ADAM FINN* An information processing perspective is used to develop hierarchical and divergent models of how individuals process print ads. An aggregation across individuals generated related audience-level models, which were operationalized by using Starch scores and extended to incorporate specific ad charocteristics. Confirmatory tests indicate that these models provide a substantial advance over previous data-driven approaches to analyzing readership scares. Print Ad Recognition Readership Scores: An Information Processing Perspective As reported by Starch (1966). research on the effectiveness of print ads began around 1900. The early use of ads in memory experiments soon evolved into systematic recognition and recall procedures for testing the effectiveness of print ads. Recognition measures of print ad readership first were provided commercially by Gallup and then by Daniel Starch, who founded the current syndicated service in 1932. The continued success of this service and recent validation research (Zinkham and Gelb 1986) both indicate that recognition readership scores provide useful information about the effectiveness of ads. Starch not only provides its well-known "noted," "associated," and "read most" scores, but also reports on the noting of component parts of larger ads (Starch 1966, p. 12). Normally included are a "seen" score for the major illustration, a "noted" score for the signature, and a "read some" score for the major block of copy. The modem era of academic investigations of the impact of print ad characteristics on recognition readership scores was ushered in by Twedt (1952). Since then, academic and proprietary studies have been done over and over again (Neu 1983). Typically, the Starch scores for a sample of ads are regressed on a set of coded ad characteristics. Causa! relationships are assumed to underlie statistically significant ad characteristics, so normative conclusions are drawn, ultimately to be used in guidelines for the design of print advertising (Stansfield 1969). Despite the number of such studies, several concerns can be raised about this stream of data-driven research. First, it has been challenged on the grounds that recognition scores are neither valid nor reliable measures of print ad effectiveness (Clancy, Ostlund, and Wyner 1979; Singh and Cole 1985). Second, without a conceptual model of the relationship between the various Starch scores, the scores always have been analyzed alone, not simultaneously. More powerful analytical techniques have not been used. Finally, there has been an overreliance on exploratory procedures and modest ad sample sizes, without recognition of the danger of drawing causal inferences (Armstrong 1970). A new approach to the relationship between print ad characteristics and recognition readership scores is described in the following sections. First, the nature of ad readership scores is reconsidered, with the suggestion that they be viewed from an information processing perspective. This perspective now dominates theoretical research on advertising effects (Alwitt and Mitchell 1985) and is of practical relevance because information processing concepts parallel the sequence of decisions advertisers make (Shimp and Gresham 1983). Information processing theory then is used to develop hierarchical and divergent models of how an individual processes a print ad. By aggregation across individuals, these microlevel models generate related audience-level models, which are operationalized by using Starch scores. A review of prior readership research is used to incorporate specific ad characteristics into these models. Confirmatory tests of the models then are conducted on data *Adam Finn is Assistant Professor, Department of Marketing and Economic Analysis, Faculty of Business, University of Alberta. The author expresses his appreciation to Starch-INRA-Hooper, Inc. for kindly providing access to the data for the study, and to Roy Howeil, Ruth Bolton, Jordan Louviere. and several anonymous JMR reviewers for their useful comments and suggestions. 16B Journal of Marketing Research Vol. XXV (May 1988). 168-77 PRINT AD RECOGNITION SCORES obtained from Starch-INRA-Hooper. Finally, the implications of the research are discussed. NATURE OE PRINT AD RECOGNITION SCORES Once the regular testing of print ads was accepted in the 1950s, a controversy arose over the relative merits of recognition and aided recall testing of print ads. Recognition scores usually were several times aided recall scores. In 1955 the Advertising Research Foundation conducted a field experiment to compare recognition, aided recall, and reader interest scores for ads in an issue of Life magazine. This experiment, commonly referred to as the PARM study, found that recognition scores were very reliable and, at least in the short term, did not decline with the passage of time from exposure to the ads. In contrast, aided recall scores were less reliable and declined quickly with time from exposure. The observed reliability and stability of recognition scores troubled many observers. First, if recognition scores were measures of memory, they should fade, not remain stable. Second, recognition studies of magazines including bogus ads (Marder and David 1961) and comparisons of claimed noting with observed exposure (Lucas 1960) both confirmed that many individuals falsely claim recognition. Research on this false claiming found that it increased with interest in the product being advertised (Appel and Blum 1961). that it increased with deviations from the Starch interview procedure (Starch 1966, Ch. 3), and that social desirability effects accounted for only a small proportion of false claims (Clancy, Ostlund, and Wyner 1979). One consequence of these concerns has been suggestions for improving recognition methodology, including the use of control groups (Appel and Blum 1961) and forced-choice recognition methods (Singh and Churchill 1986). A second result has been a claim that recognition scores are a biased measure of memory. Bagozzi