Early Childhood Development: Building Blocks for Life Briefing Paper Greater Twin Cities United Way Research & Planning July 2010 Contact: Elizabeth A. Peterson, Ph.D. petersonl@unitedwaytwincites.org 1 Table of Contents Why it Matters…………………………………………………………………………………2 Brain Development…………………………………………………………………………...2 Early Care and Attachment Developmental Prime Time External Factors: The Impact of Poverty………………………………………………….6 Prenatal Care Health Care Food Insecurity Environmental Pollutants Family Relations and Stress Neighborhood Effects Emergent Literacy: Key to Lifelong Success……………………………………………14 Where the Kids Are: Demographics & Other Data……………………………………...15 Best Practices…………………………………………………………………………………18 Home Visiting Programs Quality Childcare and Education References………………………………………………………………………………..……24 2 Early Childhood Development: Building Blocks for Life What the best and wisest parent wants for his own child, that must the community want for all its children. –John Dewey I. Why it Matters Early childhood is a critical time. These early, formative years serve as the foundation for all of life’s later endeavors. If, as a society, we fail to meet the needs of our young children, it is not just the children who suffer. We as a society suffer as well. Their success is our success. A recent report puts the cost of each high school dropout at $292,000 (Sum, Khatiwada, McLaughlin, & Palma, 2009). Dropping out of high school is not a singular event, but rather a culmination of many factors, beginning in early childhood. Supporting children and parents in these early years will have impacts into elementary school, high school, early adulthood, and beyond. Even officials of the Investment in human capital Federal Reserve Bank are weighing in—from a financial breeds economic success not perspective. They argue that early childhood only for those being educated, development programs should be viewed as economic but also for the overall development initiatives because the data around its economy. return on investment (ROI) is so compelling (Grunewald --Art Rolnick (Federal Reserve) & Rolnick, 2006; Rolnick & Grunewald, 2003). There are many factors that affect child development, resilience, and ability to thrive. Ensuring these are in place will improve the likelihood of positive outcomes despite challenging or threatening circumstances. These factors include emotionally responsive caregiving, a well-organized home environment, and well-developed intellectual and language capabilities. The best way to support children’s healthy development and longterm success is to reduce the level of stress they experience and provide support to the family (Shore, 1997). II. Brain Development Much has been made of all the advances in brain research in the last decade or so. Most of what has been learned serves to confirm what was already known and further build on it. We know that the first years of life are very important. The experiences that children have in the first 10 years influence how their brains will be wired as adults. By age 2, a child’s brain is as active as an adult’s and by age 3 the brain is more than twice as active as an adult’s—and stays that way for the first 10 years of life. Not only are children’s brains more active, they also have higher levels of neurotransmitters which assist in the formation of synapses. This and other attributes of the early brain (e.g., synapse density, glucose utilization) suggest that young children are primed for learning—particularly as infants and toddlers (Shore, 1997). 3 Early Care and Attachment The ways that parents, families, Brain research has also shown the critical role that and other caregivers relate and early caregiving plays. This care has a long-lasting respond to young children, and the ways that they mediate their impact on development, including ability to learn children’s contact with the as well as capacity to regulate emotions. Babies environment, directly affect the thrive when they receive warm responsive care, formation of neural pathways. and there are biological mechanisms responsible --Rima Shore for babies’ positive responses to attentive caregiving. When parents mimic and reinforce their infants’ positive emotional responses (e.g., smiling, laughing. babbling), they are influencing the development of patterns of connected neurons. A strong, secure attachment to a parent or other nurturing caregiver has a lasting protective function. This protective factor helps to “immunize” infants against the adverse effects of stress and trauma they will certainly experience at some point in their lives. Research has found that children respond to stress by releasing cortisol, a steroid hormone. Children with chronically high levels of cortisol are likely to experience more developmental delays than other children. But children who receive warm, responsive, nurturing care in the first year of life are less likely to respond to stress by producing cortisol. This early attachment and the attending protective factor has a long-term effect. Elementary school children who experienced strong attachment as infants are less likely to exhibit behavior problems when they encounter stressful situations. In general, babies with secure attachment tend to thrive and show more resilience throughout their lives, including greater self-reliance, greater capacity for emotional regulation, and stronger social competence, including empathy (Sroufe, 2005). Indeed, few things are of greater importance than attachment in early childhood. According to Alan Sroufe (2005), a University of Minnesota professor who has studied attachment for many years: It [attachment] is an organizing core in development that is always integrated with later experience and never lost. . . . Infant attachment is critical, both because of its place in initiating pathways of development and because of its connection with so many critical developmental functions—social relatedness, arousal modulation, emotional regulation, and curiosity, to name just a few. Attachment experiences remain, even in this complex view, vital in the formation of the person. (p.365) Shore (1997) reports that the kind of attachment infants form with their primary caregiver by age 1 can predict: Teacher ratings of child performance Behavior problems Quality of relationships with peers in preschool Social competency in a summer camp setting School achievement at age 16 There are three commonly used attachment classifications: secure, anxious-avoidant, and anxious-resistant. Simply put, a child with secure attachment behaves in a manner that reflects a trusting relationship with the caregiver. The caregiver is viewed as a reliable source of love: The infant is unhappy when she leaves and comforted when she 4 returns. A child with anxious-resistant attachment typically is not comforted by the caregiver. For example, if picked up, the child may resist by biting, screaming, whining, and arching his or her back. A child with anxious avoidant attachment tends to behave as if the caregiver isn’t present (Gopnik, 2009; Shore, 1997; Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005). The quality of attachment is based primarily on the quality of care the child receives— most particularly caregiver responsivity in the first year. Mothers of securely attached infants are consistently more cooperative and sensitive when interacting with their infants in feeding and play situations. They are more responsive to their baby’s cries and they tend to hold their baby in a more affectionate manner (Sroufe et al., 2005). Developmental Prime Time Much attention has been given in the last few For the vast majority of kids in normal years to “critical periods” for development, with homes, all they will need in order to the implication that if it doesn’t happen now, the develop strong brain architecture is the child is lost forever. In fact, the brain has kind of rich experience they