Journalof P. Sheldon Media©Psychology : Unwillingness-to-Communicate 2008 Hogrefe 2008; & Vol. Huber 20(2):67–75 Publishers The Relationship Between Unwillingness-to-Communicate and Students’ Facebook Use This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Pavica Sheldon Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA Abstract. A survey with 172 students was conducted at a large southern research university to examine how unwillingness-to-communicate in interpersonal communication influences gratifications sought and gratifications obtained from Facebook use. The study investigated the relationship between two dimensions of unwillingness-to-communicate (approach-avoidance and reward) and different motives of Facebook use. In addition, it examined the relationship between unwillingness-to-communicate and the behavioral and attitudinal outcomes of Facebook use (e.g., the number of hours spent on Facebook, duration of use, the number of Facebook friends, satisfaction with Facebook). Results of multiple regression analysis revealed that respondents who felt anxiety and fears in their face-to-face communication used Facebook to pass time and feel less lonely more than other respondents, but they had fewer Facebook friends. Overall, this paper finds evidence that people who are involved in online relationships are those who are willing to communicate in real life, rather than the opposite. Such results seem to justify the rich-get-richer hypothesis, which states that the internet primarily benefits extraverted individuals. Our results are in contrast to findings that socially anxious individuals are more likely to form relationships online. Keywords: unwillingness-to-communicate, Facebook, online relationships, uses and gratifications Introduction Created in February 2004 by Mark Zuckerberg, a Harvard undergraduate student, Facebook, with its 21 million registered users and 1.6 billion page views each day, is one of the fastest growing social network sites (Needham & Company, 2007). At the most basic level, online social networks are internet communities where individuals interact, often through profiles that represent themselves to others. A recent survey showed that 93% of college students had a Facebook account. Most of them used Facebook on a daily basis, spending on average 47 minutes a day on the site (Sheldon, in press). Although much has been published about the risk of being addicted to, and spending too much time on, Facebook, little is known about personal characteristics of people who use the website. Past research (Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000) showed that internet users who avoided face-to-face interaction, or found it less rewarding, chose the internet as a functional alternative channel to fulfill interpersonal needs. Studying online relationships and networking is thus important not only because of their growing prevalence, but also because they provide opportunities to test existing theories of interpersonal and mediated communication in virtual environments. Lea and Spears (1995) write that scholars have “concentrated primarily on romance, friendship, and marriage among young, white, middle-class, heterosexual Westerners whose relationships are conducted in © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers the open . . .” (x), and there is much research to be done in “electronic relationships.” As college students spend more time online than previous generations, it is important to know about the gratifications they seek and obtain from the new media. Similarly, Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) suggested that with the widespread use of computer-mediated communication (CMC), we need better understanding of personal and social attributes that affect why people use CMC and the outcomes of CMC-related behavior. CMC “blurs” traditional boundaries between interpersonal and mass communication offering new opportunities for the way individuals relate to one another (Parks & Floyd, 1996). Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) used the unwillingness-to-communicate concept in their internet-use research. Unwillingness-to-communicate is “a chronic tendency to avoid and/or devalue oral communication and to view the communication situation as relatively unrewarding” (Burgoon, 1976, p. 60). Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) found that internet users who were socially anxious and avoided face-to-face interaction chose the internet as a functional alternative channel. This study examines how the unwillingness-to-communicate in interpersonal communication influences gratifications sought and gratifications obtained from Facebook use. The study investigates the relationship between two dimensions of unwillingness-to-communicate (approachavoidance and reward; Burgoon, 1976) and different motives of Facebook use. In addition, it examines the relationship between unwillingness-to-communicate and the beJournal of Media Psychology 2008; Vol. 20(2):67–75 DOI 10.1027/1864-1105.20.2.67 68 P. Sheldon: Unwillingness-to-Communicate This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. havioral and attitudinal outcomes of Facebook use (the number of hours spent on Facebook, duration of use, the number of Facebook friends, the number of people never met in person, the