and Silk (1983) present the most sophisticated development of this view in their reanalysis of the PARM data. After controlling for a "biasing" effect of interest in the ad, they found recognition "noted" scores to be very reliable measures of the cognitive memory for ads. However, this may not be the only reasonable conceptualization of recognition scores. They may be indicative of how an ad has been processed by an audience. First, Starch labels its service as a readership, not a memory, report. It attempts to measure what people observe and read during consumption of print media (Starch 1966, p. 10). Second, Gallup was looking for an objective measure of interest in printed material (Lucas and Britt 1963. p. 57) and assumed that readership under natural exposure conditions would reflect the degree of interest. Hence, it is not surprising that Wells (1964) found both reader interest in and ratings of the attractiveness of ads to be correlated strongly with noting scores and that Bagozzi and Silk (1983) found the interest confound. Finally, in evaluating the PARM study, Lucas (I960) concluded that recognition scores were "a 169 rough indicator of reader behavior, perhaps a guide to some kind of psychological contact more substantive than mere visual exposure." Here, an individual's recognition responses are assumed to reflect three components. One is the processing that the ad actually received. The second is systematic error due to such effects as the decay in memory of that processing and the tendency to report how an ad would have been processed. The third is random measurement error. Both random and systematic error can lead to false claiming by individuals. Even if the proportion of individual random error variance is high, however. Starch scores stiil would be expected to be very reliable because Starch aggregates across the responses of about 100 individuals. The real issue is the validity of Starch scores when used in cross-sectional research on ad effectiveness. The systematic bias in Starch scores due to memory effects was shown to be modest by Bagozzi and Silk (1983). Systematic bias due to yea-saying reports of how an ad would have been processed should be correlated with the aggregate processing the ad actually received. It would inflate scores, but it should not be a significant problem in cross-sectional studies of characteristics contributing to the effectiveness of print ads. CONSUMER INFORMATION PROCESSING OE A PRINT AD The development of a model of print ad readership begins at the micro level with individual information processing. This approach has several advantages. By interrelating the various readership responses, a tnicrolevel model identifies how aggregate readership scores are interrelated and so should be analyzed together. In contrast, prior research has been ad hoc. Researchers have either ignored all but one score (Bagozzi and Silk 1983; Sparkman 1985), as though the scores were equivalent, or treated readership scores independently, as though they were unrelated (Hanssens and Weitz 1980; Holbrook and Lehmann 1980; Rossiter 1981). A micro-level mode! also provides a framework for structuring an assessment of prior research. Finally, it can help to identify other ad characteristics that influence readership scores. The information processing literature supports both hierarchical and divergent models of the processing a print ad receives from an individual. Both models begin with the assumptions that consumers act to accomplish their own goals and have limited processing capacity. As a result, capacity is devoted to available information to the extent that it is believed likely to facilitate attainment of those goals (Bettman 1979, p. 2). Hierarchical Processing Model The hierarchical model is a strictly bottom-up processing interpretation of the four levels of audience involvement in advertising suggested by Greenwald and Leavitt (1984). The successive levels of preattention, focal attention, comprehension, and elaboration require increasing processing capacity. In strictly bottom-up pro- 170 JOURNAL OF AAARKETING RESEARCH, MAY 1988 cessing, if processing at one level fails to evoke the next highest level, processing of the ad is terminated and the capacity is allocated to some other task. Beginning at the bottom level, a consumer is exposed to an ad because of its size and location within a print vehicle. This preattention processing enables the consumer to determine that the ad is not a continuation of what was being processed previously. The consumer can terminate processing at this level, in which case the ad produces little or no enduring effect, or can continue to process the ad in a manner that will contribute most efficiently to achieving his or her goals. This processing is done at the focal attention level, requiring just enough additional capacity to determine what the ad is about. Eye movement studies indicate this step is achieved most economically by processing the pictorial material first. About 90%.of viewers fixate the dominant picture in an ad before they fixate the copy (Kroeber-Riel 1984). The enduring effect of focal attention is the formation of a visual image of the ad, making subsequent recognition possible. If the visual image generated by focal attention evokes the higher comprehension level of involvement, the consumer analyzes the image to form propositions assigning it meaning. The enduring effects are propositional traces in memory. Finally, if the highest elaboration level of involvement is evoked, the consumer's full capacity is used to respond cognitively to the ad, generating personal connections and imagery. The enduring effect is a conceptual integration of the meaning found in the ad with prior knowledge. The hierarchical model is represented by the continuous lines in Figure 1. Figure 1 TWO MODELS OF INDIVIDUAL INFORA\ATION PROCESSING OF A PRINT AD Advertisement Characterfslics Information Processing Constructs Enduring Effects Ad size and location in a print vehicle Littie or none