will get from everyday interactions. amazing plasticity and ability to compensate for --National Scientific Council on the Developing injury or lost opportunity. With few exceptions, Child the window of opportunity remains open for years (National Scientific Council on the Developing Child, 2007; Shore, 1997; Wilder Research, 2008). Developmental Domain Visual & Auditory Development Window of Opportunity 0-5 years Components Vision, 0-6 months Hearing, 0-6 months Language Development 0-10 years Learn language, 0-3 years Physical & Motor Development 0-12 years Large motor skills Fine motor skills Emotional & Social Development 0-12 years Emotional attachment Emotional intelligence Children Need…. Objects in a variety of shapes and colors, at varying distances. Movement. A variety of sounds. A language-rich environment— talk, sing, and read to them. Respond to their babbling. Opportunities for gross and fine movements: balls, blocks, crayons. Patient caregivers. Early nurturing to learn empathy, happiness, hopefulness, and resiliency. Children are born ready to learn and will learn whatever comes next in their developmental continuum. They’re like little (and growing) learning machines. It is important to point out that there is no specific point in time where children are “ready to learn.” Instead, readiness falls along a continuum throughout childhood and the school years, rather than at any single point in time. There is a prolonged neurodevelopmental timetable throughout childhood and adolescence in which the brain exhibits a gradual development (Peterson, 1994). Readiness is a process, with many phases, rather than a singular event, or switch (i.e., ready, not ready). 5 III. External Factors: The Impact of Poverty The negative effects of poverty are pervasive, cumulative, and increase with age (Shore, 1997). Children who are raised in poverty show a negative impact even when they are born healthy and free of medical problems. They tend to show gradual declines in mental, motor, and socio-emotional development; they have poorer quality relationships with their caregivers; and they are more likely to exhibit anxious attachment. In preschool, they are more likely to have problems getting along with other children and functioning on their own. By the time they start school they are more likely to need special education services and as they progress through school they are more likely to be held back. Everyone is well aware of the achievement gap between white students and students of color (i.e., African Americans, American Indians, and Hispanics). This achievement gap is highly correlated to parental education and family income. When these two demographic variables are examined, much (though not all) of the variation between racial/ethnic groups is explained. What many people do not realize, however, is that the gap is already in place before children enter kindergarten (Fryer & Levitt, 2004, 2006). Numerous studies have found that most of the inequality in cognitive skills and differences in behavior come from family and neighborhood sources rather than from schools (Berliner, 2009). Evans and Schamberg (2009) provide evidence that living in poverty results in chronically elevated physiological stress, which in turn affects working memory. Working memory is essential to language comprehension, reading, and problem solving; it is a critical prerequisite for long-term storage of information. The longer the period of childhood poverty, the higher the stress load is during childhood, the greater the longterm effect on working memory. Children growing up in poverty are, as a rule, exposed to more risk factors than children growing up in middle-income households. Evans and English (2002) report that exposure to one risk factor generally has a negligible impact on children, while exposure to two or more risks has a cumulative, adverse psychological impact. Not surprisingly, the environment of poverty is characterized by exposure to cumulative, adverse, physical and social stressors. The housing is noisier, more crowded, and of lower quality. People living in poverty experience elevated levels of family turmoil, greater child-family separation, and higher levels of violence. According to Dearing (2008), the economic costs of childhood poverty in the United States could be as high as $500 billion a year—about 4% of the U.S. gross domestic product. The impact of poverty manifests in many ways, including the avenues of prenatal care, health care, food insecurity, environmental pollutants, family relations and family stress, and neighborhood characteristics. Each of these factors is discussed in turn. 6 Prenatal Care Disparities based on poverty begin even before birth. For example, preterm infants are born to African-Americans 58% more frequently than they are to Whites and very preterm infants are born to African-Americans 246% more frequently. In contrast, Hispanics, who have poverty and uninsurance rates comparable to African-Americans, typically have longer gestation periods and infants that often weigh more at birth than their white counterparts (Berliner, 2009). This is sometimes referred to as “the Latina Paradox.” Researchers in the public health field (McGlade, Saha, & Dahlstrom, 2004) attribute the positive outcomes to informal systems of prenatal care, composed of family, friends, community members, and lay health workers. They suggest that “this informal system confers protective factors that provide a behavioral context for healthy births” (p. 2062). They further note that Latinas in the United States are losing this protection and suggest it could be maintained through community-based informal care systems. Birth weight is important because children born at low birth weight (< 2500 g), very low birth weight (< 1500 g), and extremely low birth weight (< 1000 g) are more likely to have cognitive and behavioral problems—especially those born at very low or extremely low birth weights. Part of the problem may relate to higher use of alcohol and cigarettes frequently found in poor neighborhoods—the most consistent findings of impairment in children are those related to mothers who smoked and drank throughout pregnancy (Berliner, 2009). Alcohol intake during pregnancy is one of the most common causes of brain damage before birth. Even one drink a day is discouraged, as there are no known safe alcohol intake levels during pregnancy (CDC, 2009; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2005). Alcohol can cause a wide range of damage to the gestating child, resulting in many different disorders (referred to as the fetal alcohol spectrum disorder). The damage varies depending on the amount of alcohol consumed as well as the particular time in the pregnancy. Mild damage includes decreased intellectual function, visual problems, and high pain tolerance. Prevalence is difficult to estimate, though some studies indicate that up to 20% of children are exposed to alcohol prenatally. Epidemiological studies estimate an overall fetal alcohol incidence of 1 in 500 births, occurring in 2-6 births per 1,000 Caucasians; 6 per 1,000 African-Americans; and up to 20 per 1,000 American Indians (Minnesota Department of Health, 2005). In addition to preterm births and alcohol use, maternal obesity can affect the gestating baby, and weight problems are more prevalent in low-income populations, as are stress and anxiety during pregnancy. Mothers who are anxious, depressed, or both tend to have children with a higher rate of sleep disturbances and temperament and attention disorders (Berliner, 2009). Other prenatal conditions can affect the long-term health and ability of the child as well: If flu shots are not free, poor people do not receive them as often as higher income people. Influenza during pregnancy is associated with higher rates of schizophrenia, and influenza in the first trimester is associated with rates of schizophrenia seven times the normal rate in the population (Berliner, 2009). 