Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Global Environmental Change journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/gloenvcha Developing sustainability-oriented values: Insights from households in a trial of plug-in hybrid electric vehicles Jonn Axsen a,*, Kenneth S. Kurani b a b School of Resource and Environmental Management, Simon Fraser University, 8888 University Drive, Burnaby, BC, Canada V5A 1S6 Institute of Transportation Studies, University of California at Davis, 2028 Academic Surge, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA 95616, USA A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T Article history: Received 23 August 2011 Received in revised form 29 August 2012 Accepted 30 August 2012 Available online 29 September 2012 This paper explores the possibilities of consumer transitions to sustainability-oriented values. We draw from sociological and psychological literature to develop a conceptual framework that reflexively links an individual’s values and self-concept to their behaviors. We inductively explore the consideration, and in some cases development, of sustainability-oriented values in a small number of narrative accounts of peoples’ encounter with a plug-in hybrid electric vehicle during a multi-week trial. Because a plug-in hybrid vehicle substitutes electricity for gasoline, it is a technology that potentially symbolizes sustainability-oriented values. We classify participating households according to Schwartz’s 10 motivation types, where households associate sustainability with different motivations, namely benevolence, universalism or self-direction. We categorize households into three groups: those that demonstrate no interests in sustainability-oriented values, those that demonstrate interest in developing such values during their plug-in hybrid vehicle demonstration experience, and those that were already committed to sustainability-oriented values and behaviors. We observe that households open to change are more likely to develop sustainability-oriented values if: (i) their self-concept is open to change (liminal), either as a temporary transitional state or sustained as a value, (ii) they associate sustainability with broader motivational values that are already central to their self-concept, in this case benevolence, universalism or self-direction, and (iii) they experience positive social support for new, sustainability-oriented values within their social networks. Our exploratory findings imply that sustainability-oriented values can be developed in households who did not previously express them. Value change opens new possibilities for sustainable consumer behavior, practices, and policy. ß 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Sustainability Values Consumer behavior Plug-in hybrid vehicle Narrative 1. Introduction This paper explores the potential for consumer transitions to sustainability-oriented values and behaviors. Following the lead of values researchers in psychology (Schwartz, 1994; Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987), we take values to be stable, though not immutable, personally held beliefs that guide behavior and perceptions across a variety of situations. By behavior, we mean conscious actions as well as routinized practices. Thus, ‘‘sustainability-oriented values’’ refers to any durable motivations that guide a person to enact behaviors that are perceived as supporting sustainability goals. The framing of these values and goals will differ across individuals, social groups and cultures. A widespread transition to sustainability-oriented values could accelerate and broaden the uptake of sustainable behaviors. Values and values change also relate to environmental policy; * Corresponding author. Present address: 904 Britton Drive, Port Moody, BC, Canada V3H 3S5. Tel.: +1 778 782 9365; fax: +1 778 782 4683. E-mail address: jaxsen@sfu.ca (J. Axsen). 0959-3780/$ – see front matter ß 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2012.08.002 sustainability-oriented values could help garner support for (or avoid resistance to) policy enactment (Huijts et al., 2012), and values may shift as a result of the enactment of new policy (Axsen et al., 2009; Bardi and Goodwin, 2011). Context-shaping policies are needed to facilitate transitions to low-carbon societies, e.g., regulating vehicle manufacturers or mandating urban density, which will inevitably change individual behaviors and perceptions, e.g., using new vehicle technologies or taking up cycling and walking. Such shifts in behaviors, and enactment of the policy itself, can shape the values held by individuals. If the values people hold can change, policy makers must understand the dynamics of consumer (and voter) values in order to design policy that is feasible and effective in the long run (Norton et al., 1998)— particularly for policies that directly affect consumers. We focus this paper on values because they are by definition more durable and consistent than attitudes or norms—and thus more likely linked to durable and consistent shifts in consumer behavior. However, the role of values and other antecedents of behavior are variously conceived; we provide several selected definitions in Table 1. Economists model aggregated consumer utility functions for different products, e.g., Train (1980), but do not comment on the J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 Table 1 Selected definitions of several potential antecedents to behavior. Concept Selected definition Source Preference Consumer tastes for products (or their attributes) given their budget level Evaluation of an object, event or behavior An individual’s perception of self and how they fit into the modern world, which is socially negotiated over time and across social contexts Perceptions of what behavior is common (descriptive norms) or socially expected or desirable (injunctive norms) Personally held concepts that transcend specific situations or objects and guide behavior and perceptions Jackson (2005) Attitude Self-concept (identity) Norm Value Ajzen (1991) Giddens (1991) Cialdini (2003) Schwartz (1994) origins and dynamics of preferences that underlie utility functions and have had little to say about values (Jackson, 2005). Models from social psychology represent consumer attitudes as behavioral precursors. Multiple iterative elaborations of attitude-behavior models have inserted the concept of intentions between attitude and behavior, and added ‘‘beliefs’’ and ‘‘evaluations’’ as antecedents to attitudes, norms as antecedents to intentions (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980; Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975), and ‘‘perceived behavioral control’’ as a precursor to norms, intentions, and behavior (Ajzen, 1991). However, consistent findings of attitude-behavior gaps suggest that other factors and processes need be addressed (Gill et al., 1986; Oskamp et al., 1991; Scott and Willits, 1994). Other behavioral models have integrated values as behavioral antecedents (Schwartz, 1994; Stern et al., 1995), but use of terminology varies. Values are regularly conflated with other concepts (Hitlin and Piliavin, 2004), perhaps because values both influence and are influenced by behavior (Jackson, 2005). Although research suggests a strong link between sustainability-oriented behaviors and values, little is known about the processes of value change (Dietz et al., 2005). In this study we seek to understand how consumers’ relate their values to their behaviors and assessments of a technology potentially perceived as being sustainability-oriented. A methodological contribution is our use of in-depth, semi-structured interviews as part of a ‘‘behavioral trial,’’ whereas previous value research has relied primarily on surveys or experiments (Dietz et al., 2005). We draw from narrative accounts constructed from repeated interviews with participants in a plug-in hybrid vehicle demonstration project in northern California. Participating households drove a plug-in hybrid vehicle as part of their household fleet of cars for four weeks. Previously, we utilized these data to explore processes of interpersonal influence in car buyers’ assessments of the vehicle (Axsen and Kurani, 2011, 2012a). Here we draw from over 70 h of interviews to inductively learn about participant framing of sustainabilityoriented values in the context of a hypothetical purchase of a plug-in hybrid vehicle and how such values may develop. Next, we briefly survey relevant literature to build a conceptual framework then apply it to the participant narratives. We note that the limited sample size and particular context of this exploratory study may limit the generalizability of our specific findings. However, our results can be used to reconsider conceptualizations of values and value change, and generate hypotheses for future research. 