Layout of ad and dominant illustration Image formation Secondary elements ol ad: Headline, logo a other illustrations Proposition formation Detail of ad copy, etc. Integration with existing knowledge Divergent Processing Model Though accepting that the dominant pictorial material in an ad is processed first, Edell and Staelin (1983) offer an altemative model. They argue that any farther processing is conditional on the framing of the ad, the consistency between its pictorial material and verbal messages. They suggest that framing is required for elaborative processing to occur, in effect rejecting the strictly hierarchical model in favor of a divergent processing model, indicated by the broken lines in Figure I. Mitchell (1983) proposed that once a print ad has received attention, subsequent processing depends on the goals of the individual. If the goal is to evaluate the product suggested by the picture, the individual will form verbal representations and engage in elaboration. If other goals are dominant, the consumer will not engage in verbal product-related elaborations. Instead, the visual secondary elements of the ad will be processed to comprehend further the pictorial material. Different individuals may process the same ad differently. AUDIENCE READERSHIP OF PRINT ADS To generate audience-level readership models that are consistent with the micro models requires an aggregation Two Model Hierarchial; variants: Divergent, across consumers, from an individual to an audience level. The simplest approach to this aggregation is to assume homogeneity. With this stringent assumption, the hierarchical and divergent models yield analogous audiencelevel models. Relaxing the homogeneity assumption introduces more complexity, so only the simplest form of heterogeneity is considered here. If some individuals process hierarchically and others divergently, aggregation will produce a mixed model at the audience level. Hence, hierarchical, divergent, and mixed audience-level models are consistent with current consumer information processing theories. The audience-level constructs in these models are designated as exposure received (ExpR), attention received (AttR), comprehension attained (CompA), and elaboration attained (ElabA). Starch scores, which aggregate over individuals, are at the right level to provide measures of these constructs. The constructs can be linked to particular Starch scores by their differential enduring effects. The enduring effect of focal attention is the formation 171 PRINT AD RECOGNITION SCORES of a visual image of the ad, so recognition of ad pictorial content is indicative of processing at the focal-attention level (Greenwald and Leavitt 1984, p. 590). Thus Starch noted and seen scores appear to be indicators of AttR. Noted scores often have been referred to as measures of the attention-getting power of ads (Diamond 1968; Valiente 1973; Wells 1964). The enduring effect of comprehension is the formation of propositional traces assigning meaning to the ad, so recognition of such propositions is indicative of processing at the comprehension level (Greenwald and Leavitt 1984). Because a link to a sponsor is the most universal of such propositions, recognition of the sponsor's name and signature may be indicative of comprehension-level processing. If so. Starch associated and signature scores appear lo be suitable indicators of CompA. The enduring effect of elaboration is an integration of ad meaning with prior knowledge, so free recall of ad content is indicative of such processing (Greenwald and Leavitt 1984). Starch does not measure recall, but read most and read some scores may be an acceptable alternative. Elaboration requires additional processing time and these scores indicate the duration of processing. Read most scores correlate strongly with ad reading times measured by means of an eye camera (Starch 1966, p. 26). When raised, read most scores always have been considered indicative of a higher level of readership involvement than either noted or associated scores (Rossiter 1981; Valiente 1973). With little or no enduring effect of preattention, there is no retrospective indicator of ExpR. Therefore, as shown in Figure 2, ExpR must be omitted from the models to be estimated. This omission is not expected to bias the assessment of the effects of ad characteristics on AttR, because the models including ExpR are all recursive (Blalock 1982, Ch. 5). In Figure 2, theoretical constructs are represented as ellipses and their measures are represented as rectangles. Specifically, TI, represents AttR, 1^2 represents CompA, and T), represents ElabA; K, through Yf, are the Starch score indicators of the constructs. The differences between the models are in the structural relations. For the hierarchical model, p.,, for the direct causal path from AttR to ElabA would be zero. For the divergent model, P32 for the causal path from CompA to ElabA would be zero. For the mixed model, all three structural parameters would be positive and significant. Comparing the fit of these models does not allow the drawing of defmitive inferences about how individuals process ads, because one cannot distinguish between heterogeneity and altemative models. The same mixed model is obtained if every individual can process both hierarchically and divergently or if groups of individuals process each way. However, these models can be compared with baseline models representing prior conceptualization in the field (Sobel and Bohmstedt 1985) to determine whether the information processing perspective adds insight at the audience level. Several research- Figure 2 EXTENDED MODEL OF THE AUDIENCE READERSHIP OF PRINT ADS Advertise men 1 Characteristics Audience Readership Constructs Starch Score Irtdlcators Ad Size Front/Back Cover Facing Ads Right/Left Color Illustration Size Photo/Art Bleed Residual ers have analyzed noted, associated, and read most scores independently (Hanssens and Weitz 1980; Holbrook and Lehmann 1980; Rossiter 1981), implicitly modeling the scores as