number of times logging into account, and satisfaction with Facebook). We assume that willingness to communicate offline will in some way be related to willingness to communicate online, but no assumptions are made concerning the direction of this relationship. Hence, the present study is primarily exploratory in nature. The study is grounded in uses and gratification theory and its extensions – rich-get-richer and social compensation hypotheses. Uses and Gratifications Theory Uses and gratifications theory takes into account that audiences differ in the gratifications they seek from the mass media. The most frequently found needs and gratifications have been classified as follows: diversion (escape from problems, emotional release), personal relationship (social utility of information in conversation, substitute of the media for companionship), personal identity (value reinforcement, self-understanding), and information (McQuail, Blumler, & Brown, 1972). These classifications, however, were developed for audiovisual media use and must be extended for internet use. During the last 15 years, researchers have developed different motivational scales for internet use. Morris and Ogan (1996) found that the internet fulfills interpersonal and mediated needs. Needs traditionally fulfilled by media are social interaction, passing time, habit, information, and entertainment (Flaherty, Pearce, & Rubin, 1998). Interpersonal needs fulfilled by media are a feeling of being less lonely, relationship maintenance, problem-solving, and persuasion (Flanagin & Metzger, 2001). Other researchers (LaRose, Mastro, & Eastin, 2001) found that the expectation of finding enjoyable activities online predicted the amount of consumption. Song, La Rose, Eastin, and Lin (2004) identified virtual community as a “new” gratification that emphasized communication with people met through the internet. In contrast to this definition of virtual community, relationship maintenance focused on maintaining relationships with existing acquaintances (Song et al., 2004). Research suggests that the patterns and motives behind online communication usage are, in part, a function of demographic and personality variables. Ward and Tracey (2004) hypothesized that persons who become involved in online relationships are those with difficulties in face-toface communication. Online, they can communicate with others anonymously at their own pace. Online relationships provide minimized social risk, as one does not have to meet other participants face-to-face (Curtis, 1997). McKenna (1998) also found that socially anxious individuals were more likely to form relationships online. Yet other studies found that anxiety and loneliness were not in fact indicators of who is more likely to form an online relationship (BoneJournal of Media Psychology 2008; Vol. 20(2):67–75 brake, 2002; McCown, Fisher, Page, & Homant, 2001; Peter, Valkenburg, & Schouten, 2005). These results suggest that individuals who form online relationships tend to be no less socially skilled than those who do not form online relationships (McCown et al., 2001). Introversion resulted in less frequent, rather than more frequent, online communication (Peter et al., 2005). However, the more time people spend online communicating with one other person, the more new relationships they are likely to form with other persons as well (Bonebrake, 2002). Rich-Get-Richer Hypothesis and Unwillingness-to-Communicate These results are indicative of two general, opposing hypotheses that have been proposed to explain the relationship between internet use and psychological well-being (Kraut, Patterson, Lundmark, Kiesler, Mukopadhyay et al., 1998). The first, the rich-get-richer hypothesis, states that the internet primarily benefits extraverted individuals (Kraut, Kiesler, Boneva, Cummings, Helgeson et al., 2002). The social compensation hypothesis, in contrast, proposes that the internet benefits introverts more (McKenna & Bargh, 2000). Studies based on the social compensations hypothesis showed that the anonymity and reduced cues prevalent on the internet might stimulate online self-disclosure, because there is no fear of being ridiculed or rejected (Derlega, Metts, Petronio, & Marqulis, 1993; Pennebaker, 1989). This may be particularly appealing to introverts when trying to open up. Morahan-Martin and Schumacher (2003) found that lonely people perceive the anonymity of the internet as liberating. In fact, Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) concluded that unwillingness-to-communicate, a measure closely associated with alienation, introversion, communication apprehension, and reticence (Burgoon, 1976), led to greater use and reliance on internet communication tools. Unwillingness-to-Communicate Burgoon (1976) created a two-dimensional scale to measure unwillingness-to-communicate: (a) approach-avoidance, and (b) reward. Approach-avoidance (UCS-AA) identifies the “degree to which individuals feel anxiety and fears about interpersonal encounters” (p. 63). Reward reflects the “degree to which people perceive that friends and family do not seek them out for conversation and opinion, and that interactions with others are manipulative and untruthful” (Burgoon & Hale, 1983, p. 240). Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) used unwillingness-to-communicate in their internet use research. They found that internet users who were socially anxious and avoided face-to-face interaction chose the internet as a functional alternative channel. Armstrong and Rubin (1989) found that radio callers were less © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. P. Sheldon: Unwillingness-to-Communicate 69 willing to communicate in face-to-face interaction and perceived face-to-face communication to be less rewarding. Recently, Ma and Leung (2005) investigated the effects of unwillingness-to-communicate on self-disclosure in ICQ (“I seek you”) conversations. They found opposite results. People who were less willing to communicate in real life also tended to be less open in disclosing their opinions and beliefs online. How (un)willingness to communicate in real life relates to (un)willingness to communicate in social networking sites, however, has yet to be addressed. networking sites like Facebook, MySpace, and MyYearbook to make new friends and stay close to old friends and family. They are the “Look at Me” generation who post personal profiles with photos online. Previous research, however, has mostly been theory-free and has not systematically considered motives underlying media use as they have been studied in the context of, for instance, the uses and gratifications approach. Taking this previous research and especially the concept of unwillingness-to-communicate into account, our first research question asks: Social Networks and Facebook – RQ1: What are the motives for Facebook use and how does unwillingness-to-communicate in a real life context relate to different motives of Facebook use? Social networks represent one of three types of cyber communities (besides chat systems, such as instant messaging, and blogs; Coley, 2006). The main purpose of social networks is making new friendships or to maintain those that already exist. Online social networks encompass online dating sites, as well as popular social networking websites like MySpace, Xanga, Live Journal, and Facebook. The difference between chat rooms and social networking sites is that the majority of communication in online social networks takes place asynchronously and within the network of “friends” that the user has established (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007). Social network sites can be oriented toward workrelated contexts (e.g., LinkedIn.com), romantic relationship initiation (e.g., Friendster.com), or connecting those with shared interests, such as music or politics (e.g., MySpace.com). The Pew Research Center (2007) found that the internet’s major benefit is in helping people tap into social networks. One of these networks is Facebook, an internet site created in February 2004 by Mark Zuckerberg, a Harvard undergraduate student. Facebook enables its users to present themselves in an online profile, accumulate “friends” who can post comments on each other’s pages, and view each other’s profiles. Facebook members can also join virtual groups based on common interests, see what classes they have in common, and learn about others’ hobbies, interests, tastes, and romantic relationship statuses through the profiles (Ellison et al., 2007). Motives for Facebook Use Coley (2006) says that most students use Facebook for fun, to organize parties, and to find dates. They like the opportunity to find others with similar interests, students with whom they are in class together, and with whom feel a sense of community and connectedness. Another motive for Facebook use is that students are already online, and checking Facebook thus becomes a routine matter. According to the Pew Research Center (2007), young adults who have grown up with personal computers, cell phones, and the internet (“Generation Next”) use social © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Behavioral and Attitudinal Outcomes of Facebook Use According to the uses and gratifications approach, a person’s social and psychological characteristics influence not only motives for communicating (their gratifications sought), but also gratifications obtained. While gratifications sought are measured as what the audience’s reasons are for using Facebook, gratifications obtained are measured by determining what audiences feel they get out of using Facebook. In this study, we measure Facebook use as the amount of Facebook use and the duration of Facebook use (Rubin, 1993) as well as the frequency of updating the Facebook profile. We measured the number of friends people have on Facebook and the percentage of friends they have never met in person. As Palmgreen and Rayburn (1985) suggested, this study measured users’ satisfaction with Facebook gratifications and how much they would miss the site if it suddenly disappeared. Following these previous studies, our second research question asks: – RQ2: To what extent can unwillingness-to-communicate predict behavioral and attitudinal outcomes of Facebook use? Method Sample To address these questions, we conducted a survey with 172 students at a large research university. The sample for the survey consisted of students enrolled in two large, interpersonal communication classes. Of the sample surveyed, 93% (N = 160) of students had a Facebook account and 7% (12) currently did not have the account. Of those who