2. Values and dynamics 2.1. Insights from psychology: categorizing values Two thorough review papers outline the complexity and confusion surrounding values. Dietz et al. (2005) start with a 71 New Shorter Oxford English Dictionary definitions of values: the worth of a thing; liking for a thing; or, ‘‘principles of moral standards of a person or social groups, the generally accepted or personally held judgment of what is valuable and important in life’’ (p. 339). They go on to add that values are different than preferences, beliefs (understandings of the world), or roles (patterns of behavior across social situations). Hitlin and Piliavin (2004) also attempt to explain values by what they are not; values are more abstract than attitudes, more controllable than personality traits, less context-dependent than norms, and less biological than needs. We apply a definition similar to that provided by Schwartz and Bilsky’s (1987): values are personally-held concepts that transcend specific situations or objects and guide behavior and perceptions. Schwartz (1994) provides one of the most widely cited value frameworks. At the broadest level, values can be categorized by two dimensions: self-enhancement versus self-transcendence and conservation (or tradition) versus openness to change. Schwartz maps ten ‘‘motivational types’’ (Table 2) onto these two dimensions. Finally, 56 values are mapped onto the ten motivational types using empirical data from 44 countries. For example, the value of ‘‘wealth’’ is associated with the motivational type ‘‘power’’ which is aligned with high ‘‘self-enhancement.’’ Schwartz (1994) did not explicitly include sustainabilityoriented values; the value of environmental protection (a related but more specific value) was found to fit within the ‘‘universalism’’ motivational type, which corresponds with high self-transcendence and openness to change. Urien and Kilbourne (2011) have linked sustainability-oriented behaviors to a lack of self-enhancement values such as achievement and power. However, we suspect that sustainability-oriented values and behaviors could possibly align with a wider variety of motivation types; we speculate on such alignment using Schwartz’s ten motivation types in the third column of Table 2. For example, sustainability could be associated with benevolence (wanting to reduce environmental impacts on the local community) and universalism (avoiding global impacts on people and ecosystems). Engaging in sustainable behaviors could also be framed as a challenge and thus be linked to values of achievement and self-direction. In other words, two different individuals may have very different motivations for taking on sustainability-oriented values and behaviors, and one person may be driven by multiple motives. However, because some of these values may conflict with one another, one person would not likely subscribe to all the motivations in Table 2. Other researchers have developed various schemas to relate value types to pro-environmental and pro-sustainability behaviors. Schultz (2000, 2001) and Stern and Dietz (1994) identify three broad value classes for environmental concern: self-interest or egoistic (concern for self and kin), humanistic altruism (concern for other humans), and biospheric altruism (concern for other species). A fourth, suggested by Kempton et al.’s (1995) in-depth exploration of American environmental values, is religion and spirituality—though this has received less attention in the literature. Such models of pro-environmental behavior typically portray the effect of values as mediated by other antecedents of behavior. For example, Stern et al.’s (1999) value-belief-norms model depicts how an individual’s ascription to egoistic, altruistic or biospheric values influences their beliefs about the environment, which in turn influences their awareness of consequences and feelings of responsibility. These beliefs are represented to in turn influence their personal norms and finally their behavior. Most psychology-based models represent an individual’s values as static (Bardi et al., 2009). The few psychology-based insights offered into value change are largely speculative. For example, Rokeach (1968) suggested that values would change when an individual realized that their existing values were inconsistent. J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 72 Table 2 Schwartz’s ten motivational types of values and possible links to sustainability. Type Definition (Schwartz, 1994) Possible motives to adopt or support ‘‘sustainable’’ behaviors 1. Power ‘‘Social status and prestige, control or dominance over people and resources.’’ ‘‘Personal success through demonstrating competence according to social standard.’’ ‘‘Pleasure and sensuous gratification for oneself.’’ ‘‘Excitement, novelty and challenge in life.’’ ‘‘Independent thought and action—choosing, creating and exploring.’’ ‘‘Understanding, appreciation, tolerance, and protection for the welfare of all people and for nature.’’ ‘‘Preservation and enhancement of the welfare of people with whom one is in frequent personal contact.’’ ‘‘Respect, commitment, and acceptance of the customs and ideas that traditional culture or religion provide.’’ ‘‘Restraint of actions, inclinations, and impulses likely to upset or harm others and violate social expectations or norms.’’ ‘‘Safety, harmony, and stability of society, of relationships, and of self.’’ Controlling others’ resource use 2. Achievement 3. 4. 5. 6. Hedonism Stimulation Self-direction Universalism 7. Benevolence 8. Tradition 9. Conformity 10. Security Hitlin and Piliavin (2004) draw from attitude change research (e.g., Petty and Cacipoppo, 1986) to hypothesize that because values are inherently relevant to the individual, value change is likely to occur through active processing and reflection (central route) rather than more automated peripheral routes observed for non-relevant attitudes. Bardi et al. (2009) use Schwartz’s 10 motivation, two-dimensional framework to explore value change across repeated surveys of the same samples. They find that value change, defined as changes in importance or ranking of a given value, is more likely to occur among respondents that experience significant life changes in the inter-survey period. Values are also thought to be changeable through engagement with new social groups, through changes in behavior, e.g., following a new law, social norm, or employment role, or through direct persuasion (Bardi and Goodwin, 2011). 2.2. Insights from social theory: reflexivity, self-concept and values With that background from more ‘‘individualist’’ perspectives, we look for further insights on value change from social theory. We are motivated by prior findings of the importance of interpersonal interactions in shaping attitudes toward pro-environmental technologies and in consideration of sustainability-oriented behaviors more broadly (Axsen and Kurani, 2012a,b). And as noted in the previous summary of literature from psychology, changes in values have been linked to processes of social influence and changes in social expectations (Bardi and Goodwin, 2011). To contextualize social processes of value change, we first orient values as a component of an individual’s self-concept or identity—their perceptions of who they are. Following Brewer and Roccas (2001) and Hitlin (2003), we conceptualize values as the core or ‘‘cohesive force’’ within personal self-concept. Although research linking values and self-concept is limited (Hitlin and Piliavin, 2004), preliminary research suggests that identity can play a stronger role in sustainable behavior than attitudes (Stets and Biga, 2003), that environmental values are linked to natureoriented identity (Schultz et al., 2004), and that only values central to self-concept will influence sustainable behavior (Verplanken and Holland, 2002). Thus, we expect there to be some correspondence between sustainability-oriented identities, values, and behaviors. Framing values within self-concept is also useful because there has been relatively more discussion on the dynamics and social negotiation of self-concept or identity (Burke, 2006; Deaux and Martin, 2003; Giddens, 1991). In particular, Giddens (1991) explains how individuals must actively create their self-concept, taking on an ongoing, dynamic ‘‘reflexive project’’ to define and Setting and accomplishing energy efficiency goals Enjoying sustainable behaviors in themselves Trying novel behaviors and technologies Becoming independent of a ‘‘polluting’’ system Preserving the biosphere for all humans and animals Preserving environment for family and others in social network, community or nation Adhering to cultural values by preserving ecosystems Following sustainable behaviors demonstrated by others Minimizing personal risk of environmental collapse express oneself. If an individual’s behavior is shaped by their efforts to establish and develop a self-concept, particular behaviors are grouped into lifestyle: packages of behaviors (conscious and routinized) that the individual (and their reference group) associates with their self-concept. A given individual may subscribe to several lifestyles across different reference groups, e.g., family, co-workers and recreational friends. This characterization of identity and lifestyle has been applied to sustainable consumption behaviors (Spaargaren, 2003; Spaargaren and Van Vliet, 2000), and environmentalists’ negotiation of behavior amidst conflicting agendas (Evans and Abrahamse, 2009). We note that in addition to our present use of self-concept, there are various alternative conceptualizations of identity including definitions based on social roles such as gender, race, and nationality (Burke and Tully, 1977; Stryker, 1980, 1987)—we presently do not follow role-based definitions, but acknowledge the potential importance of roles among other social factors. In the context of Giddens’ (1991) approach, we conceptualize values as the ‘‘cohesive force within’’ self-concept (Hitlin, 2003); that is, an