7 Health Care Families living in poverty are much more likely to have unmet health needs. Not surprisingly, health coverage is highly correlated to income: Families with higher incomes are much more likely to have health insurance than low-income families. Income is also highly correlated with race, and as can be seen in the accompanying graphic, American Indians, Blacks, and Hispanics have uninsurance rates two to three times higher than Whites and Asians. Source: Minnesota Department of Health, 2010. People lacking health care coverage are much more likely to forego needed medical care because of the cost—including not filling prescriptions or seeing a doctor when sick. This problem has been exacerbated in the current economic downturn: A September 2009 survey conducted by the Kaiser Family Foundation found that over half (56%) of all households (not just those uninsured or underinsured) are putting off medical care because of costs. Dental care is an area of critical importance. Children in households at or below poverty are much less likely to obtain annual dental care—more than 1 in 3 had not seen a dentist in the last year, a rate 2.5 times greater than children in upper income households. This is important because untreated dental cavities interfere with learning (Berliner, 2009) as pain interferes with focus and dampens concentration. If cavities go untreated, more serious conditions could evolve, resulting in school absenteeism. Children from lower income households are also more likely to have undiagnosed vision problems. However, even when vision problems are identified, follow-up care is less likely. Berliner (2009) reports that low-income families follow up on problems uncovered in school vision screenings at about half the rate of more affluent families. The bottom line is: Children in low-income families are six times more likely to be in less than optimal health compared to their higher income peers, and are more likely to experience a wide variety of illnesses and injuries (Berliner, 2009). These illnesses and injuries can impact development, learning, and the long-term trajectory of the child. 8 Food Insecurity Proper nutrition is critical, most especially in the first few years of development. A nutritious diet early in life results in greater intellectual functioning and higher levels of education later in life (Berliner, 2009). The United States Department of Agriculture published a report in September of 2009, focusing specifically on food insecurity in children. According to the USDA (2009), food insecurity is associated with numerous negative outcomes for children, as detailed below: Impact of Food Insecurity on Children Poorer health More stomach aches, frequent headaches, and colds Higher hospitalization rates Iron deficiency anemia Behavioral problems Lower physical functioning Poorer psychosocial functioning and psychosocial development Higher rates of depressive disorder and suicidal symptoms More anxiety and depression Larger numbers of chronic health conditions More “internalizing” behavior problems Lower math achievement and other achievement gains in kindergarten Lower math and reading gains from kindergarten to third grade Lower arithmetic scores and higher likelihood of repeating a grade The USDA study found an average food insecurity rate for children of 8.7% nationally and 5.7% in Minnesota. The organization Feeding America (2009) did further analyses of the USDA data and developed estimates of food insecurity specifically for children under age 5. They estimate that 17.3% of children under 5 are food Food Insecurity (2007) insecure nationally and the 17.3% rate in Minnesota is estimated at 13% (with a range from 10% 13.0% to 16% when standard error is 11.0% 9.5% taken into consideration). 8.7% Nationally, food insecurity 5.7% increased to 14.6% in 2008 from 11.0% in 2007 (USDA, 2008, 2009). Updated data are not available for children, but it All Households Children under 18 Children under 5* is likely their food insecurity U.S. Minnesota rates have increased on a parallel track. *Analysis conducted by Feeding America, 2009. Not surprisingly, food insecurity is much more prevalent among low-income households than in moderate- or high-income households. In addition, compared to the general population, food insecurity is nearly three times as common in households with children headed by single women; twice as common in Black households; and almost twice as 9 common in Hispanic households (Berliner, 2009). This is a critical issue for the community. Food insecurity can have serious, long-term consequences for babies and toddlers, putting them at risk for poor health, increased hospitalizations, and developmental delays. For children under age 3, it is likely that nutritional deficiencies will affect their entire school career as well as their employment experiences (Berliner, 2009). According to Dr. J. Larry Brown (Brown, Beardslee, & Prothrow-Stith, 2008), from the Harvard School of Public Health and Founding Director of the Center on Hunger and Poverty: There exists no “safe” level of inadequate nutrition for healthy, growing children. . . . . When children attend school inadequately nourished, their bodies conserve the limited food energy that is available. Energy is first reserved for critical organ functions. If sufficient energy remains, it then is allocated for growth. The last priority is for social activity and learning. As a result, undernourished children become more apathetic and have impaired cognitive capacity. Environmental Pollutants Environmental pollutants do not affect all people equally. The two major sources of mercury in the environment are coal-fired power plants and municipal waste incinerators, and poor communities are much more likely to be located near these two mercury emitters than middle- or upper-class communities. Mercury contamination results in cognitive and behavior problems, including hyperactivity and loss of focus (Berliner, 2009). Impact of Lead Poisoning Lead is one of the most well-known pollutants affecting children. Deteriorated lead paint and leadcontaminated house dust are found in approximately 4.5 million homes with young children (Berliner, 2009). These older buildings (built before 1950) are now home to large numbers of poor and minority children. There is no safe level of lead in the human body. Lead poisoning, even at low levels, can have lifelong effects and is associated with negative outcomes for children, including impaired cognitive, motor, behavioral, and physical abilities, including learning disabilities, attention deficit disorder, lower IQ, antisocial Source: Olympio et. al., 2009 behavior, headaches, and seizures (Binns, Campbell, & Brown, 2007; Olympio, Gonçalves, Günther, & Bechara, 2009). 