had an account, 43% (n = 74) were men and 57% (n = 98) were women. The average age of respondents was 20 (M = 19.92, SD = 1.23). Journal of Media Psychology 2008; Vol. 20(2):67–75 70 P. Sheldon: Unwillingness-to-Communicate This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Measurement We asked participants to complete the questionnaire on their Facebook use. Participation was voluntary, and students received credit if they filled in the survey. Overall, they spent approximately 5–7 min on the survey. Students who never had a Facebook account and stopped answering questions after the first elimination also received credit but were excluded from further analysis. The survey measured: – Basic demographics – Unwillingness-to-communicate (Burgoon, 1976) – Gratifications of Facebook use: entertainment, escape, passing time, coolness, relationship maintenance, social interaction, virtual community, companionship – Facebook use: the amount of use, duration of use, the number of Facebook friends, the number of people never met in person, the frequency of logging into one’s account, satisfaction with Facebook Demographics Respondents indicated whether they were male or female, and were asked to give their age. Unwillingness-to-Communicate In this study, we used a 20-item unwillingness-to-communicate scale (Burgoon, 1976). Two dimensions, approachavoidance (UCS-AA) and reward (UCS-R), each contained 10 items. Items were measured with a 5-point Likert scale, with 5 = strongly agree and 1 = strongly disagree. We then summed items into two scales. High scores implied that the respondents were anxious or fearful about interpersonal encounters. The mean score of all the items for the UCS-AA dimension was 2.52 (SD = .76, Cronbach’s α = .88), whereas the mean score for the UC-R dimension was 1.76 (SD = .59, Cronbach’s α = .85). Motives A pool of gratification items was assembled from prior internet gratifications studies (Flaherty et al., 1998; Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000; Flanagin & Metzger, 2001). However, items were redefined so that they fit Facebook users’ needs (e.g., “To post a message on my friend’s wall,” “To see which of the people I know joined Facebook”). In the questionnaire, respondents were asked how much they use Facebook for the given reasons. A 5-point Likert scale was used in rating 38 gratifications items, ranging from “5” (exactly) to “1” (not at all). Factor analyses extracted factors related to gratifications of the internet. The factor analysis used a principal component solution and varimax rotation and specified the retention of factors with eigenvalues Journal of Media Psychology 2008; Vol. 20(2):67–75 greater than 1.0. This resulted in six factors that accounted for 60% of the variance (Table 1). Facebook Use and Attitudes Facebook use was measured as the amount of Facebook use and the duration of Facebook use (Rubin, 1993), as well as the frequency of logging into one’s account and of updating the Facebook profile. The measure of relationship development was the number of friends that respondents had on Facebook (and the percentage of friends they had never met in person). Users’ satisfaction with Facebook gratifications was assessed with a single item: “Overall, how satisfied are you with the job that Facebook does in providing you with the things you are seeking?” Response options ranged from extremely satisfied (5) to not at all satisfied (1). Results The goal of this study was to examine the motives for Facebook use and how motives relate to two dimensions of unwillingness-to-communicate, approach-avoidance, and reward. The study also investigated to what extent unwillingness-to-communicate could predict the behavioral and attitudinal outcomes of Facebook use. Students in this sample reported that, on average, they spent 47 min a day on Facebook. The majority of students (50%) changed their profile every few months. 19% changed their profile every day, and another 19% changed it one-to-three times per week. The majority of students had between 200 and 350 Facebook friends. RQ1: Unwillingness-to-Communicate and Facebook Motives The first research question asked for the motives of Facebook use and about the relationship between unwillingness-to-communicate in a real life context and various motives of Facebook use. Results of factor analysis yielded six interpretable factors or motives for Facebook use (see above and Table 1). – Factor 1 was labeled Relationship Maintenance (eigenvalue = 10.73). It contained six items (e.g., “To send a message to a friend,” “To post a message on my friend’s wall”) and accounted for 31% of the total variance after rotation. This factor depicted a use of Facebook to maintain relationships with existing acquaintances (Song et al., 2004). – Factor 2, Passing Time (eigenvalue = 3.94), contained four items (e.g., “To occupy my time,” “To pass time when bored”) and accounted for 11.2% of the total variance. In © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers P. Sheldon: Unwillingness-to-Communicate 71 Table 1. Motives for Facebook use: primary factor loadings Loading Eigenvalue Variance α 10.73 31 .90 3.94 11.2 .83 1.84 5.2 .80 1.62 4.6 .84 1.48 4.2 .76 1.41 4 .76 Factor 1: Relationship Maintenance To send a message to a friend .74 To post a message on my friend’s wall .70 