture of the community. Wilson (1968) argues that local political culture plays an important role in the creation of a certain type of police department. Political culture has little direct impact on the actual decision making of police officers, but it does impact organizational and policy issues, and choosing a chief. Because the chief plays an important role in controlling officers’ behavior via policy, the community gains control over officers through the local political culture. Wilson (1968) states that city managers and mayors were concerned about illegal business and those political leaders placed pressure on the chief to intervene, and the chief emphasized illegal business issues to his officers. The local political culture and chief led to the creation of a certain type of police department. According to Wilson (1968), there are three types of departments: legalistic, watchmen, and service. Each one of the departments emphasizes a different function of police. Legalistic departments focus on exercise of formal authority. They emphasize the law enforcement function. Service departments emphasize the service function. Officers in these departments tend to resolve problems informally. Finally, watchmen departments emphasize the order maintenance function. These officers use a combination of formal and informal sanctions to execute the order maintenance function. The type of department then influences how officers interact with citizens. Wilson (1968) explains that there are four types of discretionary situations and responses are based on whether or not the police have frequent or infrequent contact with citizens and 26 whether or not the situation is formal or informal. Liederbach and Travis (2008), argue that frequent, formal intervention is common in legalistic departments. These police departments stress law enforcement and these officers issue many citations and make more arrests. Service departments also intervene regularly but the solutions and issues tend to be less formal. The emphasis on informality is consistent with officers in service agencies. Finally, officers in watchmen agencies do not intervene often with citizens, and when interventions do occur they tend to be a mix of formal and informal. Thus, officers in these agencies emphasize the order maintenance function. Figure 2.7: Diagram of Wilson’s theory of police behavior Community Local political culture Dept. type Primary police function Chief Wilson’s theory has been assessed, at least in part, several times. The results have been mixed. Langworthy (1985) assessed the relationship between arrest rates and government type, i.e., what Wilson identified as a good government run by a city manager as opposed to a more traditional mayoral system of government. He found that it was indeed the case that arrest rates for marginal crimes varied based on the government type. While not a perfect test of Wilson’s theory, Langworthy’s test did partially support Wilson’s conception of police behavior. Crank (1990) also found support for the government-arrest hypothesis. He found that rural municipalities were significantly more likely to have higher arrest rates for trespassing, disorderly conduct, motor vehicle theft, and cannabis control. In urban environments, police in areas that were run by city managers were significantly more likely to make arrests for motor 27 vehicle theft, but not for the other crime categories. Additionally, Smith (1984) found that correlates of arrest varied depending on the police style of the department. He concluded that any explanation of legal force would be incomplete without a discussion of police organizational characteristics. While some scholars have found limited support for Wilson’s theory, other scholars have not found the same results when testing Wilson’s theory. Several studies have failed to find a relationship between political culture and police organization (Hassell, Zhao, and Maguire, 2003; Liederbach and Travis, 2008; Zhao and Hassell, 2005; and Zhao, Ren, and Lovrich, 2010). Zhao and Hassell (2005) went on to claim that this consistent lack of a finding of a correlation between political structure and police style was evidence of falsification of Wilson’s theory. However, Liederbach and Travis (2008) were not as quick to signal the end of Wilson’s theory. Instead, they argued that no assessment had fully replicated Wilson’s original test and that each test, including theirs, was subject to issues of model misspecification. The primary model misspecification is that no study to date had adequately examined the correct combination of communities. Often communities were fairly constant in assessments of Wilson’s theory. In sum, three scholars created a theory that places an emphasis on the impact of the community. Black outlines the social dynamics of communities that lead to the amount of formal criminal justice processing. Duffee argued that qualities of a community, vertical relations and horizontal articulation, led to the creation of a certain type of criminal justice system, which dictated how offenders are processed. Finally, Wilson stated that local political culture has an impact on the type of police department and the chief; who sets policy that dictates how officers handle situations. 28 Conflict Theory The final theoretical perspective is the conflict perspective. Conflict theory argues that macro-social group dynamics influence the behavior of criminal justice actors. According to Hagan (1989), conflict theory argues that extra-legal factors like race or gender will play a larger role in criminal justice decision making. Scheingold’s (1984) conception of conflict theory argues that increases in victimization, both direct and vicarious victimization via media, increase fear, which leads to increased punitive decision making. Under Scheingold’s (1984) model extra-legal factors play a large role in vicarious victimization; vicarious victimization occurs due to increased media attention, which is the result of increased levels of crime. Additionally, Scheingold (1984) argues that punitive policy responses create yet more increased media attention on crime. Thus, culturally driven fear creates a picture of a typical offender which leads to punitive punishments and punitive decision making toward that group. Figure 2.8: Diagram of Scheingold’s theory of policy creation Increased crime victimization non-punitive response non-punitive policy FEAR Increased media attention vicarious victimization punitive response punitive policy More recently, conflict theory has emerged as a form of the minority group threat hypothesis/perspective. Under this perspective, as certain minority groups increase in size they become more threatening and laws are created to keep that minority population in check (Kane, 2002). Kane (2002) argues that as threatening groups increase in size police are deployed to those areas and use stricter enforcement practices. While he specifically applies the minority 29 group threat hypothesis to police decision making, the same idea can be applied to the other branches of the criminal justice system. According to Hagan (1989), conflict theory is examined when scholars look at extra-legal explanations of criminal justice outcomes. Many scholars have examined the impact of these extra-legal variables such as age, race, ethnicity and gender on criminal justice decision making. Empirical assessments of these relationships have been mixed (see below). Summary There are a variety of criminal justice theory perspectives. These theories are broken down into 5 classifications based on the primary predictive variables. These five theory categories are: situational, actor, organizational/workgroup, community and conflict perspectives. Gottfredson and Gottfredson and Steffensmeier and colleagues created situational theories that stress the importance of the context in which the criminal event occurs in order to explain why individuals are processed through the system. Eisenstein and Jacob and Klinger both created theories that emphasize the role of the workgroup on actor decision making. Black, Duffee, and Wilson all created theories that emphasize the role of community as a predictive factor of the criminal justice process. Black emphasized the dynamics of societal interaction, and Duffee emphasized the two elements of the community, vertical relations and horizontal articulation. Wilson argued that local political culture influence police departments and through departments the actions of officers. Lastly, several conflict perspectives have emerged each emphasizing the role of societal conflict on criminal justice decision making. Some perspectives apply a system-wide approach. For example, Gottfredson and Gottfredson, Duffee, and Black all created theories to explain the process of criminal justice generally. Other scholars created theories that focus on a particular branch of the criminal justice 30 system, for example, Steffensmeier and colleagues, Eisenstein and Jacob, Klinger, and Wilson. Some of the decision-specific theories can be applied to other components of the criminal justice system. Focal concerns theory was created as a way to explain judicial discretion. However, the concepts of blameworthiness, protection of the community, and other practical concerns are applicable to other criminal justice decision points and decision makers. For example, an officer making an arrest could very easily be deciding who to arrest based on all three of the factors discussed in focal concerns theory. Additionally, when some decision-specific theories are considered together the key components appear to be applied at multiple stages of the criminal justice system. For example, both workgroup theories are decision-specific but examining the theories in concert shows how the concept of a workgroup can apply across the system. The workgroup may change slightly, but the concept is consistent across the system. Research Synthesis in Criminal Justice Now that each of the theoretical perspectives has been discussed, it is important to take stock of the current empirical knowledge on criminal justice decision making. The research synthesis takes two primary forms, narrative literature reviews and meta-analysis.1 Criminal justice has traditionally used narrative reviews to assess correlates of decision-making (see Gottfredson and Gottfredson, 1989; National Research Council, 2004, 2008; Riksheim and Chermak, 1993; Sherman, 1980; and Spohn, 2000). With the exception of Gottfredson and Gottfredson, each of the aforementioned reviews focuses on one decision point and at times on the effects of one particular variable (see Spohn, 2000).2 The use of meta-analysis has been much more limited. Meta-analysis has been primarily used to examine correlates of sentencing, but recently meta-analysis was used to examine the effect of suspect race on arrest (see Daly and 1 The merits and difficulties of each form of synthesis are discussed in detail in the next chapter. Since Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989) has been discussed in detail above there is no need to include another discussion of their work. 2 31 Bordt, 1995; Kochel et al., 2011; Mitchell, 2005; Pratt, 1998; and Whatley, 1996). Each of these meta-analyses, discussed in detail below, focuses both on only one decision point and on one particular relationship. Correlates of decision-making have been a long-standing research interest of criminal justice science for some time. The vast majority of synthesis literature has been decisionspecific; emphasis has been given to one particular decision point. The discussion that follows will first discuss narrative literature reviews. Finally, this section concludes with a discussion of the current meta-analytic studies on criminal justice decision making. Narrative Reviews Arrest Several studies have summarized the correlates of arrest. The first study to systematically evaluate the correlates of arrest was Sherman (1980). Sherman’s study was followed by Riksheim and Chermak (1993). Finally, the National Research Council (2004) reviewed the correlates of arrest as part of a larger examination of all aspects of policing research. Each of these studies is a narrative review and each will be reviewed in detail below. Sherman Sherman’s (1980) study was the first narrative review of the correlates of police officer decision making. Sherman (1980) separates explanations of officer behavior into five classifications: individual, situations, organizational, community and legal. With regard to individual explanations, Sherman (1980) found that less experienced officers, male officers, black officers, better educated officers, officers with greater job satisfaction, and officers that had negative views of black individuals were more likely to make arrests. Situational factors that increased the odds of arrest included: when officers proactively enter into a police-citizen 32 encounter, when more officers are present at the police-citizen encounter, when suspects are disrespectful, when the victim does not know the offender, when suspects are younger, when suspects are male, and when suspects are members of the lower class. Additionally, when the complainant prefers arrest, and when the complainant is respectful, an officer is more likely to make an arrest. He also notes that legal variables play a significant role in predicting arrest. Offense seriousness, and the amount of evidence increase the odds of arrest. There was a large amount of research on individual and situational effects on arrest. Sherman (1980) states that there has been little examination of the organizational effects on arrest behavior. Finally, Community variables have also been neglected in terms of examination regarding their impact on arrest behavior. Riksheim and Chermak Riksheim and Chermak (1993) separate arrest predictors into four categories: individuallevel, situational-level, organizational-level, and community-level. They depart from Sherman slightly by collapsing legal-based explanations into situational-level variables. They organize their discussion of correlates as pre- and post-1980. The increased amount of research changed the impact of individual factors. Riksheim and Chermak (1993) found that an officer’s education level had a mixed impact. They found that some research showed that an individual officer’s education had no impact on arrest, but some departments with better-educated officers had fewer arrests. They cited other research, which reported that the impact of education was conditional on situational factors. Officer experience as a predictor of arrest also became more muddled. Some studies found evidence that was consistent with Sherman, and others found that there was no effect of officer experience on arrest. Post-1980 research appears to contradict earlier findings with regard to the effect of 33 officer race, gender and attitudes. Riksheim and Chermak (1993) found that these variables had no effect on arrest in studies conducted post-1980. However, this finding may be a methodological artifact due to the majority of pre-1980 studies focusing on bivariate relationships and post-1980 there was an increase in the use of multivariate statistics. Situational characteristics are likely the most commonly studied correlate of officer decision making and there is little consistency in findings from before 1980 to those findings from studies after 1980. Riksheim and Chermak (1993) found that the research was consistent with respect to the effect of lower class individuals; they were more likely to be arrested. They found that results were mixed on the impact of victim initiated police-citizen encounters in terms of arrest. Some studies found police were less likely to arrest when a victim calls the police in domestic disturbance calls; other studies found no effect. Research conducted in the 1980s indicates many mixed findings. Gender, race, and age have had mixed findings. A largely equal number of studies found that males were more likely to be arrested while other studies found no effect. Race was one of the most commonly studied variables in arrest research, yet the effect of race was inconclusive. A large number of studies found no effect of race and only a handful found that minorities were more likely to be arrested, and one study found that whites were more likely to be arrested. According to Riksheim and Chermak (1993), age has generally been a nonsignificant predictor of arrest. But, some studies have found that age played a role with certain groups such as females, and adult misdemeanants. Other situational variables have shown mixed effects. The overwhelming majority of research indicates that complainant preference influences arrest, but this factor had no effect in some situations such as department type. Overall, research cited by Riksheim and Chermak (1993) demonstrates that disrespectful suspects are more likely to be arrested. Other research 34 has indicated that suspect demeanor has no effect in certain situations. Victim and offender relationship has also produced mixed findings. In many studies cited by Riksheim and Chermak (1993) when the victim and offender do not know each other the suspect is more likely to be arrested. Studies of police-citizen encounters involving alleged domestic violence found that victim-offender relationship did not have a significant effect. This finding may be the result of changing societal attitudes or department policies. The location of the incident has shown to have mixed results as well. Some research indicated that there was no difference between incidents that were in public areas and private areas. Other research showed that males were arrested more often in public locations than females. Police were more likely to arrest for more serious incidents. This effect held even after creating more detailed scales than felony vs. misdemeanor. However, some research cited by Riksheim and Chermak (1993) found that the presence of an injury did not significantly increase the likelihood of arrest. A new variable, examining the influence of police supervisors, had mixed results. For some department types this increased arrest, while for others it decreased arrest odds or had no effect. Few organizational-level variables have shown an effect on arrest behavior. The other issue with organizational-level variables is that they have not been extensively studied and some of the measures are crude at best. Research on department deployment strategies has found mixed results. According to research cited by Riksheim and Chermak (1993), one study found no effect of deployment strategy on arrest. Another study found that deploying officers to smaller assignment areas was associated with higher odds of arrest in areas with high violent crime. Also, target specific deployments, and more aggressive police tactics appear to increase arrest rates. The number and length of shift durations do not appear to influence arrest productivity. Research on department type has produced mixed results; some research has found 35 that certain department types increase arrest. Other studies have found no effect of department type on arrest. Finally, the effect of department size is also unclear; an equal number of studies have found that officers in larger departments were less likely to arrest as studies that have found no effect of department size on arrest. Like organizational-level variables, research on community-level variables has been limited. There has been some study of community-level economic variables; research cited by Riksheim and Chermak (1993) indicates that police have higher arrest rates in poorer communities. Many different measures of poverty appear to indicate the same findings. City manager style government appears to increase arrests rates for specific crimes, which include: property crime, DWI, trespassing, disorderly conduct, and motor vehicle crimes. Research cited by Riksheim and Chermak (1993) was inconclusive on percent non-white. Some research found that this variable was correlated with increased arrest rates. While other research showed that when this variable was measured as percent Hispanic the results varied based on the location of the police department, urban versus rural. Finally, Riksheim and Chermak (1993) cited contradictory research regarding the effect of neighborhood crime rates and victimization rates on arrest. National Research Council The National Research Council (2004) evaluated the majority of the currently available information regarding correlates of police behavior. They argued that police action is most heavily influenced by legal variables, particularly seriousness of the offense, evidence of wrongdoing, and willingness to file a complaint. The committee also stated that there is consistent evidence to indicate that high crime areas or disadvantaged areas will increase the odds of arrest. Finally, the committee found that there is some evidence that police agencies are 36 influenced by court rulings but the committee also argues that there is evidence that agencies try and circumvent rulings as well. These three factors were the only influences on police behavior that showed consistent effects across studies using the narrative review approach. For all other explanations of police behavior there was either mixed findings or not enough research to draw a conclusion with confidence. Summary Taken together these narrative reviews begin to create a picture that emphasizes situational-level factors such as suspect demeanor and legal variables. What is consistent across these literature reviews is inconsistency. At some points in time scholars were willing to assert with more conviction that certain factors are significant and as time progresses confusion appears to increase. In this case, more data seems to be cumbersome for traditional narrative reviews. Another issue with these narrative reviews is that they are reporting on multiple studies that use the same data over and over. This fact may be contributing to the confusion rather than helping to eliminate it. Simply because even within the same data set, findings appear to disagree with each other, for example, research cited by Riksheim and Chermak (1993) showed that some research from the Police Services Study found that race was a significant factor in arrest behavior while other research did not find an effect for race. Sentencing Spohn (2000) reviewed the correlates of sentencing, but her review focuses on how race influences sentencing. All other correlates are always put in the context of the effect of race on sentencing. Meta-analytic reviews of sentencing have discussed other issues with sentencing. They are discussed after the narrative literature review section. 37 Spohn (2000) reviews sentencing literature to examine the impact of race and ethnicity on sentencing. Of the studies reviewed, Spohn (2000) found that 43.2 percent of state-level studies found an effect for black vs. white defendants and 27.6 percent of state-level studies found an effect for Hispanic vs. white defendants. Additionally, she found that 55.5 percent of studies found a significant effect of race on the in/out decision and 23.1 percent of studies found an effect of race on the length of sentencing decision. Finally, 41.7 percent of state-level studies found an impact for ethnicity on the in/out decision and one study found an effect of ethnicity on the length of the sentence. It is also important to note that many studies that did not find race and/or ethnicity had main effects did find interaction effects of race and/or ethnicity combining with other factors. Spohn (2000) also reviewed the impact of race and ethnicity on federal sentencing studies. Research cited by Spohn (2000) showed that 68.2 percent of studies of federal-level sentencing data found an effect of race. Also, 47.6 percent of studies of federal-level sentencing data found an effect of ethnicity. Three of seven studies that examined the in/out decision found significant effects for race and three of seven studies found a significant effect for ethnicity. Finally, six of nine studies on race found significant effect of race on the length of sentence and two of eight studies on ethnicity found a significant effect of ethnicity on the length of sentence. According to Spohn (2000), based on the above picture, race appears to play a role in sentencing. Parole revocation The status of review research on parole revocation is very limited. Summary research on parole often barely mentions the topic of parole revocation and instead focuses more on racial disparities and parole. For example, Latessa and Smith (2007), Travis and Lawrence (2002) and the National Research Council’s (2008) discussions of parole fail to explain the correlates of 38 parole revocation. Caplan (2007) reviewed literature surrounding parole but focused entirely on release and not revocation. Instead each of these research endeavors has focused on other issues of parole such as release or the elimination of parole as a release mechanism. Additionally, Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989) offer limited information on the correlates of revocation. Research cited by Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989) states that prison performance predicts parole success or failure. This measure is similar to the idea of criminal history and is consistent with one of Gottfredson and Gottfredson’s (1989) three main correlates of criminal justice decision making. In sum, narrative literature reviews have shown limited consistencies across the system. For example, Gottfredson and Gottfredson (1989) found victim-offender relationship, seriousness of the offense and criminal history were the primary explanatory factors across the system. Aside from the influence of these variables, narrative literature reviews have been inconsistent across the system. At the arrest decision, literature reviews are rather exhaustive. Yet, at the sentencing decision, narrative reviews have focused almost exclusively on the effects of race and have virtually ignored the effects of other theoretically relevant variables. Finally, the status of synthesis of research on parole revocation is very limited to the point of nonexistence. Reviews of literature on parole generally have focused on general issues with parole or the release decision not the revocation decision. Meta-Analysis Meta-analysis has had some use in criminal justice decision-making research. Metaanalysis has primarily been used to examine the correlates of sentencing. Daly and Bordt, (1995) examine the effects of gender on sentencing. Mitchell (2005) and Pratt (1998) both examine the effects of race on sentencing. Finally, Whatley (1996) examines the effects of 39 victim blameworthiness. More recently, Kochel, Wilson, and Mastrofski (2011) analyzed the effects of race on the decision to arrest. Gender It had long been argued that gender played a role in sentencing decisions. Daly and Bordt (1995) examined the effects of gender on sentencing meta-analytically. They found that even after controlling for legally relevant variables, such as prior record, gender remained a significant correlate of decision-making. The vast majority of studies that showed gender effects showed effects that favored women. Additionally, gender effects were a more common result than race effects. Finally, Daly and Bordt (1995) found that gender effects were more likely to be found when the study had a high proportion of non-white defendants. The authors do conclude that gender effects may be contextual depending on the context of the criminal justice decision. Race When examining the effect sizes of race, prior record and seriousness of the offense, Pratt (1998) found that only measures of offense seriousness had a significant impact on measures of sentence length. Given the consistent finding across studies that prior record plays a large role, Pratt’s findings are somewhat surprising. Pratt argues that prior record may not have been a significant correlate due to a measurement issue. He states that the measures of prior record were often interval measures which implies a linear relationship and the relationship between prior record and sentence length may be non-linear, hence no relationship. Pratt (1998) argues that his non-significant race finding may also be due to measurement issues. There are two majors ways that race was operationalized across studies: white/black, and white/nonwhite. Pratt also created a third, other category, which acted as a catch all for any other race 40 conceptualizations. Pratt (1998) found that there was a significant difference between the operationalization of race and that variables impact on sentence length. Specifically, he found that white/non-white operationalization had the greatest likelihood of finding a significant effect of race on sentence length. Thus, it may be possible that measures of race were masking a real effect (Pratt, 1998). Unlike Pratt’s meta-analysis, Mitchell’s (2005) meta-analytic examination of sentencing correlates found that race was a significant factor in sentencing. Mitchell (2005) found a small but statistically significant effect of race even after controlling for sentence severity.3 African Americans were punished more severely than white Americans. Additionally, he found that the magnitude of the effect size varied depending on several measurement issues. For example, larger effect sizes were found with less precise measures of criminal history. Race appeared to have a stronger influence on sentencing when worse measures of criminal history were used in a particular study. Even after controlling for other variables such as use of a weapon, trial type, type of defense counsel, and socio-economic, a small but statistically significant race effect persisted (Mitchell, 2005). Kochel et al. (2011) examined the effect of race on officer decision to arrest. They found that effect sizes indicated that race was a significant predictor of officer arrest decisions. They found that African Americans were significantly more likely to be arrested. This effect persisted even after controlling for a variety of relevant moderating variables. These control variables included whether the study examined domestic violence, other legally relevant variables such as seriousness of the offense, victim request for arrest, presence of a witness, quality of evidence, use of alcohol or drugs, and prior record. In addition, they controlled for extra-legal variables 3 Mitchell (2005) analyzed studies that used multiple dependent variables unlike Pratt (1998), who examined only studies that used sentence length as a metric outcome. 41 such as demeanor of the suspect and they examined whether or not the effect of race on arrest changed over time. In all of these instances the effect of race remained. Victim characteristics Another consideration in sentencing is victim responsibility. To examine this issue, Whatley (1996) meta-analyzed studies investigating the effects of victim characteristics on victim blameworthiness. He found that victims were more likely to be blamed when: they wore revealing clothing, had questionable character, and had a close relationship with their attacker. He did not support the hypothesis that unattractive victims would be more likely to be blamed. In sum, meta-analysis has been used to examine issues with sentencing and, very recently, arrest. Through the use of meta-analysis, scholars were able to make sense of the mixed effects of several variables and make definitive conclusions regarding those variables. Daly and Bordt (19955) found that there was a gender effect on sentencing. While Pratt’s (1998) study did not find significant race effects on sentencing, Mitchell’s (2005) meta-analysis did find a small but significant effect of race on sentencing decisions. Finally, Whatley (1996) found that victim characteristics played a role in victim blameworthiness. Chapter Summary Meta-analysis is a new technique that could be used to garner new information regarding the effects of predictors of criminal justice decision making on various decisions. The majority of synthesis research has taken the form of narrative literature reviews. With the exception of a few relationships, the results of these reviews have often ended with the call for more research to assess conflicting information. Meta-analysis has had limited use in the area of sentencing but has not been used extensively in any other area of criminal justice decision making. Metaanalysis may be able to shed new light on correlates of decision-making across the system and 42 assess the validity of criminal justice theory. If correlates of decision-making are consistent across the criminal justice system a grand theory approach may be more appropriate. However, if there are few consistent correlates across decisions and the strongest relationships are predictors that are unique to a particular decision, then a decision-specific approach may be most appropriate. Given the above review, several factors emerge for the entire system under various domains or groups of variables. Legal variables appear to consistently influence decision making across the system. These variables include measures of seriousness of the offense, prior record and victim-offender relationship. When crimes were considered more serious, suspects had longer records and the victim and offender did not know one another the system was more punitive. Suspect characteristics, such as race, ethnicity, age and gender, have also been studied repeatedly but have had mixed results. Due to the conflicted nature of these results, they would be ideal to include in a meta-analysis. Other variables that have produced conflicted results include criminal justice actor characteristics, such as actor race, age/experience, gender, ethnicity, education level4, and cynicism. The aforementioned variables all tend to focus on the incident level of case processing. However, research has produced mixed results with more macro level constructs. These variables that have produced inconsistent results at the macro level include: neighborhood poverty levels, family structure, and racial heterogeneity. These variables are system-wide and are often measured at each stage of the criminal justice system. There are a number of factors that are unique to a particular decision for a variety of reasons.5 At the arrest decision a number of factors have been examined that are unique to the arrest decision. The first unique set of factors is organizational characteristics. Measures that 4 While, judges have a similar education level, this does vary for police officers and probation officers. Some factors are considered unique simply because they are not measured at multiple stages of the criminal justice system. For example, demeanor may be applicable at multiple stages but is only measured at the arrest stage. 