10 Also of concern are PCBs (associated with decreased cognitive function in early childhood) and pesticides. Air quality is also an issue and is reflected in increased hospital admissions for asthma. Inner cities, where the poor are most frequently concentrated, tend to have the highest levels of smog, which in turn affects the health of these community residents to a much greater extent than is experienced in the wealthier suburbs (Berliner, 2009). Family Relations and Stress Violence It is estimated that some sort of serious family violence occurs annually in 10 to 20 percent of families. This violence is more likely to be directed against females than males and it correlates with poverty such that it is more common in lower income households (Berliner, 2009). This is not to say that violence does not occur in higher income families, but rather that it is more prevalent in lower income families. Nor is this a surprising finding, given the multiple stresses that families in poverty experience. Family violence, even if not directed at children, has a strong effect on children. Parents in a violent, volatile relationship are less able to nurture the development of their offspring. Parental victims of violence may be emotionally withdrawn (which with infants could impede attachment). Children who grow up in violent families frequently have social and emotional problems, including higher rates of depression, aggressive behavior, and anxiety, and as they enter school are more likely to have lower social competence and poorer academic performance. Exposure to household violence has also been found to impact children’s physical health, putting them at greater risk for allergies, asthma, gastrointestinal problems, headaches, and flu (Berliner, 2009). Mental Illness and Maternal Depression Familial mental illness can also have a negative impact on child development. Maternal depression, in particular, has been found to have significant effects on development. Mental Illness & Poverty Research has found that mental illness is more prevalent in low-income households than in wealthy households. While the base rate of mental illness is around 3% in wealthier communities, it is more than twice that (7%) in low-income communities. It is not only that mental illness tends to make people poor (increased medical costs as well as potential difficulty getting and keeping a job) but poverty on its own has been found to be a causal factor in making people mentally ill. --David C. Berliner In fact, infants as young as 3 months of age are able to detect depression in their mothers, potentially compromising the infant’s social, emotional, and cognitive functioning (Weinberg & Tronick, 1998). According to Knitzer, Theberge, and Johnson (2008), maternal depression is a significant risk factor affecting the well-being and school readiness of children. Maternal depression is relatively widespread, and like mental illness in general (see textbox), is disproportionately represented in low-income households across all ethnicities. Maternal depression affects parenting in two primary ways: It impedes the development of healthy relationships and it compromises the ability of the mother to perform basic parenting functions. 11 The impact of maternal depression on children varies according to the child’s age when it occurs: Maternal depression during infancy has a greater impact on a child’s development than depression at later ages. The impact on the child is also dependent on the severity and length of the depression (Knitzer, Theberge, & Johnson, 2008). The negative effects of maternal depression on the developing child can start even as early as pregnancy. Maternal depression is linked to poor birth outcomes such as low birth weight, prematurity, and obstetric complications. Maternal depression while the child is an infant predicts increased cortisol levels at preschool ages, and as previously discussed (see ”Early Care and Attachment”) high cortisol levels are associated with anxiety, withdrawal, and decreased social competence (Knitzer, Theberge, & Johnson, 2008). Maternal depression is also linked to negative relationships in early childhood as well as reduced language ability (which is key to early school success): Depressed mothers lack the energy to carry out consistent routines such as reading to their children. In addition, their ability to have fun with their children, performing the simple but critically important elements of singing, playing, and cuddling, is severely compromised (Knitzer, Theberge, & Johnson, 2008). It is thus, perhaps, not surprising that 3-year-old children whose mothers were depressed in their infancy perform more poorly on cognitive and behavioral tasks. Maternal depression is also linked to health and safety concerns: Depressed mothers are less likely to breastfeed, and even when they do breastfeed they tend to do so for shorter periods of time. Depressed mothers are also less likely to follow the back-tosleep guidelines for prevention of SIDS, and are less likely to use basic safety precautions such as socket covers and car seats. They are also less likely to follow routine preventive health practices and are more challenged when managing chronic health conditions such as asthma. The impact of maternal depression should not be underestimated. It carries a stronger risk of child behavior problems than many other risks that are frequently tracked, including smoking, binge drinking, and emotional as well as physical domestic violence (Knitzer, Theberge, & Johnson, 2008). As mentioned, mothers in low-income households are more likely to experience depression. Overall, about 5% of children in 2004 had a parent with some depressive symptoms. That increases to about 15% in families receiving food stamps, and to 20% for families receiving Temporary Assistance for Needy Families (Child Trends Data Bank, 2009). Often women suffering from maternal depression are reluctant to seek treatment because of the stigma associated with depression and seeking help. There is also a general distrust of mental health agencies, and to further complicate things, they are often reluctant to take medications because they fear it will interfere with their parenting (Knitzer, Theberge, & Johnson, 2008). 12 Neighborhood Effects Where you live affects how well you learn. Living in a severely disadvantaged Research studies have confirmed that neighborhood reduces the later neighborhood economic status is strongly verbal ability of Black children on average by ≈ 4 points, a magnitude associated with children’s acquisition of skills in that rivals missing a year or more general and language in particular (Berliner, of schooling. 2009; Sampson, Sharkey, & Raudenbush, 2008; --Sampson, Sharkey, & Raudenbush Sastry & Pebley, 2008; Sharkey, 2009). In fact, poverty results in about a quarter of a standard deviation loss in verbal ability. Residing in neighborhoods of concentrated poverty (bottom quartile of income) has a cumulative and negative effect on verbal development and achievement. Studies suggest that living in a low-income neighborhood may have a greater effect on inequality in school test scores than coming from a low-income family (Berliner, 2009). It appears that the primary factor is the absence of more affluent neighbors rather than the presence of low-income neighbors. For children whose family income is in the top three quintiles, growing up in a highpoverty neighborhood versus a low-poverty neighborhood (e.g., a poverty rate of 25% vs. 5%) increases the chances of downward mobility by 52% (Sharkey, 2009). There is a strong racial component here—only a small percentage of White children live in highpoverty neighborhoods while a majority of Black children do: Approximately two-thirds of Black children grow up in these high-poverty neighborhoods compared to about 5% of White children. Even health is affected by neighborhood location (see “Environmental Pollutants”). Why? In part it is due to the lack of opportunities found in poor, racially segregated urban neighborhoods. Schools are often underfunded, and residents of poor neighborhoods have less political influence than residents of affluent neighborhoods. The quality of public amenities is poorer, the effectiveness of the police is compromised by a number of factors including distrust, and there is much greater exposure to violence, gangs, toxic soil, and polluted air (Sharkey, 2009). Additionally, poor neighborhoods are often stressful and hazardous. Parents may be more focused on their children’s safety than their cognitive development. The children, in turn, are less likely to be exposed to well-educated, successful, adult role models who provide examples of the value in verbal, reading, and problem-solving skills. Children’s cognitive and verbal skills may also be affected by the language environment to which they are exposed in their neighborhood: In concentrated poverty neighborhoods, children may be less exposed to adults and peers who speak standard English and also less exposed to hearing language in general due to the restrictions in social interaction imposed by parents concerned about safety (Sastry & Pebley, 2008). 