measures of three unrelated constructs. Other researchers have analyzed only the noted scores (Bagozzi and Silk 1983; Sparkman 1985), implicitly modeling all the scores as indicators of a single construct. Reconsideration of Prior Research •' To incorporate specific ad characteristic effects into these audience models, the findings of prior readership research were reconsidered from an information processing perspective. That view suggests ad size and locational characteristics should have an indirect impact on AttR through their effect on ExpR. Layout and pictorial characteristics should have a direct impact on AttR and, through it, weaker indirect effects on CompA and ElabA. Secondary ad elements should influence CompA and copy characteristics should influence ElabA, but a failure to control for AttR would make identifying such effects difficult. The direct effect of an ad characteristic on CompA (ElabA) would be identifiable only by markedly different effects on AttR and CompA (ElabA). Table I summarizes the ad characteristic fmdings re- 172 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, AAAY 1988 Table 1 FREQUENCY OF SIGNIFICANT FINDINGS IN PRIOR RESEARCH RELATING AD CHARACTERISTICS TO PRINT AD READERSHIP SCORES Ad characteristics Size and location Ad size Front/back pages Cover position Facing: ad/editorial Right/left page Layout and pictorial Color Illustration size Photo/art Bleed/no bleed Other characteristics Headline: Words Phrases Nouns Verbs Adjectives Determiners Type size Product reference Personal reference Question form Benefit Product as object Copy: Amount Readability Benefits + AttR indicator' No. of findings'^ ns 14 9 4 2 2 0 2 1 2 2 13 8 5 5 0 4 2 5 0 0 0 2 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 2 0 0 0 8 1 1 1 2 2 6 4 0 3 4 2 0 0 0 I 0 0 0 1 0 0 I 0 0 1 I 1 0 2 2 6 4r 0 0 0 4 2 3 2 0 CompA indicator No. of findings 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 ElabA indicator' No. of findings + ns X 4 0. 3 0 •1 2 0 1 0 0 0 6 3 0 0 0 1 4 3 1 3 0 0 0 0 0 3 2 1 3 1 2 0 0 0 5 2 1 I 5 6 6 5 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 0 0 2 I 0 0 0 0 0 0 6 2 3 3 I 2 4 5 3 3 4 2 1 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 4 0 0 0 1 0 4 1 1 2 0 2 3 0 1 0 0 I 0 2 0 0 I 2 3 1 3 2 2 2 0 0 1 1 0 ® 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 'Starch "noted" but includes some Ad-Chart "noticed" results. "Mainly Starch "associated." •^Mainly Starch "read most" but includes some Ad-Chart "read half or more." "Plus sign indicates positive and significant, ns indicates not significant, and minus sign indicates negative and sigtiificant. ported in the modern academic literature.' Five size and location characteristics have had consistently significant effects on AttR, slightly less consistent effects on CompA, and still less consistent effects on ElabA. In order of strength of evidence, these characteristics are ad size, front rather than back pages, a cover position, facing other ads rather than editorial, and right rather than left page. They are grouped in Figure 2, as their effects on AttR are assumed to be indirect through ExpR. Four pictorial characteristics have had the same pattern of ef- 'Included are ad characteristics for which significant effects were reported in studies by Ferguson (1935). Warner and Franzen (1947), Twedt (1952). Anderson (I960), Gardner (1961). Frankel and Soiov (1962), Yamanaka (1962), Gardner and Cohen (1964), Troldahl and Jones (1965). Assael, Kofron, and Burgi (1967). Myers and Haug (1967), Tumbull and Carter (1968). Diamond (1968), Valiente (1973), Fletcher and Winn (1974), Surlin and Kosak (1975), Holbrook and Lehmann (1980), Hanssens and Weitz (1980), Rossiter (1981), Soley and Reid (1983a.b), Blunden, Clarke, and MacDougal! (1984). and Spaiicman (1985). Though most of these researchers used Starch scores, some used similar scores from Ad-Chart (Stansfield 1969, p. 1232). fects, consistent with direct effects on AttR. In order of strength of evidence, they are color, illustration size, photo rather than drawn illustration, and bleed. Evidence is insufficient to justify confirmatory testing of other characteristics effects. CONFIRMATORY TESTS Data Starch-INRA-Hooper provided the Starch score data for 1981 issues oi Business Week (December 28), Reader's Digest (August), Hot Rod (May), and Family Circle (October 13) in response to a request for access to data for four different magazines. These magazines yielded a total of 266 Starch score observations for full-page or larger ads, including both male and female scores for the 45 ads in Reader's Digest. As Starch scores are essentially proportions, with many observations in the lower quartile, the data were subjected to a tail-stretching arcsine transformation (transformed score = 20 arcsin [square root (Starch score/100)]) that stabilizes their variance (Cohen and Cohen 1975, p. 254). The covariances, vari- 173 PRINT AD RECOGNITION SCORES Table 2 VARIANCE-COVARIANCE A N D CORRELATION MATRICES FOR TRANSFORMED STARCH SCORES' Y, Y. Y, Y, Y. Y, Noted Seen Y, Y^ 9.226 .999 .956 .944 .716 .683 9.263 9.316 .956 .946 .712 .683 Associated Y, 8.578 8.619 8.723 .989 .703 .674 Signature Read some Y, 8.117 8.173 8.266 8.010 .696 .664 5.168 5.169 4.939 4.683 5.653 .919 Read most Ye 4.760 4.781 4.566 4.313 5.012 5.264 'Correlations below, variances along, and covariances above the diagonal. ances, and correlations for these transformed scores are reported in Table 2.