5 43 are organizational characteristics are measures of organizational hierarchy and police agency size. The arrest decision point also officers several unique situational factors that are not necessarily found in other decision points. These factors include measures of poor citizen demeanor, citizen intoxication, citizen non-compliance,6 interaction-phase criminal conduct and lay perceptions of mental disorder.7 While some of these factors may not seem unique to the arrest decision, for example suspect demeanor, organizational hierarchy, or police agency size, they are not measured in studies of other decision points. It is possible that court defendants or parolees may have a poor demeanor or parole officers may operate within certain organizational hierarchies or within agencies of varying size, however, these variables are not measured in studies of sentencing or parole revocation, and thus, these variables included as unique. Like arrest, sentencing offers a number of unique factors due to the nature of the sentencing decision. Like arrest, some “unique” factors are unique because they are not measured at the other stages. These unique characteristics are broken down into court characteristics and case characteristics. Items included under court characteristics include measures such as: jurisdiction of the court, the geographic trial location, and the court size. Jurisdiction examines Federal versus state, county, or local jurisdiction. Geographic trial location focuses on regional differences in case processing. Finally, court size refers to the amount of cases processed through a particular court. All three are applicable at any decision point, but are not measured at other decision points.8 Items included under case characteristics 6 Some research has separated citizen compliance from demeanor. It may be possible that these measures will be correlated (see Novak et al. 2002). If that is the case, a measure of compliance will be treated as a measure for demeanor. 7 Engel and Silver (2001) established that officer perceptions of mental illness were not clinical definitions of mental illness but may influence officer decision making in arrest situations. Lay perceptions of mental illness are considered unique to arrest because at the sentencing and parole revocation stage the actor will likely have an official report and would not need to rely on lay opinions. 8 Court size would need to be reoperationalized to the volume of police citizen encounters or case loads of parolees revocated to be applied to arrest or parole revocation decisions. 44 include measures such as: mode of conviction, education level of the defendant, employment status and type of counsel. Finally, parole revocation also presents a number of unique measures. These factors are divided into offender characteristics and parole characteristics. Items included under offender characteristics include measures such as: education level and employment status.9 The unique parole characteristic is program completion. 9 Education level and employment status are considered unique factors for sentencing and parole revocation because these considerations are not measured at the arrest decision so they are not system-wide and thus are unique to these two particular decision points. 45 CHAPTER 3 META-ANALYSIS: BENEFITS AND CRITIQUES There have been many attempts to examine correlates of criminal justice decision making. The purpose of this dissertation is to examine these correlates in the context of the criminal justice system as a whole and determine if theoretical perspectives should focus on the entire system or specific decisions. While there have been many examinations of criminal justice decision making there have been limited attempts to synthesize the research and what has been done has had several problems. First, other than Gottfredson and Gottfredson, all attempts at research synthesis have been specific to a particular decision. This creates a myopic approach that prevents scholars from seeing the larger picture, i.e. that factors that influence decision making may be consistent across the system. The second limitation, as seen in the previous chapter, is that the majority of these reviews have been narrative, or vote counting-based reviews, which in and of themselves are problematic. In the pages that follow, issues of synthesis literature are discussed. First, there is a discussion of the problems of narrative literature and vote-counting reviews. Second there is a discussion of the benefits of meta-analysis. Third, there is a discussion the problems with metaanalysis and the solutions to these problems. Scholars have always strived to make sense, and take stock of their fields of research. Cullen (2011) issued a call for scholars in the fields of criminology and criminal justice to advance the field by taking stock of research and creating a better understanding of these fields through research synthesis. Many issues in research, such as sampling and measurement methods, create inconsistencies in findings and these inconsistencies lead to a state of confusion or “scientific chaos” (Hunt, 1997, p. 1). In an attempt to make order from the chaos, scholars 46 would conduct a massive narrative literature review or a vote-count review in order to make sense of inconsistent findings (Hunter and Schmidt, 1990). However, these traditional methods of research synthesis are flawed. Traditional Narrative Literature Reviews Traditionally, research on criminal justice decision making was reviewed via the narrative literature review format. These reviews permeate research on arrest and sentencing (see National Research Council, 2004; Riksheim and Chermak, 1993; Sherman, 1980; Spohn, 2000). Under these narrative reviews a scholar gathers all available research on a particular topic, evaluates the research, and makes a determination as to the importance of certain relationships. While these types of reviews provide valuable insights, the method is a flawed and antiquated approach to synthesis research. The first problem is the subjective nature of narrative reviews (Pratt, 2002; Wolf, 1986). Since the review is based solely on the researcher’s evaluation of the literature, any conclusions will potentially be biased based on the researcher’s examination of the literature. Essentially, two scholars could read the same inconsistent evidence and come to opposite conclusions. This qualitative judgment has no standardized process and this can possibly bias the evaluations. A second and related issue is there is no set process to conduct a narrative literature review (Jonson, 2010). This means that there is no procedure for finding, collecting and evaluating the studies for a narrative review. As a result, bias may be introduced as a result of a scholar consciously or unconsciously omitting studies from the review. This omitted study bias creates a subjective review, which may not necessarily be able to be replicated. For example, the National Research Council’s discussion of correlates of arrest fails to mention any articles using 47 data from the Cincinnati Police Observation study. The omission of these studies is a blatant example of omitted study bias in narrative literature reviews. A third issue of narrative reviews is that a large number of studies makes the technique cumbersome and potentially decreases the accuracy of the findings (Hunter and Schmidt, 1990; Jonson, 2010). When the literature for a particular topic becomes too voluminous, a researcher may fail to include studies. The researcher may choose to use a sub-sample of studies in order to make the review more manageable (Jonson, 2010). There are several problems with this approach. First, the narrative review is no longer a complete review. Second, omitting studies creates a selection bias and possibly leads to inaccurate results because critical studies may have been missed or omitted entirely. Vote-counting Another type of traditional research synthesis is the vote-count approach. Under the vote-count approach, a researcher tallies the results of studies (Hunt, 1997; Hunter and Schmidt, 1990). A researcher divides the findings of studies into three groups: positive relationships, negative relationships and no relationships. Whichever group is the largest becomes the interpretation of the relationship for the review. Unlike narrative reviews, vote-counting does have some objective means of evaluating relationships. However, like narrative reviews, votecounting has the same potential for selection bias because of omitted studies. A second and equally large problem is that vote-counts treat all studies equally (Jonson, 2010). A study with a sample size of 40 is weighted the same as a study with a sample size of 1,000. Since sampling error decrease with larger samples, more weight should be placed on the study with a larger sample size since there is more confidence in those results (Hanushek and Jackson, 1977). Another related problem is that vote-counting does not account for varying magnitudes of 48 findings (Hunt, 1997; Jonson, 2010). For example, suppose a study found a weak positive effect and another found a strong negative effect for the same relationship. Under the vote-counting scheme each study carries the same weight. Essentially the two findings would cancel each other out rather than the stronger negative effect taking precedence. Thus, while vote-counting is a slightly more objective approach than narrative literature reviews, it still suffers from flaws and there is a need for a better way to synthesize research. Meta-Analysis as an Alternative Meta-analysis is an analytical technique that can overcome the inherent problems in narrative and vote-counting reviews. Meta-analysis is a technique designed to make sense of and evaluate a large amount of information. Meta-analysis is still a relatively young technique; it was developed in the 1970s (Hunt, 1997). According to Glass (1976), meta-analysis is a statistical method for the purpose of integrating and evaluating the findings of multiple studies. Jonson (2010) comments that “[meta-analysis involves a] five-step process: 1) formulating the problem, 2) collecting the data, 3) evaluating the data, 4) synthesizing the data, and 5) presenting the findings” (p. 73). This technique has become increasingly more common in the fields of criminology and criminal justice (Pratt, 2010; Wells, 2009). Wells (2009) found that the majority of meta-analyses in criminology and criminal justice have been conducted since the 1990s. Furthermore, Jonson (2010) states that meta-analysis is a common technique of the broader social science research community. However, not all scholars have been advocates of meta-analysis. Eysenck (1976) referred to the technique as an “exercise in mega-silliness” (p. 517). With this disagreement in mind it is important to discuss the strengths and weaknesses of the technique. 49 Advantages of Meta-Analysis Meta-analysis has several advantages over traditional synthesis techniques. According to Pratt (2001) there are four advantages of meta-analysis; first, meta-analysis can derive exact measures of strength for a particular relationship called an effect size. Second, meta-analysis can use multivariate analysis to examine if relationships change under different methodological conditions. Third, other scholars may replicate meta-analyses. Fourth, meta-analysis is dynamic, as opposed to static, and when new studies are conducted they can easily be added to existing meta-analyses and the results may be updated. Additionally, Jonson (2010) stated that meta-analysis is a better approach for policy analysis and consequently meta-analysis can assess the implications of policy much better than traditional literature reviews. Effect size First, meta-analysis provides an exact measure of strength of the relationship between two variables. The effect size measure is superior to narrative literature reviews because traditional reviews can at best vote count (Pratt, 2001). Vote counting may be biased by multiple studies using the same single data set. The effect size measure gets around this problem by creating an effect size per data set. The effect size provides a precise measure of strength and direction for the relationship between the two variables (Hunt, 1997; Lipsey and Wilson, 2001; Pratt, 2001). Additionally, Jonson (2010), states that effect sizes account for variation in sample sizes. Thus the studies with larger, more representative samples are granted more weight than smaller samples. Under vote-counting each sample is given the same weight regardless of sample size. The effect size essentially creates a better understanding of the true relationship between the predictors and the outcome variable (Hunt, 1997; Hunter and Schmidt, 1990; Jonson, 2010). 50 Methodological adjustments Second, meta-analysis can adjust magnitude measures to account for varying research methodologies. According to Pratt (2001), Meta-analysis allows for the separation of studies into groups based on methodological differences and calculates effect sizes for each group. Meta-analysis allows the researcher to examine if certain pertinent methodological factors influence the relationship of interest. These methodological considerations include: demographic information (age, race and gender), study characteristics (longitudinal versus cross sectional, study location, or year published), and measurement of the outcome variable (Durlak and Lipsey, 1991; Jonson, 2010; Lipsey and Wilson, 2001). For example, Pratt (1998) found that the effect of race on arrest was contingent on how race was measured (see discussion of Pratt, 1998 in Chapter 2). Replication Third, according to Pratt (2001), “coding decisions in a meta-analysis are ‘public’” (p. 31). Coding procedures are described in detail with the study and coding sheets are often published with the study (Jonson, 2010). Because coding decisions are public, the results may be replicated and research integrity is preserved. In addition to a public coding process, the eligibility criteria for the meta-analysis are also included and thus another aspect of the metaanalysis may be replicated (Jonson, 2010). The transparent nature of meta-analysis allows for any other scholar to replicate the findings and thus ensure research integrity. This transparency is not found with traditional literature reviews where inclusion criteria are not provided. Consequently, the sample in a traditional literature review is potentially subject to selection bias. 51 Dynamic Finally, meta-analysis is a dynamic syntheses method. As such, when other studies are written they can be added to the meta-analysis and the information can continually be reevaluated (Pratt, 2001). Weisburd, Telep, Hinkle and Eck (2010) recently reviewed the status of problem oriented policing as part of the Campbell Collaborative’s systematic review process. Their review specifically mentioned that as new studies emerged they would be added to the review and the meta-analysis on the effectiveness of problem oriented policing would continually be updated. This example demonstrates the dynamic nature of meta-analysis. As new studies emerge on a particular topic, in this case problem oriented policing, new information can be added to the meta-analysis and new effect sizes may be calculated with relative ease. Thus, meta-analysis allows for the most up-to-date and accurate reviews and synthesis of information possible. Critiques of Meta-Analysis Meta-analysis is not a solution to research synthesis that is free from flaws. However, scholars have created ways to account for these potential problems and adjust the analyses. There are three problems for this meta-analysis: 1) the file drawer problem, 2) the apples-andoranges problem, and 3) the issue of independence of studies. The following section will discuss each problem and potential solutions. File drawer problem According to Rosenthal (1991), the process of publishing research biases meta-analysis. There is an emphasis on publishing findings that support research questions and thus, academic journals may not want to publish null findings. These null studies are therefore sitting in the 52 bottom of a scholar’s file drawer. Consequently, meta-analyses may be biased in the direction of finding an effect. If these unpublished studies included, effect size calculations may be different. While this is a valid criticism, according to Jonson (2010) this problem is equally valid for narrative literature reviews and vote-counting studies. Pratt (2010) contends that this problem may not be as serious of an issue because of the nature of tenure and promotion amongst scholars in academe. The “publish or perish” phenomenon does not create an incentive for research to sit in a file drawer. A scholar will not continue to be employed if he/she sits on null findings and does not make an attempt to get them published. Additionally, given the research surrounding certain variables in criminal justice research, for example, suspect demeanor, null findings may question the validity of past research and are certainly worth publishing. With these considerations in mind, it appears that the file drawer problem may not be as large of a problem as originally perceived. There are two approaches to examine this problem. The first is to try and retrieve unpublished studies (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001). These unpublished reports may take the form of government reports, dissertations, conference presentations, and directly contacting well-known scholars in the field for any additional studies. By using these methods, there is potentially less bias in meta-analytic calculations. The second method is to statistically examine for bias using the fail-safe N statistic (Rosenthal, 1991). This statistic calculates how many null findings would be necessary to render the effect size statistically zero (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001). Higher values on a fail-safe N statistic indicate that a large number of studies would be necessary to nullify the effect size. As a result, metaanalyses with higher fail-safe N values can be interpreted as more likely to be valid because it would take an unlikely number of null findings to invalidate the findings. 53 Apples-and-oranges problem The second problem with meta-analysis is the apples-and-oranges problem. This problem is related to varying measures of independent and dependent variables (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001; Pratt, 2001, 2002). If analyzed studies are not measuring constructs in the same manner then it may be inappropriate to meta-analyze them together. Meta-analysis assumes that each study is measuring the same variables in the same way (Hunter and Schmidt, 1990). If this is not the case then the measures are not necessarily tapping the same idea. Consequently, the effect size is meaningless (Hunter and Schmidt, 1990). For example, Mitchell (2005) used multiple measures of sentence severity that included: in/out decisions, sentence length, ordinal scales of severity, discretionary release (reversed coded so that positive values indicated more punitive punishments) and discretionary punitiveness. Critics would argue that meta-analyzing these measures would be inappropriate because each is different. However, proponents would argue that on all of these scales increasing values indicated more punitive sentencing. Thus, while individual measures of punishment are slightly different, the overall indication of each measure was in the same direction and each measure is tapping into the same construct. A related issue to the apples-and-oranges problem is the quality of the research being analyzed. Critics have argued that more rigorous studies, for example longitudinal and classical experiment studies, should not be meta analyzed with less rigorous studies such as quasiexperimental and non-experimental designs (Logan and Gaes, 1993). Varying quality of methodological rigor means that studies will vary in internal and external validity and this can bias the effect size. Critics argue that these meta-analysis attempts statistically improve poor research (Jonson, 2010). This idea is commonly referred to as “garbage in, garbage out,” which means that poor data will produce poor statistics. There are two solutions to this issue. The first 54 is to include only studies that measure all variables the same way and/or have greater methodological rigor. However, this solution would severely limit the sample. The alternative solution is to statistically control this issue. By statistically controlling the issue, methodological rigor may be a factor influencing the relationships between independent variables and the dependent variable. Independence of Effect Sizes A third problem for this particular meta-analysis is independence of effect sizes. According to Pratt (2000), current studies in criminal justice and criminology use multiple models that can each produce multiple effect sizes. This is especially true in arrest research given that a large amount of knowledge of arrest is produced via large systematic social observations like the Project on Policing Neighborhoods, the Cincinnati Study of Police Behavior and the Police Services Study. The lack of independence creates errors in estimates, which decrease the variance and increase the likelihood that an effect size will be significantly different from zero (Pratt, 2000). In other words, the likelihood of making a type II error increases dramatically. In order to deal with this issue, scholars suggest using a system of selection criteria to select the best possible representation. This process has been used in previous meta-analyses of criminal justice decision making (see Kochel et al., 2011, and Mitchell, 2005). While some scholars have suggested the use of independence adjustment or hierarchical linear modeling to deal with these issues, Lipsey and Wilson (2001) suggest that if possible using one representative per data set is the preferred method. 55 Chapter Summary Meta-analysis has been used for a variety of research questions. Most recently, metaanalysis has been used to evaluate criminological theory. Some of the meta-analytically evaluated theories include: Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime, social learning theory, deterrence theory, and bio-social predictors of crime (Paternoster, 1987; Pratt and Cullen, 2000; Pratt, Cullen, Blevins, et al., 2002; Pratt, Cullen, Blevins, et al. 2006; Pratt, Cullen, Sellers et al., 2010; Pratt, McGloin and Fearn, 2006; Walters, 1992). Each of these evaluations used meta-analysis to determine the overall worth of the theory and the efficacy of each of the components of the theory. For example, Pratt and Cullen (2000) found that low self-control had an effect size over .20 and stated that self-control is “one of the strongest known correlates of crime” (p. 952). Meta-analysis serves to create an alternative to evaluate theories other than purely narrative literature reviews. Meta-analysis can also be used to evaluate the relative strength of known predictors of a relationship. For example, Pratt and Cullen (2005) used meta-analysis to evaluate a number of macro-level predictors of crime, such as unemployment, racial heterogeneity, regional effects, and residential mobility among others. Pratt and Cullen (2005) examined the effects of 23 different variable types on crime. This same principle can be applied to the determinants of criminal justice decision making. Meta-analysis provides a method for synthesizing a large number of studies. It is superior to traditional narrative literature reviews and vote-counting reviews in a number of ways. First, meta-analysis allows for precise measures of strength of relationships via the effect size. This statistic indicates whether the relationship between an independent variable exists and the strength of that relationship. Second, meta-analysis allows for methodological adjustment. 56 Under traditional research synthesis, each study was given the same weight regardless of the quality of the study. Meta-analysis can correct for this and adjust the effect size based on methodological considerations. Third, meta-analysis can be replicated. Coding decisions are public because coding protocols are often included with the presentation of results. This allows another scholar to replicate the process or modify the process used in the original meta-analysis. For example, Mitchell replicated Pratt’s meta-analysis of the sentencing literature. The two scholars came to different conclusions (see Chapter 2). Finally, meta-analysis is dynamic. With meta-analysis, when more studies are generated new effect sizes for those studies can be calculated and a new average effect size can be calculated and the relationship can be reevaluated. Mitchell was able to add additional studies that had been published between his study and Pratt’s. With narrative literature reviews an entirely new narrative literature review would have to be conducted and that process would be long and involved. Meta-analysis offers a faster and more efficient alternative for updating a particular body of knowledge. While there are many benefits to meta-analysis there are several problems, but scholars have worked to correct these known issues. The first problem is commonly known as the “filedrawer” problem. This problem involves the bias of results in favor of a relationship because of the nature of the publication process. This problem is corrected in two ways, first the metaanalyst should do his/her best to find as many unpublished studies as possible, and second they should calculate a fail-safe N statistic (see Chapter 4). The second problem is the “apples-andoranges” problem. This problem refers to a bias that is created when a meta-analyst is not comparing similar constructs. Because of the nature of research not every study is identical. As a result, there is a possibility that the measures being meta-analyzed may not be the same for a variety of reasons both on a measurement level and a study design level (i.e. longitudinal versus 57 cross sectional etc.). To combat this problem a meta-analyst can either remove studies of low methodological rigor or they can include moderator variables and treat methodological rigor as a factor in the relationship between the independent and dependent variables. The final problem is the potential lack of independence of effect sizes. This issue is the result of massive data gathering projects. When large amounts of money are used to generate data, scholars often do not want to publish just one paper from the data, but instead publish multiple papers from one data set. This creates a problem for meta-analysis because it requires that each effect size be generated from an independent source. To combat this problem Kochel et al. (2011) and Mitchell (2005) suggest that selection criteria be created to select the best possible representation of a particular data set (see chapter 4 for the specific selection criteria). With these benefits and problems in mind, the next chapter outlines the statistical procedures for this dissertation. 58 CHAPTER 4 METHODS With the benefits, and potential problems, of meta-analysis in mind, this chapter outlines the statistical procedures used in this dissertation. First, there is a discussion of the research questions. Second, there is an examination of the sampling procedures and a discussion of the ways to acquire unpublished studies as an attempt to counter the file-drawer problem (see Chapter 3 for a discussion of the file-drawer problem). Third, there is a discussion of the dependent, independent and moderating variables that will be used in the statistical analysis. Fourth, analytical procedures are discussed. Fifth there is a discussion of how the analysis will indicate which correlates are significant, how it was determined whether criminal justice theoretical perspectives should be system-wide or decision-specific. This chapter concludes with a brief discussion of the limitations of this study. Research Questions This dissertation is examining the current status of criminal justice theory via an examination of correlates of criminal justice decision-making. In an effort to examine this idea, this dissertation proposed to examine several decision points in the criminal justice system in order to determine the answers to the following questions: 1. What factors influence decision making at arrest, sentencing, and parole revocation? 2. Given these factors, are they consistent across the system as a whole? 3. Given the presence or lack of consistency, should theories of criminal justice focus on the system as a whole or a particular branch/decision point? These questions are designed to assess the correlates at each stage and then the entire system. As a result of this process it is possible to determine what factors influence each decision point and 59 if those are the same influential factors across the system or if factors vary based on the decision of interest. Sample of Studies The first step in the process of meta-analysis is to collect all relevant studies. Several search strategies were employed to gather all the relevant studies. First, the bibliographies of the current synthesis studies were scoured to gather all possible studies. However, for any particular decision, the most recent narrative review is between six and ten years old. Consequently, other search strategies were utilized. The second search strategy was to search various criminal justice and social science research databases. Separate keyword searches were performed on the following databases: Criminal Justice Abstracts, Criminal Justice Periodical Index, Dissertation Abstracts Online, ERIC, National Criminal Justice Reference Service, PsycINFO, Social Sciences Index, Sociological Abstracts, and SocINDEX. These are the most common databases searched in criminology and criminal justice meta-analyses. The search terms for these searches were kept intentionally broad due to the generalist nature of the research questions discussed. For arrest, the search terms used “police” “arrest NOT cardiac” “decision or decisions, or ‘decision making’”. For sentencing, the search terms used were “sentencing” and “decision, decisions or ‘decision making’”. For parole revocation, the search terms used were “revocation” and “decision, decisions or ‘decision making’. When compared to existing meta-analyses and narrative reviews these search procedures produced virtually identical study lists. The third search strategy was to search all relevant peer-reviewed academic journals issue by issue to look for more recent information that may not yet be in the databases.10 Jonson 10 Due to the thoroughness of the searches themselves, journal search was limited to online-first articles as those already in print were in the databases and covered by second search strategy. 60 (2010) performed a Google search to find unpublished data and the tactic did glean a few state and national reports. Thus, the fourth search strategy performed was a Google search to look for unpublished reports. Because of the broad nature of the searches used Google searches were impractical. While they were performed, only the first ten pages of results were searched because search result pages numbered in the thousands and the results went too far off topic to have any utility past this point. A negligible number of potential studies were actually gleaned from this method. In total, the raw number of results from these various methods was over 3000 for arrest, over 1000 for sentencing and approximately 500 hits for parole revocation (all three estimates are inclusive of duplicates). After each search was performed, abstracts and titles were inspected for topic appropriateness. For arrest, 236 articles were determined to have a potential to be coded, and of those, 62 were deemed codeable and subsequently coded. A total of 54 separate data sets or studies were found. The reason for the difference is that some articles used the same data set to produce the results and could not all be included in the meta-analysis due to dependency issues (see below). For sentencing, 396 articles were determined to have potential, and of those, 65 were deemed codeable, and a total of 46 separate data sets or studies were collected. It is important to note that 20 of the 396 studies were removed because they dealt with juveniles. Since the juvenile adjudication process is substantively different from adults and are two separate systems, studies focusing on juveniles were not included in analyses of sentencing and parole revocation. However juvenile focused studies were included in arrest because the arrest process is not substantively different for juveniles or adults. Juveniles are arrested by the same officers that arrest adults. Finally, for parole revocation, a total of 82 studies were thought to have potential, 61 and of those 17 were coded, which yielded 19 separate data sets/studies. Surprisingly, parole revocation research did not utilize massive data sets to produce multiple studies as often as arrest or sentencing articles. Inclusion Criteria For studies to be included in this meta-analysis they must have met the following criteria. First, the study must have examined one of the three decisions of interest i.e. arrest, sentencing, or parole revocation, and the measurement of those decision points must be consistent with the discussion in the dependent variable section (see below). Second, the study must include at least one measure per system-wide domain for the independent variable. These independent variable domains include measures for offense severity11, and suspect/defendant/offender characteristics.12 Third, the study must provide the necessary information to calculate an effect size. These inclusion criteria are relatively lax in an effort to include more studies as opposed to limiting the number of studies. Including multiple studies allows for better examination of moderating variables such as methodological rigor. Selection Criteria Meta-analysis requires every effect size to be independent. If effect sizes are not independent then the mean effect size may be inflated and produce biased results in favor of finding an effect (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001). Because of the nature of the studies that were analyzed, this was not always possible if every possible. Every article did not pertain to one data set. For example, many sentencing research articles were generated from sentencing data from 11 Domain refers to a grouping of predictors for a particular outcome. Criminal justice actor characteristics were not included as very few studies include these measures and including this requirement would needlessly shrink the sample size. Community characteristics were not included here because they are often aggregate measures and the nature of decision-making research tends to be more micro level analysis and as a result valid studies may be needlessly eliminated if community characteristic measures were made mandatory. 