The Mind of the Terrorist: A Review and Critique of Psychological Approaches Author(s): Jeff Victoroff Reviewed work(s): Source: The Journal of Conflict Resolution, Vol. 49, No. 1 (Feb., 2005), pp. 3-42 Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/30045097 . Accessed: 22/06/2012 18:20 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. Sage Publications, Inc. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Journal of Conflict Resolution. http://www.jstor.org The Mind of the Terrorist A REVIEW AND CRITIQUE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL APPROACHES JEFFVICTOROFF Departmentof Neurologyand Psychiatry Universityof SouthernCaliforniaSchool of Medicine This articlereviewsthe stateof the artof availabletheoriesanddataregardingthe psychology of terrorism. Dataandtheoreticalmaterialwere gatheredfrom the world'sunclassifiedliterature.Multipletheories andsome demographicdatahave been published,butvery few controlledempiricalstudieshave been conductedinvestigatingthe psychological bases of terrorism.The field is largelycharacterizedby theoretical speculationbased on subjectiveinterpretationof anecdotalobservations.Moreover,most studiesandtheories fail to takeintoaccountthe greatheterogeneityof terrorists.Manypractical,conceptual,andpsychological barriershave slowed progressin this importantfield. Nonetheless,even at this early stage of terrorism studies,preliminaryreportssuggestthatmodifiablesocial andpsychologicalfactorscontributeto the genesis of the terroristmind-set.Psychologicalscholarshipcouldpossibly mitigatetheriskof catastrophicattack by initiatingthe long overduescientific study of terroristmentalities. Keywords: terrorism;terrorist;psychiatry;psychology;sociology; homelandsecurity Terrorism has surelyexisted since before the dawn of recordedhistory(Merariand Friedland 1985). Human nature has not changed. However, three interlocking trends have significantly changed the nature and degree of the threat: the globalization of commerce, travel, and information transfer, which puts economic disparities and ideological competition in sharp relief and facilitates cooperative aggression by far-flung but like-minded conspirators; the ascent of religious fundamentalism as an aggrieved competitor with the market-economic, democratic, and secular trends of modernity; and the privatization of weapons of mass destruction, putting the potential of macroterrorist acts into the hands of small groups or even individuals (Hoffman 1998; Laqueur 1999; Enders and Sandler 2000). September 11, 2001, is one result-and probablya warningof events to come (Gunaratna2002). It perhapswould not be an exaggeration to state that these fast-evolving trends together constitute a clear and present danger to the security of civilization (Stern 1999). AUTHOR'SNOTE:This work was supportedin partby a grantfrom the FreyaFoundationfor Brain, Behavior,andSociety.I gratefullyacknowledgecriticalreviewsof this manuscriptby JessicaSternandTodd Sandler.Itrepresentsa revisionof a lecturefirstpresentedat the annualmeetingof the AmericanNeuropsychiatricAssociation, San Diego, California,March2002. Addressreprintrequeststo victorof@usc.edu. Vol.49 No. 1, February JOURNAL 20053-42 OFCONFLICT RESOLUTION, DOI: 10.1177/0022002704272040 © 2005 Sage Publications 4 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION It would seem appropriatefor the scholarlydisciplinesof psychology andpsychiatry to bringtheirintellectualresourcesto bearon the politicalproblemof terrorism,a problemthat-stripped to thebasics-is one of atypicalhumanbehavior.Apartfroma drive for truth,political psychological theory advises that the better a targetgroup understandsthe roots of the terroristmind-set,the betterthatgroupmay developpolicies to effectively manage the risk (Wardlaw1989; Clayton, Barlow, and BallifSpanvill1998). Despitethe compellingneedfor suchan understanding,manytheoretical andpracticalimpedimentshave delayed,andperhapseven derailed,the objective scientific psychological study of terrorism(Reich 1998; Horgan2003). Indeed, the following questionmust be asked:to what degreeare leadingpsychologicaltheories of terrorismsupportedby valid concepts and objective research?A comprehensive review of the literaturesuggests that a lack of systematicscholarlyinvestigationhas left policy makersto design counterterrorismstrategieswithout the benefit of facts regardingthe origin of terroristbehavior-or, worse, guidedby theoreticalpresumptions couched as facts. Investigatingthe terroristmind may be a necessaryfirst step towardactualizingmodernpolitical psychology's potentialfor uncoveringthe bases policy. of terroristaggressionand designing an optimumcounterterrorism Informationfor this articlewas derivedfrom a review of the unclassifiedliterature on psychosocial aspects of terrorism,including peer-reviewedarticles, books and book chapters,news reports,and personalcommunicationswith terrorismexperts. Scholarlyarticleswere identifiedby a searchfor the term terrorismin the following databases:PsychINFO (1887-2003), Sociological