individual’s self-concept builds around multiple highpriority or core values (or motivations). As noted in the previous section, an individual is not likely to subscribe to all of Schwartz’s (1994) ten motivation categories or 56 values. Thus, an individual’s self-concept is likely to be more firmly based on only a subset of such values that are of high priority to the individual. Further, Giddens’ (1991) notion of reflexivity is based on the dynamics of expression and negotiation of one’s self-concept, implying that the core values that make up this self-concept can be developed or changed. Here, by value change we mean changing the importance or prioritization of values as they relate to self-concept. To further aid this discussion, we consider the concept of liminality—how open an individual’s self-concept and values are to change. Liminality is related to Schwartz’s ‘‘openness to change’’ dimension of values, but is a broader concept. According to Turner (1969), liminality is a state characterized by ‘‘ambiguous and indeterminate attributes’’ either through a temporary transition or sustained conditions. Thus liminality could be sustained through the embodiment of values Schwartz identifies with openness to change. But there are many other possible sources of liminality. At a societal level, new behaviors and norms can emerge during a liminal period (Swidler, 1986). Though less research has been conducted on understanding liminality at the individual level, we speculate that liminality is heightened at transitional points in one’s life, such as changing relationship status, changing jobs, or moving residence. As noted in the previous section, the importance of life changes to value change is already being discovered in studies of social psychology (Bardi et al., 2009). Liminality may also J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 73 change in response to experimenting with that behavior. Therefore, we sought to identify participants from across a spectrum of prior values, i.e., some do not ascribe to sustainability-oriented values, some are committed to such values, and of primary interest, some are considering making a transition toward sustainability-oriented values. To structure our exploration, we presume that values can change and that values change can occur, in part, through changes in behavior. Drawing from the above discussion of value change and identity, we utilize the following conceptual framework linking sustainability-oriented values to sustainable practices: be supported by a household having more access to resources such as time and money (or income) to afford experimentation in lifestyle behaviors. Giddens’ framework implies that not only can values (and selfconcept) serve to guide behavior, but behavior can also be means of trialing and socially learning about one’s values. For example, engagement in a sustainable behavior, e.g., driving a plug-in hybrid vehicle, could be a trial of a more fundamental shift toward sustainability-oriented values and self-concept. Experimentation with a new behavior associated with a new value may cause the individual to solidify, modify, or reverse their commitment to the value and related lifestyle. We distinguish Giddens’ perspective on lifestyle and our present focus on values from practice theory. Practice theory also views sustainable consumption as practices, but defines practices as the routinization of related bodily and mental activities that are socially constructed and reflexively refined (Reckwitz, 2002; Ropke, 2009). Practices, such as driving automobiles, are constructed and sustained by the individual practitioners (Shove, 2010)—by engaging in the practice, the individual normalizes and sustains it. For example, Shove (2004) uses practice theory to explain the co-evolution of air-conditioning and food refrigeration with increasing cultural standards and expectations of comfort. Practice theory is useful for the description of society becoming locked-in to particular patterns of consumption, and emphasizes the challenges of overcoming such normative behaviors to increase the uptake of more sustainable practices. The approach we take looks at processes of behavioral lock-in versus change and development at the household level through the examination of values expressed by household members. We use the term ‘‘practice’’ to refer to any ongoing behavior, rather than in the more formal definitions provided by practice theory. Regarding the fourth and fifth points, sustainability-oriented values might be manifest by driving an electric-drive vehicle, e.g., a plug-in hybrid vehicle. Driving an electric-drive vehicle can be both an expression of sustainability-oriented value and a trial that informs, reinforces or negates the individual’s commitment to that value. 2.3. Conceptual framework of value change 3. Methodology The concept of ‘‘sustainability-oriented’’ values is an intentionally broad concept that may include a variety of proenvironmental and pro-societal motivations. We have reviewed several models of and perspectives on value categories that could have guided the statement of specific hypotheses and experimental designs. However, we use an inductive, semi-structured interview methodology to learn whether and how participants discuss and express sustainability-oriented values. In this sense, having convened conversations with our participants, we listened for whether their narratives convey values that are attached to a particular sustainability-oriented behavior and processes of value 3.1. Sample and context 1. An individual constructs their self-concept around one or more core value(s). 2. An individual’s self-concept is developed and maintained in a social context. 3. Sustainability-oriented values are more open to development or change if the individual is in a liminal state, which can be transitional or sustained. 4. Core values and self-concept are manifest as lifestyle: packages of behaviors that are shaped by, and are expressions of, a particular value or set of values. 5. Values, self-concept and construction of and engagement in lifestyle behaviors are reflexively related—influence iteratively flows in multiple directions. To explore understandings of sustainability-oriented values and processes of value change, we draw from 10 households (18 individuals) who participated in a plug-in hybrid vehicle demonstration project conducted at the University of California, Davis (full methodology detailed in Axsen (2010)). Participating households resided in the Sacramento, California area, and did not receive any incentives other than the opportunity to drive a plug-in hybrid vehicle. The vehicle is a Toyota Prius converted to be powered in part from an additional 5 kWh battery recharged using Table 3 Characteristics of 10 participating households (18 participants). Surname First name Dominant lifestyle(s) Household income Age Earhart Fort Betty Brett Julie Craig Siobhan Rupert Amy Adam Katrina Ethel Ed Silvia Larry Cheryl Darren Pat Melissa Billy Career Social, recreation Education, recreation Environment, technology, social Environment, technology, social Social (family) Social (family) Social (family), technology Education, social (family) Social (family) Social (family), career, technology Social (family), career Social (family), environment, technology Social (family), environment, technology Career, social (family) Career, social (family) Student, social Recreation, social $50–59k $100–124k 30s 40s 20s 40s 40s 40s 40s 60s 30s 50s 30s 30s 40s 30s 50s 50s 20s 40s McAdam Noel Petrov Potter Ranchero Rhode Stashe Woods >$150k $80–89k $40–49k >$150k $100–124k >$150k $100–124k $100–124k 74 J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 any 110-V outlet. Each household’s trial lasted four to six weeks. We expected the use of this vehicle to stimulate household consideration of sustainability-oriented values for at least two reasons. First, electric-drive vehicles, and in particular the Toyota Prius, are typically perceived as being pro-environmental symbols in California (Heffner et al., 2007). Second, automobiles in general can by highly symbolic objects (Steg, 2005), and automobile use is typically considered to be a conspicuous act with strong relations to personal identity (Shove and Warde, 2002). Table 3 details the sample by age and income; all names are pseudonyms. Although households were drawn from one region in northern California, the range of socio-economic, demographic and attitudinal attributes among participants approximate those of a representative, U.S.-wide sample of new vehicle buyers (Axsen and Kurani, 2009). We intentionally selected this sub-sample from a broader pool of households in an attempt to include households with different social and demographic characteristics. Table 3 also classifies each individual by one or more lifestyle types they demonstrated to researchers during the four to five interviews that took place. We categorize each household according six lifestyle types derived from a survey of households and pro-environmental behavior (Axsen et al., 2012). 