12 62 Pennsylvania and the years of data often overlap. The same data cannot be continually used as it would bias the results. As a result, some criteria are necessary to select an article that best represents that particular data set. This dissertation used criteria modified from Mitchell (2005) and Kochel et al. (2011). The criteria for selecting the ideal representation of a particular data set were: 1) codeability- the particular article that provides the most information for coding purposes was included, 2) number of control variables- the article that has the greatest number of control variables was included, and 3) sample size- the article with the largest sample size was included because that represents the largest number of cases and larger samples are generally seen as more representative. These selection criteria are consistent with current meta-analyses of criminal justice decision making. Dependent Variable The overall dependent variable in this study is criminal justice decision processing. The definition in Chapter 1 established that criminal justice examines “governmental social control premised on punishment or blameworthiness” (Duffee and Allan, 2007, p. 8). As such, criminal justice is the study of processing. More simply, criminal justice is the study of discretion. It seeks to examine what causes people to be pushed further through the system and others are released or not pursued through the system. Operationalization of this concept varies depending upon the decision point of interest. Because of the nature of this study, each decision point presents a different operationalization of the dependent variable. For example, a study of sentencing would not operationalize criminal justice processing in the same manner as a study of arrest. They are looking at different steps in the criminal justice process. However, many studies measure process outcomes similarly in that they measure the dependent variable dichotomously as yes or no. 63 The arrest decision, which is defined as a police officer taking a person into custody in order to be processed further into the criminal justice system, measures processing as a yes/no dichotomy. In this dissertation, studies used for the arrest analysis, measured the arrest dichotomously. The decision to only use studies that measure arrest dichotomously is to eliminate studies that examine use of force or a continuum of law enforcement sanctions as they are outside the scope of the arrest decision, and could lead to a problem with heterogeneity (see below for a discussion of heterogeneity). The sentencing decision point is more complex than the arrest decision point from an operationalization perspective. Sentencing is defined here as a judge’s determination of the appropriate punishment for an individual. Many studies of sentencing focus on the incarceration decision as an in/out dichotomy. That is, studies focus on the decision to incarcerate or not. Yet, these same studies often also collect information on the length of the incarceration term. Mitchell (2005) used multiple sentencing measures, this strategy was not used in this dissertation. This decision was made to avoid both dependency and the apples-and-oranges problem. By limiting the sample in this way, the issue of dependency is avoided. There is no overlap between metric sentence length measures and dichotomous in/out scales that apply to the same set of defendants. This limitation still allows for a sufficiently large sample, but avoids any chance that the dependent variables are not compatible with one another. Additionally, limiting to the in/out decision required fewer artificial transformation procedures of effect sizes. Like sentencing, parole revocation is more complex, in terms of measurement, than arrest. For this study, parole revocation is defined as a parole officer's or parole board’s pursuit of re-incarceration of an individual serving the remainder of their sentence under parole supervision. This decision point has been measured dichotomously using the yes/no for a 64 measure of parole revocation. Since the previous two decisions used a dichotomous dependent variable measure, it is appropriate that the same standard is applied to the parole revocation decision. The reasons for using only a revoke/no revoke are the same as in sentencing, to avoid dependency issues, and the apples-and-oranges problem. Independent Variables With the research questions in mind, there should be some variables that are common to each branch of the system, for example, race of the criminal justice actor and other variables that are decision-specific, for example attorney type. The following discussion will explain what variables should function system-wide and which variables are unique to a particular decision point. System-wide variables Several factors have consistently been predicted to explain decision making across the system. Each of these factors constitutes a separate domain of independent variables in this meta-analysis. The four principle domains cover offense seriousness, criminal justice actor characteristics, suspect/defendant characteristics, and community characteristics. Items under the seriousness domain include measures of seriousness of the offense (e.g. felony versus misdemeanor), and prior record. Items included under the criminal justice actor domain would include measures of race, ethnicity, gender, cynicism, professionalism/education,13 and experience of the criminal justice actor. Items included under the suspect/defendant characteristic domain would include race, ethnicity, age, gender, and social class. Finally, items included under the community conditions domain include poverty, family structure, community racial composition and other socio-economic predictors. 13 Education and professionalism are combined because they are generally measured interchangeably see Smith (1984). 65 Arrest While the above domains do apply to the arrest decision, there are several domains that appear to be uniquely applied to the arrest decision. These two arrest specific domains are organizational characteristics, and other situation characteristics. Items included under the organizational characteristics domain include measures of organizational hierarchy and police agency size. Items included under other situational characteristics include measures of suspect demeanor, intoxication, suspect non-compliance, interaction-phase crime, and lay perceptions of mental illness. It should be noted that items included under these domains are “unique” because they are only used in arrest research but are applicable to other decision points. Prior arrest research has examined these factors, but studies of sentencing or parole revocation have not included these measures. As a result, they are “unique” to arrest for analytical purposes. Sentencing As with arrest, the sentencing decision adds other domains that are unique to the sentencing decision. These new domains include court characteristics, and case characteristics. Items under the court characteristics domain include measures of jurisdiction of the trial (e.g. federal versus local court), Trial location (e.g. region of the United States in which the trial took place, or a particular region of the state examined for the study), and court size (e.g. the number of judges in a particular jurisdiction). Items under the case characteristics domain include measures of mode of conviction (e.g. plea versus guilt at trial) and type of counsel (e.g. as public versus private counsel). While case characteristics are unique to the sentencing decision, the court characteristics are similar theoretically speaking to organizational characteristics described in the previous section. Like arrest, these court characteristics are unique because they are not measured at other stages. 66 Parole revocation The parole revocation decision presents another set of variables that are unique to the particular decision. These new factors are divided into two domains: offender characteristics, and parole conditions. Factors included under the offender characteristics domain include: education level of the offender (e.g. ordinal scale or a series of dummy variables), employment status (e.g. ordinal scale or as a series of dummy variables). The new factor under the parole condition domain is program completion. Again, like the previous two decision-specific domain discussions, offender characteristics can be applied to other domains, but they are not measured in studies of the other decision points. Moderating Variables Moderating variables are similar to control variables but in this case they are variables that pertain to the studies themselves and not necessarily the theories within the study. This is one of the advantages of meta-analysis because it allows for considerations of items such as whether the study utilized a particular data collection method, or if the data were collected in a particular time period. Each variable is discussed below. Study characteristics: The present meta-analysis coded data on several study characteristics. First, the publication year and decade were recorded. Additionally, the decade and year of data collection were also recorded. The type of report (e.g. book, book chapter, journal article, or conference paper), and the location of the study (what region of the United States: Northeast, South, Midwest, Southwest, and Pacific/West) were recorded. A variable was created for whether or not the study was multi-jurisdictional/national. 67 Methodological quality: The second domain of moderating variables was related to the methodological quality. Since this meta-analysis did not want to eliminate all studies of poor quality, the alternative solution is to examine the impact of methodological quality on outcome variables. Methodological quality was assessed via the type of data collected. The duration of the study was recorded. In order to account for the possibility of seasonality bias, a dummy variable was created to examine whether or not data collection was continuous or had periodic breaks. For example, the Cincinnati Police Observation Study was conducted year round as opposed to the Project on Policing Neighborhoods that collected data over two consecutive summers. Interaction effects are an important consideration in decision-making research. For example, many studies have found that it is not necessarily young offenders, racial/ethnic minorities or males that alone are significantly more likely to be incarcerated, but rather it is the interaction of all three so that young minority males are most likely to be severely punished (Spohn, 2000). Wooldredge (1998) argues that studies of sentencing need to improve measurement and include measures of interaction effects. Therefore, a dummy variable was created to indicate whether or not the effect size was generated from a study that measured interaction effects. Pratt (1998) found that the manner in which race was measured had an impact on the effect of race. A moderating variable that notes the way in which race is measured was recorded (coded as a three-category variable, where 1 = Black/White, 2 = White/non-White, and 3 = some other racial classification scheme). Because race and ethnicity are closely related and at times used interchangeably, a moderating variable noting the measurement of ethnicity was also 68 created (coded as a three-category variable, where 1 = Hispanic/non-Hispanic, 2 = Black/White/Hispanic, and 3 = some other ethnicity classification scheme). Arrest research has two additional moderating variables that apply to the arrest analysis. First, systematic social observation is a well-respected form of data collection and studies not using this data collection method may not be as methodologically sound. However, not all police research has been conducted via systematic social observation. A dummy variable was created to measure whether or not the effect size was generated from data recorded via systematic social observation. This variable applies only to the arrest assessment. Second, an issue that has been raised in research on arrest is the operationalization of disrespect or demeanor. According to Klinger (1994), suspect demeanor was not operationalized properly and as a result what was actually interaction-phase crime that should have accounted for the arrest was measured as suspect disrespect. Due to this critique, another moderating variable was created to account for this operationalization issue. This variable was coded as 0 when there was no interaction-phase crime measure and 1 when an interaction-phase crime measure was present. Reliability of Coding It is important to produce coded results that are reliable. One of the benefits of metaanalysis is that it can be reproduced because meta-analysts are proponents of transparency and replication. This dissertation evaluated coding reliability using the Yeaton-Wortman method. Yeaton and Wortman (1993) suggest that agreement is equal to the total number of agreements divided by the total number of comparisons. Thus, if reliability is checked and the two coders agree 50 times over 100 total points of comparison then the amount of agreement would be equal to 0.50. Values of 1.00 indicate total agreement and values of 0.00 indicate a complete lack of 69 agreement. The principle coder selected thirteen studies at random and had those studies coded by a reliability coder. After the thirteen studies were double coded, Yeaton and Wortman’s method was applied. The level of agreement between the reliability coder and the principle coder was .92. This value is well above any threshold where reliability is questioned. Analytic Strategy In order to address the research questions listed above, a series of analyses were conducted. First, a series of univariate statistics were calculated. These statistics are designed to generate a general description of the sample. Second, effect size measures were calculated between the independent variables of interest and the appropriate dependent variables. Next, a mean effect size was calculated for each relationship. Third, heterogeneity of effect sizes and the fail-safe N were evaluated in order to address common criticisms of meta-analysis, namely the apples-and-oranges problem and the file-drawer problem. Each of the techniques is described in detail below. Effect size measure Consistent with the current meta-analysis literature (see for example: Berlin, and Colditz, 1990; Brind, Chinchilli, Severs, et al., 1996; Cosgrove, Sakoulas, Perencevich, et al., 2003; Fleiss 1993; Fleiss and Berlin, 2009; Gaugler, Duval, Anderson, et al., 2007; Geddes, and Lawrie, 1995; Greenland, 1987; Greenland, 1993; Haddock, Rindskopf, and Shadish, 1998; Kochel et al., 2011; Lasky-Su, Faraone, Glatt, et al., 2005; Lipsey and Wilson, 2001; Lӧsel and Schmucker, 2005; Mitchell, 2005; Tenback, van Harten, van Os, 2009), the measure of effect size estimate used here was a logged odds ratio. This effect size estimate was chosen for several reasons. First, logged odds ratios were the most common effect size available for all three groups of studies. Second, logged odds ratios are symmetrical around zero making them easily 70 interpreted regarding significance and directionality (Hanushek and Jackson, 1977). Third, formulas exist for transforming other test statistics (such as t, F, and χ2) into a logged odds ratio value (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001). Additionally, Hedges and Olkin (1985) provide conversion formulas for probit coefficients and variances. Fourth, logged odds ratios can be used even with the presence of covariates and “inferences about the [odds ratios] may be drawn in the usual ways” (Fleiss and Berlin, 2009, p. 245). Sixth, logged odds ratios can be easily transformed into their more interpretable odds ratio counterpart by simply taking the natural anti-log of the logged odds ratio, giving them easy interpretation. The natural anti-log, also known as exponent B, is easily interpreted as an increase in the dependent variable given an increase in the independent variable. After each effect size was calculated, it was then weighted by the inverse of the variance. This step was taken to ensure that studies with larger samples are given more influence. Effect sizes generated from larger samples are given more weight because larger samples are assumed to be more representative of the population and thus, more accurate. The weighting procedure follows those outlined by Lipsey and Wilson (2001) where weights are equal to the product of the effect size, in this case the natural log of the odds ratio, and the inverse of the variance. The inverse of the variance is equal to: wLOR = ______abcd______ ab(c+d) + cd(a+b) where wLOR = the inverse variance weight for the effect size a = when both variables have a value of zero b = when the independent variable has a value of zero and the dependent variable has a value of one c = when the independent variable has a value of one and the dependent variable has a value of zero d = when both variables have a value of zero 71 Once each effect size has been weighted using the inverse variance method, a mean effect size was calculated. The mean effect size is equal to the sum of the weighted effect sizes divided by the sum of the inverse of the variance for each weighted effect size (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001). In order to determine that the mean effect size was significantly different from zero, a 95% confidence interval was calculated. If the confidence interval contains zero, then that particular mean effect size was considered non-significant. If zero was not contained within the confidence interval, then the mean effect size was considered significant at the .05 level and that relationship was included in the grand theory versus decision-specific discussion.14 Independence of effect size measures There is a large chance that effect sizes drawn may not be independent of one another. As a result, the effect sizes generated from these data sets may not be independent. Inclusion of dependent effect sizes will bias the mean effect size. This issue was combated by selecting only one measure per variable per data set. This method is consistent with current criminal justice decision-making meta-analyses. Heterogeneity of measures A criticism of meta-analysis is that meta-analysts combine measures that are too dissimilar to be meta-analyzed properly. If measures generating effect sizes are not similar or tapping into the same constructs, then the effect sizes estimates are effectively meaningless. However, meta-analysts are aware of this issue and they created Q statistics to combat this specific problem (Pratt, 2001). The Q-statistic uses a chi-square distribution. According to Lipsey and Wilson (2001) the formula used to assess heterogeneity of effect size estimates is as follows: 14 All effect size calculations, Q, and moderating variable analyses were conducted using STATA macros available on David Wilson’s website http://mason.gmu.edu/~dwilsonb/ma.html 72 Qes = ∑(wi(ESi – ES+) 2) where wi is the inverse variance weight for effect size i. ESi is the individual effect size and ES+ is the weighted mean effect size. Once Qes is calculated for each effect size, all values are summed. The summed value is designated Qobt. Values of Qobt were compared to critical values of the chi-square distribution with k-1 degrees of freedom where k is the number of effect size estimates. Values found to be significant were considered heterogeneous. According to Pratt (2001), values found to be significant may potentially be too dissimilar to be combined. However, Lipsey and Wilson (2001) suggest that significant Q values simply represent data gathered from varying populations. Effect sizes generated from heterogeneous populations can be adjusted and reanalyzed once outliers are removed (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001). When Q values were significant, several steps were taken. First, Bonta et al. (1998) suggest that any value greater than three standard deviations from the mean should be considered an outlier and removed. Second, estimates that are discontinuous from the rest of the distribution were also removed (Durlak and Lipsey, 1991). Once the outliers were removed, a new mean effect size, weighted mean effect size, and confidence interval were calculated. Both results, one with outliers and one without outliers, are presented. Additionally, random effects modeling was used for effect size analyses. Random effects modeling generates larger confidence intervals than fixed effects modeling, and is a more conservative approach (Lipsey and Wilson, 2001).15 15 For mean effect sizes both the fixed and random effects results are presented in the next chapter. 73 Fail-safe N statistic As discussed above, a critique of meta-analysis is that studies with null findings are less likely to be published and rather will sit in a scholars file drawer collecting dust. Given the current environment of scholars at universities, it seems unlikely that someone would hold a paper in a file drawer. Moreover, null findings that question the validity of previous research are equally worthy of publication. Nonetheless, meta-analysts have developed statistics to combat this file-drawer problem critique, the fail-safe N statistic. This statistic estimates the number of studies with alternative findings necessary to render the current effect size to the point of insignificance (Wolf, 1986). The larger the value of the fail-safe N statistic, the more studies with alternative findings that would be necessary to find a non-significant effect size (Hunter and Schmidt, 1990; Lipsey and Wilson, 2001; Pratt, 2001). According to Lipsey and Wilson (2001) the fail-safe N formula is as follows: k0 = k[(ESk/ESc)-1] where: k = number of studies used to calculate the mean effect size ESk = the mean effect size ESc = the alternative effect size value of non-significance The values used for ESc varied depending on the particular calculation. The value used was the minimum value that would incorporate the null value, zero, within a 95% confidence interval. Conclusions The analyses discussed above were designed to assess the average strength of correlates of decision-making at three distinct decision points across the criminal justice system. The ultimate goal of this assessment is to examine the support for various criminal justice theories and to work toward the plausibility of a general theory of criminal justice. In order to achieve 74 these goals, each relationship was assessed via the mean effect size process. Correlates that generate significant effect sizes were retained for each particular decision point. Those that did not generate a significant mean effect size were eliminated from consideration toward a general theory of criminal justice. After determining which correlates of decision-making were significant, an assessment was made to examine the existence of a grand theory approach versus decision-specific approach to theories of criminal justice. When mean effect sizes for various predictors are significant at multiple decision points and have sufficiently large fail-safe N values, then a grand theory of criminal justice is the recommended pathway. It is not necessary that correlates function in the same manner at each decision point. It is conceivable that the same variable may act differently depending on the decision point. For example, it is possible that officers are more likely to push women through the formal criminal justice process and judges are more likely to be lenient at sentencing because their action removes women from their familial responsibilities for a longer period of time than an arrest. In this case, the same variable, gender, is functioning differently at two different decision points. Yet the end result is that gender matters at both decision points. The alternative approach to criminal justice theory organization is a series of decisionspecific theories. These theories would emphasize decision-specific variables or each decision point would emphasize different predictor variables. If decision-specific variables produce larger effect sizes or each decision point has a unique set of significant effect sizes, with sufficiently large fail-safe N values, then decision-specific theories will be most appropriate for theories of criminal justice. 75 Limitations There have been some concerns raised about meta-analysis. These concerns about metaanalysis are three main problems: 1) the publication bias, 2) the incompatibility problem and 3) the lack of independence of effect sizes. Each of these problems has been discussed in detail above and solutions were proposed. Another issue is that the data are limited to mostly published materials and as a result this study will be biased by the same biases in those other studies when assessing the correlates of decision-making. This problem is commonly known as “garbage in, garbage out”. A flaw with this meta-analysis, and any meta-analysis, is that the effect sizes are only as good as the data used to generate them. There is no cure for this issue other than going through the moderating variable analysis. Missing variables are another related issue, and they happen for a multitude of reasons. Since this study is a meta-analysis there is even less control over missing variables. Omitted variable bias is even more of an issue. To combat this issue, random effects modeling was used to generate larger confidence intervals and thus, more conservative estimates. Finally, these analyses cannot examine interaction effects. The present analysis cannot include interaction effects because those measures are potentially dependent with the noninteractive variables. For example, the inclusion of race_X_gender interaction effect may cause dependency issues with the separate measures of race and gender. The best this meta-analysis can do is to assess potential differences in studies that used interaction effects and those that did not via moderating variable analysis. 76 CHAPTER 5 RESULTS This dissertation set out to answer three questions. First, what factors influence three decisions across the criminal justice system? Each of these decisions, arrest, sentencing, and parole revocation, are assessed and discussed. The second question examined in this dissertation is, given significant factors, are they consistent across the system or are significant factors unique to a particular decision point? The third question examined is, should criminal justice theory take a whole-system or decision-specific approach when formulating theories of criminal justice? The second and third questions will be discussed in the next chapter. This chapter will discuss the results from the meta-analysis described in the previous chapter. The first part of this chapter is a discussion of the general publication characteristics of each sample. The second part is a discussion of the results of the analysis of each of the decisions. The discussion of each decision is subdivided into sample characteristics, effect sizes and moderating variable analysis. It is important to note that effect sizes and confidence intervals in tables were left as logged odds ratios. This was done for ease of interpretation of the confidence intervals as logged odds ratios are symmetrical around zero and thus, any confidence interval containing zero is not significant at 95 percent confidence. Publication Characteristics As shown in Table 5.1, the analysis of arrest was based on 54 studies. The majority of studies were published recently. Approximately 96 percent of studies were published since 1990. However, only 70.3 percent of studies began data collection during that same time frame. This is likely due to large data collection efforts like POPN, PSS, or the Cincinnati study, which are still being used to generate publications today. The vast majority of data were generated 77 from studies published in journals. Geographically, studies were scattered. The Midwest region was where the largest number of studies were conducted, 27.80 percent. The next most common geographic categories were the South and multi-site studies (18.50 percent of studies each). The least common study location was the Pacific/Northwest category. Finally, the majority of authors were university/college professors and the most common discipline was criminology/criminal justice. As shown in Table 5.1, the sentencing analysis was based on 46 studies. The majority of studies were published recently. Specifically, 66.70% of studies were published since 2000. However, some studies were based on older data. Approximately one quarter of studies were based on data gathered in the 1960s or 1970s. Sentencing studies tended to be scattered across the United States. The most common study region was the Midwest, 34.80 percent, followed by the South, 28.30 percent. Eight studies were based on data collected in multiple states or federal court. The most common publication type was a journal, with about 70 percent of studies published in journals. The vast majority, of authors (95.30%) were affiliated with universities or colleges. Finally, approximately 70 percent of authors worked in the criminology/criminal justice discipline. Table 5.1 also presents the general publication information for the parole revocation analysis. It should be noted that studies of parole revocation were the least consistent. The parole revocation analysis presented the smallest sample size, with only 19 accepted studies. Additionally, the parole revocation publication analysis had a fair amount of missing data given the small sample size. The results indicate that the majority of studies were published recently. 63.10 percent of studies had been published since 2000. The majority of data collection has taken place since 1990. As with previous decisions, studies are scattered throughout the United 78 States. Parole revocation studies were most commonly conducted on the West Coast, 33.30 percent, followed by the Midwest, 22.20 percent. The most common publication type was an academic journal, 73.70 percent. Finally, authors were primarily affiliated with universities and colleges and the most common discipline was criminology/criminal justice or mixed, 36.80 percent each. 79 Table 5.1: Publication Characteristics Publication Characteristic Publication Decade 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010 Sentencing N % Parole Revocation N % 1 1 20 32 0 1.90 1.90 37.00 59.30 0.00 0 6 9 27 3 0.00 13.30 20.00 60.00 6.70 1 3 3 5 7 5.30 15.80 15.80 26.30 36.80 Publication Type Journal Dissertation/Thesis Agency Report Other 37 11 6 0 68.50 20.40 11.10 0.00 32 12 1 1 69.60 26.10 2.20 2.20 14 2 3 0 73.70 10.50 15.80 0.00 Data Gathered 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2 6 8 20 18 3.70 11.10 14.80 37.00 33.30 2 10 4 24 6 4.30 21.70 8.70 52.20 13.00 0 0 3 5 8 0.00 0.00 18.80 31.80 50.00 Geographic Region of the Study Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed/Nationwide 9 10 15 7 3 10 16.70 18.50 27.80 13.00 5.60 18.50 4 13 16 4 1 8 8.70 28.30 34.80 8.70 2.20 17.40 1 3 4 3 6 1 5.60 16.70 22.20 16.70 33.30 5.60 Author Affiliation University/College State Agency Mixed Other 51 0 1 2 94.40 0.00 1.90 3.70 41 0 2 0 95.30 0.00 4.70 0.00 15 1 3 0 78.90 5.30 15.80 0.00 N Arrest % 80 Table 5.1 continued Publication Characteristic Author Discipline Criminology/Criminal Justice Psychology Sociology Social Work Mixed Other 34 1 9 1 3 6 N 54 N Arrest % 63.00 1.90 16.70 1.90 5.60 11.10 Sentencing N % Parole Revocation N % 29 0 10 0 2 1 7 1 2 1 7 1 46 69.00 0.00 21.30 0.00 4.80 2.40 36.80 5.30 10.50 5.30 36.80 5.30 19 Arrest Sample characteristics Table 5.2 describes the sample characteristics for the arrest study analysis. As shown in Table 5.2, the average sample size was 60,423.61 cases and across all 54 studies, 3,262,875 cases were studied. The mean number of agencies involved in each study was 4.311 agencies. The mean study length was 21.67 months. Approximately, 20 percent of studies reported at least one analysis with interaction effects, a quarter of studies used systematic social observation, and nearly all studies presented information on control variables and used continuous data collection, 92.60 and 98.10 respectively. Finally, approximately 70 percent of studies collected information on offense specific arrests. Table 5.2 also describes what variables were collected in various studies. These variables are separated into 4 groups: suspect characteristics, officer characteristics, seriousness characteristics, and other characteristics. For suspects, the majority of studies collected information on race, gender and age of suspects, 83.30, 72.20, and 66.70 respectively. Approximately 30 to 25 percent of studies reported information on suspect ethnicity, demeanor, 81 or the presence of interaction phase crime. Finally, few studies collected information on suspect compliance, or whether the suspect was mentally ill, 16.70 and 5.60 percent, respectively. Officer characteristics were not commonly recorded. Officer race and experience measures were most common. Each was recorded in 20.40 percent of studies. Officer gender was slightly less common 18.50 percent of studies. Officer education was recorded in 16.70 percent studies. As shown in Table 5.2, the mean number of seriousness indicators was 1.407 per study. Weapon use and a general seriousness measure, most commonly measured as felony versus misdemeanor, were recorded in 33.30 and 31.50 percent studies respectively. An intoxication measure was recorded in 29.60 percent of the studies. Evidence and victim preference for arrest were recorded in about one quarter of studies, 25.90 and 24.10 percent respectively. Finally, other characteristics were relatively uncommon. Less than 10 percent of studies recorded organization measures and slightly more than 10 percent recorded information on community variables. However, it should be noted that there was very little consistency in these measures and therefore, they are not included in the effect size analysis. 