SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY & SPINAL REFLEXES Objectives After completing this practical you should be able to: 1. Define the terms: sensory receptor, sensory unit, peripheral receptive field, modality of sensation, sensory adaptation, generator potential, referred pain, and phantom pain. 2. Name at least four different categories of sensory receptors. 3. Name and describe the function and location within the skin of the five primary types of cutaneous receptors. 4. Describe how to utilise a two-point touch threshold test to measure tactile acuity. 5. Name and describe the conduction velocity and degree of myelination of the four primary types of sensory afferent nerve fibres. 6. Explain the pattern of change in peripheral sensations which are observed with ischaemia. 1. CUTANEOUS SENSATION: Four independent modalities of cutaneous sensation have traditionally been recognised - warmth, cold, touch, and pain (pressure is excluded because it is mediated by receptors deep in the dermis, and the sensations of itch and tickle are usually excluded because of their mysterious origin). Mapping of these sensations on the surface of the skin has revealed that the receptors are not generalised throughout the skin but are clustered at different points (have a punctate distribution). Since the punctate distribution is different for each of the four sensory modalities, this suggested that each sensation was mediated by a different sensory receptor, and this view was supported by the histological identification of different cutaneous receptors. However, this identification has not been possible for all modalities. Excision of areas of the skin corresponding to ‘cold’, ‘warm’ and ‘pain’ spots failed to reveal distinct receptors for those modalities; later work showed that these sensations are mediated by bare nerve endings of very small diameter, not visible in conventional histological sections. A. Mapping the Temperature Receptors of the Skin. Procedure 1. With a ball point pen, draw a square 2 x 2 cm on the ventral surface of the subject's forearm. 2. With the subject's eyes closed, gently touch an ice-cold metal rod to different points in the square. Mark the points of cold sensation with a dark dot. 3. Heat the metal rod to about 45°C in a water bath, wipe the rod, and repeat the mapping procedure, drawing open circles at the points where heat sensation is felt (use a different colour or different symbol). 4. Reproduce this map in your laboratory report. B. Adaptation of Temperature Receptors. Many of our sense receptors respond strongly to acute changes in our environment and then cease responding when these stimuli become constant. This phenomenon is known as sensory adaptation. Our sense of smell, for example, quickly adapts to the odours of the laboratory, and our touch receptors soon cease to inform us of our clothing, until these stimuli change. When one hand is placed in warm water and another in cold water, the perceived strength of the stimulus gradually diminishes until both types of temperature receptors have adapted to their new environment. If the two hands are then placed in water at an intermediate temperature, the hand that was in the cold water will feel warm, and the hand that was in the warm water will feel cold. The "baseline," or "zero" of the receptors has obviously changed. The sensations of temperature are therefore not absolute but relative to the baseline previously established by sensory adaptation. Procedure 1. Place one hand in warm water (about 40°C) and the other in cold water ~ 12°C and leave them in the water for about 3 minutes. 2. Now place both hands in lukewarm water (about 22°C), and record your sensations below C. The Two-Point Threshold in Touch Perception. The density of touch receptors in some parts of the body is greater than in other parts; therefore, the areas of the sensory cortex (postcentral gyrus of the central fissure) that correspond to different regions of the body are of different sizes. Those areas of the body that have the largest density of touch receptors also receive the greatest motor innervation; the areas of the motor cortex (precentral gyrus) that serve these regions are correspondingly larger than other areas. Therefore, a map of the sensory and motor areas of the brain reveals that large areas are devoted to the touch perception and motor activity of the face (particularly the tongue and lips) and hands, whereas relatively small areas are devoted to the trunk, hips, and legs (Fig. 1). The density of touch receptors is measured by the two-point threshold test (Fig. 2). The two points of a pair of adjustable calipers are simultaneously placed on the subject's skin with equal pressure, and the subject is asked if two separate contacts are felt. If so, the points of the divider are brought closer together, and the test is repeated until only one point of contact is felt. The minimum distance at which two points of contact can be felt is the two-point threshold. Figure 1: Motor and sensory areas of the cerebral cortex. (a) Motor areas that control skeletal muscles and (b) sensory areas that receive somatesthetic sensations. Figure 2: The two-point touch threshold test. If each point touches the receptive fields of different sensory neurones, with one neurone in between, two separate points of touch will be felt. Procedure 1. Beginning with the calipers wide apart and the subject's eyes closed, determine the two-point threshold on the back of the hand. (Randomly alternate the two-point touch with one-point contacts, so that the subject will not try to second guess the examiner.) 