3.2. Semi-structured interviews Our interview methodology is ethnographic in the sense outlined by Atkinson and Hammersley (1994). We explore research questions rather than testing specific hypotheses, work with uncoded data, investigate a limited number of cases, and interpret participants’ verbal description in effort to understand their behavior. However, our approach is better described as semistructured because we included structured exercises in the interviews, which can serve to create a more efficient method overall (McCracken, 1988). In open-ended components we developed a natural dialog with participants, and sought to establish trust and rapport. Though a prepared list of topics provided some structure to the interviews, the researcher was free to pursue and explore new insights that arose during the interview, even if it required temporary departure from that list. Each interview lasted from 1 to 2 h, with each household participating in four to five interviews over their four to six-week trial of the plugin hybrid vehicle. Each interview was conducted in the respondents’ home in effort to create as comfortable and neutral a social encounter as possible for the household. Prior to their trial of the plug-in hybrid vehicle, the households’ first interview elicited their vehicle purchase histories, future vehicle purchase intentions (if any) and expectations of the plug-in hybrid vehicle (if any)—this information helped to construct the beginning of the households’ narratives as well as providing initial context for demonstrating values and motivations. Researchers also collected information about the household’s social network using a method described by Hogan et al. (2007). This social network information informed a related project on social influence, summarized by Axsen and Kurani (2012a), though this information is also entirely relevant for the present analysis of value change given the socially constructed nature of values in our conceptual framework (Section 2.3). During their vehicle trial, each household completed several tasks. Bi-weekly interviews elicited information about the household’s ongoing experiences with the vehicle. Participants completed a two-part web-based survey that had been previously completed by a large, representative U.S. sample (Axsen and Kurani, 2009). Participants also completed a social episode diary, reporting verbal or non-verbal interactions with other people relating to the plug-in hybrid vehicle; the instrument and results are summarized by Axsen and Kurani (2011). The final interview elicited the household’s narrative of their overall experience with the plug-in hybrid vehicle, including use of the vehicle and learning and assessment of the vehicle. Participants also completed an ‘‘influence’’ ranking exercise to communicate which experiences (with the vehicle, social, or otherwise) had more influence on their perceptions of plug-in hybrid vehicle technology. 3.3. Narrative construction Eliciting data in narrative form can be a highly effective way to understand subjective experience, challenge research preconceptions and cultivate new perspectives on social phenomena (Burnett, 1991). Narratives can be a form of ‘‘thick’’ description that illuminate behavioral patterns that might be missed by more tightly focused, deductive research approaches (Geertz, 1973). Further, a narrative approach is consistent with Giddens’ (1991) reflexivity perspective (summarized in Section 2.2), where individuals come to understand themselves by linking their past, present and future into a cohesive storyline. Thus, the constructing and telling of narratives may help discover how an individual reflexively relates their plug-in hybrid vehicle experience to their values and potentially how the individual may consider and develop new values. A well-formed narrative includes: a goal-state; goal-related, chronological events; a logical, causal flow of events; and, demarcation signs such as ‘‘at first’’ and ‘‘by the end’’ (Burnett, 1991; Gergen and Gergen, 1987). Of course, eliciting meaningful, coherent narratives can be challenging. This is one reason we also employed structured exercises to provide context for the participant’s story—researchers utilized relevant results from these exercises to inform the constructed narratives. Further, the order and wording of the final interview questionnaire intentionally guided a narrative response: it was structured around the goal-state of their plug-in hybrid vehicle assessment, prompts were ordered chronologically (beginning with the participant’s initial expectations, moving to their vehicle trial experiences, then concluding with their assessment), and an influence ranking exercise elicited their perceptions of causality between their experiences and assessment. By constructing narratives for each household, the researchers serve as instruments themselves (McCracken, 1988). Researchers reviewed interview recordings and complete transcripts of each interview and produced a summary relating to value change, then integrated these summaries with information from their online survey responses and structured exercises. We analyzed narratives from all 10 households, and looked for patterns relating to the dynamics of values, self-concept and behavior. In particular, we look for their understanding of and commitment to sustainabilityoriented values and behavior and how these values may develop or change in some cases. We summarize several illustrative narratives in the next section. 4. Results 4.1. Framing of sustainability-oriented values Our conceptual framework (Section 2.3) and semi-structured method allowed participants to frame their perceptions of sustainability-oriented behaviors and values in their own terminology (if they had anything to say about sustainability at all). That is, we did not impose analytic categories at the time of the interviews. However, we do impose such a structure to frame the results (and summaries of narratives) using what we have learned. To start, we found that participants’ engagement with sustainability-oriented values did not correspond with the altruistic, biospheric and spiritual distinctions promulgated by prior J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 researchers (Schultz, 2001; Stern et al., 1999), or Schwartz’s (1994) mapping of environmental protection onto universalist motivations. Rather, we found Schwartz’s broader framework of 10 motivational types a useful starting point for describing participants’ ‘‘core’’ values (which form their self-concepts). Through our methodology of narrative construction, participants connected commitment to a future plug-in hybrid vehicle purchase to sustainability through three different core values. As we will discuss, in addition to universalism, participants also linked sustainability-oriented values with the core values of benevolence, i.e., benefitting socially closer individuals such as friends and family, and self-direction, i.e., providing a challenge that involved individualistic learning and development. Sustainability did not form its own core value, but rather fit in with other, higher level motivations, i.e., Schwartz’s ten motivational types. We differentiate the ten households into three categories according to their commitment or openness to sustainabilityoriented values in Table 4. First are households that are not presently motivated by sustainability-oriented values and demonstrate no interest in developing such values. Second are those open to exploring sustainability-oriented values. Third are those previously committed to sustainability-oriented values. Each household differed in terms of which core values were demonstrated via their narrative of experience and social interactions pertaining to the plug-in hybrid vehicle trial. Looking across these households, we identify three conditions associated with those in the second group, i.e., those open to the development of new, sustainability-oriented values. These households were found to develop or strengthen their commitment to sustainability-oriented values by the end of their plug-in hybrid vehicle trial when they start with a moderately to highly liminal self-concept, come to associate sustainability with one of their existing core values, and experience positive social support for the new lifestyle practice and values. Next we utilize household narratives to illustrate these conditions. We summarize illustrative narratives from the participant groups that did not demonstrate interest in value change, and then elaborate on all four households exploring sustainability-oriented values. By focusing on these households we garner more understanding of how sustainability-oriented values can be developed. 