82 Table 5.2: Arrest Sample Characteristics Variable General Study Characteristics Agencies Offense Specific (Yes=1) Control Variables Systematic Social Observation Interaction Effects Length Continuous Data Collection Size Mean/% S 4.31 68.50 92.60 25.90 18.50 21.67 98.10 60423.61 7.25 Suspect Characteristics Suspect Race Measure Suspect Gender Measure Suspect Age Measure Suspect Ethnicity Measure Demeanor Measure Interaction Phase Crime Measure Compliance Measure Mental Illness Measure 83.30 72.20 66.70 27.80 29.60 24.10 16.70 5.60 Officer Characteristics Officer Race Measure Officer Gender Measure Officer Education Measure Officer Experience Measure 20.40 18.50 16.70 20.40 Seriousness Characteristics Seriousness Measure Evidence Measure Intoxication Measure Weapon Use Measure Victim Preference Measure Total Serious Measures 31.50 25.90 29.60 33.30 24.10 1.41 Other Characteristics Organization Variables Measured Community Variables Measured 9.40 11.10 N 54 83 Total 29.19 149133.61 1.27 3262875 Effect sizes Table 5.3 displays results for the arrest effect size analysis. Suspect race, gender and ethnicity significantly influenced arrest. Specifically, black suspects were approximately 1.40 times more likely to be arrested than white suspects, male suspects were approximately 1.50 times more likely to be arrested than female suspects and Hispanic suspects were approximately 1.25 times more likely to be arrested than non-Hispanic suspects across studies. Age and mental illness were not significantly related to arrest. Additionally, all three of the characteristics were all significant at p < .001. No officer characteristics were significantly related to arrest. Seriousness measures are all significant and in the expected directions. The strongest seriousness indicator was strength of evidence. As amounts of evidence increased, arrest chances were almost 7 times higher. When the victim preferred arrest, the odds of arrest increased 4.10 times. Felonies were approximately 2.50 times more likely to end in arrest than misdemeanors. Finally, suspect intoxication and the presence or use of a weapon increased the odds of arrest by 1.53 and 1.58 times respectively. Suspect demeanor measures were also influential in arrest outcomes. Demeanor was not stronger than the effect of interaction phase crime. Demeanor increased the odds of arrest by almost 200 percent, but when a suspect committed an interaction phase crime the odds of arrest increased 3.17 times across studies. Additionally, when suspects were considered noncompliant, their odds of arrest increased 1.79 times. 84 Table 5.3: Mean Effect Sizes Arrest Mean ES W Mean ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max 42 3253123 0.66 0.33 (0.01) 0.221 (0.02) 0.438 (0.03) Without Outliers*** 41 3239557 0.52 0.33 (0.01 0.222 (-0.02) 0.438 (0.03) Suspect Male*** 38 3213071 0.83 0.40 (0.16 0.29 (0.12) 0.52 (0.19) Without Outliers *** 37 3199505 0.38 0.40 (0.16) 0.29 (0.12) 0.52 (0.19) Suspect Age 35 2120709 0.06 -0.001 (0.001) -0.004 (0.001) 0.003 (0.001) Suspect Ethnicity*** 15 2858798 0.26 0.22 (0.02) 0.104 (0.02) 0.343 (0.03) Suspect Mental Illness 4 -0.51 -0.47 (-0.34) -1.43 (-0.77) 0.49 (0.09) Officer Variables Officer Black 11 1822723 -0.07 -0.035 (0.01) -0.097 (-0.01) 0.027 (0.02) Officer Male 10 1787322 0.32 0.095 (-0.020) -0.069 (-0.05) 0.26 (0.01) Officer Experience 8 1733917 0.12 0.009 (0.003) -0.018 (0.002) 0.037 (0.01) Seriousness Variables Seriousness*** 14 19782 1.91 0.925 (0.34) 0.585 (0.27) 1.265 (0.40) Variable k Suspect Variables Suspect Black*** N 4364 85 Table 5.3 continued 1728460 Mean ES 2.63 W Mean ES 1.93 (0.67) 95% CI Min 1.38 (0.59) 95% CI Max 2.49 (0.74) 17 64711 0.60 0.43 (0.38) 0.27 (0.30) 0.59 (0.45) Weapon Use*** 15 73113 0.59 0.46 (0.34) 0.30 (0.25) 0.62 (0.42) Arrest Preference*** 13 12811 1.45 1.41 (1.57) 0.87 (1.36) 1.94 (1.77) Demeanor Variables Demeanor*** 16 150640 0.03 0.69 (0.02) 0.36 (0.002) 1.02 (0.04) Without Outliers*** 13 150050 0.68 0.64 (0.02) 0.31 (0.001) 0.97 (0.04) Interaction Phase Crime** 9 43810 1.81 1.15 (0.19) 0.35 (0.01) 1.96 (0.36) Compliance** 9 6794 0.66 0.58 (0.45) 0.15 (0.31) 1.02 (0.58) Variable k N Evidence*** 12 Intoxication*** (Fixed effects results in parentheses) * sig p< .05 ** sig p<.01 *** sig p< .001 Fail-safe N analysis The fail-safe N statistic determines the number of studies necessary to render the effect size to a value of non-significance. Higher values indicate that more studies with null values are required to nullify the mean effect size. The value chosen for the negligible value varied depending on the particular mean effect size. A fail-safe N value was calculated for each 86 significant weighted mean effect size value. Overall, there is confidence in these significant effect sizes. The average fail-safe N value was 36. There was some variation in fail-safe N values amongst the various significant mean effect sizes. The “suspect black” finding would require another 86 studies to render the finding presented above to an insignificant value.16 The effect of suspect gender was also a stable finding; the fail-safe N value for gender was 93. Ethnicity was the least stable significant suspect characteristic with a fail-safe N value of 15. While there is some variation amongst these significant suspect characteristics, they are relatively stable. All of the seriousness measures are stable as well. The relationship between seriousness of the offense and arrest would require another 25 null effect sizes, and the evidence effect size would require an additional 30 null effect sizes to render these findings non-significant. Additionally, the intoxication effect size’s fail-safe N value is 28, and the weapon use effect size fail-safe N value is 31. The arrest preference effect size would require an additional 21 effect sizes to render that finding non-significant. Finally, the fail-safe N values for various demeanor measures were the least stable. The fail-safe N value for demeanor is 13. The mean effect sizes for interaction-phase crime and compliance are much less stable than the fail-safe N for demeanor. The values are 4 and 3 respectively. Moderating variable analysis An attempt was made to dig deeper into the effect size analysis above by conducting an analysis of moderating variables. The moderator variable analysis for arrest is limited. The primary limitation is lack of variation in moderating variables. Due to the consistency of study variables there is little variation in the variables found in studies. As a result, effect sizes used in the moderating variable analysis are largely lumped into one category and very few in the others. 16 Where applicable fail-safe N values are reported with outliers removed. 87 A second limitation is there were few mean effect sizes with enough k values for meaningful moderator analysis. Using a cutoff of a k value of 15, the variables that could be further analyzed were: suspect race, suspect gender, suspect ethnicity, suspect intoxication, suspect weapon use, and suspect demeanor. Due to a technical issue with the STATA software, too many cases had to be removed in order to analyze suspect age. As a result, that analysis was not conducted. Tables 5.4-5.9 below display the results of the moderating variable analysis. Across all moderator analyses, variables that were significant in the mean effect size analysis remained significant across moderator categories.17 Table 5.4 displays the full results of the suspect race moderator analysis. Effect sizes generated by authors that were categorized as “other” discipline did not report significant race effect sizes. Studies conducted in the South did not report significant race effect sizes, but all other geographic regions did report significant race effect sizes. The mean effect size for studies using data gathered in the 1980s did not report a significant race effect. However, it should be noted that the k value for this finding is small. Across other moderating variables, race remained significant. There were only a handful of nonsignificant categories and the majority of these were based on a small number of effect sizes, one to three, which made these results less meaningful. The findings for the moderator analysis of gender effect sizes are similar to the findings for race in that, across moderator categories, mean effect sizes remained significant. The full results are displayed in Table 5.5. Like race, mean effect sizes for studies conducted by those in the “other” discipline category were not significant. Upon closer examination of when studies were published, only those published between 2000 and 2009 produced significant mean effect sizes for gender. However, studies based on data gathered in the 1960s, 1970s, and 1980s did 17 The findings discuss those results that are contrary to the general finding. 88 not produce significant mean effect sizes. Only two studies were based on data from the 1960s and 1980s, and six studies used data from the 1970s. Finally, the remaining moderator categories either produced mean effect sizes for gender that remained significant or those that were not significant were based on a negligible number of studies. Table 5.6 displays the full results for the ethnicity moderating variable analysis. Relative to race and gender there are significantly fewer effect sizes making up the suspect ethnicity moderator analysis. The inclusion of Hispanic status as an empirical variable is still a relatively modern addition. The majority of studies analyzed here were published between 2000 and 2009 and a few in the 1990s. Nevertheless, there were a sufficient number of effect sizes to conduct a moderating variable analysis. The pattern found in these results is similar to the previous moderator analyses for individual suspect characteristics. Specifically, the majority of mean effect sizes across categories are significant and those that are not are often limited to one or two effect sizes to generate the mean effect size. Significant mean effect sizes were found among those that were conducted by individuals in criminal justice or mixed disciplines. Studies conducted in the Northeast and the Southwest produced significant mean effect sizes for ethnicity. Hispanics were significantly more likely to be arrested according to studies conducted between 2000 and 2009. However, it should be noted that the smaller number of studies spread over more categories creates a small number of effect sizes generating the mean effect size in this finding. Only state/local reports reported significantly more Hispanics being arrested than non-Hispanics. Studies that included a measure of age found that Hispanic status increased arrest. Two seriousness measures had a sufficient number of effect sizes to perform a meaningful moderator analysis, suspect intoxication and suspect weapon use. Table 5.7 displays 89 the full results for the moderating variable analysis for suspect intoxication. Consistent with previous moderator analyses, mean effect sizes across moderator categories are generally significant. Those mean effect sizes that were not significant are those that were created from one or two effect sizes. One exception is that mean effect sizes generated from studies that did not measure gender and those that did not measure age did not produce significant mean effect sizes for suspect intoxication. Table 5.8 displays the full results for suspect weapon use. For this analysis all mean effect sizes were significant across moderator categories except for a handful of categories that had one to three effect sizes to create the mean effect size. The final variable examined for moderator variable analysis was suspect demeanor. The full results for this analysis are displayed in Table 5.9. The results here are similar to all of the previous moderator results for the arrest decision. Specifically, mean effect sizes are generally significant across moderator categories and those that are not significant are often generated from one to three effect sizes. There were some unique findings. The mean effect size for studies conducted by sociologists did not find demeanor significant. Studies conducted in the Northeast and Midwest did not find demeanor significantly predicted arrest. Studies based on data from the 1980s did not find demeanor significantly predicted arrest. Studies that did not measure intoxication did not produce a significant mean effect size for demeanor. Finally, both studies that did and those that did not record a measure of interaction phase crime produced significant mean effect sizes for demeanor and arrest. 90 Table 5.4: Suspect Race Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k Discipline Criminal Justice/Criminology 26 Psychology 0 Sociology 9 Social Work 0 Mixed 3 Others 6 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 0.34 0.00 0.52 0.00 0.35 0.12 0.22 0.00 0.26 0.00 0.07 -0.04 0.45 0.00 0.77 0.00 0.62 0.28 0.06 0.00 0.13 0.00 0.35 0.08 Publication Decade 1970 1980 1990 2000 1 1 15 28 0.63 0.71 0.34 0.30 0.12 -0.59 0.09 0.19 1.14 2.02 0.59 0.42 0.26 0.66 0.13 0.06 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 6 7 14 7 3 8 0.27 -0.06 0.49 0.43 0.51 0.15 0.05 -0.47 0.27 0.25 0.22 0.00 0.48 0.35 0.71 0.61 0.79 0.29 0.11 0.21 0.11 0.09 0.15 0.07 Data Gathered 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2 6 4 16 17 0.41 0.52 0.31 0.43 0.25 0.07 0.09 -0.13 0.21 0.12 0.75 0.96 0.75 0.64 0.39 0.17 0.22 0.23 0.11 0.07 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or Local Report 30 9 6 0.23 0.33 0.46 0.11 0.15 0.27 0.36 0.50 0.64 0.06 0.09 0.10 91 Table 5.4 continued Moderator Variable Agency Number 1 2 3 4 8 9 20 24 28 k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 28 1 1 1 1 3 1 1 1 0.39 0.21 0.63 0.53 0.31 1.33 0.94 0.45 0.71 0.26 -0.26 0.19 -0.29 -0.31 0.51 0.27 -0.08 -0.57 0.51 0.68 1.08 1.35 0.92 2.14 2.16 0.98 1.99 0.06 0.24 0.23 0.44 0.32 0.42 0.62 0.27 0.65 Offense Specific No Yes 16 29 0.45 0.16 0.26 0.16 0.64 0.39 0.10 0.06 Control Variables No Yes 4 41 0.52 0.32 0.08 0.21 0.97 0.42 0.23 0.05 SSO Use No Yes 32 13 0.29 0.49 0.18 0.24 0.40 0.74 0.06 0.13 Interaction Effect Use No Yes 37 8 0.35 0.25 0.23 0.00 0.46 0.51 0.06 0.13 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 1 44 0.21 0.34 -0.35 0.22 0.77 0.45 0.29 0.06 Offender Gender Recorded No Yes 7 38 0.42 0.32 0.05 0.21 0.79 0.43 0.19 0.06 Offender Age Recorded No Yes 11 34 0.26 0.36 0.07 0.23 0.45 0.49 0.10 0.07 92 Table 5.4 continued Moderator Variable Offender Ethnicity Recorded No Yes k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 30 15 0.24 0.42 0.11 0.27 0.36 0.56 0.06 0.07 Officer Race Recorded No Yes 34 11 0.38 0.23 0.25 0.58 0.51 0.40 0.07 0.09 Officer Gender Recorded No Yes 36 9 0.35 0.30 0.21 0.10 0.49 0.51 0.07 0.11 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 30 15 0.32 0.33 0.20 0.15 0.44 0.52 0.06 0.09 Evidence Recorded No Yes 31 14 0.27 0.44 0.15 0.26 0.38 0.61 0.06 0.09 Intoxication Recorded No Yes 32 13 0.35 0.31 0.21 0.06 0.48 0.56 0.07 0.13 Weapon Use Recorded No Yes 35 10 0.36 0.26 0.23 0.01 0.50 0.50 0.07 0.13 Victim Preference Recorded No Yes 35 10 0.30 0.43 0.19 0.18 0.42 0.69 0.06 0.13 Demeanor Recorded No Yes 30 15 0.27 0.49 0.16 0.27 0.39 0.70 0.06 0.11 93 Table 5.4 Continued Moderator Variable Interaction Phase Crime Recorded No Yes Compliance Recorded No Yes k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 35 10 0.34 0.29 0.22 0.03 0.45 0.55 0.06 0.13 37 8 0.31 0.50 0.20 0.17 0.42 0.83 0.06 0.17 Organization Recorded No Yes 41 3 0.35 -0.10 0.28 -0.23 0.43 0.04 0.04 0.07 Community Recorded No Yes 41 4 0.33 0.32 0.22 0.02 0.44 0.62 0.06 0.15 94 Table 5.5: Suspect Gender Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k Discipline Criminal Justice/Criminology 23 Psychology 0 Sociology 7 Social Work 0 Mixed 3 Others 5 0.42 0.00 0.38 0.00 0.80 0.15 0.28 0.00 0.10 0.00 0.46 -0.08 0.55 0.00 0.66 0.00 1.13 0.37 0.07 0.00 0.14 0.00 0.80 0.15 Publication Decade 1970 1980 1990 2000 1 1 13 23 0.53 0.53 0.22 0.45 -0.09 -0.78 -0.03 0.31 1.15 1.85 0.47 0.59 0.32 0.67 0.13 0.07 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 3 5 13 7 2 8 0.53 0.65 0.29 0.56 0.50 0.24 0.20 0.17 0.04 0.32 0.13 0.04 0.87 1.14 0.55 0.80 0.86 0.44 0.17 0.25 0.13 0.12 0.19 0.10 Data Gathered 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2 0.38 6 0.29 2 -0.03 13 0.42 15 0.46 -0.05 -0.17 -0.54 0.19 0.29 0.80 0.75 0.48 0.66 0.64 0.22 0.24 0.26 0.12 0.09 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or Local Report 26 6 6 0.13 0.32 0.31 0.42 0.80 0.77 0.08 0.12 0.12 95 ES 0.28 0.56 0.54 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.5 continued Moderator Variable Agency Number 1 2 3 8 9 20 24 28 22 0.56 1 0.23 1 0.53 1 0.93 3 -0.19 1 0.23 1 0.40 1 0.53 0.42 -0.27 0.01 0.33 -0.74 -1.36 -0.24 -0.74 0.71 0.73 1.04 1.52 0.37 1.83 1.04 1.80 0.07 0.25 0.26 0.30 0.28 0.81 0.22 0.65 Offense Specific No Yes 14 24 0.36 0.42 0.15 0.28 0.57 0.57 0.11 0.07 Control Variables No Yes 3 35 0.50 0.40 -0.02 0.28 1.01 0.52 0.26 0.06 SSO Use No Yes 27 11 0.40 0.39 0.28 0.12 0.53 0.67 0.06 0.14 Interaction Effect Use No Yes 31 7 0.43 0.29 0.30 0.01 0.57 0.56 0.07 0.14 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 1 37 0.23 0.41 -0.36 0.29 0.82 0.53 0.30 0.06 Offender Age Recorded No Yes 8 30 0.28 0.45 0.08 0.32 0.48 0.57 0.10 0.07 Offender Ethnicity Recorded No Yes 26 12 0.30 0.53 0.15 0.37 0.44 0.70 0.07 0.08 k 96 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.5 continued Moderator Variable Officer Race Recorded No Yes k ES 27 11 0.41 0.38 0.27 0.20 0.56 0.56 0.07 0.09 Officer Gender Recorded No Yes 30 8 0.41 0.38 0.27 0.17 0.56 0.60 0.07 0.11 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 26 12 0.39 0.44 0.25 0.21 0.53 0.66 0.07 0.11 Evidence Recorded No Yes 26 12 0.29 0.60 0.16 0.42 0.42 0.79 0.07 0.09 Intoxication Recorded No Yes 27 11 0.40 0.41 0.27 0.16 0.54 0.65 0.07 0.12 Weapon Use Recorded No Yes 31 7 0.43 0.31 0.30 0.06 0.56 0.56 0.07 0.13 Victim Preference Recorded No Yes 31 7 0.38 0.50 0.26 0.21 0.51 0.80 0.06 0.15 Demeanor Recorded No Yes 24 14 0.41 0.39 0.27 0.17 0.54 0.61 0.07 0.11 Interaction Phase Crime Recorded No Yes 29 9 0.40 0.41 0.28 0.11 0.53 0.71 0.06 0.15 97 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.5 continued Moderator Variable Compliance Recorded No Yes k ES 30 8 0.41 0.33 0.29 0.02 0.54 0.64 0.06 0.16 Organization Recorded No Yes 34 3 0.43 0.00 0.33 -0.22 0.54 0.22 0.05 0.11 Community Recorded No Yes 35 3 0.39 0.54 0.27 0.17 0.51 0.91 0.06 0.19 98 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.6: Suspect Ethnicity Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k Discipline Criminal Justice/Criminology 8 Psychology 0 Sociology 4 Social Work 0 Mixed 2 Others 1 0.22 0.00 0.18 0.00 0.49 0.25 0.05 0.00 -0.20 0.00 0.10 -0.19 0.40 0.00 0.56 0.00 0.87 0.69 0.09 0.00 0.19 0.00 0.19 0.27 Publication Decade 1990 2000 3 12 0.30 0.25 -0.84 0.12 1.44 0.39 0.58 0.07 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 3 2 2 4 3 1 0.49 -0.10 0.21 0.35 0.21 0.11 0.20 -0.37 -0.01 0.17 -0.01 -0.43 0.79 0.17 0.44 0.54 0.44 0.65 0.15 0.14 0.11 0.05 0.11 0.28 Data Gathered 1980 1990 2000 3 2 10 0.30 0.28 0.25 -0.84 -0.16 0.10 1.43 0.72 0.40 0.58 0.22 0.08 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or Local Report 4 5 6 0.07 0.17 0.38 -0.15 -0.07 0.22 0.28 0.41 0.53 0.11 0.12 0.08 Agency Number 1 4 12 1 0.25 0.44 0.11 -0.18 0.40 1.05 0.07 0.31 99 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.6 continued Moderator Variable Offense Specific No Yes k ES 4 11 0.15 0.26 -0.39 0.12 0.70 0.40 0.28 0.07 SSO Use No Yes 13 2 0.25 0.30 0.12 -0.84 0.39 1.44 0.07 0.58 Interaction Effect Use No Yes 11 4 0.26 0.23 0.10 -0.09 0.42 0.55 0.08 0.16 Offender Gender Recorded No Yes 3 12 0.31 0.24 -0.04 0.09 0.67 0.40 0.18 0.08 Offender Age Recorded No Yes 5 10 0.23 0.27 -0.02 0.10 0.48 0.44 0.13 0.09 Officer Race Recorded No Yes 10 5 0.22 0.31 0.05 0.03 0.39 0.53 0.09 0.11 Officer Gender Recorded No Yes 10 5 0.21 0.31 0.03 0.11 0.38 0.52 0.09 0.10 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 12 3 0.24 0.43 0.10 -0.14 0.38 1.00 0.07 0.29 Evidence Recorded No Yes 8 7 0.22 0.28 0.00 0.09 0.43 0.47 0.11 0.10 100 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.6 continued Moderator Variable Weapon Use Recorded No Yes k ES 14 1 0.24 0.44 0.11 -0.17 0.38 1.05 0.07 0.31 Victim Preference Recorded No Yes 14 1 0.24 0.44 0.11 -0.17 0.38 1.05 0.07 0.31 Demeanor Recorded No Yes 12 3 0.27 0.15 0.12 -0.26 0.42 0.57 0.08 0.21 Interaction Phase Crime Recorded No Yes 13 2 0.26 0.25 0.11 -0.19 0.41 0.68 0.08 0.22 Community Recorded No Yes 14 1 0.22 0.57 0.09 0.13 0.35 1.01 0.07 0.23 101 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.7: Suspect Intoxication Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k ES Discipline Criminal Justice/Criminology 13 0.41 Psychology 0 0.00 Sociology 2 0.76 Social Work 0 0.00 Mixed 1 0.48 Others 1 0.43 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 0.18 0.00 0.12 0.00 -0.21 -0.16 0.64 0.00 1.40 0.00 1.16 1.01 0.12 0.00 0.33 0.00 0.35 0.30 Publication Decade 1980 1990 2000 1 4 12 0.40 0.68 0.38 -1.06 0.26 0.20 1.86 1.10 0.56 0.74 0.21 0.09 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 1 6 4 2 1 3 -0.26 0.52 0.28 0.57 0.62 0.52 -0.62 0.19 0.07 0.16 0.18 0.34 0.10 0.85 0.49 0.98 1.07 0.70 0.18 0.17 0.11 0.21 0.23 0.09 Data Gathered 1970 1980 1990 2000 3 0 12 2 0.78 0.00 0.48 0.12 0.23 0.00 0.27 -0.27 1.34 0.00 0.70 0.52 0.28 0.00 0.11 0.20 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis 15 2 0.48 0.12 0.31 -0.24 0.65 0.49 0.08 0.19 102 Table 5.7 continued Moderator Variable Agency Number 1 2 3 4 8 9 20 21 24 28 k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 9 1 0 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 0.38 0.59 0.00 0.62 0.48 0.00 1.44 -0.26 0.74 0.40 0.10 0.17 0.00 0.13 -0.01 0.00 0.08 -0.68 0.24 -1.02 0.58 1.00 0.00 1.12 0.96 0.00 2.80 0.16 1.24 1.82 0.11 0.21 0.00 0.25 0.25 0.00 0.69 0.22 0.25 0.73 Offense Specific No Yes 5 12 0.63 0.32 0.36 0.13 0.90 0.51 0.14 0.10 Control Variables No Yes 2 15 1.08 0.41 0.13 0.24 2.02 0.57 0.48 0.08 SSO Use No Yes 11 6 0.30 0.65 0.11 0.38 0.50 0.91 0.10 0.13 Interaction Effect Use No Yes 15 2 0.39 0.74 0.23 0.22 0.56 1.27 0.09 0.27 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 1 16 0.59 0.41 0.06 0.24 1.12 0.59 0.27 0.09 Offender Race Recorded No Yes 3 17 0.01 0.48 -0.35 0.33 0.37 0.62 0.18 0.08 103 Table 5.7 continued Moderator Variable Offender Gender Recorded No Yes k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 6 11 0.24 0.50 -0.05 0.31 0.53 0.69 0.15 0.09 Offender Age Recorded No Yes 6 11 0.25 0.49 -0.05 0.30 0.55 0.67 0.15 0.09 Offender Ethnicity Recorded No Yes 16 1 0.41 0.62 0.24 0.03 0.58 1.22 0.09 0.30 Officer Race Recorded No Yes 14 3 0.47 0.37 0.26 0.05 0.68 0.68 0.11 0.16 Officer Gender Recorded No Yes 12 5 0.61 0.27 0.38 0.07 0.84 0.48 0.12 0.10 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 9 8 0.52 0.37 0.25 0.16 0.79 0.59 0.14 0.11 Evidence Recorded No Yes 10 7 0.40 0.49 0.16 0.22 0.63 0.76 0.12 0.14 Weapon Use Recorded No Yes 8 9 0.27 0.52 0.04 0.33 0.50 0.71 0.12 0.10 Victim Preference Recorded No Yes 6 11 0.23 0.58 0.01 0.37 0.46 0.78 0.12 0.11 104 Table 5.7 continued Moderator Variable Demeanor Recorded No Yes k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 9 8 0.33 0.55 0.09 0.24 0.58 0.81 0.13 0.13 Interaction Phase Crime Recorded No Yes 12 5 0.32 0.64 0.14 0.36 0.51 0.91 0.09 0.14 Compliance Recorded No Yes 13 4 0.45 0.41 0.24 0.10 0.66 0.72 0.11 0.16 Organization Recorded No Yes 15 2 0.52 0.12 0.32 -0.28 0.73 0.52 0.10 0.20 Community Recorded No Yes 15 2 0.42 0.49 0.23 0.11 0.61 0.87 0.10 0.19 105 Table 5.8: Suspect Weapon Use Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k ES Discipline Criminal Justice/Criminology 11 0.56 Psychology 0 0.00 Sociology 0 0.00 Social Work 1 0.27 Mixed 1 0.34 Others 2 0.35 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 0.34 0.00 0.00 -0.33 -0.99 -0.09 0.78 0.00 0.00 0.88 1.66 0.78 0.11 0.00 0.00 0.31 0.68 0.22 Publication Decade 1990 2000 5 10 0.55 0.44 0.27 0.20 0.83 0.67 0.14 0.12 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 2 5 2 1 0 6 0.41 0.73 0.71 0.34 0.00 0.35 0.07 0.23 0.32 -0.96 0.00 0.11 0.75 1.23 1.10 1.63 0.00 0.59 0.17 0.26 0.20 0.66 0.00 0.12 Data Gathered 1970 1980 1990 2000 1 3 9 2 1.16 0.59 0.42 0.30 0.39 0.24 0.21 -0.04 1.93 0.95 0.63 0.65 0.39 0.18 0.11 0.18 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis 13 2 0.47 0.44 0.30 -0.04 0.65 0.92 0.09 0.24 Agency Number 1 2 8 9 24 7 1 1 1 2 0.80 0.02 0.34 0.64 0.43 0.46 -0.91 -0.95 0.19 0.10 1.15 0.95 1.67 1.09 0.77 0.18 0.47 0.66 0.23 0.17 106 Table 5.8 continued Moderator Variable Offense Specific No Yes k ES 3 12 0.66 0.39 0.33 0.23 1.00 0.55 0.17 0.08 SSO Use No Yes 13 2 0.44 0.70 0.28 0.11 0.61 1.29 0.08 0.30 Interaction Effect Use No Yes 14 1 0.41 1.16 0.27 0.41 0.56 1.91 0.07 0.38 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 1 14 0.02 0.48 -0.92 0.31 0.04 0.64 0.48 0.08 Offender Race Recorded No Yes 6 9 0.48 0.49 0.20 0.25 0.77 0.73 0.15 0.12 Offender Gender Recorded No Yes 8 7 0.50 0.47 0.23 0.22 0.77 0.72 0.14 0.13 Offender Age Recorded No Yes 5 10 0.55 0.46 0.23 0.23 0.88 0.68 0.17 0.11 Officer Race Recorded No Yes 14 1 0.48 0.02 0.31 -0.92 0.64 0.96 0.08 0.48 Officer Gender Recorded No Yes 13 2 0.54 0.18 0.36 -0.18 0.72 0.54 0.09 0.19 107 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.8 continued Moderator Variable Seriousness Recorded No Yes k ES 12 3 0.40 0.96 0.25 0.43 0.55 1.49 0.07 0.27 Evidence Recorded No Yes 12 3 0.48 0.35 0.31 -0.27 0.65 0.97 0.09 0.32 Intoxication Recorded No Yes 7 8 0.50 0.44 0.28 0.14 0.72 0.74 0.11 0.15 Victim Preference Recorded No Yes 8 7 0.38 0.73 0.23 0.38 0.54 1.07 0.08 0.18 Demeanor Recorded No Yes 13 2 0.44 0.70 0.28 0.11 0.61 1.29 0.09 0.30 Interaction Phase Crime Recorded No Yes 11 4 0.41 0.73 0.25 0.31 0.57 1.14 0.08 0.21 Compliance Recorded No Yes 14 1 0.48 0.02 0.31 -0.92 0.64 0.96 0.08 0.48 Organization Recorded No Yes 14 1 0.52 0.21 0.34 -0.20 0.71 0.61 0.09 0.21 Community Recorded No Yes 13 2 0.45 0.57 0.29 0.02 0.62 1.12 0.09 0.28 108 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.9: Suspect Demeanor Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k Discipline Criminal Justice/Criminology 10 Psychology 0 Sociology 5 Social Work 0 Mixed 0 Others 1 0.70 0.00 0.37 0.00 0.00 1.03 0.35 0.00 -0.34 0.00 0.00 0.11 1.06 0.00 1.08 0.00 0.00 1.95 0.18 0.00 0.36 0.00 0.00 0.47 Publication Decade 1970 1980 1990 2000 1 0 9 6 1.03 0.00 0.52 0.73 0.08 0.00 0.03 0.30 0.20 0.00 1.01 1.17 0.49 0.00 0.25 0.22 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 1 4 6 2 0 3 0.01 0.62 0.52 1.00 0.00 0.90 -0.73 0.02 -0.01 0.28 0.00 0.43 0.74 0.12 1.06 1.72 0.00 1.37 0.38 0.31 0.27 0.37 0.00 0.24 Data Gathered 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 1 5 3 6 1 1.03 1.09 0.07 0.67 0.89 0.32 0.45 -0.43 0.28 0.08 1.74 1.73 0.56 1.06 1.70 0.36 0.33 0.25 0.20 0.41 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or Local Report 13 2 1 0.74 0.51 0.89 0.30 -0.35 -0.39 1.19 1.36 2.17 0.23 0.44 0.65 109 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.9 continued Moderator Variable Agency Number 1 2 3 20 24 k ES 12 1 1 1 1 0.48 0.49 1.03 1.26 1.17 0.18 -0.27 0.32 -0.02 0.39 0.79 1.25 1.74 2.55 1.95 0.16 0.39 0.36 0.65 0.40 Offense Specific No Yes 10 6 0.70 0.74 0.24 0.05 1.16 1.44 0.24 0.36 Control Variables No Yes 3 13 1.09 0.57 0.44 0.26 1.75 0.88 0.33 0.16 SSO Use No Yes 3 13 0.61 0.74 -0.21 0.30 1.43 1.18 0.42 0.22 Interaction Effect Use No Yes 11 5 0.77 0.52 0.37 -0.09 1.18 1.12 0.21 0.31 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 1 15 0.49 0.72 -0.63 0.36 1.61 1.08 0.57 0.18 Offender Race Recorded No Yes 1 15 0.20 0.74 -1.00 0.39 1.40 1.09 0.61 0.18 Offender Gender Recorded No Yes 2 14 0.08 0.81 -0.57 0.49 0.74 1.14 0.34 0.16 110 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.9 continued Moderator Variable Offender Age Recorded No Yes k ES 3 13 0.71 0.69 -0.11 0.32 1.54 1.06 0.42 0.19 Offender Ethnicity Recorded No Yes 13 3 0.80 0.33 0.44 -0.31 1.17 0.96 0.19 0.33 Officer Race Recorded No Yes 13 3 0.80 0.46 0.35 -0.31 1.26 1.23 0.23 0.39 Officer Gender Recorded No Yes 13 3 0.80 0.46 0.35 -0.31 1.26 1.23 0.23 0.39 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 7 9 0.70 0.69 0.17 0.26 1.24 1.11 0.27 0.22 Evidence Recorded No Yes 10 6 0.65 0.78 0.13 0.22 1.17 1.34 0.26 0.29 Intoxication Recorded No Yes 8 8 0.47 0.96 -0.11 0.45 0.96 1.47 0.27 0.26 Weapon Use Recorded No Yes 13 3 0.71 0.64 0.33 0.00 1.09 1.27 0.19 0.32 Victim Preference Recorded No Yes 9 7 0.28 0.97 0.01 0.68 0.56 1.26 0.14 0.15 111 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.9 continued Moderator Variable Interaction Phase Crime Recorded No Yes k ES 7 9 0.71 0.66 0.27 0.18 1.14 1.14 0.22 0.24 Compliance Recorded No Yes 9 7 0.77 0.62 0.29 -0.05 1.25 1.28 0.25 0.34 Community Recorded No Yes 14 2 0.71 0.64 0.34 -0.19 1.08 1.46 0.19 0.42 112 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Sentencing Sample characteristics Table 5.10 describes the sample characteristics for the sentencing study analysis. As shown in Table 5.10, the average sample size contained 49,382.72 cases and across all 46 studies, 2,271,605 cases were studied. The mean number of court systems involved in each study was 1.83. The mean length of the study period was 37.89 months. Approximately 30 percent of studies reported at least one analysis with interaction effects, and nearly all studies used presented information on control variables and used continuous data collection, 95.70 and 87.00 percent respectively. Finally, approximately 33 percent of studies collected information on offense specific cases. Table 5.10 also describes what variables were collected in various studies. These variables were separated into 4 groups: suspect characteristics, judge characteristics, seriousness characteristics, and other characteristics. For suspects, the majority of studies collected information on race, gender, and age of suspects, 84.80, 69.60, and 87.00 percent respectively. Approximately 35 and 20 percent of studies reported information on suspect ethnicity, and suspect employment. Judge characteristics were not commonly recorded. Judge race was most common. Judge race was recorded in 10.90 percent of studies. Judge gender was less common with only 6.40 percent of studies reporting information on the gender of the judge. Judicial experience was also not a common variable. Only 6.5 percent of studies recorded judge experience measures. Measures of seriousness generally took the form of two variables, seriousness of the offense and prior record. Each was recorded in 91.30 and 95.70 percent of studies respectively. Weapon use information was recorded in approximately 40 percent of studies, and drug use was 113 rarely recorded, with only 6.5 percent of studies measuring drug use. The average number of seriousness measures was 2.00 per study. Overall, other court characteristics were also not common. Federal court study, court organization measures, and community measures were each recorded in approximately 6.5 percent of cases. Mode of conviction and attorney type measures were more common, and were reported in 60.90 and 32.60 percent respectively of all studies. 114 Table 5.10: Sentencing Sample Characteristics Variable General Study Characteristics Number of Court Systems Offense Specific Control Variables Interaction Effects Length Continuous Data Collection Size Mean S 1.83 32.60 95.70 28.30 37.89 87.00 49,382.72 3.97 Defendant Characteristics Defendant Race Measure Defendant Gender Measure Defendant Age Measure Defendant Ethnicity Measure Defendant Mentally Ill Measure Defendant Employment Measure 84.80 69.60 87.00 34.80 0.00 21.70 Judge Characteristics Judge Race Measure* Judge Gender Measure Experience 10.90 6.40 6.50 Seriousness Characteristics Seriousness Measure Weapon Use Measure Drug Use Measure Prior Record Measure Total Seriousness Measures 91.30 41.30 06.50 95.70 2.00 Other Court Characteristics Federal vs. Local Mode of Conviction Measure Attorney Type Measure Court Organization Measures Community Measures 6.40 60.90 32.60 6.50 6.50 N 32.65 136,070.28 0.87 46 *one study included information on ethnicity of the judge but it was included with race 115 Total 2,271,605 Effect sizes Table 5.11 presents the findings from the effect size analysis. Defendant race, gender and ethnicity are each significant. Specifically, black defendants were 1.23 times more likely to be incarcerated over white defendants, males were 1.63 times more likely to be incarcerated than females and Hispanics were 1.29 times more likely to be incarcerated than non-Hispanics. However, age and employment status were not significant predictors of incarceration. Additionally, like arrest, judge characteristics were not significantly related to the outcome of interest. It is noteworthy that the effect size for judge race approached significance, .073, and that black judges were more lenient than white judges. This finding is the only criminal justice actor variable that approaches significance. Prior record had a stronger overall effect than seriousness of the offense. Specifically, those with longer prior records were almost 2 times more likely to be incarcerated. Seriousness of the offense increased the odds of incarceration by approximately 1.5 times. Drug use also increased incarceration (1.45 times). With regard to other court characteristics, only mode of conviction was significant. Those that opted for a plea were 1.29 times less likely to be incarcerated. Attorney type and court size correlations were not significant but were in the expected directions. 