13 IV. Emergent Literacy: Key to Lifelong Success Learning to read starts a child on the long pathway to success. It is particularly important that a child is able to read by grade 3, since that is the turning point when schools begin to expect children to read to learn. But From the child’s first, halting attempts to learning to read begins much earlier than decipher letters, the experience of reading third grade or even kindergarten. As soon as is not so much an end in itself as it is our an infant can sit on a caregiver’s lap, the act best vehicle to a transformed mind, and, of learning to read may begin. Sitting on a literally and figuratively, to a changed lap, surrounded by love, being read to: This brain. --Maryanne Wolf is the beginning of a long process called emergent or early literacy. Children are not born knowing that there is a name, or label, for everything in the world. But children gradually begin to name the parts of their world—typically beginning with the people who care for them. The “aha!” moment that everything has its own name usually comes at around 18 months of age. Maryanne Wolf, professor of child development at Tufts University, director of the Center for Reading and Language Research, and author of Proust and the Squid: The Story and Science of the Reading Brain, writes, [T]he more children are spoken to, the more they will understand oral language. The more children are read to, the more they understand all the language around them, and the more developed their vocabulary becomes. This intertwining of oral language, cognition, and written language makes early childhood one of the richest times for language growth (p. 84). Reading to a child can make a significant difference: Children who are “well-read-to” (at least five times a week), when asked to tell a story, used more literary language than unread to children, and they also used more sophisticated syntactic forms, longer phrases, and relative clauses. They were also better able to understand the oral and written language of others—an important foundation for the comprehension skills that will develop in the coming years (Wolf, 2008). Children who are read to are gaining vocabulary skills, but more than that, they begin to practice the complex use of analogy (e.g., “Her cheeks were like rose petals”). Simple similes such as those found in Curious George aid the child in performing such sophisticated cognitive operations as size-based comparisons and depth perception (Wolf, 2008). Reading depends to a great degree on biological development and the brain’s ability to connect and integrate various sources of information (visual, auditory, linguistic, and conceptual). The speed with which this occurs depends on the sheathing wrapped around a brain cell’s axons. For most children, this is not sufficiently developed until school age—between 5 and 7 years. On the road to reading, however, they gradually learn that words make up sentences, and syllables make up words, and sounds (phonemes) make up syllables. 14 Poetry and Mother Goose rhymes help children learn phonemes. Children ages 4 and 5 are beginning to differentiate the first sounds of words—one of the reasons the Dr. Seuss books are so popular: Go, Dog, Go! and “I do not like green eggs and ham. I do not like them Sam I am” are catchy phrases with built-in redundancies that help children parse phonemes. This, in turn, facilitates learning to read (Wolf, 2008). Children who grow up without these literary advantages are already behind when they enter kindergarten. The language and environment children grow up in has a significant impact. A frequently cited study by Hart and Risley (1995, 2003) found huge language differences as early as age 3: At 3 years of age, children growing up in families receiving welfare had an average vocabulary size of 525 words. Children in working-class families had an average vocabulary size of 749 words, and “The importance of simply children in professional families had an average being talked to, read to, and vocabulary size of 1,116 words. Extrapolating from their listened to is much of what early data, Hart and Risley estimate that by age 5, children language development is all from impoverished language environments have heard about.” 32 million fewer words spoken to them than the -Maryanne Wolf average middle-class child. Hart and Risley further found that vocabulary use at age 3 was predictive of language skill at ages 9 and 10. Children who come to kindergarten in the bottom quartile generally remain behind the other children—both in vocabulary growth and reading comprehension (Wolf, 2008). According to Hart and Risley (2003), “Estimating the hours of intervention needed to equalize children’s early experience makes clear the enormity of the effort that would be required to change children’s lives. And the longer the effort is put off, the less possible the change becomes.” V. Where the Kids Are: Demographics & Other Data There are approximately 241,000 children ages 0-5 in the nine-county area. Of those, 30,505 (12.7%) live at or below 100% of poverty, and 68,758 (28.5%) live below 200% of poverty. (Poverty numbers do not include foster children, children in institutional group quarters, and homeless children without shelter.) Source: U.S. Census (2008). 15 Kindergarten Readiness The Minnesota Department of Education conducts an annual assessment of kindergarten readiness based on a representative sample of Minnesota kindergartners as they enter school in the fall. They categorize children in five domains, detailed below. Five Domains of Kindergarten Readiness Assessment Physical development (e.g., self-care tasks, eye-hand coordination) The arts (e.g., participates in group music experiences, uses a variety of art materials) Personal and social development (e.g., interacts with others, shows curiosity, empathy and caring, self-direction) Mathematical thinking (e.g., beginning shape recognition, understanding of quantity) Language and literacy (e.g., comprehends stories read aloud, speaks clearly, beginning understanding of concepts about print) Each domain has a number of indicators. For each indicator within a domain, a child is rated as Not Yet, In Process, or Proficient. A rating of Not Yet means that a child cannot perform the indicator (i.e., the indicator represents a skill, knowledge, behavior, or accomplishment the child has not yet acquired). A rating of In Process means that the child does not yet consistently perform the indicator, but there is some presence of the skill, knowledge, behavior, or accomplishment. It is in development but not yet demonstrated reliably or consistently. A rating of Proficient means that the child reliably and consistently demonstrates the skill, knowledge, behavior, or accomplishment. More than 6,000 kindergartners from 96 randomly selected schools were assessed in the 2008 assessment, representing more than 10% of the kindergartners starting the 2008-2009 school year. Overall, the large majority of children are rated either “in process” or “proficient.” Between 4% and 13% were assessed as “not yet.” 16 Kindergarten Readiness by Domain 61% 49% 44% 49% 47% Proficient In Process Not Yet 4% 35% Physical Development 43% 41% 43% 41% 8% 11% 12% 13% The Arts Personal & Social Development Mathematical Thinking Language & Literacy Source: Minnesota Department of Education (2009). As can be seen, children are most likely to be at least in process in the domain of physical development. The largest proportion of children assessed “not yet” fell into the language and literacy domain. These results are reasonably consistent with the findings of prior years. There is a small trend towards an increase in students assessed “not yet,” but the increase is not outside the margin of sampling error (i.e., it could represent sampling error rather than an actual change from the prior years). Being “in process” is not a bad thing. It is to be expected that young children will develop at different paces, showing proficiency in some areas while others are still in process. Greater concern and intervention is warranted for those “not yet” developing proficiency. 