4.2. Households committed to private or sustainability-oriented values Households in the first and third groups maintained their initial values over the course of their plug-in hybrid vehicle trial—they 75 either demonstrated no interest in developing sustainabilityoriented values or were already committed to such values. First, the Noels, the Petrovs, Betty Earhart and the Stashes each began and ended their plug-in hybrid vehicle trial with no interest in sustainability-oriented values. They framed the practice of driving the vehicle according to self-enhancement values such financial savings, safety and status. Consider Adam Petrov as an example. He is a retired maintenance supervisor in his sixties who spends most of his time doing handy-man ‘‘small jobs’’ for friends and acquaintances. He primarily assessed the vehicle based on the performance of the engine and battery as he would assess one of his power tools. He demonstrated strong commitment to the traditions of his family, church and career, and benevolence in sharing his skills with those socially close to him. However, Adam demonstrated no interest in or understanding of sustainabilityoriented values and did not interpret the plug-in hybrid vehicle according to sustainability or benevolence. Further, the Petrovs were surprised to find the plug-in hybrid vehicle stimulated little interest in their social network, which itself consists of likeminded individuals who also emphasize traditional values. Any vehicle-related conversations that did occur only addressed technical aspects of the vehicle such as fuel costs and handling. By the end of their trial the Petrovs lacked interest, openness and social support to consider a transition to sustainability-oriented values. In contrast, the McAdams and the Rhodes began and ended their plug-in hybrid vehicle trial with strong commitments to sustainability-oriented values. For example, Larry and Cheryl Rhode consider themselves to be ‘‘tree huggers’’ that have been dedicated to sustainability-oriented practices for many years— such values are well integrated into their lifestyle and social network. They are eager to explain that they practice organic gardening, use only compact fluorescent light bulbs, and send their son to a preschool that espouses environmental values and includes special classes on environmentally friendly technologies. Larry in particular is highly competitive in his career as well as his efforts to achieve high fuel economy in his own hybrid vehicle and in the plug-in hybrid vehicle trial he was happy to ‘‘beat the fleet’’ of other plug-in hybrid vehicle drivers’ fuel economy. His commitment to sustainability-oriented practices is motivated by strong values of achievement and self-direction. The Rhodes also have many sustainability-oriented friends and acquaintances, and Larry frequently goes out of his way to tell others about his hybrid car and the plug-in hybrid vehicle during his trial as part of ‘‘getting the word out.’’ In short, the Rhodes were already engaged in a sustainability-oriented lifestyle, supported by like-minded friends and acquaintances. The practice of driving the plug-in hybrid Table 4 Three patterns of value orientation. Household (by interest in sustainability-oriented values 1. No interest: Betty Earhart The Noels The Petrovs The Stashes Melissa Stashe 2. Exploring: The Forts Ethel Potter The Rancheros Billy Woods 3. Committed: The McAdams The Rhodes Demonstrated core values (Schwartz’s motivation type) Sustainability associated with: Liminality Sustainable-oriented values Social network support Acceptance of value Achievement Tradition, conformity Tradition, benevolence Security Conformity Universalism – – – – Mod Low Mod Low High No No No No No No No No No No Hedonism, security, benevolence, self-direction Self-direction, stimulation Security, benevolence Stimulation, conformity Benevolence High Yes Yes Universalism, self-direction Benevolence Benevolence High Mod High Yes No No Yes No No Benevolence Achievement, self-direction Benevolence Self-direction Low Low Yes Yes Yes Yes 76 J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 vehicle fit their existing core values and their existing commitment to sustainability-oriented values in particular. 4.3. Households open to sustainability-oriented values Billy Woods, the Rancheros, Ethel Potter, and the Fortes each exhibited liminality—openness to shifting their self-concepts and develop new, sustainability-oriented core values. However, in only two of these four households did this openness to value change at the time of their plug-in hybrid vehicle trial end with the development of new sustainability-oriented values. The following narrative summaries illustrate why, following the three conditions identified above. 4.3.1. Why did Billy Woods reject sustainability-oriented values? Billy began his vehicle trial in a liminal state. He was recently divorced and had a new girlfriend. He tries and takes part in many different recreational activities. He has friends and casual acquaintances from a variety of social groups, including coworkers, golfing friends, and friends from bars and nightclubs. He earns a relatively high income and has the flexibility to work from home or his office. Billy was not committed to a particular routine, set of activities, or social group—he was dabbling in a variety of practices across different social groups. Billy is highly motivated by stimulation values—seeking novel experiences—as well as conformity, where he is constantly seeking to understand the norms of his social networks so that he can align with them. Billy’s stimulation and conformity-based values are in part what guide him to consider sustainability-oriented values in the first place. He quickly understands how an electric-drive vehicle is seen as an environmental symbol, and he wants to learn if this symbol (that is novel to him) might fit with his self-concept and social reference groups. Prior to the vehicle trial, Billy had few thoughts of sustainability; he did not consider sustainabilityoriented values when engaging in activities, and he did not devote any significant time or resources to sustainable practices. His conceptualization of sustainability is linked to vague ideas such as ‘‘overall cleaner, livable cities’’ and framed more by benevolence rather than universalism (‘‘I focus on my own country. . .let [others] worry about their own’’). However, during his trial he briefly engaged with the sustainability aspects of the plug-in hybrid vehicle. Billy’s ultimate rejection of sustainability-oriented values results from social feedback. Billy was disappointed to find mixed responses to the plug-in hybrid vehicle across his social groups. Many of his relatives and acquaintances seemed disinterested in the vehicle altogether or were unable to grasp what it is. Of the friends and acquaintances that were interested, most focused on technical functions or performance. Billy intentionally mentions sustainability concerns with one particularly influential group of coworkers, asking them: ‘‘Why would you buy a hybrid. . .mainly to protect the environment or from a consumer standpoint?’’ When his coworkers declare a lack of support for the environment, Billy concurs and drops further consideration, saying: ‘‘To buy it just to protect the environment is probably not something I’d do at this time.’’ Thus, while Billy’s liminality makes him receptive to experimenting with the car, exploring the values associated with it, and the question of a transition to sustainability-oriented values, he abandons this consideration when he sees an apparent lack of support and interest in his social network. While he associated his vague concepts of sustainability with the motivation of benevolence, the motivation to conform to others’ perspectives proved more important the end. 4.3.2. Why did the Rancheros reject sustainability-oriented values? Ed and Silvia Ranchero’s self-concepts were liminal in a transitional sense—they were moving through a phase of opening some new lifestyles and closing off others. Their recent marriage and new child were shifting their priorities. Ed had had to change from his powerful pickup truck to buying a family sedan: ‘‘I need a four-door to get in and out easily. . .to put my kid in something that’s safe. . .reliable.’’ They also became more interested in sustainability issues through the motive of benevolence, in particular relating to their child’s future. Ed explains: ‘‘I want to do my part in reducing global warming. . .to be more environmentally friendly. I wanted my daughter to have something. . .an environment that is free of pollution when she grows up. . .that definitely became more important when she was born. [After having a child] your focus totally changes from being on yourself to being on somebody else.’’ Further, Ed repeatedly spoke with his wife and coworkers about his concerns of the environmental impacts and unsustainability of energy use. At the same time, other aspects of the Rancheros’ self-concept were becoming less liminal as their family patterns increasingly centralize around their baby daughter. They emphasize security values relating to their household; for example, the Rancheros are too cautious (worried about leaving the car plugged in overnight) and resource-constrained (in time and effort) to integrate regular plug-in hybrid vehicle recharging into their daily schedule. They state that they perceive the immediate needs and safety of their daughter as more important than indirectly benefitting her through long-term environmental benefits or cost savings. Silvia becomes ‘‘paranoid’’ that the vehicle charger could start a fire, and Ed points out that his ‘‘main concern is [his] daughter’’ although he admits, ‘‘maybe my concern [about recharging] is without founding.’’ Thus, the Rancheros only plug-in their plug-in hybrid vehicle on a few occasions, and end up using much less electricity and thus much more gasoline than they would have had they regularly charged the vehicle. By the end of their plug-in hybrid vehicle trial, Ed and Silvia were surprised and ‘‘sort of disappointed’’ with how little interest the vehicle generated among their friends and coworkers. They expected friends and coworkers to be ‘‘hyped’’ about the vehicle, yet most demonstrated only casual interest or none at all. Ed notes: ‘‘I was probably expecting more people to be more open minded about it. . .to be excited about it. . .but not everybody thinks the same way.’’ The Rancheros explain that electric-drive vehicles are beyond the realm of typical discussions, even about vehicles, within their groups, and their social contacts were not openly committed to sustainability-oriented values or behaviors. Ed imagines that ‘‘if people were [more] excited about it. . .the [plug-in hybrid vehicle trial] would have been a little bit different.’’ In summary, the Rancheros volunteered for the vehicle study during a window of time when their marriage and new child had opened their lives to many changes. The Rancheros’ interest and understanding could have supported a shift to sustainabilityoriented values. However, the conflict between increasing familyoriented values (particularly the security of their daughter) with the new perceived risk posed by the vehicle, plus a lack of countervailing social support quashed any serious commitment to value change. 4.3.3. Why did Ethel develop sustainability-oriented values? Ethel’s self-concept became increasingly liminal as each of her eight children grew up and moved out, and as her household income and financial stability improved. For much of her life she has been interested in new scientific breakthroughs and technologies, and recently became more concerned about energy issues such as oil dependence and emissions: ‘‘We’re such pigs with J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 energy.’’ She associates her sustainability concerns with a universal perspective: ‘‘When you look at the rest of the world. . .there [are] some simple things you can do. We’re just living the way we live, gobbling up energy. . .it just doesn’t seem right.’’ Prior to her plug-in hybrid vehicle trial, Ethel’s interest in sustainability issues prompted her to consider similar technologies and talk to others about their effectiveness. She had heard about waiting lists for hybrid vehicles, and one coworker had warned her about battery toxicity and electricity generation impacts for electric-powered vehicles. Ethel’s core values include self-direction and stimulation—she feels she is now better able to realize the latter value because her increasing disposable income gives her the ‘‘luxury’’ to more actively consider and try out new behaviors such as adopting solar panels and electric-drive vehicles. During her trial, Ethel is disappointed by the lack of broad support across her social network—‘‘it’s amazing how little people notice [the vehicle]. . .especially the fact that it’s a plug in.’’ However, she felt inspired by the ‘‘interest, enthusiasm and encouragement’’ offered by several key friends and acquaintances, such as two of her favorite daughters, and a repairman that visits her home. Ethel likes how the vehicle brought her into a contact with a new friend from her craft class, where the technology served as a ‘‘way to talk to her more. . .we’re getting to know each other. . .we have a lot in common.’’ By trying the plug-in hybrid vehicle, Ethel became better able to articulate her own values and contrast them with the motives of others—such as her boss’s purchase of a sports car: ‘‘For the first time I saw her car today. . .it just to me is so typical. I was looking at that car thinking, do I feel jealous? No, at this time in my life, I’d rather be making an impact in some way, a positive impact rather than driving some gas guzzling [sports car]. Maybe 10 years ago I would have thought, ‘Oh, I want that car.’ [Now] I just want a car that makes sense. . .and actually may make a difference.’’ Such support and reflection helped Ethel to solidify her initial interests in sustainability-oriented values, empowering her to take additional actions and make commitments. For example, during her vehicle trial she committed to install solar panels on her roof and she was excited about one day being able to use the generated electricity to power a plug-in vehicle. In addition to being an act motivated by universalism, the development of sustainabilityoriented values was consistent with Ethel’s motives of selfdirection. 4.3.4. Why do the Forts develop sustainability-oriented values? The Forts sustain a state of lifestyle liminality. Brett, Selena, and their two children form a tight family unit; their social circles closely overlap, and they typically consult only one another regarding vehicle purchases and other issues—potentially resulting from a lack of proximate extended family. This insular social arrangement seems to buffer the Forts from external social pressures. Self-direction forms one of the Forts’ core values; they experiment with several alternative and perhaps conflicting motivations, including hedonism, security and benevolence. The Forts embody hedonistic values in their ‘‘bigger is better’’ living and recreational activities; they own and operate three large lightduty trucks and initially dreamt of an even larger one, and regularly haul and drive dirt bikes and all-terrain vehicles for recreation. At the same time, the Forts were increasingly shifting toward sustainability-oriented household behaviors, such as reducing water use and buying efficient appliances—driven by benevolent concerns about avoiding local impacts from ‘‘smog’’ 77 and ‘‘ozone depleting’’ substances. Selena has recently led their household to become more environmentally aware in general: ‘‘We use environmentally friendly dish washing soap and laundry soap. . .turn the shower off halfway. . .we recycle everything. . .[Selena’s] become very green over the years.’’ The Fort’s perspective on vehicles is also heavily influenced by security motives. After Selena twice experienced automotive accidents where she was in a smaller car hit by a larger truck, the Forts resolved to buy only large vehicles: ‘‘mass always wins.’’ When the daughter, Julie, received her driver’s license the Forts bought her a new, powerful pickup truck solely for the purpose of safety. As a tightly knit household, the Forts explain that their most influential social experiences occur with close family, and they typically don’t look beyond each other when considering vehicle purchases. Indeed, most of their conversations about the plug-in hybrid vehicle took place with one another. Despite their generally inward focus, Selena explains they were initially worried about the Prius being viewed as a ‘‘weenie car’’ in other social groups. They were surprised to find mostly positive reinforcement by acquaintances and strangers—in particular a hybrid-driving family that enthusiastically waved to the Forts on the highway. The Forts were at first baffled by the experience (which they rated as being highly influential), and tried to understand the significance of the event: Brett: ‘‘the wave on the freeway. . .was actually pretty cool.’’ Julie: ‘‘we don’t get waved at. . .frequently by strangers.’’ Brett: ‘‘you know, [when] you’re driving a truck. . .someone in a [hybrid car] doesn’t normally wave at you. . .they may flip something at you. . .but it won’t be a wave.’’ Julie: ‘‘[it was like] we would be part of the family if we had [a hybrid vehicle].’’ Brett: ‘‘I think [this type of experience] makes you feel good about doing something good. . .one of those feel good moments.’’ Similarly, Julie contrasted the positive social support she received for the plug-in hybrid vehicle, with the ‘‘dirty looks’’ she would get when driving her large pickup truck—like the ‘‘dirty looks from vegans when you’re wearing leather.’’ When Brett and Julie drove the plug-in hybrid vehicle, they saw the world from this other perspective: ‘‘I have found myself looking at trucks, saying, ‘Wow, he’s taking a lot of gas.’’’ By the end of their plug-in hybrid vehicle trial, the Forts made commitments to purchase a hybrid vehicle to strengthen their commitment to sustainability-oriented value. The excitement of this new commitment diminished the importance of motivations that seemed to be of higher priority in their first interview, including concerns about security and hedonistic goals of owning even bigger trucks. Experiences of social support (and lack of ridicule) served to further legitimize the Forts growing environmental interests and commitment to sustainability-oriented values. In this validation, the Forts were not so much conforming to the values of others, but instead finding a lack of the social resistance they expected. 