^ Operational measures of the ad characteristics employed in the study were chosen by considering the definitions used in prior research, while recognizing the need to control for the number and size of pages in the four magazines. Each ad was coded independently for the following characteristics by two judges. Possible discrepancies were resolved by going back to the ad (e.g., cover position, left/right). The final inter-rater Pearson or Spearman correlation coefficients are reported in parentheses. Ad size. Coded as I for a full-page ad, 2 for a doublepage spread, and 3 for gatefoids and other ads of a full page or larger accompanied by inserts (.89). Color. Treated as an interval variable, with black and white ads coded as 1. black and white plus a single color as 2, black and white plus two colors as 3, and full color as four (.98). Front/back. After adjustment to include inserted alphabetical sections, the page on which the ad appeared was divided by the total number of pages in each magazine and the resulting proportion was subjected to the arcsine transformation (1.0). Illustration size. Combined photo and illustration space in the ad as a proportion of the page size of the magazine (.71). Photo or art. The number of photos in the ad divided by the total number of illustrations, giving a proportion that was subjected to the arcsine transformation (.89). Cover position. A O-l variable with 1 for any one of the inside front, inside back, or outside back covers (1.0). Left/right. A 0-1 variable coded as I for a right page location (1.0). 'These correlations are higher than expected; the noted and associated correlation of .95 exceeds tbe recent reports of .87 (Rossiter 1981) and 83 (Zinkham and Gelb 1986). However, the correlation pattem is consistent with tbe proposed correspondence rules. Covariance data were used for model estimation. Facing ad/editorial. A 0-1 variable with 1 for ads facing editorial pages (1.0). Bleed. A 0-1 variable with 1 indicating the use of bleed (1.0). Estimation of the Starch Scores Relationship Models Because of the complexity of the full model in Figure 2, the models of the relationship between Starch scores were evaluated before the extended model was estimated (Bagozzi 1983). To identify the models, the structural relation residuals for AttR (Ci). CompA (^2). and ElabA (l,i) were assumed uncorrelated and X,. \ , . and \f, were fixed at 1, making the scales for the AttR (TI,), CompA (T)2), and ElabA (TI,) respectively the same as Chose for the transformed noted (/,), signature (K^), and read most (Yf,) scores. To help evaluate the fit of these models, a null model (Bentler and Bonett 1980) and the two prior practice baseline models also were estimated. For the first baseline model, the structural parameters were all fixed at zero to give three unrelated constructs. For the second, all six Starch scores were modeled as indicators of a single construct. The fit indices obtained from LISREL VI maximum likelihood estimation of these models are reported in Table 3. All models failed to fit according to the chi square likelihood ratio criterion, a statistic known to be sensitive to violations of the distributional assumptions and to give infiated rejections of a known model at this sample size (Boomsma 1982). However, judged against the simulation results reported by Anderson and Gerbing (1984), all three information-processing-based models provide a reasonably good fit to the data. In addition, they all provide a highly satisfactory improvement in fit over both of the baseline models.^ The mixed and divergent models are superior to the hierarchical model on all fit indices. As the mixed model is the more general 'Against the better fitting one-common-conslruci baseline model, tbe degree of freedom adjusted (p) and unadjusted (A) fit coefficients of Sobel and Bohmstedl (1985) for the hierarchical, divergent, and mixed models were respectively p = .921, A = .930; p = .935, A = .941; and p = .927. A = .944. Table 3 FIT OF MODELS FOR TRANSFORMED STARCH DATA Model d.f. Null (uncorrelated 15 random variables) Baseline (three unrelated 9 constructs) Baseline (one common 9 construct) Discriminant (combine 8 AttR & CompA) 7 Hierarchical 7 Divergent 6 Mixed Model ftt statistics P 8-fi- a.gf.i. x' rmr 3894.2 .000 .226 -.083 5.540 846.7 .000 .591 .(M7 4.707 749.5 .000 .656 .198 .538 436.9 52.3 44.3 41.9 .409 .823 .850 .835 .163 .122 .058 .005 .000 .000 .000 .000 .775 .941 .950 .953 174 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, AAAY 1988 Table 4 PARAMETER ESTIAAATES FOR AUDIENCE READERSHIP OF PRINT ADS MODELS Model parameter Noted-AttR Seen-AttR Signature-CompA Associated-CompA Read some-ElabA Read most-ElabA AtlR-CompA AttR-ElabA CompA-ElabA Residual of AttR Residual of CompA Residual of ElabA Ad size-AttR _ Front/back-AttR Cover-AttR Facing ads-AttR Right/left page-AttR Color-AttR Illustration size-AttR Photo/art-AltR Uses bleed-AttR Front/back-ElabA Color-ElabA Unique noted Unique seen Unique signature Unique associated Unique read some Unique read most Mixed relationship model: ML' Symbol K Xi , 9» 'I'l «h • l.ff 1.005 1.0^ 1.056 1.084 I.O' .881 .356 .182 9.222 .672 2.143 (.003) (.010) (.039) (.019) (.108) (.116) (.822) (.064) (.224) 7L3 7M •Yis 7l6 7l7 -ym Extended deierminant modets Original: ML' 1.0" 1.004 (.003) l.O' 1.056 (.010) 1.084 (.039) - l.O^ .880 (.019) .358 (.107) .180 (.116) 5.286 (.471) .675 (.064) 2.142 (.224) 1.354 (.668) .589 (.207) 1.902 (.754) -.881 (.602) .776 (.343) .807 (.144) 2.778 (.688) • -.038 (.164) -.334 (.378) TfM •y» «€„ 9*44 ec.6 .004 (.007) .011 (.007) .183 (.028) -.006(025) .222 (.138) .638 (.130) .003 .012 .185 -.008 .220 .640 (.007) (.007) (.028) (.025) (.138) (.130) Modified: ML' 1.0' 1.004 (.003) I.O^ 1.056 (.010) 1.106 (.039) 1.0^ .880 (.019) .466 (.101) .152 (.107) 5.286 (.471) .676 (.065) 1.878 (.197) 1.355 (.668) .589 (.207) 1.902(754) -.881 (.602) .776 (.343) .807 (.144) •; 2.780 (.688) -.038 (.164) -.334 (.378) -.451 (.126) -.384 (.081) .003 (.007) .012 (.007) .187 (.028) -.010 (.025) .108 (.127) .733 (.123) Modified: UV 1.0' 1.005 l.O' 1.056 1.082 1.0"^ .880 .428 .187 5.429 .680 1.945 1.731 .491 1.423 -.968 .701 .837 2.369 -.002 -.323 -.447 -.310 .004 .001 .183 -.008 .232 .630 Fit parameters 41.88 (6 d.f.) .953 g.f.i. a.g.f.i. .83* rmr 'Maximum likelihood parameter (standard error in parentheses) "Unweighted least squares parameter. 'Parameter fixed to equal 1. .tm model, it is used in subsequent analysis. The parameter values and standard errors for the mixed model are reported in the first column of Table 4. The presence of a small negative variance for the associated uniqueness (6644) was cause for some concern. However, compared with a standard error of .025, the estimate of - . 