116 Table 5.11: Sentencing Mean Effect Sizes W Mean ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max 0.17 0.21 (0.28) 0.11 (0.27) 0.30 (0.29) 2243915 0.48 0.489 (0.29) 0.334 (0.29) 0.645 (0.31) 39 2214935 -0.02 0.001 (-0.002) -0.004 (-0.002) 0.002 (-0.001) Suspect Ethnicity*** 16 1481682 0.22 0.25 (0.16) 0.16 (0.13) 0.34 (0.18) Suspect Employment 10 20694 0.45 0.45 (0.45 -0.17 (-0.17) 1.07 (1.07) Judge Characteristics Judge Race 7 268988 -0.17 -0.16 (-0.10) -0.33 (-0.15) 0.01 (-0.06) Judge Gender 5 252702 0.09 0.14 (0.002) -0.10 (0.00) 0.39 (0.004) Judge Experience 4 200920 0.00 -0.001 (0.01) -0.02 (0.01) 0.02 (0.01) Seriousness Characteristics Seriousness*** 24 1991280 0.58 0.39 (0.02) 0.34 (0.02) 0.43 (0.02) Without outliers*** 22 1438972 0.35 0.25 (0.02) 0.22 (0.02) 0.29 (0.02) Variable k N Mean ES Defendant Characteristics Suspect Black*** 39 2247871 Suspect Male*** 32 Suspect Age 117 Table 5.11 continued W Mean ES 0.67 (0.23) 95% CI Min 95% CI Max 0.54 (0.22) 0.81 (0.24) 0.61 (0.22) 0.48 (0.21) 0.74 (0.23) 0.55 0.37 (0.01) 0.04 (-0.001) 0.71 (0.01) 974747 -0.82 -0.83 (-0.69) -2.05 (-0.74) 0.40 (-0.65) 326882 -0.04 -0.02 (-0.03) -0.19 (-0.08) 0.15 (0.01) Mode of Conviction* 28 1292317 -0.08 -0.26 (-0.02) -0.46 (-0.04) -0.06 (0.01) Without outliers* 26 -0.11 -0.20 0.004 -0.38 (-0.02) -0.02 (0.03) Variable k N Prior Record*** 44 2214373 0.73 Without outliers*** 42 2193655 0.66 Drug Use* 4 17164 Other Court/Case Characteristics Court Size 5 Counsel Type 15 905324 Mean ES (Fixed effects results in parentheses) * sig p< .05 ** sig p<.01 Fail-safe N analysis Consistent with the fail-safe N results for the arrest analysis, the fail-safe N values for sentencing are generally stable. Fail-safe N values for suspect race and gender were 47 and 70 respectively. Ethnicity was the least stable finding in the suspect characteristics domain with 29 additional studies needed to nullify the ethnicity effect size. Seriousness of the offense and prior record were both very stable findings. Their fail-safe N values were 129 and 160. The effect 118 size for drug use produced the smallest fail-safe N value of any decision point; the value is less than 1. Clearly, the results for drug use should be interpreted with caution. Finally, the fail-safe N value for the mode of conviction effect size was 5. Like arrest, the decision-specific variables produced the smallest fail-safe N values. Overall, these findings, like arrest, are stable and can be viewed with confidence, except where noted. The mean fail-safe N value was 90 additional effect sizes. Moderating variable analysis A series of moderating variable analyses were also performed for the sentencing decision. Like the arrest analyses, these analyses were limited to a handful of variables that had a sufficient number of effect sizes for meaningful analysis. The results for each analysis are displayed in Tables 5.12-5.17. Table 5.12 displays the results of the moderating variable analysis for defendant race. There is a similar pattern for the moderator analysis of sentencing outcomes as the arrest outcome. For defendant race, the majority of mean effect sizes were significant across moderator categories and the analysis follows the same pattern that African Americans were more severely punished. However, there are several other noteworthy findings. Race was not a significant predictor of sentencing for studies that used multiple study locations. These multiple location studies were often studies of sentencing at the federal level. Race was also not significantly related to sentencing for studies that used data gathered in the 1970s or the 1980s. Race was also not related to sentencing in offense specific studies or studies that did not use continuous data. Race was not significantly related to sentencing in studies that measured suspect employment or counsel type. 119 Table 5.13 displays the results of the moderating variable analysis for defendant gender. Results are similar to previous moderator analyses. The majority of mean effect sizes are significant across moderator categories. Those that are not significant are generally the result of a small number of effect sizes used to generate the mean effect size. Males were not significantly more likely to be sentenced to prison in studies conducted in the Northeast, South, Pacific, or those using multiple locations or the federal level. Gender was not a significant predictor of sentencing in studies based on data gathered in the 1970s or the 1980s. Like the suspect race analysis, gender was not a significant predictor of sentencing in studies that measured suspect employment. Table 5.14 displays the results for defendant ethnicity moderator analysis. Unlike race and gender, the ethnicity analysis is based on a much smaller number of effect sizes. The majority of mean effect sizes were significant across moderator categories. Those that were not were generally limited to only a few effect sizes to create the mean effect sizes. However, unlike the previous two analyses, Hispanics were significantly more likely to be incarcerated only in studies that had multiple locations or Federal court. There was little variation in the other variable categories. Table 5.15 displays the results for the moderator analysis for prior record. Generally, patterns here were similar to previous moderator discussions. Prior record was not a significant predictor of sentencing for studies published since 2010 and studies conducted in the Southwest. It should be noted that these findings are based on a small number of effect sizes. All other mean effect sizes were significant across moderator categories. Those that were not were based on a negligible number of effect sizes. 120 The final two moderator analyses were conducted on attorney type and mode of conviction. Table 5.16 displays the results for attorney type and Table 5.17 displays the results for mode of conviction. Mean effect sizes for attorney type are generally not significant across moderator categories. However the one exception was in the data gathered moderator. The results show that studies conducted in the 1960s were significantly more likely to find that those represented by private attorneys were more likely to be incarcerated. In the 1970s defendants were significantly more likely to be incarcerated when represented by public defenders or court appointed counsel. Mean effect sizes for mode of conviction were generally significant. Those that were not significant were often based on a small number of effect sizes. Studies published between 2000 and 2009, and studies based on data collected in the South, found that people that pled guilty were significantly less likely to be sentenced to prison. Significant mean effect sizes were found for studies based on data from the 1990s indicating those that took a plea were less likely to be incarcerated. Pleas also produced significantly less prison sentences in studies that were offense specific. 121 Table 5.12: Defendant Race Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k ES Discipline Criminology/Criminal Justice 21 0.20 Psychology 0 0.00 Sociology 11 0.19 Social Work 0 0.00 Mixed 2 0.34 Other 1 0.34 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 0.12 0.00 0.08 0.00 -0.04 -0.01 0.28 0.00 0.30 0.00 0.54 0.68 0.04 0.00 0.06 0.00 0.15 0.17 Publication Decade 1980 1990 2000 2010 6 8 21 3 0.28 0.18 0.16 0.44 0.07 0.03 0.08 0.25 0.48 0.33 0.25 0.64 0.10 0.08 0.04 0.10 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 4 7 16 3 1 8 0.36 0.19 0.17 0.34 0.48 0.11 0.20 0.06 0.06 0.17 0.17 -0.01 0.52 0.32 0.29 0.51 0.79 0.23 0.07 0.07 0.06 0.09 0.16 0.06 Data Gathered 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2 10 3 18 6 0.54 0.12 0.75 0.20 0.31 0.08 -0.09 -0.24 0.07 0.11 0.99 0.34 0.39 0.32 0.51 0.23 0.11 0.16 0.06 0.10 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or Local Report Other 30 7 1 1 0.20 0.22 0.17 0.35 0.10 0.03 -0.28 -0.10 0.30 0.41 0.62 0.80 0.05 0.10 0.23 0.23 122 Table 5.12: continued Moderator Variable Agency Number 1 17 23 k 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 37 0.21 1 0.25 1 0.18 0.11 -0.27 -0.35 0.31 0.77 0.71 0.05 0.27 0.27 Offense Specific No Yes 30 0.21 9 0.21 0.10 -0.03 0.31 0.46 0.05 0.13 Control Variables No Yes 2 0.26 36 0.21 -0.31 0.12 0.66 0.31 0.20 0.05 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 6 0.10 33 0.23 -0.14 0.12 0.35 0.33 0.13 0.05 Defendant Gender Recorded No Yes 8 0.30 31 0.20 0.02 0.10 0.57 0.30 0.14 0.05 Defendant Age Recorded No Yes 7 0.28 32 0.19 0.05 0.09 0.50 0.30 0.11 0.05 Defendant Ethnicity Recorded No Yes 25 0.17 14 0.26 0.07 0.14 0.28 0.39 0.05 0.06 Defendant Employment Status Recorded No Yes 32 0.23 7 0.06 0.13 -0.21 0.33 0.34 0.05 0.14 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 4 0.37 35 0.19 0.06 0.08 0.68 0.29 0.16 0.05 123 ES Table 5.12: continued Moderator Variable Prior Record Recorded No Yes k 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 2 0.35 37 0.20 -0.03 0.10 0.73 0.30 0.19 0.05 Mode of Conviction Recorded No Yes 20 0.22 18 0.18 0.07 0.04 0.36 0.32 0.07 0.07 Counsel Type Recorded No Yes 31 0.21 7 0.17 0.11 -0.03 0.32 0.37 0.05 0.10 37 0.23 2 0.13 0.13 0.33 0.05 -0.51 0.24 0.19 0.11 -0.11 0.31 0.50 0.05 0.15 Organization Recorded No Yes Community Recorded No Yes ES 36 0.21 3 0.19 124 Table 5.13: Defendant Gender Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k Discipline Criminology/Criminal Justice 20 Psychology 0 Sociology 8 Social Work 0 Mixed 2 Other 1 0.51 0.00 0.45 0.00 0.18 0.74 0.30 0.00 0.14 0.00 0.37 -0.19 0.72 0.00 0.76 0.00 -0.54 1.68 0.11 0.00 0.16 0.00 0.90 0.48 Publication Decade 1980 1990 2000 2010 1 6 21 3 0.13 0.46 0.49 0.66 -0.48 0.17 0.34 0.29 0.74 0.75 0.64 1.02 0.31 0.15 0.08 0.19 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 3 6 12 4 1 6 0.45 0.37 0.66 0.68 0.45 0.21 -0.07 -0.01 0.38 0.21 -0.43 -0.16 0.98 0.75 0.94 1.16 1.33 0.58 0.27 0.19 0.14 0.24 0.45 0.19 Data Gathered 1970 1980 1990 2000 4 4 18 6 0.30 0.46 0.52 0.52 -0.18 -0.02 0.30 0.16 0.79 0.95 0.73 0.88 0.25 0.25 0.11 0.18 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or Local Report Other 23 7 1 1 0.50 0.41 0.71 0.47 0.33 0.12 -0.02 -0.26 0.67 0.71 1.44 1.20 0.09 0.15 0.37 0.37 125 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.13: continued Moderator Variable Agency Number 1 17 23 30 0.49 1 0.53 1 0.51 0.32 -0.29 -0.32 0.65 1.36 1.34 0.08 0.42 0.42 Offense Specific No Yes 29 0.19 3 0.45 0.33 -0.10 0.66 0.99 0.08 0.28 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 6 0.43 26 0.50 0.07 0.33 0.79 0.68 0.19 0.09 Defendant Race Recorded No Yes 1 0.77 31 0.48 -0.29 0.33 0.18 0.64 0.54 0.08 Defendant Age Recorded No Yes 4 0.66 28 0.47 0.21 0.30 1.11 0.63 0.23 0.08 Defendant Ethnicity Recorded No Yes 18 0.48 14 0.50 0.26 0.27 0.70 0.73 0.11 0.12 Defendant Employment Status Recorded No Yes 27 0.50 5 0.43 0.33 -0.01 0.66 0.87 0.08 0.22 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 3 0.51 29 0.49 -0.01 0.33 1.03 0.65 0.27 0.08 Prior Record Recorded No Yes 2 0.19 30 0.51 -0.40 0.35 0.78 0.67 0.30 0.08 k 126 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.13: continued Moderator Variable Mode of Conviction Recorded No Yes 14 0.50 16 0.46 0.25 0.24 0.75 0.68 0.13 0.11 Organization Recorded No Yes 30 0.50 2 0.32 0.34 -0.25 0.67 0.90 0.08 0.30 Community Recorded No Yes 29 0.50 3 0.37 0.34 -0.11 0.67 0.85 0.09 0.25 k 127 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.14: Defendant Ethnicity Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k Discipline Criminology/Criminal Justice 11 Psychology 0 Sociology 4 Social Work 0 Mixed 0 Other 0 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 0.33 0.00 0.15 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.23 0.00 -0.01 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.43 0.00 0.30 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.05 0.00 0.08 0.00 0.00 0.00 Publication Decade 1990 2000 2010 2 -0.14 11 0.32 3 0.15 -0.47 0.21 -0.07 0.20 0.43 0.37 0.17 0.06 0.17 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 3 2 3 3 0 5 0.22 0.26 0.28 0.21 0.00 0.27 -0.02 -0.04 .-.00946 -0.03 0.00 0.09 0.46 0.56 0.57 0.44 0.00 0.46 0.12 0.15 0.15 0.12 0.00 0.09 Data Gathered 1980 1990 2000 2 11 3 0.19 0.27 0.19 -0.15 0.15 -0.04 0.53 0.39 0.42 0.17 0.06 0.12 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis 14 2 0.26 0.21 0.15 -0.03 0.37 0.46 0.06 0.13 Agency Number 1 17 23 14 1 1 0.24 0.27 0.30 0.14 -0.05 -0.04 0.35 0.60 0.64 0.05 0.17 0.17 128 Table 5.14: continued Moderator Variable Continuous Data Collection No Yes 2 0.29 14 0.24 0.06 0.14 0.52 0.34 0.12 0.05 Defendant Race Recorded No Yes 1 0.22 15 0.25 -0.21 0.16 0.64 0.53 0.22 0.05 Defendant Gender Recorded No Yes 1 0.09 15 0.26 -0.27 0.15 0.45 0.37 0.18 0.06 Defendant Age Recorded No Yes 2 0.24 14 0.25 -0.02 0.14 0.50 0.37 0.13 0.06 Defendant Employment Status Recorded No Yes 15 0.25 1 0.22 0.16 -0.21 0.35 63799.00 0.05 0.22 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 1 0.27 15 0.25 -0.06 0.15 0.60 0.34 0.17 0.05 Prior Record Recorded No Yes 2 0.32 14 0.24 0.05 0.15 0.60 0.34 0.14 0.05 Mode of Conviction Recorded No Yes 5 0.28 11 0.23 0.12 0.10 0.45 0.36 0.08 0.07 Counsel Type Recorded No Yes 13 0.23 3 0.41 0.13 0.16 0.32 0.65 0.05 0.13 k 129 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.14: continued Moderator Variable Organization Recorded No Yes 14 0.27 2 0.11 0.17 -0.16 0.36 0.38 0.05 0.14 Community Recorded No Yes 15 0.25 1 0.30 0.15 -0.03 0.34 0.63 0.05 0.17 k 130 ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.15: Prior Record Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable Discipline Criminology/Criminal Justice Psychology Sociology Social Work Mixed Other k ES 27 0 10 0 2 1 0.70 0.00 0.74 0.00 0.57 0.20 0.53 0.00 0.44 0.00 -0.17 -0.69 0.88 0.00 1.05 0.00 1.30 1.08 0.09 0.00 0.15 0.00 0.38 0.45 Publication Decade 1980 1990 2000 2010 5 9 26 3 0.72 0.46 0.75 0.48 0.24 0.18 0.59 -0.06 1.21 0.74 0.91 1.02 0.25 0.14 0.08 0.28 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 4 12 15 4 1 8 0.61 0.77 0.75 0.15 2.14 0.49 0.25 0.55 0.54 -0.26 1.43 0.22 0.97 0.99 0.96 0.56 2.85 0.77 0.18 0.11 0.11 0.21 0.36 0.14 Data Gathered 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2 9 4 24 5 0.57 0.84 0.30 0.72 0.73 -0.15 0.50 -0.11 0.55 0.32 1.28 1.17 0.72 0.90 1.14 0.37 0.17 0.21 0.09 0.21 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or Local Report Other 30 12 1 1 0.63 0.60 1.90 1.32 0.50 0.39 .1.258 0.68 0.76 0.81 2.53 1.96 0.07 0.11 0.32 0.33 131 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.15: continued Moderator Variable Agency Number 1 17 23 k ES 42 1 1 0.71 0.35 0.64 0.56 -0.53 -0.24 0.85 1.23 1.52 0.08 0.45 0.45 Offense Specific No Yes 29 15 0.66 0.76 0.50 0.51 0.82 1.02 0.08 0.13 Control Variables No Yes 2 41 0.55 0.69 -0.04 0.55 1.14 0.82 0.30 0.07 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 6 38 0.84 0.67 0.46 0.52 1.21 0.82 0.19 0.08 Defendant Race Recorded No Yes 7 37 0.54 0.72 0.22 0.58 0.86 0.87 0.16 0.07 Defendant Gender Recorded No Yes 14 30 0.77 0.66 0.51 0.51 1.02 0.82 0.13 0.08 Defendant Age Recorded No Yes 7 37 0.57 0.71 0.23 0.57 0.90 0.86 0.17 0.07 Defendant Ethnicity Recorded No Yes 30 14 0.82 0.44 0.66 0.22 0.98 0.66 0.08 0.11 Defendant Employment Status Recorded No Yes 36 8 0.69 0.68 0.55 0.37 0.83 1.00 0.07 0.16 132 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.15: continued Moderator Variable Seriousness Recorded No Yes k ES 4 40 0.54 0.71 0.07 0.56 1.01 0.85 0.24 0.07 Mode of Conviction Recorded No Yes 14 30 0.46 0.77 0.25 0.62 0.66 0.91 0.10 0.07 Counsel Type Recorded No Yes 30 14 0.65 0.74 0.49 0.49 0.81 1.00 0.08 0.13 Organization Recorded No Yes 42 2 0.79 0.30 0.57 -0.34 0.87 0.93 0.08 0.32 Community Recorded No Yes 42 2 0.67 0.98 0.54 0.41 0.81 1.54 0.07 0.29 133 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.16: Attorney Type Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable Discipline Criminology/Criminal Justice Psychology Sociology Social Work Mixed Other k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 7 0 3 0 5 0 0.10 0.00 -0.52 0.00 0.15 0.00 -0.22 0.00 -1.05 0.00 -0.44 0.00 0.42 0.00 0.01 0.00 0.73 0.00 0.16 0.00 0.27 0.00 0.30 0.00 Publication Decade 1980 1990 2000 5 2 8 0.05 -0.23 0.01 -0.36 -0.65 -0.23 0.45 0.20 0.25 0.21 0.22 0.12 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 1 5 6 1 0 2 0.48 0.15 -0.17 -1.18 0.00 -0.08 0.12 -0.08 -0.41 -1.86 0.00 -0.32 0.83 0.37 0.07 -0.50 0.00 0.16 0.18 0.11 0.12 0.34 0.00 0.12 Data Gathered 1960 1970 1980 1990 2 5 0 8 0.57 -0.33 0.00 0.02 0.09 -0.64 0.00 -0.20 1.06 -0.02 0.00 0.23 0.25 0.16 0.00 0.11 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis 14 -0.03 1 -0.08 -0.26 -0.79 0.20 0.63 0.12 0.36 Offense Specific No Yes 9 6 -0.25 -0.41 0.23 0.28 0.12 0.18 134 -0.01 -0.07 Table 5.16: continued Moderator Variable Control Variables No Yes k 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 1 0.48 14 -0.07 0.06 -0.22 0.89 0.08 0.21 0.08 Defendant Gender Recorded No Yes 6 9 0.13 -0.06 -0.17 -0.24 0.43 0.11 0.15 0.09 Defendant Age Recorded No Yes 4 0.19 11 -0.10 -0.07 -0.27 0.46 0.07 0.14 0.09 Defendant Ethnicity Recorded No Yes 10 0.11 5 -0.28 -0.14 -0.62 0.36 0.05 0.13 0.17 Defendant Employment Status Recorded No Yes 14 0.00 1 -0.53 -0.17 -1.36 0.18 0.30 0.09 0.42 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 3 -0.01 12 -0.04 -0.44 -0.27 0.42 0.19 0.22 0.12 Mode of Conviction Recorded No Yes 5 0.02 10 -0.04 -0.29 -0.25 0.34 0.17 0.16 0.11 Organization Recorded No Yes 14 -0.03 1 -0.08 -0.26 -0.79 0.20 0.63 0.12 0.36 135 ES Table 5.17: Mode of Conviction Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k ES Discipline Criminology/Criminal Justice 17 -0.49 Psychology 0 0.00 Sociology 7 0.22 Social Work 0 0.00 Mixed 1 0.31 Other 0 0.00 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE -0.80 0.00 -0.33 0.00 -1.50 0.00 -0.17 0.00 0.77 0.00 2.11 0.00 0.16 0.00 0.28 0.00 0.92 0.00 Publication Decade 1980 1990 2000 2010 4 7 15 2 0.20 -0.24 -0.32 -0.39 -0.43 -0.71 -0.58 -1.12 0.83 0.23 -0.06 0.33 0.32 0.24 0.13 0.37 Geographic Region Northeast South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 5 4 10 1 1 7 -0.07 -1.23 -0.28 0.11 -0.86 0.61 -0.69 -1.96 -0.77 -1.23 -2.26 -0.03 0.56 -0.51 0.22 1.50 0.54 1.26 0.32 0.37 0.25 0.71 0.71 0.33 Data Gathered 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 2 6 3 13 4 -0.13 0.07 -0.18 -0.44 0.12 -1.22 -0.66 -0.97 -0.85 -0.58 0.96 0.79 0.62 -0.03 0.83 0.56 0.37 0.41 0.21 0.36 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or Local Report 22 5 1 -0.19 -0.34 -0.04 -0.52 -0.99 -1.43 0.14 0.31 1.35 0.17 0.33 0.71 Agency Number 1 23 27 1 -0.24 -0.62 -0.45 -1.61 -0.04 0.37 0.11 0.51 136 Table 5.17: continued Moderator Variable Offense Specific No Yes k ES 21 7 -0.28 -0.17 -0.48 -0.67 -0.08 0.34 0.10 0.26 Control Variables No Yes 1 26 -0.44 -0.24 -1.37 -0.45 0.49 -0.03 0.48 0.11 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 3 25 -0.51 -0.23 -1.12 -0.44 0.10 -0.01 0.31 0.11 Defendant Race Recorded No Yes 1 27 0.11 -0.27 -0.84 -0.49 1.05 -0.06 0.48 0.11 Defendant Gender Recorded No Yes 8 20 0.16 -0.34 -0.39 -0.65 0.71 -0.03 0.28 0.16 Defendant Age Recorded No Yes 4 24 -0.14 -0.24 -0.80 -0.53 0.52 0.05 0.34 0.15 Defendant Ethnicity Recorded No Yes 15 13 -0.25 -0.18 -0.65 -0.53 0.08 0.17 0.19 0.18 Defendant Employment Status Recorded No Yes 25 3 -0.31 0.44 -0.53 -0.32 -0.10 1.20 0.11 0.39 Seriousness Recorded No Yes 1 27 0.31 -0.27 -1.30 -0.47 1.92 -0.06 0.82 0.10 137 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Table 5.17: continued Moderator Variable Prior Record Recorded No Yes k ES 2 26 0.30 -0.31 -0.40 -0.52 0.99 -0.10 0.36 0.11 Counsel Type Recorded No Yes 18 10 -0.21 -0.35 -0.46 -0.72 0.04 0.02 0.13 0.19 Organization Recorded No Yes 24 4 -0.29 -0.05 -0.52 -0.59 -0.07 0.48 0.11 0.27 Community Recorded No Yes 27 1 -0.24 -0.62 -0.45 -1.61 -0.04 0.37 0.11 0.51 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE Parole Revocation Sample characteristics Table 5.18 describes the sample characteristics for the parole revocation study analysis. As shown in Table 5.18, the average sample size was 8,979.16 cases and across all 19 studies, 170,604 cases were studied. The mean number of parole revocation systems involved in each study was 1.11 agencies. The mean length of study period was 15.33 months. Approximately 15 percent of studies reported at least one analysis with interaction effects, and nearly all studies used continuous data collection (88.90%). Approximately six percent of studies collected information on offense specific cases (5.60%). Table 5.18 also describes what variables were collected in various studies. These variables are separated into 2 groups: offender characteristics, and seriousness characteristics. For suspects, the majority of studies collected information on race, gender, and employment status of offenders, 84.20, 52.60, and 52.60 percent respectively. About 37 percent of studies 138 collected information on offender age. One quarter of studies recorded information on ethnicity of offenders and 21 percent of studies recorded information on offender education. No studies included in this analysis recorded measures of parole officer characteristics. The most common seriousness measure was a risk/prior record measure. Eighty percent of studies recorded a measure of risk (78.90%). Slightly more than half of studies recorded information on the seriousness of the offense and roughly one quarter recorded information on a need measure, 52.60 and 26.30 percent respectively. Very few studies (5.30%) recorded information on weapon use. The average number of seriousness measures was 1.26 per study. 139 Table 5.18: Parole Revocation Sample Characteristics Variable Mean/% General Study Characteristics Number of PR Systems 1.11 Offense Specific 5.60 Control Variables 63.20 Interaction Effects 15.80 Length 15.33 Continuous Data Collection 88.90 Size 8,979.16 Suspect Characteristics Suspect Race Measure Suspect Gender Measure Suspect Age Measure Suspect Ethnicity Measure Suspect Education Measure Suspect Employment Measure 84.20 52.60 36.80 26.00 21.10 52.60 Seriousness Characteristics Seriousness Measure Evidence Measure Weapon Use Measure Risk/Prior Record Measure Need Measure Total Serious Measures 52.60 0.00 05.30 78.90 26.30 1.26 N 19 140 S Total 0.74 7.554 20,856.20 0.45 170,604 Effect sizes Table 5.19 presents the effect sizes for parole revocation. As shown in table 5.19, only two variables are significantly related to parole revocation, seriousness and risk. Seriousness increased the odds of revocation 1.18 times and those parolees with greater risk were 1.15 times more likely to be revoked. No other measures were significantly related to parole revocation. It is noteworthy that the effect size for offender race approached significance (p<0.051). This finding that race was nearly significant is consistent with findings from the other two decisions that indicated that race was a significant predictor of decision-making. Further, this race finding may be an artifact of the small number of studies assessed. Table 5.19: Mean Effect Sizes Parole Revocation N Mean ES W Mean ES 95% CI 95% CI Min Max Variable k Offender Characteristics Offender Black 16 157177 0.76 0.22 (0.19) 0.00 (0.13) 0.44 (0.25) Without outliers 15 126541 0.15 0.15 (0.19) 0.00 (0.13) 0.31 (0.25) Offender Male 10 149928 -0.39 -0.04 (0.11) -0.36 (0.06) 0.29 (0.16) Offender Age 7 21385 -0.16 -0.02 (-0.02) -0.05 (-0.03) 0.01 (-0.01) Offender Ethnicity 5 115738 -0.01 0.04 (0.13) -0.13 (0.09) 0.21 (0.17) Offender Employment 10 19689 -0.29 -0.29 (-0.24) -0.75 (-0.38) 0.17 (-0.11) 141 Table 5.19 Continued Variable k N Mean ES Offender Education 6 45652 -0.49 W Mean ES -0.28 (-0.18) 95% CI 95% CI Min Max -0.83 (-0.37) 0.26 (0.01) Seriousness Characteristics Seriousness** 10 154527 -0.12 0.17 (0.20) 0.05 (0.16) 0.28 (0.25) Risk/Prior Record*** 16 167528 0.63 0.14 (0.05) 0.09 (0.05) 0.20 (0.05) Without Outliers*** 15 136892 0.38 0.13 (0.05) 0.08 (0.05) 0.19 (0.05) Need** 4 1095 0.14 0.47 (0.49) 0.16 (0.25) 0.79 (0.73) (Fixed effects results in parentheses) * sig p< .05 ** sig p<.01 *** sig p< .001 Fail-safe N analysis The results of the parole revocation fail-safe N analysis indicate that the results were less stable than results of the other decisions. This is likely due to the smaller k values. Risk had the largest fail-safe N value; 25 additional studies would be necessary to nullify the risk mean effect size. There was no substantive difference between the risk fail-safe N analysis with or without outliers. Seriousness and need had similar fail-safe N values, 4 and 2 respectively. The results regarding these two variables should be used with caution given the small fail-safe N value. The value for risk appears to be stable but the values for seriousness and need cannot be considered stable. The mean fail-safe N value was 14 additional studies. 142 Moderating variable analysis The moderator analysis for parole revocation is limited to offender race and risk effect sizes. The limited analysis is due to the small number of studies in parole revocation generally and offender race and risk were the only two variables that had a sufficient number of effect sizes for meaningful moderator analysis. As with previous moderator variable analyses discussed above, the general trends of significance mirror those at the mean effect size level. Thus, across moderator categories, mean effect sizes for offender race are generally not significant. However, there were some exceptions. The full results are found in Table 5.20. Studies conducted by those working in either “sociology” or “social work” categories produced a significant mean effect size. Those two groups were found that black individuals were more likely to be revocated. Significant mean effect sizes were also found in studies that were conducted in the South. Black individuals in the South were more likely to be revocated. Studies that had a continuous data collection generated a significant mean effect size. Those studies that coded both race and a measure of risk had a significant mean effect size. Finally, studies that measured race as white/non-white produced a significant mean effect size. Like Pratt (1998), the effects of race on parole revocation may be masked by measurement issues, while those that measured race as white/black did not produce a significant mean effect size. Risk was also measured in a sufficient number of studies to perform further analysis. As with previous moderator analyses, the general pattern of significance follows the mean effect size significance. In other words, the majority of mean effect sizes across moderator categories were significant for the risk variable. Table 5.21 displays the full results for this analysis. Studies based on data gathered between 2000 and 2009 did not produce a significant mean effect size, but studies based on data collected in the 1980s and 1990s did produce a significant mean 143 effect size. All other categories of moderator variables produced significant effect sizes or were based on one to two effect size values. 144 Table 5.20: Offender Race Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable k ES Discipline Criminal Justice/Criminology 6 -0.15 Psychology 1 0.48 Sociology 2 1.34 Social Work 1 0.99 Mixed 6 0.11 Other 1 0.00 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE -0.56 -0.27 0.60 0.05 -0.22 -1.53 0.26 1.24 2.08 1.92 0.44 1.53 0.21 0.38 0.38 0.48 0.17 0.78 Publication Decade 1970 1980 1990 2000 2010 1 1 3 5 7 0.48 0.99 0.07 0.15 0.21 -0.38 -0.04 -0.48 -0.40 -0.16 1.35 2.01 0.62 0.69 0.58 0.44 0.52 0.28 0.28 0.19 Geographic Region Northwest South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 0 3 4 2 6 1 0.00 0.43 0.02 -0.66 0.10 9.91 0.00 0.12 -0.25 -1.40 -0.10 7.41 0.00 0.74 0.30 0.08 0.32 12.40 0.00 0.16 0.14 0.28 0.10 1.27 Data Gathered 1980 1990 2000 2 5 8 0.06 0.22 0.16 -0.62 -0.25 -0.22 0.74 0.68 0.53 0.35 0.24 0.19 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or local report 13 2 2 0.20 0.72 0.03 -0.05 -0.08 -0.64 0.45 1.52 0.70 0.13 0.41 0.34 Agency Number 1 4 15 1 0.12 9.91 -0.03 7.41 0.27 12.40 0.08 1.27 145 Table 5.20: continued Moderator Variable Control Variables No Yes k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 5 12 0.37 0.17 -0.07 -0.10 0.80 0.44 0.22 0.14 Interaction Effects No Yes 14 3 0.28 0.10 0.00 -0.40 0.57 0.59 0.15 0.25 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 2 14 -0.31 0.26 -0.84 0.03 0.23 0.50 0.27 0.12 Risk Recorded No Yes 4 13 -0.14 0.38 -0.59 0.10 0.30 0.67 0.23 0.14 Need Recorded No Yes 13 4 0.16 0.45 -0.09 -0.03 0.41 0.94 0.13 0.25 Race Measure White/Non-white White/Black 8 9 0.43 0.13 0.03 -0.15 0.83 0.40 0.20 0.14 Offender Gender Exclusively Male Exclusively Female Mainly Male 4 1 10 0.48 0.21 0.10 0.06 -0.68 -0.19 0.89 1.10 0.39 0.21 0.46 0.15 146 Table 5.21: Risk Moderator Analysis Moderator Variable Discipline Criminal Justice/Criminology Psychology Sociology Social Work Mixed Other k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 5 0 2 1 7 1 0.32 0.00 0.17 1.62 0.21 -0.38 0.05 0.00 -0.07 0.69 0.09 -1.75 0.60 0.00 0.41 2.28 0.33 0.99 0.14 0.00 0.12 0.34 0.06 0.70 Publication Decade 1980 1990 2000 2010 3 2 5 5 1.46 0.22 0.24 0.09 1.00 0.05 0.06 -0.02 6.27 2.56 2.61 1.64 0.23 0.09 0.09 0.05 Geographic Region Northwest South Midwest Southwest Pacific/Northwest Mixed 1 2 3 2 6 1 1.37 0.23 0.21 1.11 0.12 4.39 0.51 0.06 0.03 0.19 0.01 1.61 2.23 0.41 0.39 2.03 0.22 7.16 0.44 0.09 0.09 0.47 0.05 1.42 Data Gathered 1980 1990 2000 2 5 7 0.21 0.28 0.12 0.00 0.13 -0.01 0.42 0.44 0.25 0.11 0.08 0.07 Publication Type Journal Graduate Dissertation/Thesis State or local report Other 10 2 3 1 0.12 1.26 0.74 0.80 0.03 0.67 0.42 0.28 0.21 1.84 0.99 1.32 0.04 0.30 0.15 0.26 Agency Number 1 4 14 1 0.20 4.39 0.10 1.61 0.29 7.16 0.05 1.42 147 Table 5.21: continued Moderator Variable Control Variables No Yes k ES 95% CI Min 95% CI Max SE 6 10 0.84 0.14 0.59 0.04 1.09 0.23 0.13 0.05 Interaction Effects No Yes 13 3 0.37 0.07 0.24 -0.09 0.50 0.23 0.07 0.08 Continuous Data Collection No Yes 1 14 0.80 0.18 0.27 0.09 1.33 0.28 0.27 0.05 Need Recorded No Yes 11 5 0.21 0.45 0.10 0.21 0.32 0.69 0.06 0.12 Race Measure White/Non-white White/Black 7 8 0.49 0.17 0.27 0.06 0.71 0.28 0.11 0.06 Offender Race Mainly Black Mixed 2 12 0.62 0.15 0.32 0.06 0.93 0.23 0.16 0.05 Offender Gender Exclusively Male Exclusively Female Mainly Male 3 1 10 0.19 -1.42 0.20 0.05 -2.32 0.09 0.33 -0.52 0.32 0.07 0.46 0.06 148 Chapter Summary This chapter presented the results of the meta-analysis that was conducted as a part of this dissertation. Across decision points, the majority of studies were published since 1990 and were published in journals. The majority of studies used data that were gathered relatively recently. Finally, studies were conducted across the United States and were not limited to a single jurisdiction. There was less consistency with independent variables across studies. Race, gender, and measures of seriousness of the offense were measured often across decisions. Measures of other variables were more limited. Age was measured in a majority of studies of arrest and sentencing but not parole revocation. Suspect ethnicity has been a more recent addition to studies of decision-making and is measured in a few studies across decision points. Criminal justice actor characteristics were not measured in parole revocation studies, but were measured in a few of the studies arrest and sentencing. Community characteristics were not measured often at any decision point. The effect size analysis indicates that measures of seriousness are significant across decision points. The correlations between seriousness variables and the three dependent variables estimated some of the largest mean effect sizes across decision points. Suspect characteristics were also significant across decision points. Race was significantly related to arrest and sentencing, but approached significance in the parole revocation analysis. The findings indicated that black individuals were significantly more likely to be processed further through the system. Gender was also significant at two decision points, arrest and sentencing. Males were more likely to be arrested and incarcerated. Ethnicity was also a significant factor. Hispanic individuals were more likely to be arrested and sentenced to prison. 149 Each decision point also had significant decision-specific results. At arrest, significant mean effect sizes were found for all measures of suspect demeanor. At sentencing, the mode of conviction was significant. More specifically, those that chose to take a plea were significantly less likely to be sentenced to prison than those that went to trial. Finally, the analysis of parole revocation indicated that those with a greater level of need were more likely to be revoked. Several system-wide and decision-specific variables were not significant. Criminal justice actor characteristics and community characteristics were not significant across decision points. Suspect/defendant age was also not significant across decision points. The decisionspecific variable counsel type was not significant at sentencing. Finally, the moderator analysis does not appear to nullify the effect size findings. 