2. Repeat this procedure with the palm of the hand, fingertip, and back of the neck. 3. Describe your findings below. Clinical Significance: Sensory information from the cutaneous receptors projects to the postcentral gyrus of the cerebral cortex. Direct electrical stimulation of this area therefore produces sensations similar to those felt when the cutaneous receptors are stimulated. Much of this information has been obtained by electrically stimulating the brain in patients that remain awake whilst undergoing brain surgery; the surgeon must often map the areas of the brain in order to locate the site of the lesion and avoid damage to healthy tissue. Since the cutaneous receptors are more densely arranged in the face, tongue, and hands than on the back and thighs, larger areas of the brain are involved in analysing information from the former areas than from the latter. Consequently, the areas of the brain map representing the face, tongue, and hands are larger than those representing the back and thighs. The map is also upside down, with the feet represented near the superior surface and the head represented more inferiorly and laterally in the cortex. 2. PHANTOM PAIN AND REFERRED PAIN: Receptor organs are sensory transducers, changing environmental stimuli into afferent nerve impulses. Since the action potentials in one nerve are the same as another, the perception of the sensation is determined entirely by the area of the brain stimulated, which is different for each sensory nerve. Although a given sensory nerve is normally stimulated by a specific receptor, trauma to the nerve proximal to the receptor may also evoke action potentials, and this will be interpreted by the brain as the normal sensation (e.g., seeing flashes of light or ‘stars’ when punched in the eye). A. Phantom pain: Amputees frequently report feelings of pain in their missing limbs (this is known as the phantom limb phenomenon). The nerve impulses arise at the cut ends of the nerve fibres, but the pain is perceived as coming from the amputated region of the body that normally produces action potentials along these nerves. The following experiment demonstrates how we perceive sensations that arise from impulses generated along the course of a nerve. Procedure 1. Gently tap the ulnar nerve where it crosses the median epicondyle of the elbow with a reflex hammer. 2. Describe the locations where sensation is felt. B. Referred pain: Referred pains are important clinically, particularly for deep visceral pain, which is characteristically dull and poorly localised. Here, pain initiated by impulses arising in internal organs is perceived as arising from an area of the body surface, because visceral and somatic nociceptive afferents converge on the same ascending neurone. For this reason, the deep visceral pain is referred to a surface location served by nerves from the same segmental level of the spinal cord (see Fig. 3). In ischaemic heart disease, for example, the pain is referred to the left pectoral region and shoulder areas; this is called angina pectoris. In many patients with stomach ulcers, the pain is referred to the region between the scapulae of the back. Figure 3: Referred pain. Convergence of visceral and somatic afferents onto ascending pathways. 3. DEEP PAIN: Arrest the circulation in one arm of the subject by rapidly inflating a cuff on the upper arm to 200 mm Hg. The subject now momentarily but tightly grips a tennis ball in each hand; the gripping movements, which should all be of equal strength, are repeated once per second. After about 30 seconds pain will be felt in the forearm, and perhaps in the hand, with the circulation arrested. It will probably increase in intensity and become severe in about 60 seconds. The exercise is then stopped, but the cuff is kept inflated. The pain continues practically unchanged. On sudden release of the cuff the pain vanishes within a second or two. Usually no pain is felt in the arm exercising with a free circulation. 4. EFFECTS OF ISCHAEMIA ON PERIPHERAL SENSATIONS: The subject holds his bared forearm above head level to drain the veins while a sphygmomanometer cuff is rapidly inflated to 200 mm Hg. It must be maintained at this pressure throughout the experiment. The subject then rests his forearm comfortably on the bench and closes his eyes while the experimenter repeatedly tests sensory function as follows, in the hand and forearm. Light touch sensation - touch lightly with a wisp of cotton wool; the subject says 'yes' whenever he feels the touch. Vary the rhythm and site of testing irregularly. Superficial pain sensation – jab the arm and/or forearm lightly with either the head or point of a pin; the subject reports 'head' or 'point'. Temperature sensation - touch the skin with a boiling-tube containing water at 45°C or 10°C. The subject reports 'hot' or 'cold' as the case may be. Position sensation - the experimenter waggles the subject's thumb or forefinger for a second or two, finally holding it in either a flexed or extended position. The subject reports the position. Keep the arm cuff inflated for between 30 and 35 min. (there is an ample safety margin even after this time period). The most interesting effects are experienced only after 25-30 min of ischaemia, so do not discontinue it too soon. If the superficial veins become uncomfortably distended the brachial artery cannot have been completely occluded, and the experiment should be discontinued. Before release of the occluding pressure note wrist-drop and inability to dorsiflex the wrist. On releasing the cuff, note the general subjective similarity between 'pins and needles' (here an after-effect of the ischaemia, resulting from impulses generated at the point where the cuff was pressing on the nerve trunk), the sensation when the ulnar nerve is accidentally knocked where it passes round the medial epicondyle, and the sensation of an electric shock. This type of sensation is caused by the simultaneous stimulation of fibres which normally mediate different sense modalities, so that no 'meaningful pattern' of sensory inflow is formed. 