17 VI. Best Practices Everyone wants a magic bullet, the perfect program, to make sure children enter kindergarten ready and eager to learn. There is no magic bullet. The causes and contributors to childhood problems are manifold and complex. Early Childhood Development Programs are a good start for many children, but health care and external environments also need to be addressed. Early Intervention: Children from families with the least formal education generally experience the greatest cognitive benefits from intervention programs. Importantly, the impact of early intervention is long-lasting, especially when there is follow-up throughout the elementary school years (Shore, 1997). Studies of model preschool A recent study by David Deming (2009) looked at the interventions find dramatic long-term benefits of Head Start programming. Deming improvements in long-term found that there tends to be a strong fading out in terms outcomes among program of test scores as children get older. However, in spite of participants, despite rapid fadetest score fading, the long-term impacts were strong: out of test score gains. improved high school graduation rates, college --David Deming attendance, higher employment, better health, and lower rates of teen parenthood. In fact, African-American children, who show particularly strong fade-out on test scores also show the largest long-term gains. (Deming, 2009). Deming concludes that test score gains are an incomplete measure of long-term benefits and points out that the connection between test score gains and improvements in adult outcomes is not well understood. Home-Visiting Programs Programs for parents of young children hold considerable promise for improving children’s lives—in the short run as well as long term. Delivering services to families in their own homes means that parents do not have to arrange transportation, childcare, or take time off from work. It also provides opportunity for more whole-family involvement, personalized service, individual attention, and rapport building (Sweet & Appelbaum, 2004). In general, home visitors focus on the parents rather than the children, encouraging and training parents to relate to and help their children. Home-visiting programs have been found to produce consistent and lasting results when delivered in a manner consistent with research-based best practices. A focus on parents and parenting is essential because hundreds of studies have found evidence of the relationship between early prenatal care and care in the first years of life and children’s intellectual, behavioral, and emotional outcomes. Sensitive, responsive care in the first months of life is critically important, and home visiting can help ensure that happens. When parents understand their infants and respond in ways that meet the baby’s needs, children are more likely to develop secure infant attachment and exhibit better behavioral and emotional adjustment later in life (Olds, Sadler, & Kitzman, 2007). It is important to note that home visiting is a strategy for delivering a service rather than a specific type of intervention. Home visiting programs differ and have been found to be effective for different demographic groups (e.g., single teen moms, specific ethnic groups, low-income parents); targeting different behaviors or outcomes (e.g., child abuse, school readiness, parental employment), and vary by length and intensity of 18 services, types of services provided, methods of recruitment, and who delivers the services. The most effective home-visiting programs for helping parents and children are those that start early (ideally during pregnancy), maintain visits for at least three years, visit often—at least weekly for the first six months, and have clear, focused goals and materials that support those goals. The table below details the positive outcomes associated with well-structured, professionally developed home-visiting programs as well as key components of homevisiting programs that have been found to obtain these outcomes. Positive Outcomes Associated with Home-Visiting Programs Improved maternal health (prenatally and postnatally) Improved child health Improved parents’ abilities to manage their lives Improved parents’ abilities to care for their children Lower rates of involvement in the criminal justice system (parents and children) Less harsh parenting behaviors Lower infant mortality rates Prevention of child abuse and neglect Improved child safety and fewer injuries Improved cognitive and language development Better emotional engagement with parents More sustained attention with objects Less aggressive behavior Reduced domestic violence Fewer subsequent pregnancies and births Increased inter-birth intervals Increased parental employment Increased maternal education Reduction in reliance on welfare Key Components of Successful Programs Begin during pregnancy Focus on at-risk families Focus on prevention (of low birth weight, child abuse, reliance on public assistance, learning delays, etc.) Duration of at least three years (recommend 3-5) More intensive visitation frequency at outset (weekly for the first six months) Focus on parenting Clear program goals with curricula and materials to support the goals Rigorous training of supervisors and home visitors Cultural competency Developmental screenings for the children Connect family to other resources in the community Sources: Family Strengthening Policy Center, 2007; Isaacs, 2008; Olds, 2008; Olds et al., 2007; Sweet & Appelbaum, 2004; 19 Rigorous training of home visitors is a critical element. One study found that homevisited children actually performed worse on measures of child development than their control-group counterparts. As Olds et al. (2007) state, “In spite of our best intentions, it is possible to harm those we attempt to help” (p.385). This is why training and cultural competence on the part of home visitors is so important. Home visiting generally delivers greater benefits for high-risk families than low-risk families (Olds, 2008). This is in part because many of the benefits that accrue (e.g., avoiding welfare dependence, lower rates of substance abuse, less involvement in criminal behavior) were not risk factors in the lives of lower risk (generally higher income) groups. One study found that while the cost to implement the program was the same for low-risk and high-risk families ($7,271), the benefits accruing to the low-risk group totaled $9,151 while the benefits accruing to the high-risk group totaled $41,419 (Family Strengthening Policy Center, 2007). Home visiting is also one key avenue for addressing maternal depression, which can have devastating and long-term impacts on children. Addressing Maternal/Parental Depression Promote early identification and screening. For lowincome women, make treatment more accessible. Educating parents about the effects of their depression on their children could encourage mothers who would otherwise be resistant to seek treatment. Knitzer, Theberge, and Johnson (2008) point out one potentially powerful but underutilized strategy to address parental depression is to embed explicit interventions into early childhood programs, most particularly home-visiting programs. They also recommend that efforts to address maternal depression include the following: Three types of strategies commonly used to address depression in the context of parenting young children: 1. 2. 