150 CHAPTER 6 DISCUSSION This dissertation seeks to address two major limitations within the field of criminal justice: 1) the lack of extensive reviews regarding decision-making correlates throughout criminal justice system, and 2) the general perception that criminal justice is an atheoretical field. Reviews of decision-making correlates are limited in both scope and number; additionally, the majority of these reviews focus on specific decisions within the system or on a particular variable’s (such as race and gender) impact on an outcome within the system (see Chapter 2 for a detailed discussion). While these research endeavors have been valuable contributions to the field of criminal justice research, they are limited. The only comprehensive system-wide review, which was a massive and time-consuming undertaking, was conducted by Gottfredson and Gottfredson in the late 1980s and has yet to be sufficiently updated. System-wide reviews of decision-making correlates are valuable in that they help us understand what factors influence or structure discretion. The system-wide focus also presents a fuller picture because the entire system is examined, as opposed to examining each piece independently. A second limitation of criminal justice is the general perception that criminal justice is an atheoretical field. Marenin and Worrall (1998) argue that criminal justice “has not yet achieved theoretical integrity and coherence” and that the only general consensus within the field is that theories and concepts are drawn from a variety of other disciplines—such as sociology, biology, political science—and that these derived theories coalesce and contribute to the “consolidation of the new discipline” (p. 465). An atheoretical perception reduces legitimacy of criminal justice theory and suggests a lack of development as a field. The lack of legitimacy may also reduce interest in the topic amongst scholars, which also leads to the lack of development. 151 This dissertation seeks to address both of these limitations by recreating Gottfredson and Gottfredson’s work on a smaller scale. First, correlates of decision-making are examined at multiple decision points in an attempt to examine the “bigger picture” of criminal justice decision making. Second, the correlates of decision-making are considered in the context of system-wide versus decision-specific in order to determine if criminal justice theories should be focused on the criminal justice system as a whole or by looking at each decision point separately. The purpose of this dissertation has been to examine, in the ten plus years since Marenin and Worrall’s comments, whether the field of criminal justice has begun to create a theoretical foundation. This section of the dissertation—which is broken up into five different sections—will discuss these findings and comment on their relevance to these two limitations. First, there is a summary of the findings. Second, there is a discussion of how to organize criminal justice theory given the results. Third, there is a discussion of a theory that can be gleaned from the findings of this dissertation. Fourth, there suggestions for future research in criminal justice theory are presented. Finally, there is a brief discussion of the limitations of this study. Summary of Findings The overall findings suggest a lack of consistency in decision-making research across the system, but there was some degree of similarities with respect to certain variables, such as measures of seriousness and race. Measures of seriousness were significant across the system. These measures were generally the strongest predictors of criminal justice processing. These results indicated that those who committed more serious acts were more likely to be processed through that decision point. Seriousness measures increased the likelihood of arrest between 1.53 and 6.91 times. The weakest seriousness relationship was between weapon use and arrest. 152 The results indicated that those that used a weapon were 1.53 times more likely to be arrested. The strongest relationship was between evidence and arrest. Cases with more evidence were nearly 7 times more likely to end in arrest. For sentencing, the increase in likelihood of sentencing was between 1.45 and 1.96 times across seriousness measures.18 The weakest relationship was between drug use and sentencing, which indicated that suspects that used illicit substances were 1.45 times more likely to be incarcerated. The findings indicated that prior record was the strongest predictor of incarceration; those with longer prior records were nearly 2 times more likely to be incarcerated. Results regarding seriousness measures for parole revocation were similar to the other two decision points. Risk, which is essentially prior record, was the weakest measure of seriousness. The results indicated that those with greater risk were 1.15 times to be revoked. The strongest measure of seriousness was need. Those with greater need were 1.60 times more likely to be revoked. These findings support the idea that criminal justice decision making is largely a function of seriousness of the offense. Furthermore, the idea of two criminal justice systems is supported by the finding from arrest analysis, which dichotomizes seriousness of offense as felony versus misdemeanor. This indicates that those suspected of felony crimes are significantly more likely (2.50 times) to be arrested. Additionally, measures of seriousness were generally the strongest predictors across decision points and the direction of the relationship was positive. This information indicates that it may be the case that the duality of criminal justice idea discussed by the Gottfredsons may be correct. However, the data presented here cannot provide a definitive answer. 18 The reduced effect of seriousness measures for sentencing may have been due to drug charging as a result of the “War on Drugs”. Mandatory drug penalties may have caused some defendants to be sentenced to prison when they would otherwise would not have been sentenced as harshly 153 Race appears to be a significant correlate of criminal justice decision making at multiple stages of the criminal justice system. Black individuals are significantly more likely to be processed than white individuals, 1.39 times more likely to be arrested and 1.23 times more likely to be sentenced to prison. Offender race was nearly significant at parole revocation and would likely have been significant with the addition of a small number of studies. This finding may be due to the small number of studies on parole revocation. Additionally, the moderator analysis indicates that African Americans are more likely to be revoked in the South (1.54 times more likely). This evidence indicates that race appears to be an important predictor of criminal justice decision making. Gender and ethnicity were significant at two decision points. Results indicated that males were more likely to be arrested or sentenced than females, 1.49 and 1.63 times respectively, and Hispanics were more likely to be arrested or sentenced than nonHispanics, 1.25 and 1.29 times respectively. Another important aspect of the finding that black individuals and Hispanics are more likely to be processed through the system is that it is not limited temporally. This means, potentially, disparate treatment or discriminatory treatment is still on going. Several multi-decision relationships were not significant at any decision point. Age was not significant at any decision point. It could be that the effect of age was absorbed by measures of race, gender, and ethnicity. Measures of criminal justice agent characteristics, race and gender, were also not significant.19 There are many possible explanations for this finding. First, the lack of influence for criminal justice actor characteristics could be an artifact of the small number of studies that actually measured these concepts, but the results of available studies 19 Criminal justice actor ethnicity was only measured in a few studies in either arrest or sentencing and was not included in parole revocation studies. Therefore, criminal justice actor ethnicity was included in with race for metaanalytic purposes. Hispanic was treated as black or non-white and non-Hispanic was treated as white. In the event that a study measured both officer race and officer ethnicity, officer ethnicity was dropped in favor of the officer race measure. 154 indicated that agent characteristics were not significantly related to the processing decisions. A second and related issue is that some studies examined actor race, gender or ethnicity in a way that was not conducive to meta-analysis. For example, Brown and Frank (2006) examined the effect of officer race on arrest but did so by creating a multivariate model for white, and a second model for black officers. The model containing arrests made by both black and white officers did not include officer race. Thus, their finding that black officers were more likely to arrest black suspects could not be included in this meta-analysis; there was no data available to generate an effect size for the relationship between officer race and arrest. Third, the lack of significance for criminal justice actor predictors is limited evidence of discrimination in the system. The finding that race, gender, and ethnicity of suspects/defendants is significant, but those same variables are not significant for criminal justice actors, could indicate that potential discrimination is structural and not based on individual actors. In other words, discrimination is potentially institutionalized and not just limited to a few “bad apples”. The training process may be very thorough and could cause any individual differences to be reduced or eliminated. Instead the individual criminal justice agent functions as a part of the workgroup for that particular branch of the criminal justice system (Klinger, 1996). A final interpretation of the significance of suspect demographic characteristics but not criminal justice agent demographics is that suspect race, gender and ethnicity may be proxies for other theoretically relevant but unmeasured variables. None of the studies that measured criminal justice agent factors also measured factors pertaining to social class or education, and only a handful of studies measured employment status. However, these variables have been theorized to be explanations of criminal justice actor behavior. For example, Black (1976) argues that social status, education (a measure of culture), and employment status (a measure of 155 organization) are critical factors to explaining the behavior of governmental social control. These other demographic characteristics may be correlated with race, gender, and ethnicity and act as proxy measures. Both sentencing and arrest also had unique, decision-specific measures that were significant. Both measures of demeanor were significant. Specifically, suspects that were disrespectful or non-compliant were significantly more likely to be arrested. The third demeanor related variable—interaction-phase crime—was also significant. Based on the findings presented in this study, it appears that Klinger may have been correct: interaction-phase crime does significantly increase the odds of arrest. The mean effect size of interaction-phase crime was almost twice as strong as the mean effect size of demeanor. However, the moderator analysis showed that regardless of whether a study measured interaction-phase crime, demeanor was still significant. Suspect mental illness was not significantly related to arrest, but this could be an artifact of the small number of studies that actually measured suspect mental state. The direction of the effect size indicated that officers were less likely to arrest mentally ill suspects. Sentencing also produced a unique finding. Mode of conviction was significantly related to sentencing. Those that pled guilty were 1.29 times less likely to go to prison. However, suspect employment, court size, and the type of attorney were not significantly related to sentencing. Finally, moderator analysis for all three decision points did not really create any significant modifications to the general mean effect size findings. Missing variables Another finding, when looking at the bigger picture of the results, is that there is a clear lack of consistency in decision-making research. The results generally indicate that seriousness of the offense and prior record are important at all stages examined, and race, gender, and 156 ethnicity are important at multiple stages. However, when it comes to other correlates, there is little consistency across research. Potentially relevant variables are simply not measured. This finding has implications for the “atheoretical” perception of criminal justice research. Without a clear theory or theories to provide guidance on not only what to measure, but how to measure concepts, this lack of consistency will continue, which will cause the “atheoretical” perception of criminal justice research to thrive. Criminal Justice Theory Organization Given the above results, and the general lack of effects from moderators, it appears that correlates are generally consistent across the system. Some measure of seriousness was significant at all three stages of the criminal justice system. Additionally, several variables were significant predictors (race, gender, and ethnicity) of at least two decision points. These variables also performed in the same directions at all stages examined. The effects of factors that are unique to a particular decision were not as strong as those that are generally applicable. Factors that were unique to a decision may have only been unique because they were not measured at other stages of the criminal justice system. Arrest specific factors, demeanor and mentally ill status, are not exclusive to arrest. Defendants could potentially have a less than cooperative disposition and judge may enforce a harsher punishment. Interaction-phase crime is really a measure of seriousness and the strong effect is more applicable in the context of a measure of seriousness than as a unique demeanor measure. The effects of unique sentencing factors are virtually non-existent and the one decisionspecific relationship that produced a significant effect size, mode of conviction, was weaker than any effect size in suspect characteristics, or seriousness measures. While the effects of need 157 were a strong unique factor in parole revocation, it was also a measure of seriousness. This means that need functions in a similar manner as interaction-phase crime did at arrest. Given this information, it seems most appropriate to approach criminal justice theory from a whole-system perspective versus a decision-specific approach. Given the leaning toward the whole-system approach for theory organization, the idea that criminal justice theory has one dependent variable, discretionary processing, is also supported. However, due to limited data on some unique factors, it is not prudent to dismiss them entirely, and it is probably the case that the majority of the decision making of criminal justice agents is based largely on these whole-system variables with some influence by decision-specific characteristics. The findings here do not appear to be consistent with Bernard and Engel’s conceptualization of criminal justice theory organization. Bernard and Engel’s (2001) attempt was useful and an important contribution to this area of study, but their organizational approach may miss higher order, system-wide influences on processing. A new organizational method based on primary predictor variables would prevent theories with similar theoretical explanations from being separated simply because they operationalize a dependent variable differently. For example, Klinger’s ecological theory of policing focuses on police workgroups and the influence of those workgroup norms on police decisions. Eisenstein and Jacob’s workplace theory of court processing argues that the courtroom workgroup is a critical component to sentencing. If the focus is on the dependent variable, then these two theoretically similar theories would not be grouped together. Focusing on primary correlates as an organizational approach would also bring criminal justice in line with other social science disciplines, such as criminology and sociology, and increase the scientific legitimacy of criminal justice as a discipline. 158 The theoretical categories, discussed in chapter two, represent the new organizational model proposed in this study. Theories should be grouped into the following categories: situational theories, actor theories, workgroup/organizational theories, community theories, and conflict theories. This newly proposed model breaks each type of theory into their key predictive components. Additionally, while actor characteristics, organizational, and community characteristics were not supported in the effect size analysis, they should not be omitted. These categories of predictors have not been sufficiently studied to warrant their removal as theory categories. Toward a General Theory of Criminal Justice A general explanation for the behavior of criminal justice actors would have to include indicators of seriousness and suspect characteristics—race, ethnicity, and gender. There would also seem to be some effect of unique characteristics that function as part of the general explanation. Unfortunately, the literature examined here did not provide consistent measures of community influences or organizational factors. The handful of studies that actually recorded this information did not measure with any degree of consistency. Court size, the one organizational factor that was measured somewhat frequently was not significant. However, it would be inappropriate to dismiss both organizational and community explanations and the theories based on this level of explanation such as Wilson’s theory of police behavior, and Duffee’s community theory of criminal justice, based on this one finding. Nevertheless, it appears that a theory of criminal justice should first focus on the entire criminal justice system. Second, theories of criminal justice should include measures of seriousness and suspect characteristics. 159 Future Research First, there is a general lack of research on certain variables; specifically, there is a lack of research on the effect of organizational and community factors on decision-making behavior. The examination of characteristics of criminal justice actors is very limited. Eleven studies of arrest and seven studies of sentencing examined here also measured the effects of criminal justice actors on decision making. Suspect employment was only examined in ten sentencing studies and ten parole revocation studies; this is a relatively small portion when compared to other factors like race and gender. A related issue is that other potentially relevant measures, like education and social class, were virtually unmeasured. Some studies examined a quasi-social class characteristic; however, any examination was based more on where the arrest or sentencing took place and not an individual’s social class. Future research may want to improve upon the current state of decision-making research by striving to incorporate the effect of these relevant but unmeasured variables. This would aide in potentially disentangling the effects of race, gender, and ethnicity, and how they may be serving as a proxy measure of other variables like education and social class. Second, it appears that prior record/risk is a key factor in criminal justice decision making. However, prior record/risk has not been measured at the arrest stage. If possible, it could be useful to incorporate some measure of prior record into examinations of the arrest decision. This concept could be incorporated into systematic social observation studies by asking officers if they have had repeated contact with the suspect or by checking arrest records of suspects to obtain the identity of the arresting officer. Another potential measure could be the extent of the record check when an officer performs a record check during a stop. Finally, there 160 is a very limited knowledge of ethnicity relative to other suspect characteristics. While the results are significant, perhaps as time passes and more studies measuring ethnicity are accumulated the results may change. Third, while specific variables are limited, there is a general lack of studies on parole revocation. This important final decision has very little research relative to arrest and sentencing. Given that this decision potentially pulls someone back into prison, it would be important to study the factors that influence this decision. However, based on the small number of studies analyzed here, it would appear that parole revocation is the most neutral and objective decision. Research in this area can be improved by creating a more uniform measure of revocation and consistent operational definitions of concepts. Parole revocation as a field of study was the least developed of any of the decisions examined. Finally, there is a need of greater research that uses a system-wide approach to decision making. No studies analyzed here actually took cases from arrest to parole revocation. It would be helpful, and a rich source of data, to be able to analyze cases from arrest to parole revocation to see if the same factors are influencing decision making. A limitation of the meta-analysis presented in this dissertation is that the cases examined in arrest are not necessarily the same cases examined in sentencing or parole revocation. Having data that actually examines processing from the beginning to the end of the system might be able to put an end to the wholesystem versus decision-specific approach debate. Limitations While every effort was made to minimize the limitations of this study, there were several limitations. First, it is likely that some studies may have been missed. Efforts were made to be as inclusive as possible, but some of the results should be used with caution. However, the 161 majority of significant mean effect sizes produced stable results. Second, this project was that only direct effects could be measured. It may be that some of the non-significant factors were functioning as part of an interaction effect or a non-direct effect. It may be the case that some effects were masked by unexamined interaction effects. Third, the results are limited by the quality of the studies included. However, this limitation was mitigated somewhat by the moderator analysis, which indicated a great deal of uniformity across studies. Finally, as mentioned above there were some predictors of decision-making that were simply insufficiently measured. This reduced the ability to explore all facets of the research question and truly develop a general theory of criminal justice. Finally, two large groups of factors, organizational and community, were simply not able to be examined. The third and fourth limitations can be corrected with future research. The solution to these limitations is for future research to strive to create more uniform measures and models. More complete models would correct the second limitation because it would improve the overall quality of the study by having more complete data. Further, standardized measures and models would provide effect size information that is more directly comparable and less likely to experience problems with heterogeneity. More compatible information would reduce the risk of heterogeneity in the study effect sizes. The fourth limitation can also be corrected with this proposed idea of a standardized model of decision making. By standardizing the measures and models currently omitted variables would be more likely to be included and therefore a more definitive decision could be made as to the effect of actor, organizational, and community characteristics. Variables that could be included in this standardized model would include system-wide variables: offender race, offender gender, offender ethnicity, seriousness of the offense, risk/prior record. Additionally, the model would also need to include significant 162 decision-specific variables, where applicable, such as demeanor, and mode of conviction. Finally, the standardized model should also include agent characteristics, race, gender, ethnicity, and experience, organizational characteristics, and community characteristics in order to fully determine if these factors are important to criminal justice processing. Standardized models may also help to conceptualize new criminal justice theories. Despite these limitations this dissertation has contributed to the field of decision-making research. This study examined decision making at multiple decision points, which has not been done since Gottfredson and Gottfredson’s study. Further, this dissertation improved upon the Gottfredson’s work by adding meta-analysis to the examination of correlates of decision-making. The results indicate that seriousness is the chief decision-making component of decision makers of the criminal justice system. Further, the results indicate, race, gender, and ethnicity are important factors in decision making. Black individuals, males, and Hispanics appear to be more likely to be processed through the system. Finally, this dissertation has also presented results that appear to indicate that criminal justice theory should focus on a system-wide approach versus creating decision-specific theories. Correlates that are significant predictors of decision-making are consistent across decision points. Independent variables that were significant and unique to a particular decision point were generally unique in application, not in theory. In other words, there were no theoretical reasons why, for example, a suspect/defendants demeanor would not impact a judge’s decision in the same way it appears to impact an officer’s exercise of discretion. Finally, moderator analyses do not seem to negate any of the major findings. 163 REFERENCES20 *Alpert, G.P., Becker, E., Gustafson, M.A., Meister, A.P., Smith, M.R., and Strombom, B.A. (2006). Pedestrian and Motor Vehicle Post-Stop Data Analysis. Los Angeles, CA: City of Los Angeles. *Arazan, C.L. (2007). Contextual Effects and Punishment Decisions: A Multilevel Analysis. Ph.D. Dissertation, College of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL. *Atherton, M.C. (2005). The Study of Hispanic Defendants in United States federal court: A Multi-Level Analysis of Racial Sentencing Outcomes. Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Sociology, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA. Berlin, J.A., and Colditz, G.A. (1990). “A meta-analysis of physical activity in the prevention of coronary heart disease”. American Journal of Epidemiology. 132: 612-628. Bernard, T. and Engel, R. (2001). “Conceptualizing criminal justice theory” Justice Quarterly. 18: 1-30. *Bickle, G.S. and Peterson, R.D. (1991). “The impact of gender-based family roles on criminal sentencing”. Social Problems. 38: 372-394. Black, D. (1976). The behavior of law. New York: Academic Press. Bonta, J., Law, M., & Hanson, K. (1998). “The prediction of criminal and violent recidivism among mentally disordered offenders: A meta-analysis”. Psychological Bulletin, 123: 123-142. *Bourassa, S.C. and Andreescu, V. (2009). “Decomposition of racial differences in sentencing: Application of an econometric technique to cocaine possession cases”. Journal of Ethnicity in Criminal Justice. 7: 186-204. Braithwaite, J. and Biles, D. (1980). “Empirical verification and Black’s ‘behavior of law’”. American Sociological Review. 45: 334-338. Brind, J., Chinchilli, V.M., Severs, W.B., and Summy-Long, J. (1996). “Induced abortion as an independent risk factor for breast cancer: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis”. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 50: 481-496. *Brown, M.A. (2003). Similarities and Differences in the Sentencing Decisions of Men and Women Judges in Cook County Circuit Court. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Nebraska, Omaha, NE. Brown, R. and Frank, J. (2006). “Race and officer decision making: Examining differences in arrest outcomes between black and white officers”. Justice Quarterly, 23: 96-126. 20 * indicates a reference that was used for the meta-analysis 164 *Brown, R.A., Novak, K.J., Frank, J. (2009). “Identifying variation in police officer behavior between juveniles and adults”. Journal of Criminal Justice. 37: 200-208. *Bushway, S.D. and Piehl, A.M. (2001). “Judging judicial discretion: Legal factors and racial discrimination in sentencing”. Law & Society Review. 35: 733-764. *Buzawa, E., Austin, T.L., and Buzawa, C.G. (1995). “Responding to crimes of violence against women: Gender differences versus organizational imperatives”. Crime & Delinquency. 41: 443466. Caplan, J. (2007). “What factors effect parole: A review of empirical research”. Federal Probation. 71: 16-19. *Cho, H. (2006). Effects of Arrest on Intimate Partner Violence Incidence and Revictimization: Logistic Regression and Regression Time Series Analysis of the National Crime Victimization Survey from 1987-2003. Ph.D. Dissertation, College of Social Work, Florida State University. Clay-Warner, J. and McMahon-Howard, J. (2009). “Rape reporting: ‘Classic rape’ and the behavior of law”. Violence and Victims. 24: 723-743. *Cohen, T. (2003). The New Penology: How Court Sentencing Practices Have Been Influenced by the Growing Emphasis on Actuarialism and Managerialism. Ph.D. Dissertation. Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey. Crank, J. (1990). “The influence of environmental and organizational factors on police style in urban and rural environments”. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 27: 166-189. Crank, J. and Bowman, B. (2008). “What is good criminal justice theory?”. Journal of Criminal Justice. 36: 563-572. *Crawford, C. (2000). “Race and pretextual stops: Noise enforcement in Midwest City”. Social Pathology. 6: 213-227. *Crow, M.S. and Bales, W. (2006). “Sentencing guidelines and focal concerns: The effect of sentencing policy as a practical constraint on sentencing decisions”. American Journal of Criminal Justice. 30: 285-304. Cullen, F. (2011). “Beyond adolescence-limited criminology: Choosing our future – The American Society of Criminology 2010 Sutherland address”. Criminology. 49: 287-330. Daly, K. and Bordt, R. (1995). “Sex effects and sentencing: An analysis of the statistical literature”. Justice Quarterly. 12: 141-175. Davis, K.C. (1969). Discretionary justice. Baton Rouge, LA: Louisiana State University 165 *Doran, D.E. (2007). Racial Profiling in Las Vegas: A Reexamination of Police Stop Data in Las Vegas. Master’s Thesis, Department of Criminal Justice, University of Nevada Las Vegas. Duffee, D. (1980). Explaining criminal justice: Community theory and criminal justice reform. Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn & Hain. Duffee, D. and Allan, E. (2007). “Criminal Justice, criminology and criminal justice theory”. In Duffee, D. and Maguire, E. (eds). Criminal justice theory: Explaining the nature and behavior of criminal justice. New York: Routledge. Duffee, D. and Maguire E. (2007). Criminal justice theory: Explaining the nature and behavior of criminal justice. New York: Routledge. Durlak, J. A., & Lipsey, M. W. (1991). “A practitioner’s guide to meta-analysis”. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19: 291-332. Eisenstein, J. and Jacob, H. (1977). Felony justice: An organizational analysis of criminal courts. Boston: Little, Brown. *Eitle, D. (2005). “The influence of mandatory arrest policies, police organizational characteristics, and situational variables on the probability of arrest in domestic violence cases”. Crime & Delinquency. 51: 573-597. *Eitle, D., Stolzenberg, L., and D’Alessio, S.L. (2005). “Police organizational factors, the racial composition of the police, and the probability of arrest”. Justice Quarterly. 22: 30-57. *Ekpunoboi, A.E. (1999). Judicial Decision-Making Under Michigan Sentencing Guidelines. Ph.D. Dissertation, School of Public Affairs, Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI. *Engel, R.S., and Calnon, J.M. (2004). “Examining the influence of drivers’ characteristics during traffic stops with police: Results from a national survey”. Justice Quarterly. 21: 49-90. *Engel, R.S., Calnon, J.M., Liu, L., and Johnson, R. (2004). Project on Police-Citizen Contacts: Year 1 Final Report. Harrisburg, PA: Pennsylvania State Police. *Engel, R.S., Calnon, J.M., Tillyer, R., Johnson, R., Liu, L., and Wang, X. (2005). Project on Police-Citizen Contacts: Year 2 Final Report. Harrisburg, PA: Pennsylvania State Police. *Engel, R.S., Chekauskas, J.C., and Smith, M.R. (2008). Traffic Stop Data Analysis Study: Year 2 Final Report. Phoenix, AZ: Department of Public Safety. *Engel, R.S., Cherkauskas, J.C., Smith, M.R., Lytle, D., and Moore, K. (2009). Traffic Stop Data Analysis Study: Year 3 Final Report. Phoenix, AZ: Department of Public Safety. *Engel, R.S. and Silver, E. (2001). “Policing mentally disordered suspects: A reexamination of the criminalization hypothesis”. Criminology. 39: 225-252. 166 *Engel, R.S., Sobol, J.J., and Worden, R.E. (2000). “Further exploration of the demeanor hypothesis: The interaction effects of suspects’ characteristics and demeanor on police behavior”. Justice Quarterly. 17: 235-258. *Engel, R.S., Tillyer, R., Cherkauskas, J.C., and Frank, J. (2007). Traffic Stop Data Analysis Study: Year 1 Final Report. Phoenix, AZ: Department of Public Safety. *Erez, E. and Tontodonato, P. (1989). “Patterns of reported parent-child abuse and police response”. Journal of Family Violence. 4: 143-159. *Everett, R.S. and Nienstedt, B.C. (1999). “Race, remorse, and sentence reduction: Is saying you’re sorry enough?” Justice Quarterly. 16: 99-122. Eysenck, H. J. (1978). “An exercise in mega-silliness”. American Psychologist. 33: 517. *Farrell, A., Ward, G., and Rousseau, D. (2009). “Race effects of representation among federal court workers: Does black workforce representation reduce sentencing disparities?”. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 623: 121-133 *Fearn, N. (2003). Community Context and Sentencing Decisions: A multilevel Analysis. Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, University of Missouri-St. Louis, St. Louis, MO. *Feder, L. (1996). “Police handling of domestic calls: The importance of offender’s presence in the arrest decision”. Journal of Criminal Justice. 24: 481-490. *Felson, R.B. and Ackerman, J. (2001). “Arrests for domestic and other assaults”. Criminology. 39: 655-676. Fleiss, J.L. (1993). "The statistical basis of meta-analysis". Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 2: 121-145. Fleiss, J.L. and Berlin, J.A. (2009). "Effect sizes for dichotomous data". In Cooper, H., Hedges, L. and Valentine, J. The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis (2nd Ed). New York: Russell Sage Foundation *Franklin, T.W. (in press). “Sentencing native Americans in US Federal courts: An examination of disparity”. Justice Quarterly 1-30. *Freeman, J.R. (1992). The Social Ecology of Police Discretion. Ph.D. Dissertation. Northwestern University, Evanston, IL. *Friedrich, R.J. (1977). The Impact of Organizational, Individual, and Situational Factors on Police Behavior. Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Political Science, University of Michigan. 167 *Fyfe, J.J., Klinger, D.A., and Flavin, J.M. (1997). “Differential police treatment of male-onfemale spousal violence”. Criminology. 35: 455-473. Gaugler, J.E., Duval, S., Anderson, K.A., and Kane, R.L. (2007). "Predicting nursing home admissions in the U.S: A meta-analysis". BMC Geriatrics. 7: 1-14. *Gibbs, J. (2003). Factors Affecting Police Officers’ Decisions to Arrest in Domestic Violence Situations: A Look at the Niagara Falls Police Department. Master’s Thesis, University of Niagra. Glass, G. V. (1976). "Primary, secondary, and meta-analysis". Educational Researcher, 5: 3-8. Geddes, J.R., and Lawrie, S.M. (1995). “Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: A metaanalysis”. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 167: 786-793. Gottfredson, M. and Gottfredson, D. (1989). Decision making in criminal justice: Toward the rational exercise of discretion. (2nd Ed.). New York: Plenum Press. Gottfredson, M. and Hindelang, M. (1979). “A study of the behavior of law”. American Sociological Review. 44: 3-18. Greenland, S. (1987). “Quantitative methods in the review of epidemiologic literature”. Epidemiological Reviews. 9: 1-30 Greenland, S. (1993). “A meta-analysis of coffee, myocardial infarction, and coronary death”. Epidemiology. 4: 366-374. *Griffin, T.W. (2002). The Impact of Presumptive Sentencing Guidelines on Disparity in Sentencing in Ohio. Ph.D. Dissertation, Division of Criminal Justice, University of Cincinnati. *Gumbhir, V.K. (2005). Racial Profiling in Eugene, Oregon: A Case Study in Race, Community, and Law Enforcement. Ph.D. Dissertation. Department of Sociology. University of Oregon. Haddock, C.K., Rindskopf, D., and Shadish, W.R. (1998). “Using odds ratios as effect sizes for meta-analysis of dichotomous data: A primer on methods and issues”. Psychological Methods. 3: 339-353. *Harrington, M.P. and Spohn, C. (2007). “Defining sentence type: Further evidence against use of the total incarceration variable”. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 44: 36-63. *Hadwiger, J. (1972). The Significance of Legal and Extra Legal Variables in Predicting Sentencing Outcomes Under Oklahoma’s Community Sentencing Act. Ph.D. Dissertation, Oklahoma State University. 168 Hagan, J. (1989). “Why is there so little criminal justice theory? Neglected macro- and microlevel links between organization and power.” Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 26: 116-135 Hanushek, E.A., and Jackson, J.E. (1977). Statistical methods for social scientists. Orlando, FL: Academic Press Inc. Hassell, K., Zhao, J., and Maguire, E. (2003). “Structural arrangements in large municipal police organizations: Revisiting Wilson’s theory of local political culture”. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management. 26: 231-250. *Hawkins, H.C. (2008). “Race and sentencing outcomes in Michigan”. Journal of Ethnicity in Criminal Justice. 3: 91-109. *Haynes, S.H., Ruback, R.B., and Cusick, G.R. Courtroom Workgroups and Sentencing: The effects of Similarity, Proximity, and Stability. Unpublished manuscript. Hedges, L.V., and Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. London: Academic Press Inc. *Heilburn, A.B. (1978). “Race, criminal violence, and length of parole: A new look at parole outcome”. British Journal of Criminology. 18: 53-61. *Ho, T. (2000). “Domestic violence in a southern city: The effects of a mandatory arrest policy on male-versus-female aggravated assault incidents”. American Journal of Criminal Justice. 25: 107-118. *Ho, T. (2003). “The influence of suspects gender in domestic violence arrests”. American Journal of Criminal Justice. 27: 183-195. Hubbard, D. and Pratt, T. (2002). “A meta-analysis of the predictors of delinquency among girls”. Journal of Offender Therapy. 34(3): 1-13. Hunt, J. (1997). How science takes stock: The story of meta-analysis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis. Newburk Park, CA: Sage Publications. *Jhi, K.Y., and Joo, H.J. (2009). “Predictors of recidivism across major age groups of parolees in Texas”. Justice Policy Journal. 6: 1-28. Johnson, B.D. (2003). “Racial and ethnic disparities in sentencing departures across modes of conviction”. Criminology. 41: 449-488. 169 Johnson, B.D. (2005). “Contextual disparities in guideline departures: Courtroom social contexts, guideline compliance and extralegal disparities in criminal sentencing”. Criminology. 43: 761-796. *Johnson, B.D. (2006). “The multilevel context of criminal sentencing: Integrating judge- and county-level influences”. Criminology. 44: 259-298. *Johnson, Y.W. (2010). Racial Disparity in Sentencing Outcomes: A Study of Sentencing Decisions Under Arizona’s Presumptive Sentencing Structure. Ph.D. Dissertation, Capella University, Minneapolis, MN. *Johnson, B.D. and Betsinger, S. (2009). “Punishing the ‘model minority’: Asian-American criminal sentencing outcomes in federal district courts”. Criminology. 47: 1045-1090. Jonson, C.L. (2010). The Impact of Imprisonment on Reoffending: A Meta-Analysis. PhD. Dissertation, Department of Criminal Justice, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH. *Jones, D.A. and Belknap, J. (1999). “Police responses to battering in a progressive pro-arrest jurisdiction”. Justice Quarterly. 16: 249-273. Kane, R. (2002). “The social ecology of police misconduct”. Criminology. 40: 867-896. Klinger, D. (1994). “Demeanor or crime? Why ‘hostile’ citizens are more likely to be arrested”. Criminology. 32: 475-493. *Kassenbaum, G., Davidson-Coronado, J., Perrone, P., and Allen, J. (2001). Parole Decision Making in Hawaii. Hawaii Correctional Industries. *Kassenbaum, G., Davidson-Coronado, J., Silverio, M. and Marker, N. (1999). Survival on Parole: A Study of Post-Prison Adjustment and the Risk of Returning to Prison in the State of Hawaii. Hawaii Correctional Industries. *Klinger, D. (1994). “Demeanor or crime? Why ‘hostile’ citizens are more likely to be arrested”. Criminology. 32: 475-493. *Klinger, D. (1996). “More on demeanor and arrest in Dade County”. Criminology. 34: 61-82. Klinger, D. (1997). “Negotiating order in patrol work: An ecological theory of police response to deviance”. Criminology. 35: 277-306. Kochel, T.R., Wilson, D.B., and Mastrofski, S.D. (2011). “Effect of suspect race on officers’ arrest decisions”. Criminology. 49: 473-512. *Koons-Witt, B.A. (2002). “The effects of gender on the decision to incarcerate before and after the introduction of sentencing guidelines”. Criminology. 40: 297-328. 170 *Kramer, J. and Steffensmeier, D. (1993). “Race and imprisonment decisions”. The Sociological Quarterly. 34: 357-376. Kramer, J.H. and Ulmer, J.T. (2002). “Downward departures for serious violent offenders: Local court ‘corrections’ to Pennsylvania’s sentencing guidelines”. Criminology. 40: 897-931. Kraska, P. (2006). “Criminal justice theory: Toward legitimacy and an infrastructure”. Justice Quarterly. 23:167-185. *Krauss, M.W. (1981). The Mentally Disabled Parolee: Predictors of Parole Outcome. Ph.D. Dissertation, The Florence Heller Graduate School for Advanced Studies in Social Welfare, Brandeis University, Waltham, MA. Kuhn, T. (1996). The structure of scientific revolutions. (3rd Ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kuo, S., Longmire, D., Cuvelier, S., and Chang, S. (2010). “Prosecutorial decision making in Taiwan: A partial test of Black’s behavior of law”. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology vol. 54: 1023-1046 Lasky-Su, J.A., Faraone, S.V., Glatt, S.J., and Tsuang, M.T. (2005). “Meta-analysis of the association between two polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter gene and affective disorders” American Journal of Medical Genetics. 133: 110-115. Langworthy, R. (1985). “Wilson’s theory of police behavior: A replication of the constraint theory”. Justice Quarterly. 2: 89-98. Latessa, E. and Smith, P. (2007). Corrections in the community (4th ed.) Cincinnati, OH: Anderson Publishing. *Leiber, M.J. and Blowers, A.N. (2003). “Race and misdemeanor sentencing”. Criminal Justice Policy Review. 14: 464-485. *Leinfelt, F.H. (2006). “Police officer discretion and race in an American Midwestern city: Predicting arrests, citations, warnings: A multivariate examination”. The Police Journal. 79: 327. *Liederbach, J. (2007). “Controlling suburban and small-town hoods: An examination of police encounters with juveniles”. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice. 5: 107-124. Liederbach, J. and Travis, L. (2008). “Wilson redux: Another look at varieties of police behavior”. Police Quarterly. 11: 447-467. Lipsey, M.W. and Wilson, D.B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc. 171 *Lin, J., Grattet, R., and Petersilia, J. (2010). "'Back-end sentencing' and reimprisonment: Individual, organizational, and community predictors of parole sanctioning decisions". Criminology. 48: 759-795. Logan, C. H., & Gaes, G. G. (1993). Meta-analysis and the rehabilitation of punishment. Justice Quarterly, 10, 245-263. Lӧsel, F. and Schmucker, M. (2005). “The effectiveness of treatment for sexual offenders: A comprehensive meta-analysis”. Journal of Experimental Criminology. 1: 117-146. *Lunden, R. (1987). Risk and Recidivism Among Massachusetts Parolees: An Update. Boston, MA: Massachusetts Parole Board, Research and Planning Unit. *Lundman, R.J. (1994). “Demeanor or crime? The Midwest city police-citizen encounters study”. Criminology. 32: 631-656. *Lundman, R.J. (1996). “Demeanor and arrest: Additional evidence from previously unpublished data”. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 33: 306-323. Lundman, R. J., & Kaufman, R. L. (2003). Driving while black: Effects of race, ethnicity, and gender on citizen self-reports of traffic stops and police actions. Criminology, 41: 601–626. Makrarios, M. and Pratt, T. (2012), “The effectiveness of policies and programs that attempt to reduce firearm violence: A meta-analysis”. Crime & Delinquency. 58: 222-244. Marenin, O., & Worrall, J. (1998). “Criminal justice: Portrait of a discipline in progress”. Journal of Criminal Justice, 26: 465–480. Mastrofski, S., Snipes, J., Parks, R. and Maxwell, C. (2000). “The helping hand of the law: Police control of citizen’s request”. Criminology. 38: 307-342 *Mastrofski, S.D., Worden, R.E., and Snipes, J.B. (1995). “Law enforcement in a time of community policing”. Criminology. 33: 539-563. *Mignon, S.I., and Holmes, W.M. (1995). “Police response to mandatory arrest laws”. Crime & Delinquency. 41: 430-442. *Miller, J.M., and Miller, H.V. (2011). “Considering the effectiveness of drug treatment behind bars: Findings from the South Carolina RSAT evaluation”. Justice Quarterly. 28: 70-86. Mitchell, O. (2005). “A meta-analysis of race and sentencing research: Explaining the inconsistencies”. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 21: 439-466. *Morris, P.W. (2009). Dual Arrest in Intimate Partner Violence Incidents: The Influence of Police Officer, Incident, and Organizational Characteristics. Ph.D. Dissertation, City University of New York, New York, NY. 172 *Myers, M.A. and Talarico, S.M. (1986). “Urban justice, rural injustice? Urbanization and its effects on sentencing”. Criminology. 24: 367-391. *Nobiling, T.L. (2005). A Tale of Two Cities: The Effects of Offender’s Employment Status on Sentence Severity in Chicago and Kansas City. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Nebraska, Omaha, NE. *Novak, K.J. and Engel, R.S. (2005). “Disentangling the influence of suspects’ demeanor and mental disorder on arrest”. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management. 28: 493-512. Novak, K., Frank, J., Smith, B., and Engel, R. (2002). “Revisiting the decision to arrest: comparing beat and community officers”. Crime & Delinquency. 48:70-98. National Research Council (2004). Fairness and effectiveness in policing: The evidence. Committee to Review Research on Police Policy and Practices. W. Skogan and K. Frydl, Eds. Committee on Law and Justice, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. National Research Council (2008). Parole, desistance from crime, and community integration. Committee on Community Supervision and Desistance from Crime. Committee on Law and Justice, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. *Ozgenturk, Ilyas (2009). An Analysis of the Characteristics Related to the Victims and Suspects of Domestic Violence. D.Ed. Dissertation, College of Education, Spalding University, Louisville, KY. Packer, H. (1968). The limits of the criminal sanction. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Paternoster, R. (1987). “The deterrent effect of the perceived certainty and severity of punishment: A review of the evidence and issues”. Justice Quarterly. 4: 173-217. *Patnoe, J.L. (1990). The Demographic and Ecological Distribution of Police Discretion in an Urban Area. Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Sociology, University of Arizona, Tuscon, AZ. Philips, S.W. and Sobol, J.J. (2011). “Police attitudes about the use of unnecessary force: An ecological explanation”. Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology. 26: 47-57. *Pogrebin, M.R., Poole, E.D., and Regoli, R. (1986). “Parole decision making in Colorado”. Journal of Criminal Justice. 14: 147-155. Pratt, T. C. (1998). “Race and sentencing: A meta-analysis of conflicting empirical research results” Journal of Criminal Justice. 26: 513-523. 173 Pratt, T. C. (2001). Assessing the relative effects of macro-level predictors of crime: A metaanalysis. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Criminal Justice, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH. Pratt, T. C. (2002). "Meta-analysis and its discontents". Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 35: 23-40. Pratt, T. (2010). “Meta-analysis in criminal justice and criminology: What is it, when it’s useful, and what to watch out for”. Journal of Criminal Justice Education. 21:152-168. Pratt, T., and Cullen, F. (2000). “The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: A meta-analysis”. Criminology. 38: 921-964. Pratt, T., and Cullen, F. (2005). “Assessing macro-level predictors and theories of crime: A meta-analysis”. In Tonry, M. (Ed.) Crime and justice: A review of research (Vol. 32 pp. 373450). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Pratt, T., Cullen, F., Blevins, K., Daigle, L., and Madensen, T. (2006). “The empirical status of deterrence theory: A meta-analysis”. In Cullen, F., Wright, J., and Blevins, K. (Eds.) Taking Stock: The Status of Criminological Theory: Advances in Criminological Theory (Vol. 15 pp. 367-396). New Brunswick, NJ: Transition. Pratt, T., Cullen, F., Blevins, K., Daigle, L., and Unnever, J.D. (2002). “The relationship between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder to crime and delinquency: a meta-analysis”. International Journal of Police Science and Management. 4: 344-360. Pratt, T.C., McGloin, J.M., and Fearn, N.E. (2006). “Maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy and criminal/deviant behavior: A meta-analysis”. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology. 50: 672-690. *Prozesky, L. (2009). Parole Bureaucracy: The Decision Making Process of Paroling Authorities. Ph.D. dissertation, Southern Illinois University Carbondale, Carbondale, IL. *Pruitt, C.R. and Wilson, J.Q. (1983). “A longitudinal study of the effect of race on sentencing”. Law & Society Review. 17: 613-636. Riksheim, E. and Chermak, S. (1993). “Causes of police behavior revisited”. Journal of Criminal Justice. 21: 353-382. *Robinson, A.L. and Chandek, M.S. (2000). “The domestic violence arrest decision: Examining demographic, attitudinal, and situational variables”. Crime & Delinquency. 46: 18-37. *Rodriquez, S.F., Curry, T.R., and Lee, G. (2006). “Gender differences in criminal sentencing: Do effects vary across violent, property, and drug offenses?”. Social Science Quarterly. 87: 318339. 174 Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Productions Inc. *Rydberg, J., and Terrill, W. (2010). “The effect of higher education on police behavior”. Police Quarterly. 13: 92-120. Sampson, R.J., Raudenbush, S.W., and Earls, F. (1997). “Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multileveled study of collective efficacy”. Science. 277: 918-924. Scheingold, S. (1984) The politics of law and order: Street crime and public policy. New York: Longman. *Schram, P.J., Koons-Witt, B.A., Williams, F.P., and McShane, M.D. (2006). “Supervision strategies and approaches for female parolees: Examining the link between unmet needs and parolee outcome”. Crime & Deliquency. 52: 450-471. *Schwaner, S. (1998). “Patterns of violent specialization: Predictors of recidivism for a cohort of parolees”. American Journal of Criminal Justice. 23: 1-17. Sealock, M.D. and Simpson, S.S. (1998). “Unraveling bias in arrest decisions: The role of juvenile offender type-scripts”. Justice Quarterly. 15: 427-457. Shaffer, D. K. (2006). Reconsidering drug court effectiveness: A meta-analysis. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Criminal Justice, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH. Sherman, L. (1980). “Causes of police behavior: The current state of quantitative research”. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 17: 69-100. Smith, D. (1984). “The organizational context of legal control”. Criminology. 22: 19-38. Smith, D., Visher, C., and Davidson, L. (1984). “Equity and discretionary justice: The influence of race on arrest decisions”. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology. 75: 234-249. *Smith, M.R., Makarios, M., and Alpert, G.P. (2006). “Differential suspicion: Theory specification and gender effects in the traffic stop context”. Justice Quarterly. 23: 271-295. Sobol, J.J. (2010). “Social ecology and police discretion: The influence of district crime, cynicism, and workload on the vigor of police response”. Journal of Criminal Justice. 38: 481488. *Spohn, C. (1990A). “Decision making in sexual assault cases: Do black and female judges make a difference?”. Women & Criminal Justice. 2: 83-105. *Spohn, C. (1990B). “The sentencing decisions of black and white judges: Expected and unexpected similarities”. Law & Society Review. 24: 1197-1216. 175 Spohn, C. (2000). “Thirty years of sentencing reform: the quest for a racially neutral sentencing process”. Criminal Justice: The National Institute of Justice Journal. 3: 427-501. *Spohn, C. and DeLone, M. (2000). “When does race matter? An analysis of the conditions under which race affects sentence severity”. Sociology of Crime, Law, and Deviance. 2: 3-37 *Spohn, C., Gruhl, J., and Welch, S. (1981-1982). “The effect of race on sentencing: A reexamination of an unsettled question”. Law & Society Review. 16: 71-88. *Spohn, C. and Holleran, D. (2000). “The imprisonment penalty paid by young, unemployed black and Hispanic male offenders”. Criminology, 38: 281-306. *Spohn, C. and Spears. J. (1997). “Gender and case processing”. Women & Criminal Justice. 8: 29-59. *Steen, S., Engen, R.L., and Gainey, R.R. (2005). “Images of danger and culpability: Racial stereotyping, case processing, and criminal sentencing”. Criminology. 43: 435-468. *Steffensmeier, D. and Britt, C. (2001). “Judges race and judicial decision making: Do black judges sentence differently?”. Social Science Quarterly. 82: 749-764 *Steffensmeier, D. and Demuth, S. (2001). “Ethnicity and judges’ sentencing decisions: Hispanic-black-white comparisons”. Criminology. 39: 145-178. *Steffensmeier, D. and Demuth, S. (2006). “Does gender modify the effects of race—ethnicity on criminal sanctioning? Sentences for male and female and white, black, and Hispanic defendants”. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 22: 241-261. *Steffensmeier, D. and Herbert, C. (1999). “Women and men policymakers: Does the judge’s gender affect the sentencing of criminal defendants?”. Social Forces. 77: 1163-1196. *Steffensmeier, D., Ulmer, J. and Kramer, J. (1998). “The interaction of race, age, and gender in criminal sentencing: The punishment cost of being young, black, and male”. Criminology. 36: 763-798. *Steen, S. and Opsal, T. (2007). “‘Punishment on the installment plan’: Individual-level predictors of parole revocation in four states”. The Prison Journal. 87: 344-366. *Steiner, B., Travis, L., Makarios, M., and Meade, B. “Examing the effect of sanctions on offender recidivism”. Unpublished manuscript. *Steiner, B., Travis, L.F., Makarios, M.D., and Meade, B. (2011). “Short-term effects of sanctioning reform on parole officers’ revocation decisions”. Law and Society Review. 45: 371400. 176 *Tartaro, C. and Sedemaier, C.M. (2009). “A tale of two counties: the impact of pretrial release, race, and ethnicity upon sentencing decisions”. Criminal Justice Studies. 22: 203-221. Tenback, D.E., van Harten, P.N., van Os, J. (2009). “Non-therapeutic risk factors for onset of tardive dyskinesia in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis”. Movement Disorders. 24: 2309-2315. *Teplin, L.A. (2000). “Keeping the peace: Police discretion and mentally ill persons”. National Institute of Justice Journal. 8-15. *Tillyer, R. (2008). Social Conditioning of Police Officers: Exploring the Interactive Effects of Driver Demographics on Traffic Stop Outcomes. Ph.D. Dissertation, Division of Criminal Justice, University of Cincinnati. Travis, J. and Lawrence, S. (2002). Beyond Prison Gates: The State of Parole in America. Urban Institute Justice Policy Center: Washington, DC. Ulmer, J.T. and Johnson, B.D. (2004). “Sentencing in context: A multilevel analysis”. Criminology. 42: 137-177. Ulmer, J.T., Kurlychek, M.C., and Kramer, J.H. (2007). “Prosecutorial discretion and the imposition of mandatory minimum sentences”. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 44: 427-458. *Unnever, J.D. (1982). “Direct and organizational discrimination in the sentencing of drug offenders”. Social Problems. 30: 212-225. *Van Voorhis, P., Spruance, L.M., Ritchey, P.N., Listwan, S.J., and Seabrook, R. (2004). “The Georgia cognitive skills experiment: A replication of reasoning and rehabilitation”. Criminal Justice and Behavior. 31: 282-305. *Viehe, M.E. (2003). Criminal Justice Response to Domestic Violence: A Closer Look at Los Angeles County, 1995-1998. Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Sociology, University of Southern California *Walker, J.T., Hartley, R.D., Maddan, S., VanHouten, A.C., and Ervin-McLarty, G. (2004). Sentencing Practices Under the Arkansas Sentencing Guidelines. Little Rock, AR: Arkansas Crime Information Center. Walker, S. (1992). “Origins of the contemporary criminal justice paradigm: The American Bar Foundation survey, 1953-1969”. Justice Quarterly. 9: 47-75 Walters, G.D. (1992). “A meta-analysis of the gene-crime relationship”. Criminology. 30: 595613. *Walsh, A. (1990). “Twice labeled: The effect of psychiatric labeling on the sentencing of sex offenders”. Social Problems. 37: 375-389. 177 *Wang, X. and Mears, D.P. (2010). “A multilevel test of minority threat effects on sentencing”. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 26: 191-215. Weisburd, D., Telep, C.W., Hinkle, J.C., and Eck, J.E. (2010). “Is problem-oriented policing effective in reducing crime and disorder?” Criminology & Public Policy. 9: 139-172 *Weisburd, D., Waring, E., and Wheeler, S. (1990). “Class, status, and the punishment of whitecollar criminals”. Law & Social Inquiry. 15: 223-243. Wells, E. (2009) “Uses of meta-analysis in criminal justice research: A quantitative review”. Justice Quarterly. 26: 268-294. Whatley, M.A. (1996). “Victim characteristics influencing attributions of responsibility of rape victims: A meta-analysis”. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 1: 81-95. *Wilmot, K.A. and DeLone, M.A. (2010). “Sentencing of native Americans: A multistage analysis under the Minnesota sentencing guidelines”. Journal of Ethnicity in Criminal Justice. 8: 151-180. Wilson, J.Q. (1968). Varieties of police behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wolf, F. M. (1986). Meta-analysis: Quantitative methods for research synthesis. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. *Winfree, L.T., Wooldredge, J., Sellers, C.S., and Ballard, V.S. (1990). “Parole survival and legislated change: A before/after study of parole revocation decision making”. Justice Quarterly. 7: 151-173. *Withrow, B.L. (2007). “Race-based policing: A descriptive analysis of the Wichita stop study”. Police Practice and Research: An International Journal. 5: 223-240. *Wooldredge, J. (1998). “Analytical rigor in studies of disparities in case processing”. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 14: 155-179. Worden, R. (1989). “Situational and attitudinal explanations of police behavior: A theoretical reappraisal and empirical assessment”. Law and Society Review. 23: 667-711. Worden, R. and Shepard, R. (1996). “Demeanor crime and police behavior: A reexamination of Police Services Study data”. Criminology. 34: 83-105. *Wordes, M., and Bynum, T.S. (1995). “Policing juveniles: Is there bias against youths of color?” In Kempf-Leonard, K., Pope, C.E., and Feyerherm, W. (eds). Minorities in Juvenile Justice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. *Wu. J. (2009). Immigration, Race/Ethnicity, and Differential Sentencing: An Examination of the Effects of Citizenship Status on Sentencing Outcomes in Federal Court. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Nebraska, Omaha, NE. 178 Yeaton, W. and Wortman, P. (1993). “On the reliability of meta-analytic reviews: The role of intercoder agreement”. Evaluation Review. 17: 292-309. Zhao, J. and Hassell, K. (2005). “Policing styles and organizational priorities: Retesting Wilson’s theory of local political culture”. Police Quarterly. 8: 411-430. Zhao, J., Ren, L., and Lovrich, N. (2010). “Wilson’s theory of local political culture revisited in today’s police organizations: Findings from longitudinal panel study”. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management. 33: 287-304. 179 APPENDIX A Arrest Coding Guide: (All missing data coded as 99) I. STUDY INFORMATION: A. Identification Number: [STUDID] ________ B. Study accepted or rejected? (yes=1; no=0) [INCLUD] ________ C. Eligibility criteria 1. Study is examining arrest measured dichotomously? 1=YES 0=NO 2. Includes at least one measure of offense severity? 1=YES 0=NO 3. Includes at least one measure of offender characteristics? 1=YES 0=NO 4. Includes at least one measure of officer characteristics? 1=YES 0=NO Notes [DATASET]: _____________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________________ D. Article author(s): _____________________________________________________________ E. Title: ______________________________________________________________________ F. Author affiliation: ____________________________________________________________ [AFFIL] __________ 1 = University 2 = State Agency 3 = Federal Agency 4 = Mixed 5 = Other _________________ 99 = Missing G. Author discipline: 1 = Criminal Justice/Criminology 2 = Psychology 3 = Sociology 4 = Social Work 5 = Mixed 6 = Other _____________ 99 = Missing [DISCIP] _________ H. Coder: [CODER] _________ 1 = Daniel Lytle 180 0 = Other 99 = Missing I. Funding Agency: _____________________________________________________________ 1 = Unfunded [FUND] __________ 2 = Independent agency/foundation funded 3 = State funded 4 = Federal funded 5 = Other ______________ 99 = Missing J. Publication year: Decade: 1 = 1950 2 = 1960 3 = 1970 4 = 1980 5 = 1990 6 = 2000 7 = 2010 99 = Missing [PUBYR] _________ [PUBDEC] ________ K. Geographic region of the study: 1 = Northeast 2 = South 3 = Midwest 4 = Southwest 5 = Pacific/Northwest 6 = Mixed 99 = Missing [GEOREG] ________ L. Data collection year: [START] _________ [FINISH] _________ [DATAGA] _______ Data gathered decade: 1 = 1950 2 = 1960 3 = 1970 4 = 1980 5 = 1990 6 = 2000 7 = 2010 99 = Missing 181 M. Publication type: 1 = Journal 2 = Doctoral dissertation 3 = Book 4 = Book chapter 5 = Federal report 6 = State or local report 7 = Conference paper 8 = Other ________________ 99 = Missing [PUBTYP] ________ O. Number of police agencies surveyed: [AGENUM] _______ II. SAMPLE DEMOGRAPHICS: A. Offense specific: Is the current study limited to a type of offense (1=YES, 0=NO) [TYPOFF0] __________ Specific type of offense: [TYPOFF1] __________ 1 = Drug offenses 2 = Property offenses 3 = Violent offenses 4 = DUI 5 = Mixed 6 = Other _______________ 99 = Missing/na Percent arrested [PERARR] ________ B. Age: Suspects are: 1 = Exclusively adults 2 = Exclusively juveniles 3 = Mainly adults (over 80%) 4 = Mainly juveniles (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [SUSAGE] ________ Mean age of suspects [MAGESU] ______ 182 C. Gender Suspects are: 1 = Exclusively male 2 = Exclusively female 3 = Mainly male (over 80%) 4 = Mainly female (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [SUSGEN] ________ Percent Female suspect: [PRFSUS] _______ Officers are: 1 = Exclusively male 2 = Exclusively female 3 = Mainly male (over 80%) 4 = Mainly female (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [OFFGEN] ________ Percent Female officer: [PRFOFF] _______ D. Race Suspects are: 1 = Exclusively White 2 = Exclusively Black 3 = Mainly White (over 80%) 4 = Mainly Black (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [SUSRAC] ________ Percent Suspect Non-white: [PRNWS] _________ Officers are: 1 = Exclusively White 2 = Exclusively Black 3 = Mainly White (over 80%) 4 = Mainly Black (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [OFFRAC] ________ Percent Officer Non-white [PRNWO] ________ 183 Race is measured as: 1 = White/Non-white 2 = White/Black 3 = Other _________________ 99 = Missing/not measured [RACMEA] _______ E. Ethnicity Suspects are: 1 = Exclusively Hispanic 2 = Exclusively Non-Hispanic 3 = Mainly Hispanic (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly Non-Hispanic (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [ETHSUS] ________ Percent Hispanic: [PRHISS] _________ Officers are: 1 = Exclusively Hispanic 2 = Exclusively Non-Hispanic 3 = Mainly Hispanic (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly Non-Hispanic (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [ETHOFF]_________ Percent Hispanic: [PRHISO] _________ Ethnicity is measured as: 1 = Hispanic/Non-Hispanic 2 = Other ________________ 99 = Missing [ETHMEA] _______ F. Offense Severity Sample is: 1 = Exclusively felonies 2 = Exclusively misdemeanors 3 = Mainly felonies (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly misdemeanors (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [SERFEL] ________ Percent felony: [PRFEL] __________ Evidence: 184 Was the amount of evidence measured? (1=YES, 0=NO) [EVIREC] ________ Citizen intoxication: Did the study measure citizen intoxication? (1=YES, 0=NO) [TOXREC] ________ Percent of citizens intoxicated: [PRTOX] _________ Weapon Use: Was the weapon use in the encounter recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [WEAPREC] ______ Percent of encounters involving a weapon: [PRWEAP] ________ Victim request arrest: Was victim preference recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [VPREFREC] ______ Percent of encounters where victim requested arrest: [PRVPREF] _______ Total number of offense severity measures: [SERTOT] ________ G. Demeanor Suspect demeanor: Was suspect demeanor recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [DEMREC] _______ Percent non-deferential: [PRDEM] ________ Interaction-phase crime: Was IPC recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [IPCREC] _________ Percent of encounters with IPC: [PRIPC] __________ Suspect compliance: Was suspect compliance recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [COMREC] _______ Percent of encounters where suspect was non-compliant [PRCOM] _________ F. Mental illness Mental illness: Was suspect mental illness recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) Percent mentally ill: [MIREC] _________ [PRMENIL] _______ G. Other officer characteristics Education: Is officer education recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) 185 [EDREC] _________ Officers are: 1 = Exclusively college educated 2 = Exclusively high school/GED educated 3 = Mainly college educated (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly high school/GED educated (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [COLED] _________ Percent college educated: [PRCOLED] _______ Experience: Was a measure of officer experience recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [XPREC] _________ H. Organizational characteristics: Were organizational level variables recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [ORGREC] ________ Mean number of organizational levels: [MNLEV] _________ Mean agency size: [MNSIZE] ________ I. Community