5. CONDUCTION VELOCITY OF HUMAN MOTOR NERVE: Figure 4: Representative screen cap of two compound action potentials traces generated by stimulating the ulnar nerve at the wrist (left hand trace) and elbow. If the ulnar nerve is stimulated at various points along its course, the latency of the response of the muscle which abducts the little finger (abductor digiti minimi) can be recorded. A plot of latency against the length of the conduction path will give a straight line plot, the slope of which is the conduction velocity. It is useful to record the response evoked by stimulation at elbow and wrist. By measuring the distance between points of stimulation and the time interval between the responses an average conduction velocity can be found (Fig. 4). Figure 5: Diagram illustrating the attachment points on the arm for the stimulating, recording and earth electrodes when measuring conduction velocity of the ulnar nerve. Procedure: 1) Attach the electrodes wires according to the diagram in Fig. 5 above. 2) The bar stimulus electrode is placed over the ulnar nerve, increase the stimulus voltage until a response occurs. You will probably have to explore around with the electrode before locating the optimum position. You will feel some sensation as a result of stimulation of sensory nerve fibres, but provided a good contact is maintained by the stimulating electrode there should not be any discomfort. When the latency of the response has been measured by stimulation at elbow and wrist, try the effect of changing the stimulation frequency. What is the frequency at which tetanus occurs? Clinical Significance: Nerve conduction velocity measurement, using the method shown here, is a commonly used diagnostic tool where demyelinating neuropathy is suspected. Breakdown of myelination along axons results in a slowing of action potential conduction along it such that axons within a nerve begin to conduct at slightly different velocities. Thus, the nerve subsequently loses its normal synchrony of conduction in response to a single stimulus. For example functions that depend upon the arrival of synchronous bursts of neural activity, such as tendon reflexes and vibratory sensation are lost soon after the onset of a chronic neuropathy. 6. REACTION TIMES: Figure 6: Representative screen cap of trace for visual and auditory reaction time experiments This is the time elapsing between the reception of a sensory stimulus and the acting upon it. a) Visual Reaction Time In this experiment, the computer timebase is triggered to sweep by the experimenter; the time taken for the subject to respond visually is measured. Procedure 1) On the computer screen, click Start in Sample panel. 2) The experimenter pushes the (hand-held) Start button to initiate a sweep; as soon as the subject sees the sweep beginning, he pushes the (hand-held) Stop button and so deflects the sweep. The time from the origin of the sweep to the deflection is the subject's visual reaction time and can be measured from the timebase at the bottom of the screen (Fig. 6). b) Auditory Reaction Time In this experiment, the computer timebase is again triggered to sweep by the experimenter; the time taken for the subject to respond aurally is measured. Procedure The subject has his back to the computer screen and wears the earphones supplied. Click Start on the computer screen. When the experimenter pushes the (hand-held) Start button, the subject hears a click and the sweep starts; the subject pushes the (hand-held) Stop button to deflect the sweep. This allows measurement of the reaction time as before. NOTE: The experimenter should not allow the subject to see the Start button, and before the experimenter pushes the button he calls 'ready' to the subject. At least six observations of both visual and auditory reaction times should be made; the experimenter should vary randomly the interval between calling 'ready' and pushing the start button. In order to give these figures a meaning for everyday affairs, take the shortest time for the visual reaction and calculate the distance in metres travelled in that time by a motor car driven at 50 k.p.h. (13.9 m/sec). This will give the distance traversed before you could begin to apply the brake after receiving a warning ('red') light signal when travelling at 50 k.p.h. Note that this is only part of the distance required to bring the car to a standstill. A car travelling at 50 k.p.h. can be stopped in about 10 metres from the moment of application of the brake, assuming 100% braking efficiency with optimum coefficient of friction between the tyres and the road. In a real situation the car driver cannot equal the low reaction times of the laboratory because he is not prepared by a "ready" signal. The reaction time is more likely to be about a second. Now calculate what this means in terms of distance (metres) Note also that older people (with slower reaction times) are often safer drivers than younger people, who may depend unduly on their fast responses to dangerous situations. This is explained by the better judgement ('road sense') of the older people who anticipate a possible dangerous situation and avoid the need for emergency measures ('defensive driving'). Most of our ordinary reactions are not merely responses to simple sensory stimuli, but to a complicated pattern of stimuli which must be integrated before an adequate response is possible. An intelligence test may be said to measure the speed and accuracy of such integrative responses. In a real driving situation, attention may be distracted by other stimuli, thus increasing reaction time. To measure the magnitude of this effect, repeat the visual reaction time test above, but during a mobile phone conversation about a topic that requires some concentration. Conduction Velocity (metres/sec) Reaction Time Visual (ms) Auditory (ms) Visual In terms of distance travelled (metres)