5. Discussion and conclusions This paper set out to better understand sustainability-oriented values and how such values can be developed. We reviewed literature from psychology and sociology to construct a conceptual framework to guide our empirical analysis using narratives of participants in a plug-in hybrid vehicle (plug-in hybrid vehicle) demonstration project. We frame values as the cohesive force of an individual’s self-concept, and describe identity and values are 78 J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 reflexively linked to behavior. These core values (and self-concept) are socially negotiated and defined, and more prone to change when the individual is in a liminal (transitional or open) state. Through semi-structured data collection and narrative analysis, we hear processes (or the lack thereof) of the formation of sustainability-oriented values, in distinction from the formation of attitudes or preferences. Participants do express attitudes and preferences relating to the plug-in hybrid vehicle; these attitudes are stated in relation to the specific object, i.e., a vehicle, and are relatively fluid as participants gain experience with the technology over periods of four to six weeks. In contrast, when participants talk about their concepts of sustainability, they engage broader, relatively stable beliefs and feelings that guide behavior across a range of situations. Participants tended to link their concepts of sustainability with a variety of behaviors beyond the use of the plug-in hybrid vehicle itself, such as recycling, installing solar panels, and reducing household energy use. These observations confirm that values are a useful construct for observing how perceptions of sustainability relate to behavior. We find that participants’ expression of sustainability-oriented values do not align with the egoistic, humanistic altruism and biospheric altruism categories utilized by many values researchers (Schultz, 2001; Stern and Dietz, 1994; Stern et al., 1999). Instead, we find that sustainability-oriented values can be aligned with a variety of core values. Schwartz’s (1994) ten general motivational types proves useful for categorizing participant’s core values, and how they frame sustainability-oriented values (if at all). Although Table 2 hypothesized potential links to sustainability for each of the ten motivation types, in this study we only observed links to three: universalism, benevolence and self-direction. This is a more diverse set than Schwartz’s (1994) mapping of environmental concern solely onto universalism and more values were engaged by our respondents who were open to values change, including stimulation, conformity, and security. Our finding suggests other motivational links to sustainability may be possible in other samples and contexts, and for other sustainability-oriented behaviors. These narratives also yield insights into value change. We highlight three broad conditions that facilitate the development of sustainability-oriented values among plug-in hybrid vehicle trial participants. Each condition relates to the reflexive relationship between values and behavior outlined in our conceptual framework. Here we summarize these conditions with examples observed in the vehicle demonstration study (Table 5). First, if the household is not already committed to sustainability-oriented values, their self-concept must be in a liminal state to facilitate consideration of new values. Liminality can be a temporary state of transition: Billy Woods was recently divorced and seeking new ways to structure his life, and Ed and Silvia Ranchero were newly married and working to understand their priorities with a growing family. In time, an individual’s self-concept may return to a more stable state. In other cases, liminality can be sustained: Ethel Potter increases liminality over a period of years as her children move out and her disposable household income rises, freeing up time and money to consider sustainability-oriented actions and investments. The Forts’ sustained liminality results in part from their social network structure: as a family unit they are tightly connected through mutual activities and communication while at the same time having access to a variety of social networks and lifestyles. The Forts’ maintain a tight central family that facilitates sustained experimentation with ideas from these diverse reference points. In this sense, liminality may be sustained as a core motivation for some individuals, similar to Schwartz’s (1994) motivations of self-direction and stimulation which both Table 5 Sub-components of conditions for sustainability-oriented value change. Conditions for change Examples observed in this study 1) Lifestyle liminality 2) Alignment with core values Sustainability as helping family and friends (benevolence) Sustainability as saving the world (universalism) Sustainability as personal development (self-direction) 3) Social network support Observing similar practices, e.g., HEV ownership Interpersonal support from influential social groups Demonstrated support from strangers Absence of ridicule from non-sustainabilityoriented social groups Transition in life, e.g., divorce, new child Sustained life stage, e.g., retirement Diverse social network, variety of social groups Access to resources, e.g., income and time Lack of routine in daily behavior score highly on the dimension of openness to change. Although liminality facilitates the consideration of sustainability-oriented values, it is not required once sustainability-oriented values are established. For example, the Rhodes and McAdams have solidified their self-concepts and behaviors around sustainabilityoriented values. Second, households need to be able to align the ideas of sustainability-oriented values and behaviors with their existing core values. Relating back to Schwartz’s (1994) ten motivations, households’ framing of motives for sustainable behavior varies widely, including the potential for benefits to family and friends (benevolence), the world (universalism), or personal development (self-direction). Several households demonstrated no interest or sophistication in understanding of sustainability to start with. Those that were interested in such values faced a conflict in some cases. The Rancheros decided to emphasize motivations for their baby daughter’s short term security (due to safety fears of recharging at home) rather than longer term benevolence in the sense of preserving her future environment. Bill Woods’ conformity to others’ self-enhancement based values (financial savings) quashed his temporary interest in reducing his environmental impact. In contrast, Ethel Potter and the Forts discovered that sustainability-oriented values could align quite well with their core values of self-direction and benevolence, respectively, without conflicting with other core values. Third is a perception of support for sustainability within the social network. Among the four sustainability ‘‘explorer’’ households, the presence or absence of such support is associated with their final acceptance or rejection of sustainability-oriented values. The two households that eventually rejected sustainabilityoriented values described a lack of social support (or opposition) for such a transition: Billy Woods conformed to the selfenhancement financial motivations of an influential group of coworkers, and Ed Ranchero was disappointed by the lack of environmental interest among his friends and coworkers. In contrast, the two households concluding with sustainabilityoriented values perceived social support: Ethel Potter described sustainability-oriented interest among her ‘‘favorite’’ daughters and a new friend, while the Forts were pleasantly surprised to see hybrid owners welcome them to ‘‘the family.’’ Within the context of this study, each of these conditions appears to be necessary but not sufficient to facilitate a transition to sustainability-oriented values and behaviors. Of course, it is difficult to infer causality from a limited number of observations and narratives. We thus consider our exploratory findings to be J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 insights that can inform future research with additional samples in different regions and contexts of sustainability-oriented behavior. We further acknowledge that the present focus on a highly conspicuous, high-identity, environmental symbol (a plug-in hybrid vehicle version of a Toyota Prius) may not be indicative of consumer experience with less overt, less-visible pro-environmental technologies, such as programmable thermostats, home insulation, or energy efficient appliances. Future research should explore specific processes and subcomponents of each condition identified here. Research could assess whether the likeliness and strength of liminality effects differ by types of liminal self-concept, e.g., transitional versus sustained, financial, temporal, or social contexts, and across sustainability-oriented behaviors. Further effort could explore how consumers think about and frame sustainability-oriented values according to different motivations, e.g., benevolence, universalism, self-direction, and perhaps others. Potentially, some people may cast other new or existing values into a sustainability-orientation under conditions that differ from the present research. Other studies could also assess what types of social support are most important in different networks and possibly for different sustainability practices, e.g., from closer friends and family, from coworkers, or from technical experts. Our exploratory findings do support the notion that widespread sustainability-oriented values and behaviors can develop over time, subject to particular conditions. Policymakers seeking to achieve ambitious sustainability goals, such as achieving deep reductions in CO2 emissions, will want to better understand and utilize these levers and dynamics to attain support (or lack of resistance) to sustainability-oriented policy, and to facilitate the widespread adoption of sustainable behaviors and technologies across a variety of contexts. Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank the California Air Resources Board, the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, and the 18 individuals that took part in this study. References Ajzen, I., 1991. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 50, 179–211. Ajzen, I., Fishbein, M., 1980. Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behaviour. Prentice-Hall Inc., Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Atkinson, P., Hammersley, M., 1994. Ethnography and participant observation. In: Denzin, N., Lincoln, Y. (Eds.), Handbook of Qualitative Research. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks. Axsen, J., 2010. Interpersonal Influence within Car Buyers’ Social Networks: Observing Consumer Assessment of Plug-in Hybrid Electric Vehicles (PHEVs) and the Spread of Pro-Societal Values. Institute of Transportation Studies, University of California, Davis, UCD-ITS-RR-10-15. Axsen, J., Kurani, K.S., 2009. Early U.S. market for plug-In hybrid electric vehicles: anticipating consumer recharge potential and design priorities. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board 2139, 64–72. Axsen, J., Kurani, K.S., 2011. Interpersonal influence in the early plug-in hybrid market: observing social interactions with an exploratory multi-method approach. Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment 16, 150–159. Axsen, J., Kurani, K.S., 2012a. Interpersonal influence within car buyers’ social networks: applying five perspectives to plug-in hybrid vehicle drivers. Environment and Planning A 44, 1057–1065. Axsen, J., Kurani, K.S., 2012b. Social influence, consumer behavior, and low-carbon energy transitions. Annual Review of Environment and Resources 37. Axsen, J., Mountain, D.C., Jaccard, M., 2009. Combining stated and revealed choice research to simulate the neighbor effect: the case of hybrid-electric vehicles. Resource and Energy Economics 31, 221–238. Axsen, J., TyreeHageman, J., Lentz, A., 2012. Lifestyle practices and pro-environmental technology. Ecological Economics 82, 64–74. Bardi, A., Goodwin, R., 2011. The dual route to value change: individual processes and cultural moderators. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42, 271–287. 79 Bardi, A., Lee, J., Hofmann-Towfigh, N., Soutar, G., 2009. The structure of intraindividual value change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 97, 913–929. Brewer, M.B., Roccas, S., 2001. Individual values, social identity, and optimal distinctiveness. In: Sedikides, C., Brewer, M.B. (Eds.), Individual Self, Relational Self, Collective Self. Taylor & Francis, Anne Arbor, MI, pp. 219–237. Burke, P.J., 2006. Identity change. Social Psychology Quarterly 69, 81–96. Burke, P.J., Tully, J., 1977. The measurement of role idenity. Social Forces 55, 881–897. Burnett, R., 1991. Accounts and narratives. In: Montgomery, B., Duck, S. (Eds.), Studying Interpersonal Interaction. The Guildford Press, New York, pp. 121–140. Cialdini, R.B., 2003. Crafting normative messages to protect the environment. Current Directions in Psychological Science 12, 105–109. Deaux, K., Martin, D., 2003. Interpersonal networks and social categories: specifying levels of context in identity processes. Social Psychology Quarterly 66, 101–117. Dietz, T., Fitzgerald, A., Shwom, R., 2005. Environmental values. Annual Review of Environment and Resources 30, 335–372. Evans, D., Abrahamse, W., 2009. Beyond rhetoric: the possibilities of and for ‘sustainable lifestyles’. Environmental Politics 18, 486–502. Fishbein, M., Ajzen, I., 1975. Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior. AddisonWesley, Reading, MA. Geertz, C., 1973. The Interpretation of Cultures. Basic Books, Inc., New York. Gergen, K., Gergen, J., 1987. Narratives of relationships. In: Burnett, R., McGhee, P., Clarke, D. (Eds.), Accounting for Relationships: Explanation, Representation and Knowledge. Methuen, London, pp. 269–288. Giddens, A., 1991. Modernity and Self-Identity: Self and Society in the Late Modern Age. Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA. Gill, J., Crosby, L., Taylor, J., 1986. Ecological concern, attitudes, and social norms in voting behavior. Public Opintion Quarterly 50, 537–554. Heffner, R.R., Kurani, K.S., Turrentine, T.S., 2007. Symbolism in California’s early market for hybrid electric vehicles. Transportation Research Part D-Transport and Environment 12, 396–413. Hitlin, S., 2003. Values as the core of personal identity: drawing links between two theories of self. Social Psychology Quarterly 66, 118–137. Hitlin, S., Piliavin, J., 2004. Values: reviving a dormant concept. Annual Review of Sociology 30, 359–393. Hogan, B., Carrasco, J.A., Wellman, B., 2007. Visualizing personal networks: working with participant-aided sociograms. Field Methods 19, 116–144. Huijts, N., Molin, E., Steg, L., 2012. Psychological factors influencing sustainable energy technology acceptance: a review-based comprehensive framework. Renewable and Sustainable Energy Reviews 16, 525–531. Jackson, T., January 2005. Motivating Sustainable Consumption: A Review of Evidence on Consumer Behaviour and Behavioural Change. Sustainable Development Research Network, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey, UK. Kempton, W., Boster, J.S., Hartley, J.A., 1995. Environmental Values in American Culture. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. McCracken, G., 1988. The Long Interview. Sage Publications, Newbury Park, CA. Norton, B., Costanza, R., Bishop, R., 1998. The evolution of preferences: why ‘sovereign’ preferences may not lead to sustainable policies and what to do about it. Ecological Economics 24, 193–211. Oskamp, S., Harrington, M., Edwards, T., Sherwood, D., Okuda, S., Swanson, D., 1991. Factors influencing household recycling behavior. Environment and Behavior 23, 494–519. Petty, R., Cacipoppo, J., 1986. Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. Springer-Verlag, New York, NY. Reckwitz, A., 2002. Toward a theory of social practices: a development of culturalist theorizing. European Journal of Social Theory 5, 243–263. Rokeach, M., 1968. Beliefs, Attitudes and Values. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA. Ropke, I., 2009. Theories of practice: new inspiration for ecological economic studies of consumption. Ecological Economics 68, 2490–2497. Schultz, P., 2000. Empathizing with nature: the effects of perspective taking on concern for environmental issues. Journal of Social Issues 56, 391–406. Schultz, P., 2001. The structure of environmental concern: concern for self, other people and the biosphere. Journal of Environmental Psychology 21, 327–339. Schultz, P., Shriver, C., Tabanico, J., Khazian, A., 2004. Implicit connections with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology 24 . Schwartz, S.H., 1994. Are there universal aspects in the structure and content of human values? Journal of Social Issues 50, 19–45. Schwartz, S.H., Bilsky, W., 1987. Toward a universal psychological structure of human values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53, 550–562. Scott, D., Willits, F., 1994. Environmental attitudes and behavior: a Pennsylvania survey. Environment and Behavior 26, 239–260. Shove, E., 2004. Efficiency and consumption: technology and practice. Energy and Environment 15, 1053–1065. Shove, E., 2010. Beyond the ABC: climate change policy and theories of social change. Environment and Planning A 42, 1273–1285. Shove, E., Warde, A., 2002. Inconspicious consumption: the sociology of consumption, lifestyles and the environment. In: Dunlap, R., Buttel, F., Dickens, P., Gijswijt, A. (Eds.), Sociological Theory and the Environment: Classical Foundations, Contemporary Insights. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Lanham, Maryland. Spaargaren, G., 2003. Sustainable consumption: a theoretical and environmental policy perspective. Society and Natural Resources 16, 687–701. Spaargaren, G., Van Vliet, B., 2000. Lifestyles, consumption and the environment: the ecological modernization of domestic consumption. Environmental Politics 9, 50–76. 80 J. Axsen, K.S. Kurani / Global Environmental Change 23 (2013) 70–80 Steg, L., 2005. Car use: lust and must. Instrumental, symbolic and affective motives for car use. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 39, 147–162. Stern, P., Dietz, T., 1994. The value basis of environmental concern. Journal of Social Issues 50, 65–84. Stern, P., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G., Kalof, L., 1999. A value-belief norm theory of support for social movements: the case of environmental concern. Human Ecology Review 6, 81–97. Stern, P., Dietz, T., Kalof, L., Guagnano, G., 1995. Values, beliefs, and proenvironmental action: attitude formation toward emergent attitude objects. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 25, 1611–1636. Stets, J., Biga, C., 2003. Bringing identity theory into environmental sociology. Sociological Theory 21, 398–423. Stryker, S., 1980. Symbolic Interactionism: A Social Structural Version. Benjamin Cummins, Menlo Park, CA. Stryker, S., 1987. Identity theory: development and extensions. In: Yardley, K., Honess, R. (Eds.), Self and Identity: Psychosocial Perspectives. John Wiley and Sons, Oxford, UK, pp. 89–103. Swidler, A., 1986. Culture in action: symbols and strategies. American Sociological Review 51, 273–286. Train, K., 1980. The potential market for non-gasoline-powered automobiles. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 14A, 405–414. Turner, V., 1969. The Ritual Process: Structure and Anti-Structure. Aldine Publishing, Chicago, IL. Urien, B., Kilbourne, W., 2011. Generativity and self-enhancement values in ecofriendly behavioral intentions and environmentally responsible consumption behavior. Psychology and Marketing 28, 69–90. Verplanken, B., Holland, R., 2002. Motivated decision making: effects of activation and self-centrality of values on choices and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82, 434–447.