To communicate with my friends .83 To stay in touch with friends .78 Get in touch with people I know .72 Get through to someone who is hard to reach .58 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Factor 2: Passing Time To pass time when bored .67 It is one of the routine things I do when online .61 To occupy my time .74 To check my wall after I receive e-mail from Facebook .74 Factor 3: Virtual Community Develop a romantic relationship .77 Find more interesting people than in real life .70 Find companionship .86 Meet new friends .65 To feel less lonely .52 Factor 4: Entertainment To see other people’s pictures .59 It is entertaining .56 To read other people’s profiles .67 To enjoy it .61 To see which of the people I know who joined the Facebook .62 Factor 5: Coolness It makes me cool among my peers .76 Have fun .66 It is cool .60 Factor 6: Companionship To feel less lonely .51 No one to talk or be with .75 So I won’t be alone Total variance explained = 60% .83 a previous study, the motive has been found to be particularly salient to the internet (Flaherty et al., 1998). – Factor 3, Virtual Community (eigenvalue = 1.83), consisted of five items (e.g., “To feel less lonely,” “To meet new friends”) and explained 5.2% of the total variance. This factor, as opposed to maintaining relationships with existing acquaintances, emphasized communication with people met through the internet. It was named “virtual community” following the terminology Song et al. (2004) introduced – Factor 4, Entertainment (eigenvalue = 1.62), consisted of five items (e.g., “To read other people’s profiles,” “It is entertaining”) and explained 4.6% of the total variance. However, the factor had a high mean score, suggesting that entertainment is a strong gratification sought in Facebook use. © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers – Factor 5, Coolness (eigenvalue = 1.48), consisted of three items (e.g., “It is cool,” “Have fun”) and explained 4.2% of the total variance. Like “Passing Time,” this factor has also been found to be pertinent to internet use in previous research. Charney and Greenberg (2001) introduced this terminology. – Factor 6, Companionship (eigenvalue = 1.41), consisted of three items (e.g., “To feel less lonely,” “No one to talk or be with”), and explained 4% of the total variance. It is connected with loneliness and regarded as mediated interpersonal needs (Flanagin & Metzger, 2001). Passing Time (M = 3.88, SD = 1.23) and Relationship Maintenance (M = 3.64, SD = 1.24) factors had the highest mean scores. Entertainment (M = 3.23, SD = 1.19) was also a salient factor for using Facebook. Less important reasons Journal of Media Psychology 2008; Vol. 20(2):67–75 72 P. Sheldon: Unwillingness-to-Communicate Table 2. Internet Motives Scale “I use the Facebook for the following reasons” M SD To send a message to a friend 3.62 1.19 To post a message on my friend’s wall 3.61 1.31 To communicate with my friends 3.84 1.22 To stay in touch with friends 3.92 1.22 Get in touch with people I know 3.49 1.19 Get through to someone who is hard to reach 3.37 1.31 To pass time when bored 4.10 1.07 It is one of the routine things I do when online 4.04 1.24 To occupy my time 3.52 1.33 To check my wall after I receive an e-mail from Facebook 3.85 1.29 Develop a romantic relationship 1.15 .51 Find more interesting people than in real life 1.22 .58 Find companionship 1.18 .49 Meet new friends 1.60 .87 To feel less lonely 1.30 .55 To see other people’s pictures 3.67 1.18 It is entertaining 3.58 1.21 To read other people’s profiles 3.05 1.24 To enjoy it 3.61 1.19 To see which of the people I know who joined the Facebook 2.25 1.15 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Factor 1: Relationship Maintenance Factor 2: Passing Time Factor 3: Virtual community Factor 4: Entertainment Factor 5: Coolness It makes me cool among my peers 1.52 1.12 Have fun 2.62 1.23 It is cool 2.22 1.22 To feel less lonely 1.28 .57 No one to talk or be with 1.50 .92 So I won’t be alone 1.26 .64 Factor 6: Companionship were Coolness (M = 2.12, SD = 1.19), Companionship (M = 1.35, SD = .78), and Virtual Community (M = 1.29, SD = 0.6) (Table 2). The internal consistency of the factors, assessed by Cronbach’s α, ranged from .75 to .90 and thus can be considered satisfactory (Table 1). Next, we looked at the relationship between motives for Facebook use and the two dimensions of unwillingness-tocommunicate. Pearson product moment correlations were computed. Results (Table 3) revealed a positive correlation between UCS-AA and the passing time motive, and revealed a positive correlation between UCS-AA and going on Facebook to feel less lonely. A positive association was also found between UCS-reward and the companionship motive, and a negative association was found between UCS-AA and the virtual community motive. Overall, results thus show that respondents who were unwilling to communicate offline tended to go to Facebook to pass time when bored or to feel less lonely but not to meet new friends. When entered into six regression equations, one for each motive, UCS-AA and UCS-reward were, in line with the correlational results, significant predictors of three motives for Facebook use. UCS-AA was the predictor of going on Facebook to pass time when bored (B = .29, SE = .10, β = .22, p < .01, R = .22, R² = .05, F(1, 161) = 8.12) and to feel less lonely (B = .20, SE = .10, β = .16, p < .05, R = .25, R² = .06, F(2, 160) = 10.94). UCS-reward was the significant predictor of going on Facebook to feel less lonely (B = .43, SE = .13, β = .26, p < .001). Results thus reveal that students who were anxious about face-to-face communication went to Facebook to pass time or to not be alone, but were less likely to expect to meet new people. RQ2: Unwillingness-to-Communicate and Facebook Attitudes and Behaviors The second research question asked to what extent unwillingness-to-communicate can predict behavioral and attitudinal outcomes of Facebook use. Results revealed a negative correlation between UCS-AA and the number of Facebook friends, r(172) = –.23, p < . 