characteristics: Were community level variables recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [UNITREC] _______ III. METHODOLOGICAL RIGOR ASSESSMENT A. Control Variables included: (1=YES, 0=NO) [CONVAR] _______ B. Systematic social observation used: (1=YES, 0=NO) [SSOUSE] ________ C. Includes interaction-­‐effects: (1=YES, 0=NO) [INEFUSE] _______ D. How long was the study conducted in months: E. Was data collection continuous: (1=YES, 0=NO) IV. SAMPLE SIZE A. Sample size: 186 [LENGTH] ___________ [CONTIN] ____________ [SIZE] ________________ V. EFFECT SIZE DATA A. Suspect Race: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of race and arrest [ORRACE] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVRACE] ____________ Type of statistical test for race and arrest [RACETEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Race and arrest effect size: [ESRACE] ________ B. Suspect Gender: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of gender and arrest [ORGEN] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVGEN] ____________ Type of statistical test for gender and arrest [GENTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Gender and arrest effect size: [ESGEN] ________ C. Suspect Age: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of age and arrest [ORAGE] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVAGE] ____________ Type of statistical test for age and arrest [AGETEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Race and arrest effect size: [ESAGE] ________ D. Suspect Ethnicity: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of ethnicity and arrest Inverse variance: 187 [ORHISP] ___________ [IVHISP] ____________ Type of statistical test for ethnicity and arrest [HISPTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Ethnicity and arrest effect size: [ESHISP] ________ E. Officer Race: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of officer race and arrest [ORORAC] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVORAC] ____________ Type of statistical test for officer race and arrest [ORACTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Officer race and arrest effect size: [ESORAC] ________ F. Officer Gender: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of officer gender and arrest [OROGEN] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVOGEN] ____________ Type of statistical test for officer gender and arrest [OGENTEST] ________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Officer gender and arrest effect size: [ESOGEN] ________ G. Officer Ethnicity: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of officer ethnicity and arrest [OROHIS] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVOHIS] ____________ Type of statistical test for officer ethnicity and arrest [OHISTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Officer ethnicity and arrest effect size: [ESOHIS] ________ 188 H. Offense Severity (felony v. misdemeanor): Odds ratio (logistic regression) of felony and arrest [ORFELO] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVFELO] ____________ Type of statistical test for felony and arrest [FELOTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Felony and arrest effect size: [ESFELO] ________ I. Evidence: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of evidence and arrest [OREVID] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVEVID] ____________ Type of statistical test for evidence and arrest [EVIDTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Evidence and arrest effect size: [ESEVID] ________ J. Citizen Intoxication Odds ratio (logistic regression) of intoxication and arrest [ORTOX] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVTOX] ____________ Type of statistical test for intoxication and arrest [TOXTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Intoxication and arrest effect size: [ESTOX] ________ K. Weapon Use Odds ratio (logistic regression) of weapon use and arrest [ORWU] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVWU] ____________ Type of statistical test for weapon use and arrest [WUTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 189 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Weapon use and arrest effect size: [ESWU] ________ L. Victim preference for arrest Odds ratio (logistic regression) of victim preference and arrest [ORVPREF]___________ Inverse variance: [IVVPREF]____________ Type of statistical test for victim preference and arrest [VPREFTEST]________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Victim preference and arrest effect size: [ESVPREF]________ M. Demeanor Odds ratio (logistic regression) of demeanor and arrest [ORSDEM] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVSDEM] ____________ Type of statistical test for demeanor and arrest [SDEMTEST] ________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Demeanor and arrest effect size: [ESSDEM] ________ N. Interaction Phase Crime Odds ratio (logistic regression) of IPC and arrest [ORIPC] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVIPC] ____________ Type of statistical test for IPC and arrest [IPCTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING IPC and arrest effect size: [ESIPC] ________ O. Suspect Compliance Odds ratio (logistic regression) of compliance and arrest Inverse variance: 190 [ORSCOM] ___________ [IVSCOM] ____________ Type of statistical test for compliance and arrest [SCOMTEST]_________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Compliance and arrest effect size: [ESSCOM] ________ P. Suspect Mentally Ill Odds ratio (logistic regression) of mentally ill and arrest [ORSMI] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVSMI] ____________ Type of statistical test for mentally ill and arrest [SMITEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Mentally ill and arrest effect size: [ESSMI] ________ Q. Officer Education Odds ratio (logistic regression) of officer education and arrest [OROED] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVOED] ____________ Type of statistical test for officer education and arrest [OEDTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Officer education and arrest effect size: [ESOED] ________ R. Officer Experience Odds ratio (logistic regression) of experience and arrest [ORXP] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVXP] ____________ Type of statistical test for experience and arrest [XPTEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Experience and arrest effect size: [ESXP] ________ 191 S. Rank Structure (number of organizational levels) Odds ratio (logistic regression) of rank structure and arrest [ORLEVE] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVLEVE] ____________ Type of statistical test for rank structure and arrest [LEVETEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Rank structure and arrest effect size: [ESLEVE] ________ T. Agency Size Odds ratio (logistic regression) of agency size and arrest [ORSIZE] ___________ Inverse variance: [IVSIZE] ____________ Type of statistical test for agency size and arrest [SIZETEST] _________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Agency size and arrest effect size: [ESSIZE] ________ U. Community Characteristics (crime rate) Odds ratio (logistic regression) of community and arrest [ORCOMM]___________ Inverse variance: [IVCOMM]____________ Type of statistical test for community and arrest [COMMTEST] ________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Community and arrest effect size: [ESCOMM] _______ 192 Sentencing Coding Guide: (All missing data coded as 99) I. STUDY INFORMATION: A. Identification Number: [STUDID] ________ B. Study accepted or rejected? (YES=1; NO=0) [INCLUD] ________ C. Eligibility criteria 1. Study is examining sentencing measured dichotomously? 1=YES 0=NO 2. Includes at least one measure of offense severity? 1=YES 0=NO 3. Includes at least one measure of offender characteristics? 1=YES 0=NO 4. Includes at least one measure of officer characteristics? 1=YES 0=NO [DATASET]: __________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________________ D. Article author(s): _____________________________________________________________ E. Title: ______________________________________________________________________ F. Author affiliation: ____________________________________________________________ [AFFIL] __________ 1 = University 2 = State Agency 3 = Federal Agency 4 = Mixed 5 = Other _________________ 99 = Missing G. Author discipline: 1 = Criminal Justice/Criminology 2 = Psychology 3 = Sociology 4 = Social Work 5 = Mixed 6 = Other _____________ 99 = Missing H. Coder: 1 = Daniel Lytle 0 = Other 99 = Missing [DISCIP] _________ Fill in name 193 [CODER] _________ __________________ I. Funding Agency: _____________________________________________________________ 1 = Unfunded [FUND] __________ 2 = Independent agency/foundation funded 3 = State funded 4 = Federal funded 5 = Other ______________ 99 = Missing J. Publication year: Decade: 1 = 1950 2 = 1960 3 = 1970 4 = 1980 5 = 1990 6 = 2000 7 = 2010 99 = Missing [PUBYR] _________ [PUBDEC] ________ K. Geographic region of the study: 1 = Northeast 2 = South 3 = Midwest 4 = Southwest 5 = Pacific/Northwest 6 = Mixed 99 = Missing [GEOREG] ________ L. Data collection year: [START] _________ [FINISH] _________ [DATAGA] _______ Data gathered decade: 1 = 1950 2 = 1960 3 = 1970 4 = 1980 5 = 1990 6 = 2000 7 = 2010 99 = Missing 194 M. Publication type: 1 = Journal 2 = Doctoral dissertation 3 = Book 4 = Book chapter 5 = Federal report 6 = State or local report 7 = Conference paper 8 = Other ________________ 99 = Missing [PUBTYP] ________ O. Number of courts surveyed: [AGENUM] _______ II. SAMPLE DEMOGRAPHICS: A. Offense specific: Is the current study limited to a type of offense (1=YES, 0=NO) Specific type of offense: 1 = Drug offenses 2 = Property offenses 3 = Violent offenses 4 = DUI 5 = Mixed 6 = Other _______________ 99 = Missing/na Percent sentenced to prison [TYPOFF0] _______ [TYPOFF1] _______ [PERARR] ________ B. Age: Defendants are: 1 = Exclusively adults 2 = Exclusively juveniles 3 = Mainly adults (over 80%) 4 = Mainly juveniles (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [DEFAGE] ________ Mean age of defendants [MAGEDF] ______ 195 C. Gender Defendants are: 1 = Exclusively male 2 = Exclusively female 3 = Mainly male (over 80%) 4 = Mainly female (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [DEFGEN] ________ Percent Female defendants: [PRFDEF] _______ Judges are: 1 = Exclusively male 2 = Exclusively female 3 = Mainly male (over 80%) 4 = Mainly female (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [JUDGEN] ________ Percent Female officer: [PRFJUD] _______ D. Race Defendants are: 1 = Exclusively White 2 = Exclusively Black 3 = Mainly White (over 80%) 4 = Mainly Black (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [DEFRAC] ________ Percent Defendants Non-white: [PRNWD] ________ Judges are: 1 = Exclusively White 2 = Exclusively Black 3 = Mainly White (over 80%) 4 = Mainly Black (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [JUDRAC] ________ Percent judges Non-white [PRNWJ] ________ 196 Race is measured as: 1 = White/Non-white 2 = White/Black 3 = Other _________________ 99 = Missing/not measured [RACMEA] _______ E. Ethnicity Defendants are: 1 = Exclusively Hispanic 2 = Exclusively Non-Hispanic 3 = Mainly Hispanic (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly Non-Hispanic (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [ETHDEF] ________ Percent Hispanic defendants: [PRHISD] _________ Judges are: 1 = Exclusively Hispanic 2 = Exclusively Non-Hispanic 3 = Mainly Hispanic (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly Non-Hispanic (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [JUDHIS] _________ Percent Hispanic judge: [PRHISJ] _________ Ethnicity is measured as: 1 = Hispanic/Non-Hispanic 2 = Other ________________ 99 = Missing [ETHMEA] _______ F. Offense Severity Sample is: 1 = Exclusively felonies 2 = Exclusively misdemeanors 3 = Mainly felonies (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly misdemeanors (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [SERFEL] ________ Percent felony: [PRFEL] __________ Evidence: Was the amount of evidence measured? (1=YES, 0=NO) [EVIREC] ________ 197 Weapon Use: Was the weapon use in the case recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [WEAPREC] ______ Percent of cases involving a weapon: [PRWEAP] ________ Drug and Alchohol use: Was drug and/or alcohol use recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [DRUGREC] ______ Percent of cases involving drugs or alcohol [PRDRUG] _______ Prior record/criminal history Criminal history/prior record recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) How was prior record measured? 1= 1 or more or none 2= discrete scale 3= count 4= other ________________ Total number of offense severity measures: F. Mental illness Mental illness: Was defendant mental illness recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) Percent mentally ill: [PRIORREC]______ [PRIORMEA] _____ [SERTOT] ________ [MIREC] _________ [PRMENIL] _______ G. Other court characteristics Experience: Was a measure of judicial experience recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [XPREC] _________ Mean experience [XPMEAN] _______ Federal vs. local Cases are: 1 = Exclusively Federal 2 = Exclusively local 3 = Mostly Federal (over 80%) 4 = Mostly local (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [FVLREC] ________ Percent Federal [PRFVL] _________ 198 Mode of conviction (plea v. trail conviction) Cases are: 1 = Exclusively pleas 2 = Exclusively trial convictions 3 = Mostly pleas (80%) 4 = Mostly trial convictions (80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing Percent plea [PVTCREC] _______ [PRPVTC] ________ Counsel type Defendants had: 1 = Exclusively public 2 = Exclusively private 3 = Mostly public (over 80%) 4 = Mostly private (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [PVPREC] _______ Percent public defenders [PRPVP] __________ H. Organizational characteristics: Were organizational level variables recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) Mean court size: [ORGREC] ________ [MNSIZE] ________ I. Community characteristics: Were community level variables recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [UNITREC] _______ III. METHODOLOGICAL RIGOR ASSESSMENT A. Control Variables included: (1=YES, 0=NO) [CONVAR] _______ B. Includes interaction-effects: (1=YES, 0=NO) [INEFUSE] _______ C. How long was the study conducted in months: [LENGTH] _______ D. Was data collection continuous: (1=YES, 0=NO) [CONTIN] ________ IV. SAMPLE SIZE A. Sample size: [SIZE] ___________ V. EFFECT SIZE DATA 199 A. Defendant Race: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of race and sentence Inverse variance: [ORRACE] ________ [IVRACE] ________ Type of statistical test for race and sentence [RACETEST]______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Race and sentence effect size: [ESRACE] ________ B. Defendant Gender: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of gender and sentence Inverse variance: [ORGEN]_________ [IVGEN] _________ Type of statistical test for gender and sentence [GENTEST] _______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Gender and sentence effect size: [ESGEN] ________ C. Defendant Age: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of age and sentence Inverse variance: [ORAGE] _______ [IVAGE] _________ Type of statistical test for age and sentence [AGETEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Race and sentence effect size: [ESAGE] ________ D. Defendant Ethnicity: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of ethnicity and sentence Inverse variance: [ORHISP] ________ [IVHISP]__________ Type of statistical test for ethnicity and sentence [HISPTEST] _______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING 200 Ethnicity and sentence effect size: [ESHISP] ________ E. Judge Race: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of judge’s race and sentence Inverse variance: [ORJRAC] ________ [IVJRAC] _________ Type of statistical test for judge’s race and sentence [JRACTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Judge’s race and sentence effect size: [ESJRAC] ________ F. Judge Gender: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of judge’s gender and sentence Inverse variance: [ORJGEN]_________ [IVJGEN] _________ Type of statistical test for judge’s gender and sentence [JGENTEST]_______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Judge’s gender and sentence effect size: G. Judge’s Ethnicity: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of judge’s ethnicity and sentence Inverse variance: [ESJGEN] ________ [ORJHIS]_________ [IVJHIS] _________ Type of statistical test for judge’s ethnicity and sentence [JHISTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Judge’s ethnicity and sentence effect size: H. Offense Severity (felony v. misdemeanor): Odds ratio (logistic regression) of felony and sentence Inverse variance: Type of statistical test for felony and sentence 1: r 5: t test 201 [ESJHIS] ________ [ORFELO] ________ [IVFELO] _________ [FELOTEST] ______ 2: ANOVA 3: Chi-square 4: descriptive statistic 6: p value not otherwise specified 7: Φ calculated 99: MISSING Felony and sentence effect size: [ESFELO] ________ I. Evidence: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of evidence and sentence Inverse variance: [OREVID] ________ [IVEVID] _________ Type of statistical test for evidence and sentence [EVIDTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Evidence and sentence effect size: [ESEVID] ________ J. Weapon Use Odds ratio (logistic regression) of weapon use and sentence Inverse variance: [ORWU] ________ [IVWU]___________ Type of statistical test for weapon use and sentence [WUTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Weapon use and sentence effect size: [ESWU] ________ K. Experience Odds ratio (logistic regression) of experience and sentence Inverse variance: [ORXP] _________ [IVXP] __________ Type of statistical test for experience and sentence [XPTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Experience and sentence effect size: [ESXP] ________ L. Court size Odds ratio (logistic regression) of court size and sentence Inverse variance: 202 [ORSIZE] _________ [IVSIZE]__________ Type of statistical test for court size and sentence [SIZETEST]_______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Court size and sentence effect size: [ESSIZE] ________ M. Community Characteristics Odds ratio (logistic regression) of community and sentence Inverse variance: [ORCOMM]_______ [IVCOMM]________ Type of statistical test for community and sentence [COMMTEST] ________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Community and sentence effect size: [ESCOMM] _______ N. Federal v. Local Odds ratio (logistic regression) of jurisdiction and sentence Inverse variance: [ORJUR]________ [IVJUR]_________ Type of statistical test for jurisdiction type and sentence [JURTEST]________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Jurisdiction type and sentence effect size: O. Counsel Type (public v. private) Odds ratio (logistic regression) of counsel type and sentence Inverse variance: [ESJUR] ________ [ORPVP]________ [IVPVP]_________ Type of statistical test for counsel type and sentence [PVPTEST] ________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Counsel type and sentence effect size: [ESPVP] _______ P. Plea type (plea v. trial conviction) 203 Odds ratio (logistic regression) of plea type and sentence Inverse variance: [ORPVTC] ________ [IVPVTC] ________ Type of statistical test for plea type and sentence [PVTCTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Plea type and sentence effect size: [ESPVTC] _______ Q. Drug and/or Alcohol use Odds ratio (logistic regression) of Drug/alcohol use Inverse variance: [ORDRUG] ______ [IVPVTC]________ Type of statistical test for plea type and sentence [DRUGTEST] _____ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Plea type and sentence effect size: [ESDRUG] _______ R. Prior record/Criminal History Odds ratio (logistic regression) of Drug/alcohol use Inverse variance: [ORPRIOR] _______ [IVPRIOR] _______ Type of statistical test for plea type and sentence [PRIORTEST] _____ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Plea type and sentence effect size: [ESPRIOR] _______ 204 Parole Revocation Coding Guide: (All missing data coded as 99) I. STUDY INFORMATION: A. Identification Number: [STUDID] ________ B. Study accepted or rejected? (yes=1; no=0) [INCLUD] ________ C. Eligibility criteria 1. Study is examining parole revocation measured dichotomously? 1=YES 0=NO 2. Includes at least one measure of offense severity? 1=YES 0=NO 3. Includes at least one measure of offender characteristics? 1=YES 0=NO 4. Includes at least one measure of officer characteristics? 1=YES 0=NO Data Set [DATASET]: ___________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________________ D. Article author(s): _____________________________________________________________ E. Title: ______________________________________________________________________ F. Author affiliation: ____________________________________________________________ [AFFIL] __________ 1 = University 2 = State Agency 3 = Federal Agency 4 = Mixed 5 = Other _________________ 99 = Missing G. Author discipline: 1 = Criminal Justice/Criminology 2 = Psychology 3 = Sociology 4 = Social Work 5 = Mixed 6 = Other _____________ 99 = Missing H. Coder: 1 = Daniel Lytle 0 = Other 99 = Missing [DISCIP] _________ [CODER] _________ If zero, Identify coder __________________ 205 I. Funding Agency: _____________________________________________________________ 1 = Unfunded [FUND] __________ 2 = Independent agency/foundation funded 3 = State funded 4 = Federal funded 5 = Other ______________ 99 = Missing J. Publication year: Decade: 1 = 1950 2 = 1960 3 = 1970 4 = 1980 5 = 1990 6 = 2000 7 = 2010 99 = Missing [PUBYR] _________ [PUBDEC] ________ K. Geographic region of the study: 1 = Northeast 2 = South 3 = Midwest 4 = Southwest 5 = Pacific/Northwest 6 = Mixed 99 = Missing [GEOREG] ________ L. Data collection year: [START] _________ [FINISH] _________ [DATAGA] _______ Data gathered decade: 1 = 1950 2 = 1960 3 = 1970 4 = 1980 5 = 1990 6 = 2000 7 = 2010 99 = Missing (If data collection crosses decade use starting decade for DATAGA) 206 M. Publication type: 1 = Journal 2 = Doctoral dissertation 3 = Book 4 = Book chapter 5 = Federal report 6 = State or local report 7 = Conference paper 8 = Other ________________ 99 = Missing [PUBTYP] ________ O. Number of police agencies surveyed: [AGENUM] _______ II. SAMPLE DEMOGRAPHICS: A. Offense specific: Is the current study limited to a type of offense (1=YES, 0=NO) Specific type of offense: 1 = Drug offenses 2 = Property offenses 3 = Violent offenses 4 = DUI 5 = Mixed 6 = Other _______________ 99 = Missing/na Percent revocated [TYPOFF0] _______ [TYPOFF1] _______ [PERARR] ________ B. Age: Offenders are: 1 = Exclusively adults 2 = Exclusively juveniles 3 = Mainly adults (over 80%) 4 = Mainly juveniles (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [OFFAGE] ________ Mean age of offenders [MAGEOF] ______ 207 C. Gender Parole officers are: 1 = Exclusively male 2 = Exclusively female 3 = Mainly male (over 80%) 4 = Mainly female (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [POGEN] ________ Percent Female suspect: [PRFPO] __________ Offenders are: 1 = Exclusively male 2 = Exclusively female 3 = Mainly male (over 80%) 4 = Mainly female (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [OFFGEN] ________ Percent Female offenders: [PRFOFF] _______ D. Race Parole officers are: 1 = Exclusively White 2 = Exclusively Black 3 = Mainly White (over 80%) 4 = Mainly Black (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [PORAC] ________ Percent parole officers Non-white: [PRNWPO] _______ Offenders are: 1 = Exclusively White 2 = Exclusively Black 3 = Mainly White (over 80%) 4 = Mainly Black (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [OFFRAC] ________ Percent Offender Non-white [PRNWO] ________ 208 Race is measured as: 1 = White/Non-white 2 = White/Black 3 = Other _________________ 99 = Missing/not measured [RACMEA] _______ E. Ethnicity Parole officers are: 1 = Exclusively Hispanic 2 = Exclusively Non-Hispanic 3 = Mainly Hispanic (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly Non-Hispanic (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [ETHPO] ________ Percent Hispanic: [PRHISPO] _______ Offenders are: 1 = Exclusively Hispanic 2 = Exclusively Non-Hispanic 3 = Mainly Hispanic (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly Non-Hispanic (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [ETHOFF] ________ Percent Hispanic: [PRHISOF]________ Ethnicity is measured as: 1 = Hispanic/Non-Hispanic 2 = Other ________________ 99 = Missing [ETHMEA] _______ F. Offense Severity Sample is: 1 = Exclusively felonies 2 = Exclusively misdemeanors 3 = Mainly felonies (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly misdemeanors (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [SERFEL] ________ Percent felony: [PRFEL] __________ Evidence: Was the amount of evidence measured? (1=YES, 0=NO) [EVIREC] ________ 209 Weapon Use: Was the weapon use in the encounter recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [WEAPREC] ______ Percent of encounters involving a weapon: [PRWEAP] ________ Risk: Criminal History/Risk Recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [RISKREC] _______ Need: Criminogenic Need Recorded ? (1=YES, 0=NO) [NEEDREC] ______ Total number of offense severity measures: [SERTOT] ________ G. Education Education: Is offender’s education recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [EDREC] _________ Offenders are: 1 = Exclusively college educated 2 = Exclusively high school/GED educated 3 = Mainly college educated (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly high school/GED educated (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [COLED] _________ Percent college educated offenders: [PRCOLED] _______ Parole officer’s education recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [POEDREC] _______ Officers are: 1 = Exclusively college educated 2 = Exclusively high school/GED educated 3 = Mainly college educated (Over 80%) 4 = Mainly high school/GED educated (Over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing [COLEDPO] ______ Percent college educated parole officers: [PRCOLEDPO] ____ Experience: Was a measure of parole officer experience recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [XPREC] _________ Mean officer experience [MNXP] __________ 210 H. Employment status: Offenders are: 1 = Exclusively employed full time or part time 2 = Exclusively not employed 3 = Mostly employed full or part time (over 80%) 4 = Mostly not employed (over 80%) 5 = Mixed 99 = Missing Percent employed full or part time [EMPOREC] ______ [PREMP] _________ I. Community characteristics: Were community level variables recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [UNITREC] _______ J. Program completion: Were program completion variables recorded? (1=YES, 0=NO) [PCREC] _________ Percent that have completed programming requirements [PRPC] ___________ III. METHODOLOGICAL RIGOR ASSESSMENT A. Control Variables included: (1=YES, 0=NO) [CONVAR] _______ B. Includes interaction-effects: (1=YES, 0=NO) [INEFUSE] _______ C. How long was the study conducted in months: [LENGTH] ________ D. Was data collection continuous: (1=YES, 0=NO) [CONTIN] ________ IV. SAMPLE SIZE A. Sample size: [SIZE] ___________ V. EFFECT SIZE DATA A. Offender Race: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of race and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ORRACE] ________ [IVRACE] ________ Type of statistical test for race and parole revocation [RACETEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Race and parole revocation effect size: [ESRACE] ________ 211 B. Offender Gender: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of gender and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ORGEN]_________ [IVGEN] _________ Type of statistical test for gender and revocation [GENTEST] _______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Gender and parole revocation effect size: C. Offender Age: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of age and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESGEN] ________ [ORAGE] _________ [IVAGE] _________ Type of statistical test for age and parole revocation [AGETEST] _______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Race and parole revocation effect size: [ESAGE] ________ D. Offender Ethnicity: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of ethnicity and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ORHISP]_________ [IVHISP] _________ Type of statistical test for ethnicity and parole revocation [HISPTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Race and parole revocation effect size: [ESHISP] ________ E. Offense Severity (felony v. misdemeanor): Odds ratio (logistic regression) of felony and parole revocation Inverse variance: 212 [ORFELO] ________ [IVFELO] _________ Type of statistical test for felony and parole revocation [FELOTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Felony and parole revocation effect size: F. Evidence: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of evidence and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESFELO] ________ [OREVID] ________ [IVEVID] _________ Type of statistical test for evidence and parole revocation [EVIDTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Evidence and parole revocation effect size: G. Weapon Use Odds ratio (logistic regression) of weapon use and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESEVID] ________ [ORWU] ________ [IVWU] __________ Type of statistical test for weapon use and parole revocation [WUTEST] ________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Weapon use and arrest effect size: [ESWU] ________ H. Parole Officer Education Odds ratio (logistic regression) of officer education and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ORPOED] ________ [IVPOED] ________ Type of statistical test for officer education and parole [POEDTEST] ______ revocation 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING 213 Officer education and parole revocation effect size: I. Parole Officer Race Odds ratio (logistic regression) of officer race and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESPOED] ________ [ORPORAC] ______ [IVPORAC] _______ Type of statistical test for officer race and parole revocation [PORACTEST]_____ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Officer race and parole revocation effect size: J. Parole Officer Gender: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of officer gender and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESPORAC] _______ [ORPOGEN] ______ [IVPOGEN] _______ Type of statistical test for officer gender and parole revocation [POGENTEST] ____ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Officer gender and parole revocation effect size: K. Parole Officer Ethnicity: Odds ratio (logistic regression) of officer ethnicity and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESPOGEN] _______ [ORPOETH] ______ [IVPOETH] _______ Type of statistical test for officer ethnicity and parole [POETHTEST] ____ revocation 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Officer ethnicity and parole revocation effect size: L. Offender Employment Status Odds ratio (logistic regression) of employment and parole revocation Inverse variance: 214 [ESPOETH] _______ [OREMP] _______ [IVEMP] _________ Type of statistical test for employment and parole revocation [EMPTEST] _______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Employment and parole revocation effect size: M. Offender Education Odds ratio (logistic regression) of education and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESEMP] ________ [OREDU] _________ [IVEDU] _________ Type of statistical test for education and parole revocation [EDUTEST] _______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Education and parole revocation effect size: N. Program Completion Odds ratio (logistic regression) of program completion and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESEDU] _______ [ORPROG] _______ [IVPROG] ________ Type of statistical test for program completion and parole [PROGTEST] ______ revocation 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Program completion and parole revocation effect size: O. Parole Officer Experience Odds ratio (logistic regression) of experience and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESPROG] _______ [POXP] ___________ [IVXP] __________ Type of statistical test for experience and parole revocation [XPTEST] ________ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING 215 Experience and parole revocation effect size: P. Community Characteristics Odds ratio (logistic regression) of community and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESXP] ________ [ORCOMM] _______ [IVCOMM] _______ Type of statistical test for community and parole revocation [COMMTEST] _____ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Community and parole revocation effect size: Q. Criminal History/Risk Odds ratio (logistic regression) of risk and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ESCOMM] _______ [ORRISK] ________ [IVRISK] _________ Type of statistical test for risk and parole revocation [RISKTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Risk and parole revocation effect size: [ESRISK] _______ R. Need Odds ratio (logistic regression) of need and parole revocation Inverse variance: [ORNEED] ________ [IVNEED] ________ Type of statistical test for need and parole revocation [NEEDTEST] ______ 1: r 5: t test 2: ANOVA 6: p value not otherwise specified 3: Chi-square 7: Φ calculated 4: descriptive statistic 99: MISSING Need and parole revocation effect size: [ESNEED] _______ 216