0 0 6 is small enough to be ascribed to sampling fluctuations when the true variance is positive but close to zero (van Driel I978).'' The AttR-ElabA (pa,) and AttR-CompA (pjj) structural parameter values are both highly significant, explaining the better fit of the 'Clever respecifications have been proposed for such improper solutions (Rindskopf 1983), but bave proved of little practical value (Dillon. Kumar, and Mulani 1987). Fixing the offending variance to zero or a small positive value also adds little to the interpretability of the improper solution, wbich can be evaluated for goodness offitby using the indices provided by the LISREL program (Gerbing and Anderson 1987). 201.7 (51 d.f.) .907 :066 171.1 (49 d.f.) .919 .801 .037 L(HX) LOOO .028 divergent model. The CompA-ElabA (p^j) parameter is not quite significant, explaining why the mixed model gives only a marginally better fit. Substantively, differences in AttR account for about 91% of the variance in CompA and more than 53% of the variance in EtabA. Indeed, the strong AttR-CompA (P21) relationship could be viewed as calling into question the discriminant validity of these two constructs. To assess this possibility, a model treating the noted, seen, associated, and signature scores as indicators of one construct was estimated. As shown in Table 3, this model is an improvement over the baseline models, but all three of the information-processing-based models are a substantial further improvement over it.^ 'The fit coefficients for the one-common-construct baseline model were only p = .348 and A = .417. whereas the fit coefficients were p = .888 and A = .904 for tbe mixed model and p = .901 and A = .898 for tbe divergent model. PRINT AD RECOGNITION SCORES Estimation of the Extended Model Many of the ad characteristics in the extended model are nominal or ordinal variables, raising questions about the use of the maximum likelihood estimation (Joreskog and Sorbom 1984, Ch. 4). A check of the normality of the conditional distribution of each transformed Starch score for each level of these fixed-effect independent variables found normality was not rejected at the .05 level by a Kolmogorov-Smimov test. However, normality was rejected at the . 10 level for the read most indicator. Hence, maximum likelihood estimation was employed to make the fullest use of the available data and to be able to assess the significance of the structural parameters, but with recognition that the program standard errors and chi square test would be sensitive to departures for normality. The maximum likelihood parameter and standard error estimates for the extended model are reported in column 2 of Table 4. Six of the nine ad characteristics have the expected signs and parameter values that are approximately two or more times their standard errors. In order of significance, these characteristics are color, illustration size, front/back, cover position, right page, and ad size. The modest impact of ad size may be due to the truncation of the sizes used in the study. The other characteristics have the wrong signs, but are not significant. Together the nine ad characteristics account for 42% of the variance in AttR. As is desirable, the other structural parameters and measurement relations remain virtually unchanged. However, the modification indices provided by the program indicate that for color and for a front location, the indirect effect through AttR does not account adequately for the total effect on ElabA. As reported in the third column of Table 4, a modified model found their positive effects on AttR to be accompanied by negative direct effects on ElabA. These direct effects increase the variance in ElabA accounted for from 53.7 to 58.5%. Though potentially of substantive importance, these effects on ElabA were not hypothesized and require future confirmation. The modified model also was estimated by using unweighted least squares, which can be justified without the distributional assumptions of maximum likelihood (Joreskog and Sorbom 1984). As shown in Table 4, this procedure produced a stronger effect for ad size and a weaker effect for cover position without changing the substantive conclusions.^ DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS Adoption of an information processing perspective and aggregation across individuals generated hierarchical, divergent, and mixed models of how an audience processes a print ad. Proposed links between the constructs in these audience models and particular Starch scores en- "Similar results were obtained by using other estimation methods. Of tbese. generalized least squares in EQS (Bentler 1985) gave proper solutions for all models in Table 4. 175 abled the models to be estimated with Starch data. All three information-processing-based models provided a substantially better fit to the data than did baseline mtxiels representing other views of readership scores. Thus, there is confirmation for modeling Starch scores as indicators of three interrelated processing constructs. The divergent and the mixed models provided a better fit than the hierarchical model. Estimation of the extended mixed model confirmed that four size and location (ad size, cover position, front or back of the vehicle, right or left page) and two pictorial characteristics (color and illustration size) were significant determinants of AttR. Limitations The implications that can be drawn from the results depend on the validity of the proposed links between individual information processing constructs and Starch responses. Starch associated and signature responses tap only one aspect of comprehension and Starch read some and read most responses were substituted for recall responses. A study of the relationship between level of involvement, type of processing, recall, and recognition responses to print ads is needed to resolve the validity issue. If