01, suggesting that those who were fearful of interpersonal encounters tended to have fewer Facebook friends. A positive but weak correlation existed between UCS-reward and the frequency of logging into the Facebook account, r(172) = .19, p < .05, indicating that respondents who found interpersonal communication to be less rewarding logged on to the Facebook Table 3. Correlations for unwillingness-to-communicate and Facebook motives Relationship maintenance Passing time Virtual community Entertainment Coolness Companionship .13 .25* Students (n = 172) UCS – AA –.05 .22* –.03 –.03 UCS – R –.12 .12 –.16* –.06 .09 *Correlation is significant at the .05 level, one-tailed; **correlation is significant at the .01 level, one-tailed. Journal of Media Psychology 2008; Vol. 20(2):67–75 .31** © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers P. Sheldon: Unwillingness-to-Communicate 73 Table 4. Correlations for unwillingness-to-communicate and Facebook attitudes and behaviors Subscale No. hours on Facebook No. of loggings No. of Facebook friends No. of strangers Changing profile Satisfaction .05 .05 .09 Students (n = 172) UCS-AA –.07 .01 –.23** This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. UCS – R –.13 .19* –.12 .12 .04 *Correlation is significant at the .05 level, one-tailed; **correlation is significant at the .01 level, one-tailed. account more often. Unwillingness-to-communicate in real life did not correlate with the number of hours students spent on Facebook, the number of strangers they had amongst their friends, or how satisfied they were with the site. These results were again supported by stepwise multiple regression analysis; results revealed UCS-AA as being a predictor of the number of Facebook friends respondents had (B = –.37, SE = .12, β = –.23, p < .01, F(1, 169) = 9.34, R = .23, R² = .05). UCS-reward was the predictor of the frequency of logging into Facebook accounts (B = .39, SE = .15, β = .20, p < .01, F(1, 169) = 6.84, R = .20, R² = .04). In addition to the research question posed in this study, results revealed a positive correlation between the number of hours spent on Facebook and the number of Facebook friends, r(172) = .22, p < .01. Students who spent more time on Facebook tended to have more Facebook friends (Table 4). Discussion Because little is known about the characteristics of people who use Facebook, we administered a survey with 172 students to examine how unwillingness-to-communicate in real life influences gratifications sought and gratifications obtained from Facebook use. Of the sample surveyed, 93% of students had a Facebook account and 7% did not have the account. Students in this sample reported that, on average, they spent 47 min a day on Facebook. Overall, 81% of students logged into Facebook on a daily basis. The majority of students had between 200 and 350 Facebook friends. The largest proportion of students go to Facebook to maintain relationships with people they know, for instance, by sending a message to a friend, posting a message on their friend’s wall, staying in touch with them, or getting in touch with someone who is difficult to reach. A larger proportion of students go to Facebook to pass time when bored or after they receive an e-mail notifying them of a wall posting. A significant number of students use Facebook for entertainment reasons. A smaller number of people use it to develop new relationships or to meet new people. This supports what Tewksbury and Althaus (2000) suggested: that entertainment and passing time – gratifications typically associated with television and newspaper use – prove to be significant predictors of using Facebook, an internet social network website. Generally, the findings of this study are © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers –.02 also consistent with findings of Flaherty et al. (1998) that people use computers to satisfy needs traditionally fulfilled by media (i.e., pass time, habit, information, and entertainment). It supported LaRose et al. (2001) in that the expectation of finding enjoyable activities online predicted the amount of consumption. Next, results indicate that respondents who feel anxiety and fears in their face-to-face communication use Facebook more to pass time and to feel less lonely than other respondents. However, those individuals tend to have fewer Facebook friends rather than more. These findings are inconsistent with McKenna’s (1998) study, which found that socially anxious individuals are more likely to form relationships online. In our case, the types of