3. Screening and follow up for women, usually in ob-gyn or pediatric practices. Targeted interventions to reduce depression and improve parenting, such as home visiting. Promoting awareness about the impact of maternal depression and what to do about it. Target audiences include general public, low-income communities, and early childhood and health practitioners. Link services and supports for parents and children through formal as well as informal strategies. Provide training and support to home visitors, --Knitzer, Theberge, & Johnson (2008) teachers, and childcare providers to help families get support and treatment for depression. Help parents address specific parenting challenges related to depression and other difficulties. Ensure that children in high-risk families have access to high-quality childcare programs to reinforce social and emotional skills and provide early learning opportunities. Provide clinical treatment when it is needed, in settings that families trust. Quality Childcare and Education Educationally enriched childcare can have long-lasting effects on the cognition and behavior of children. Quality childcare and education is particularly effective when it is coupled with or follows on the heels of early home visiting. 20 Research has found that early childhood education is particularly beneficial to children from low-resource families, while benefits are less clear for children from middle- and upper-class families. Low-resource families typically have limited parental education, very low family incomes, and/or parents unable to consistently provide high-quality learning opportunities for normal brain and behavioral development (Wilder Research, 2008). As more children spend more time in childcare (20-35 hours/week for most children), concerns have arisen about the quality of care provided. There is reason for concern: Not all childcare providers are equally skilled or trained, and at least one study found that only 12% to 14% of children are in childcare arrangements that promote their growth and learning (Shore, 1997). The hallmark of quality for early childcare is not very different from quality care from parents: warm, responsive, consistent, caregiving geared towards the needs of each individual child. The table below details short- and long-term outcomes of successful quality childcare and early preschool programs as well as the key components of the kinds of programs that obtain these outcomes. Outcomes of Successful Quality Childcare Improved language development and literacy Higher cognitive skills Improved fine skills development Higher levels of school readiness Lower rates of special education placement Lower rates of grade retention Higher rates of school completion Higher rates of college attendance Lower incidence of juvenile arrest Lower rates of incarceration as adults Lower rates of child abuse and neglect Fewer months on public aid (men) Higher rates of full-time employment Key Components of Quality Programs Intensive (half- or full-day five days a week) Multiple years High levels of staff education and specialized training Low staff turnover High staff-to-child ratios Smaller group sizes Administrative stability High levels of staff compensation Educational enrichment Comprehensive parent involvement Instructions to parents on effective child rearing Family support services Health services Sources for above: Ramey, Campbell, & Blair, 1998; Reynolds et al., 2001; Reynolds et al., 2007; Schweinhart et al., 2005; Shore, 1997; Temple, Reynolds, & Miedel, 2000. 21 Numerous studies have examined the public and private return on investment (ROI) for early childhood programs. The High/Scope Perry Preschool program has received particular attention because of its extensive longitudinal nature (program participants and controls have been tracked through age 40). Most studies have found extremely high rates of return: Rolnick and Grunewald (2003) estimate a 16% rate of return, while Belfield, Nores, Barnett, and Schweinhart (2006) estimate a 17% rate of return. A recent analysis by Heckman, Moon, Pinto, Savelyev, and Yavitz (2009) applied more rigorous and conservative methodologies but still found a rate of return ranging between 7% and 10%. A word of caution: Perry Preschool participants were highly disadvantaged, very lowincome, African-American children with IQs between 70 and 85 (average IQ is 100), and much of the ROI (> 90%) is based on savings from crime reduction, specifically for male participants. Caution should be used in generalizing these data to broader populations. Regardless of the actual size of the return on investment, it is clear that the earlier the investment takes place, the higher the rate of return: From: Heckman (2006). 22 Conclusion The early childhood years are critically important: The stronger the foundation built in the first few years of life, the greater the likelihood of long-term success and happiness. Poverty has an insidious and pervasive impact on this development, weaving through many areas of the child’s life: It limits their access to developmental stimulation and heightens their exposure to stress—in their physical as well as their psychological environment (Dearing, 2008). It carries a cost for society as well. Dearing reports the economic cost of childhood poverty is as high as $500 billion a year—4% of the United States gross domestic product. Early intervention can help. Research has found two strategies that are particularly effective: home visiting, to educate parents; and high-quality early childcare and education. When these two strategies are bundled and child and family support extends through the elementary school years, long-term success is even more likely. The earlier the intervention begins, the more likely it is to be effective and the greater the return on investment. 23 References Belfield, C. R., Nores, M., Barnett, S., & Schweinhart, L. (2006). The High/Scope Perry Preschool program: Cost-benefit analysis using data from the age-40 followup. Journal of Human Resources, 41(1), 162-190. Berliner, D. (2009). Poverty and Potential: Out-of-School Factors and School Success. East Lansing, MI: The Great Lakes Center for Education Research & Practice. Binns, H. J., Campbell, C., Brown, M. J., & the Advisory Committee on Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention. (2007). Interpreting and managing blood lead levels of less than 10 μg/dL in children and reducing childhood exposure to lead: Recommendations of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Advisory Committee on Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention. Pediatrics, 120, e1285-e1298. Brown, J. L.., Beardslee, W. H., & Prothrow-Stith, D. (2008, November). Impact of School Breakfast on Children’s Health and Learning: An Analysis of the Scientific Research. Commissioned by the Sodexo Foundation. http://tinyurl.com/epic0309hunger-sodexo. CDC (2009, April). What Every Woman Should Know About Alcohol and Pregnancy. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Accessed 11-18-09, http://www.cdc.gov/features/alcoholfreepregnancy/ Child Trends Data Bank. (2009). Indicator: Parental Symptoms of Depression. Accessed 10-29-09, http://www.childtrendsdatabank.org/indicators/54ParentalDepression.cfm. Dearing, E. (2008). Psychological costs of growing up poor. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 324-332. Deming, D. (2009). Early childhood intervention and life-cycle skill development: Evidence from Head Start. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 1(3), 111-134. Evans, G. W., & English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: Multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73(4), 1238-1248. Evans, G. W., & Schamberg, M. A. (2009). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and adult working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, PNAS Early Edition, www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas0811910106. Family Strengthening Policy Center. (2007, November). Home Visiting: Strengthening Families by Promoting Parenting Success. (Policy Brief No. 23). Washington, DC: National Human Services Assembly. Feeding America. (2009). Child Food Insecurity in the United States: 2005 – 2007. Chicago, IL: Feeding America. Fryer, R. G., Jr., & Levitt, S. D. (2004). Understanding the black-white test score gap in the first two years of school. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 86(2), pp. 447-464. 24 Fryer, R. G., Jr.; & Levitt, S. D. (2006). The Black-White Test Score Gap Through Third Grade. American Law and Economic Review, 8, pp. 249-281. Gopnik, A. (2009). The Philosophical Baby: What Children’s Minds Tell Us About Truth, Love, and the Meaning of Life. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. Grunewald, R., & Rolnick, A. (2006, March). A Proposal for Achieving High Returns on Early Childhood Development. Minneapolis, MN: Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (2003). The early catastrophe: The 30 million word gap by age 3. American Educator, Spring, accessed 10-29-09, http://www.aft.org/pubsreports/american_educator/spring2003/catastrophe.html. Heckman, J. J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312, 1900-1902. Heckman, J. J., Moon, S. H., Pinto, R., Savelyev, P. A., & Yavitz, A. (2009, April). The rate of return to the High/Scope Perry Preschool Program. University of Chicago: Department of Economics. Accessed 11-6-09, http://jenni.uchicago.edu/papers/cba_paper_2009-04-17_jlt.pdf Isaacs, J. (2008, September). Impacts of Early Childhood Programs: Nurse Home Visiting. (Research Brief No. 5). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution, Center on Children & Families. Kaiser Family Foundation. (2009, September). Kaiser Health Tracking Poll. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation. Knitzer, J., Theberge, S., & Johnson, K. (2008). Reducing Maternal Depression and Its Impact on Young Children: Toward a Responsive Early Childhood Policy Framework. NY: Columbia University, National Center for Children in Poverty. McGlade, M. S., Saha, S., & Dahlstrom, M. E. (2004). The Latina paradox: An opportunity for restructuring prenatal care delivery. American Journal of Public Health, 94(12), 2062-2065. Minnesota Department of Education. (2009, June). Minnesota School Readiness Study: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance, Fall 2008. St. Paul, MN: Minnesota Department of Education. 25 Minnesota Department of Health, Health Economics Program. (2010, February). Health Insurance Coverage in Minnesota, Early Results from the 2009 Minnesota Health Access Survey. St. Paul, MN: Minnesota Department of Health. Accessed 6-7-10, http://www.health.state.mn.us/divs/hpsc/hep/publications/coverage/2009resultshas.p df. Minnesota Department of Health. (2005, November). Fetal Alcohol Syndrome. Minnesota Department of Health Fact Sheet. St. Paul, MN: Minnesota Department of Health, Minnesota Children with Special Health Needs. Accessed 10-29-09, http://www.health.state.mn.us/divs/fh/mcshn/bd/fetal.htm. National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. (2007). The timing and quality of early experiences combine to shape brain architecture. Working Paper No. 5. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University, Center on the Developing Child. http://www.developingchild.net Olds, D. (2008). Preventing child maltreatment and crime with prenatal and infancy support of parents: The nurse-family partnership. Journal of Scandinavian Studies in Criminology and Crime Prevention, 9, 2-24. Olds, D. L., Sadler, L., & Kitzman, H. (2007). Programs for parents of infants and toddlers: Recent evidence from randomized trials. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(3/4), 355-391. Olympio, K. P. K., Gonçalves, C, Günther, W. M. R., & Bechara, E. J. H. (2009). Neurotoxicity and aggressiveness triggered by low-level lead in children: A review. Pan Am Journal of Public Health, 26(3), 266-275. Peterson, R. W. (1994). School readiness considered from a neuro-cognitive perspective. Early Education Development, 5(2). Accessed 11-2-09, http://readyweb.crc.uiuc.edu/virtual-library/1994/peterson.html. Ramey, C. T., Campbell, F. A., & Blair, C. (1998). Enhancing the life course for high-risk children: Results from the Abecedarian Project. In J. Crane (Ed.), Social Programs that Work (pp. 163-183). Reynolds, A. J., Temple, J. A., Robertson, D. L.., & Mann, E. A. (2001, June). Age 21 Cost-Benefit Analysis of the Title I Chicago Child-Parent Center Program: Executive Summary. University of Wisconsin-Madison: Waisman Center. Accessed 6-17-2010, http://www.waisman.wisc.edu/cls/cbaexecsum4.html. Reynolds, A. J., Temple, J. A., Ou, S., Robertson, D. L., Mersky, J., Topitzes, J. W., & Niles, M. D. (2007, March). Effects of a preschool and school-age intervention on adult health and well-being: Evidence from the Chicago Longitudinal Study. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA. Rolnick, A., & Grunewald, R. (2003). Early Childhood Development: Economic Development with a High Public Return. The Region, December 2003. 26 Sampson, R. J., Sharkey, P., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2008). Durable effects of concentrated disadvantage on verbal ability among African American children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(3), 845-852. Sastry, N., & Pebley, A. R. (2008). Family and neighborhood Sources of Socioeconomic Inequality in Children’s Achievement. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan, Population Studies Center. Schweinhart, L. J., Montie, J., Xiang, Z., Barnett, W. S., Belfield, C. R., & Nores, M. (2005). Lifetime Effects: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study Through Age 40 [Monograph]. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press. Sharkey, P. (2009). Neighborhoods and the Black-White Mobility Gap. Washington, DC: Pew Charitable Trusts, Economic Mobility Project. Shore, R. (1997). Rethinking the Brain: New Insights into Early Development. New York: Families and Work Institute. Sroufe, L.. A. (2005). Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Attachment & Human Development, 7(4), 349-367. Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The Development of the Person: The Minnesota Study of Risk and Adaptation from Birth to Adulthood. New York: Guilford Press. Sum, A., Khatiwada, I., McLaughlin, J., & Palma, S. (2009, October). The Consequences of Dropping out of High School: Joblessness and Jailing for High School Dropouts and the High Cost for Taxpayers. Boston: Northeastern University, Center for Labor Market Studies. Sweet, M. A., & Appelbaum, M. I. (2004). Is home visiting an effective strategy? A metaanalytic review of home visiting programs for families with young children. Child Development, 75(5), 1435-1456. Temple, J. A., Reynolds, A. R., & Miedel, W. T. (2000, March). Can early intervention prevent high school dropout? Evidence from the Chicago Child-Parent Centers. Urban Education, 35(1), 31-56. United States Department of Agriculture. (2008, November). Household Food Security in the United States, 2007. Washington, DC: USDA. United States Department of Agriculture. (2009, September). Food Insecurity in Households with Children: Prevalence, Severity, and Household Characteristics. Washington, DC: USDA. United States Department of Agriculture. (2009, November). Household Food Security in the United States, 2008. Washington, DC: USDA. United States Department of Health and Human Services. (2005). Dietary Guidelines for Americans. Washington, DC: HHS. Accessed 11-18-09, http://www.health.gov/dietaryguidelines/dga2005/document/html/chapter9.htm. 27 Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1998). Emotional characteristics of infants associated with maternal depression and anxiety. Pediatrics, 102(5), 1298-1304. Wilder Research. (2008). Early Childhood Business Plan Literature Review. St. Paul, MN: Wilder Research. Wolf, M. (2007). Proust and the Squid: The Story and Science of the Reading Brain. New York: Harper Perennial. 28