the proposed links are not valid, the constructs underlying Starch scores would have to be reinterpreted at a more operational level. Though there would be little consequence for the practical implications of the research, any information processing interpretation of the results would be eliminated. . Implications for Print Advertisers The results explain the continued practitioner focus on noted scores. For this sample of ads, differences in the AttR by the ads account for more than 90% of the variance in the CompA and more than half of the variance in the ElabA. Substantively, the results suggest that the AttR by print ads is determined largely by some now well-established location and illustration characteristics. Of these, only one, illustration size, is both under the direct creative control of the ad designer and not already subject to a premium. Therefore this finding is the most clearly actionable. In addition, to the extent that they have a choice, print advertisers should attempt to obtain right-page locations toward the front of magazines rather than left pages toward the back. Allocating the maximum possible proportion of ad space to pictorial material is likely to be optimal if one wants to ensure CompA and is probably optimal if one is seeking ElabA. Finally, though premiums for cover and color remain justified, after controlling for illustration size, the premium for bleed may not be justified. Implications for Future Ad Readership Research Future ad readership research should be conducted within the framework of a specific model, such as the mixed processing model presented here. Exploratory research is needed to identify other characteristics that influence ad processing. The greatest need is for the iden- 176 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, MAY 1988 tification of additional determinants of AttR. The identification of determinants of ElabA, given AttR, is a second priority. Research on the determinants of CompA, after controlling for AttR. can produce only marginal gains. Simple analytical procedures such as stepwise regression can continue to be used, but only after controlling for previously confirmed relationships. Exploratory research on AttR must control for the six confirmed characteristics. Similar research on ElabA must control for the strong effects of AttR. An information processing perspective should help to identify ad characteristics to include in this research. Additional layout and illustration characteristics should be considered as determinants of AttR. Details of ad copy, such as sentence structure, might be expected to influence ElabA directly. Similarly, aspects of the ad signature might be expected to affect CompA. Further confirmatory research is needed to generalize the results obtained here and to test exploratory findings such as the negative effects of color and front location on ElabA found here. The power of tests of the effects of particular characteristics can be increased significantly by controlling for previously established relationships. REFERENCES Alwitt, Litida F. and Andrew A. Mitchell (1985), Psychological Processes and Advertising Effects: Theory. Research and Applications. Hillsdaie, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Anderson. James C. and David W. Gerbing (1984), "The Effect of Sampling Error on Convergence, Improper Solutions, and Goodness-of-Fit Indices for Maximum Likelihood Confirmatory Factor Analysis," Psychometrika. 49 (June), 155-73. Anderson, Leo (i960), "How Research Can Boost Ad Effectiveness by 10%," Industrial Marketing, 45 (May), 39-43. Appel, Valentine and Milton. L. Blum (1961), "Ad Recognition and Respondent Set," Journal of Advertising Research, 1 (June), 13-21. Armstrong, J. Scott (1970), "How to Avoid Exploratory Research." Journal of Advertising Research, 10 (August), 2 7 30. Assael, Henry, John H. Kofron, and Walter Burgi (1967), "Advertising Performance as a Function of Print Ad Characteristics," Journal of Advertising Research. 7 (June), 20-6. Bagozzi, Richard P. (1983), "Issues in the Application of Covariance Structure Analysis: A Further Comment," Journal of Consumer Research. 9 (March), 449-50. - a n d Aivin J. Silk (1983), "Recall, Recognition, and the Measurement of Memory for Print Advertising," Marketing Science. 2 (Spring), 95-134. Bentler, P.M. (1985), Theory and Implementation of EQS: A Structural Equation Program. Los Angeles: BMDP Statistical Software. and Douglas G. Bonett (1980), "Significance Tests and Goodness of Fit in the Analysis of Covariance Structures," Psychological Bulletin. 88 (November), 588-606. Bettman, James R. (1979), An Information Processing Theory of Consumer Behavior. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company. Blalock, Hubert M., Jr. (1982). Conceptualization and Measurement in the Social Sciences. Beverly Hills. CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Blunden, Robert G., T. K. Clarke, and M. Eileen MacDougall (1984). "A Simple Method for Predicting Starch Scores by Micro Computer," in Marketing 1984: Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Administrative Sciences Association of Canada—Marketing Division, Sheila. A. Brown, ed., 41-50. Boomsma, Anne (1982), "The Robustness of LISREL Against Small Sample Sizes in Factor Analysis Models," in Systems Under Indirect Observation. Part I. K. G. Joreskog and H. Wold, eds. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company, 149-73. Clancy, Kevin J., Lyman E. Ostlund, and Gordon A. Wyner (1979). "False Reporting of Magazine Readership," Journal of Advertising Research, 19 (October), 23-30. Cohen, Jacob and Patricia Cohen (1975), Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavorial Sciences. Hillsdaie NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Diamond, Daniel S. (1968), "A Quantitative Approach to Magazine Advertisement Format Selection," Journal of Marketing Research. 5 (November). 