people who are involved in online relationships tend to be those who are willing to communicate in real life rather than the opposite. These individuals also have more Facebook friends and initiate new relationships online more than do individuals who view face-to-face communication as relatively rewarding. Hence, our results also differ from the findings by Ward and Tracey (2004) and Wolak, Mitchell, and Finkelhor (2003), who suggest that people involved in online relationships are those with difficulties in face-to-face communication. Our results seem to justify the rich-get-richer hypothesis, which states that the internet primarily benefits extraverted individuals (Kraut et al., 2002), and that introverts communicate online less often (Peter et al., 2005; Bonebrake, 2002). Although the results reveal that persons unsatisfied with their face-to-face interactions tend to have fewer Facebook friends, those individuals log into Facebook more often than others. One of the explanations may be that they do not self-disclose on Facebook enough to form new relationships, although they visit the site more often. As we know, self-disclosure is central to relationship development, and Ma and Leung (2005) found that people who are less willing to communicate in real life also tend to be less open online. Future studies should investigate to what extent respondents disclose on Facebook. In our study, less satisfaction with face-to-face communication does not significantly correlate with the number of hours students spend on Facebook, the number of strangers students have among their friends, or how satisfied they are with the site. This is opposite to what Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) found, that users who feel more valued by their friends and family and score lower on an unwillingness-to-communicate scale feel more satisfied with the inJournal of Media Psychology 2008; Vol. 20(2):67–75 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 74 P. Sheldon: Unwillingness-to-Communicate ternet. This is an interesting finding and, once again, it suggests that reasons for Facebook use may be more similar to reasons for face-to-face communication rather than for mediated communication. This study has several limitations. First, we used a convenient student sample of students enrolled in interpersonal communication classes. This increases the chance that students may be more willing to communicate face to face and, thus, score lower on unwillingness-to-communicate than other samples. The use of a convenience sample also limits the generality of the findings. The relationship between unwillingness to communicate and Facebook use is interpreted based upon previous studies that investigated similar or parallel constructs (e.g., the relationship between loneliness and internet use, and between introversion and internet use). Second, we cannot establish the causal relationship between unwillingness-to-communicate and Facebook use. Finally, most correlation coefficients indicate only weak relationships between unwillingness-to-communicate and Facebook use. Future studies should include other personal predictors for Facebook use, such as locus of control, and should test for relationships between self-disclosure and relationship development on Facebook. Studies should be conducted using other theoretical approaches, such as the social penetration theory. As Facebook has a great influence on college students and other adults around the world, more multi-method studies are needed to explain how and why Facebook is used, who uses it and other social networking sites, and, finally, what the consequences are of spending hours on the site. A structural equation model, with motives as endogenous and unwillingness-to-communicate dimensions as exogenous constructs, can be tested for significance of relationships between the two. As it is, we have learned that college students use Facebook in the same way they use interpersonal communication, primarily to maintain their relationships or pass time when bored. There is no evidence that students who are unwilling-to-communicate offline would develop more relationships online. Rather, it seems that the rich-get-richer hypothesis holds true. References Armstrong, C.B., & Rubin, A.M. (1989). Talk radio as interpersonal communication. Journal of Communication, 39, 84–94. Bonebrake, K. (2002). College students’ internet use, relationship formation, and personality correlates. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 5, 551–557. Burgoon, J.K. (1976). The unwillingness to communicate scale: Development and validation. Communication Monographs, 43, 60–69. Burgoon, J.K., & Hale, J.L. (1983). A research note on the dimensions of communication reticence. Communication Quarterly, 31, 238–248. Journal of Media Psychology 2008; Vol. 20(2):67–75 Charney, T., & Greenberg, B. (2001). Uses and gratifications of the internet. In C. Lin & D. Atkin (Eds.), Communication, technology and society: New media adoption and uses and gratifications (pp. 383–406). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton. Coley, T. (2006). Students and cyber communities. University of South Carolina. Curtis, P. (1997). Mudding: Social phenomena in text-based virtual realities. In S. Kiesler (Ed.), Culture of the internet (pp. 121–142). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Derlega, V.L., Metts, S., Petronio, S., & Marqulis, S.T. (1993). Self-disclosure. London: Sage. Ellison, N.B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook “friends”: Social capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 1143–1168. Flaherty, L.M., Pearce, K., & Rubin, R.B. (1998). Internet and face-to-face communication: Not functional alternatives. Communication Quarterly, 46, 250–268. Flanagin, A.J., & Metzger, M.J. (2001). Internet use in the contemporary media environment. Human Communication Research, 27, 153–181. Kraut, R., Kiesler, S., Boneva, B., Cummings, J., Helgeson, V., & Crawford, A. (2002). Internet paradox revisited. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 49–74. Kraut, R., Patterson, M., Lundmark, V., Kiesler, S., Mukopadhyay, T., & Scherlis, W. (1998). Internet paradox: A social technology that reduces social involvement and psychological well-being? American Psychologist, 53, 1017–1031. LaRose, R., Mastro, D., & Eastin, M.S. (2001). Understanding internet usage: A social-cognitive approach to uses and gratifications. Social Science Computer Review, 19, 395–414. Lea, M., & Spears, R. (1995). Love at first byte? Building personal relationships over computer networks. In J.T. Wood & S. Duck (Eds.), Understudied relationships: Off the beaten track (pp. 197–233). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ma, M.L., & Leung, L. (2005). Unwillingness-to-communicate, perceptions of the internet and self-disclosure in ICQ. Telematics and Informatics, 23, 22–37. McCown, J.A., Fischer, D., Page, R., Homant, M. (2001). Internet relationships: People who meet people. CyberPsychology and Behavior, 4, 593–596. McKenna, K.Y.A. (1998). The computers that bind: Relationship formation on the internet. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio University. McKenna, K., & Bargh, J. (2000). Plan 9 from cyberspace: The implications of the internet for personality and social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 57–75. McQuail, D., Blumler, J.G., & Brown, J.R. (1972). The television audience: Revised perspective. In D. McQuail (Ed.), Sociology of mass communications (pp. 135–165). Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin. Morahan-Martin, J., & Schumacher, P. (2003). Loneliness and social uses of the internet. Computers in Human Behavior, 19, 659–671. Morris, M., & Ogan, C. (1996). The internet as mass medium. Journal of Communication, 46, 39–50. Needham & Company (2007). Needham Capital Partners. Retrieved May 24, 2007, from http://www.needhamcapital.com Palmgreen, P., & Rayburn, J.D., II. (1985). A comparison of gratification models of media satisfaction. Communication Monographs, 52, 334–346. © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. P. Sheldon: Unwillingness-to-Communicate Papacharissi, Z., & Rubin, A. (2000). Predictors of internet use. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 44, 75–196. Parks, M.R., & Floyd, K. (1996). Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of Communication, 46, 80–97. Pennebaker, J.W. (1989). Confession, inhibition, and disease. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 221–244). New York: Academic Press. Peter, J., Valkenburg, P.M., & Schouten, A.P. (2005). Developing a model of adolescent friendship formation on the internet. CyberPsychology and Behavior, 8, 423–430. The Pew Research Center for the People & The Press (January 9, 2007). How young people view their lives, futures and politics. Retrieved January 16, 2007, from http://pewresearch.org/pubs/ 278/a-portrait-of-generation-next. Rubin, A.M. (1993). The effect of locus of control on communication motivation, anxiety, and satisfaction. Communication Quarterly, 41, 161–171. Song, I., LaRose, R., Eastin, M.S., & Lin, C.A. (2004). Internet gratifications and internet addiction: On the uses and abuses of new media. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 7, 384–393. Sheldon, P. (in press). Student favorite: Facebook & motives for its use. Southwestern Journal of Mass Communication. Tewksbury, D., & Althaus, S.L. (2000). An examination of motivations for using the World Wide Web. Communication Research Reports, 17, 127–138. Ward, C.C., & Tracey, T.J.G. (2004). Relation of shyness with aspects of online relationship involvement. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21, 611–623. Wolak, J., Mitchell, K.J., & Finkelhor, D. (2003). Escaping or connecting? Characteristics of youth who form close online relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 26, 105–119. 75 Pavica Sheldon is a doctoral student at Louisiana State University (LSU) in Baton Rouge, LA, USA. Originally from Croatia, Pavica received a Masters in mass communication at LSU in May 2006 and is now working toward a PhD in the Department of Communication Studies. Her research interests include media psychology, social networking, uses and gratifications, and intercultural communication. Pavica Sheldon Department of Communication Studies Louisiana State University 136 Coates Hall Baton Rouge, LA 70803 USA Tel. +1 225 573 5342 Fax +1 225 578 4828 E-mail pjuric1@lsu.edu Date of acceptance: 14 November, 2007 © 2008 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Journal of Media Psychology 2008; Vol. 20(2):67–75