376-86. Dillon. William R., Ajith Kumar, and Narendra Mulani (1987), "Offending Estimates in Covariance Structure Analysis: Comments on the Causes of and Solutions to Heywood Cases," Psychological Bulletin. 101 (January), 126-35. Edell, Julie A. and Richard Staelin (1983), "The Information Processing of Pictures in Print Advertisements," Journal of Consumer Research. 10 (June), 4 5 - 6 1 . Ferguson, Leonard W. (1935), "The Imptirtance of the Mechanical Features of an Advertisement," Journal of Applied Psychology, 19 (October), 521-6. Fletcher, Alan D. and Paul R. Winn (1974), "An Intermagazine Analysis of Factors in Advertisement Readership," Journalism Quarterly. 51 (Autumn), 425-30. Frankel, Lester R. and Bernard H. Solov (1962), "Does Recall of an Advertisement Depend on Its Position in the Magazine?" Journal of Advertising Research, 2 (December), 2 8 32. Gardner, Burleigh B. and Yehudi A. Cohen (1964), "ROP Color and Its Effect on Newspaper Advertising," Journal of Marketing Research, 1 (May), 68-70. Gardner, Howard A. (1961), "Do Back-of-the-Book Ads Pull More Inquiries?" Industrial Marketing. 46 (November), 146-8. Gerbing, David W. and James C. Anderson (1987). "Improper Solutions in the Analysis of Covariance Structures: Their Interpretability and a Comparison of Altemative Respecifications," Psychometrika, 52 (March), 99-111. Greenwald. Anthony G. and Clark Leavitt (1984). "Audience Involvement in Advertising: Four Levels." Journat of Consumer Research, It (June), 581-92. Hanssens, Dominique M. and Barton A. Weitz (1980), "The Effectiveness of Industrial Print Advertisements Across Product Categories," Journal of Marketing Research. 17 (August), 294-306. Holbrook, Morris B. and Donald R. Lehmann (1980). "Form Versus Content in Predicting Starch Scores," Journal of Advertising Research, 20 (August), 53-62. Joreskog, Karl G. and Dag Sorbom (1984), USREL VI. Mooresville, IN: Scientific Software. Kroeber-Riel. Wemer (1984), "Effects of Emotional Pictorial Elements in Ads Analyzed by Means of Eye Movement 177 PRINT AD RECOGNITION SCORES Monitoring," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 11, Thomas C. Kinnear, ed. Provo, UT: Association tor Consumer Research. 591-6. Lucas. DarTell B. (1960), "The ABCs of ARF's PARM." Journal of Marketing. 25 (July), 9-20. and Steuart H. Britt (1963), Measuring Advertising Effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company. Marder, Eric and Mort David (1961), "Recognition of Ad Elements; Recall or Projection?" Journal of Advertising Research. I (December), 23-5. Mitchell, Andrew A. (1983). "The Effects of Visual and Emotional Advertising: An Information-Processing Approach," in Advertising and Consumer Psychology. Larry Percy and Arch G. Woodside, eds. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, 197-217. Myers, James H. and Ame F. Haug (1967), "Declarative vs. Interrogative Advertisement Headlines," Journal of Advertising Research. 1 (September), 41-4. Neu. D. Morgan (1983), Private communication from Vice President of Starch-INRA-Hooper. Rindskopf, David (1983). "Parameterizing Inequality Constraints on Unique Variances in Linear Structural Models." Psychometrika. 48 (March), 73-83. Rossiter. John R. (1981). "Predicting Starch Scores," Journal of Advertising Research, 21 (October), 63-8. Shimp, Terence A. and Larry G. Gresham (1983), "An Information-I^ocessing Perspective on Recent Advertising Literature." in Current Lssues and Research in Advertising 1983: Reviews of Selected Areas. James H. Leigh and Claude R. Martin, Jr.. eds. Ann Arbor: Division of Research, Graduate School of Business Administration, University of Michigan, 39-75. Singh, Surendra N. and Gilbert A. Churchill, Jr. (1986), "Using the Theory of Signal Detection to Improve Ad Recognition Testing," Journal of Marketing Research, 23 (November), 327-36. and Catherine A. Cole (1985), "Forced-Choice Recognition Tests; A Critical Review," Journal of Advertising, 14 (3), 52-8. Sobel. Michael E. and George W. Bohmstedt (1985), "Use of Null Models in Evaluating the Fit of Covariance Structure Models." in Sociological Methodology 1985. Nancy B. Tuma. ed. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers, 152-78. Soley, Lawrence C. and Leonard N. Reid (1983a), "Predicting Industrial Ad Readership." Industrial Marketing Management, 12 (July). 201-6. and (1983b). "Industrial Ad Readership as a Function of Headline Type," Journal of Advertising, 12(1), 34-8. Sparkman. Richard M.. Jr. (1985), "Cost Effectiveness of Advertising," International Journal of Advertising, 4 (2). 13141. Stansfield. Richard H. (1969), The Dartnell Advertising Manager's Handbook. Chicago: Dartnell. Starch, Daniel (1966), Measuring Advertising Readership and Results. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company. Surlin, Stuart H. and Herman H. Kosak (1975), "The Effect of Graphic Design in Advertising on Reader Ratings," Journalism Quarterly. 52 (Winter). 685-91. Troldahl. Veriing C. and Robert L. Jones (1965), "Predictors of Newspaper Advertisement Readership." Journal of Advertising Research. 5 (March), 23-7. Tumbull, A. T. and David E. Carter (1968), "Readership of Advertisements with All-Display Typography," Journal of Typographic Research, 2 (April), 157-69. Twedt, Dik Warren (1952), "A Multiple Factor Analysis of Advertising Readership," Journal of Applied Psychology. 36 (June), 207-15. Valiente, Rafael (1973), "Mechanical Correlates of Ad Recognition," y«urna/o/^i/vert/.vi>i^/?e.searc/i. 13 (June), 1318. van Driel, Otto P. (1978), "On Various Causes of Improper Solutions of Maximum Likelihood Factor Analysis," Psychometrika. 43 (June), 225-43. Warner. Lucien and Raymond Franzen (1947). "Value of Color in Advertising," Journal of Applied Psychology, 41 (June), 260-70. Wells, William D. (1964), "Recognition. Recall, and Ratings Scales." Journal of Advertising Research, 3 (September), 2-8. Yamanaka, Jiro (1962). "The Prediction of Ad Readership Scores," Journat of Advertising Research. 2 (March), 1823. Zinkhan, George M. and Betsy D. Gelb (1986), "What Starch Scores Predict." Journat of Advertising Research. 26 (Au- gust), 45-50.