Abstracts(1974-2003), Medline (1966-2003), andLexis-NexisAcademicUniverse(1980-2003), as well as frombibliographiesof the identifiedarticles.This articlecriticallyreviewspublishedtheoriesof the psychologicalbases of terrorism,reviewsthe psychosocialdatadescribingterrorists, defines the limits of andimpedimentsto inquiryin this field, andoffers a preliminarypolitical-psychologicalclassificationof terrorism. DEFINITION AND DIMENSIONS OF TERRORIST BEHAVIOR Schmid (1983) compiled 109 academic definitionsof terrorism,suggesting that there are roughly as many availabledefinitionsas there are publishedexpertsin the field. The lack of consensusis to some extentinescapable,given the heterogeneityof terroristbehaviors,the varietyof declaredor assumedmotivations,andthe questionof point of view, a.k.a.,the "oneman'sterroristis anotherman'sfreedomfighter"problem (Jenkins1982; Hoffman 1998). Nonetheless,two common elements are usually foundin contemporarydefinitions:(1) thatterrorisminvolvesaggressionagainstnoncombatantsand(2) thatthe terroristactionin itself is not expectedby its perpetratorto accomplisha political goal but insteadto influencea targetaudienceand changethat audience'sbehaviorin a way thatwill servethe interestsof the terrorist(Badey 1998; Laqueur1999). The typology of terrorismis complex and controversialsince actorscan be characterizedacross multiplevariables.Schultz (1980) proposedseven such variables- Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 5 TABLE 1 Dimensions of Terrorism Variable Perpetratornumber Sponsorship Relationto authority Locale Militarystatus Spiritualmotivation Financialmotivation Political ideology Hierarchicalrole Willingnessto die Target Methodology Classification Individualvs. group State vs. substatevs. individual vs. pro-state/pro-establishment Anti-state/anti-establishment/separatist Intrastatevs. transnational Civilian vs. paramilitaryor military Secularvs. religious Idealisticvs. entrepreneurial Leftist/socialistvs. rightist/fascistvs. anarchist Sponsorvs. leaderversusmiddle managementvs. follower Suicidal vs. nonsuicidal Property(includingdata)vs. individualsvs. masses of people Bombing, assassination,kidnapping/hostagetaking,mass poisoning, rape,other (e.g., bioterrorism,cyberterrorism) causes, environment,goals, strategy,means, organization,and participation-that might be specified for revolutionaryversus subrevolutionaryterrorism.Post (2004) usefully dividedpoliticalsubstateterrorisminto (1) social revolutionaryterrorism,(2) right-wingterrorism,(3) nationalist-separatist terrorism,(4) religious extremistterrorism,and (5) single-issue (e.g., animalrights) terrorism,proposingthat each type tends to be associatedwith its own social-psychologicaldynamics.A more comprehensive typology is shown in Table 1, listing variablessubjectto analysis and classificationswithin those variables. Any such typology mustbe considereda heuristiccompendiumof ideal types, and classes shouldnot necessarilybe construedas dichotomous.Forexample,while many instances of collective violence unequivocallymeet the criteriafor state terrorism (e.g., the gassing of Iraqi civilians in Halabja),the distinction between state and substateterrorismcan be blurred,as in the case of pro-governmentparamilitarydeath squadsin SouthAfrica or Columbia(Hoffman1998; Stern 1999). It is an open question whethera particulartype of mind is disproportionatelyassociatedwith a given politicalcategoryof terrorism.Yetanotherchallengeto anypsychologicalinquiryinto the "mindof the terrorist"is thatterroristgroupstypicallyexhibithierarchicalorganization, with variousroles assumedwithin each level of thathierarchy(see Figure 1). Each position on such a matrixmay attractindividualswith differentpredispositions who perhapsplay theirroles because of profoundlydifferentpsychological factors. One might postulate, for example, that some leaders are more likely to be selfimaginedidealistsor altruists,othersaredrivenby messianicdelusions,othersby ethnic or religious animus,and othersby entrepreneurialambitions-a point thatseems clearwhen we intuit,for example,the differencesof psychic attributeslikely separating the three convicted terroristleaders Shoko Asahara, Abu Nidal, and Nelson Mandela.Of course, roles may blur dependingon the type of the group and its size. Nonetheless,since individualsof differenttemperamentsmightplay extremelydiffer- 6 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION -Sponsor Leader Committee Executive Middle Manage ment Follower State authority figure Selfimagined idealist Political policy maker Strategist/ technocrat Foot soldier/ action perpetrator Substate group sponsor Selfimagined messianic Military policy maker Recruiter Technician Trainer/ dispatcher Researcher/ surveyor errand runner Individual Ethnic or financial religious contributor animusdriven EntrepreneurI Supplier/ armorer Lone Wolf Transporter/ harborer Sympathizer/ fellow traveler Figure 1: Roles and Types within TerroristHierarchies NOTE:Mostterrorists,dependingon the politicalcategoryandsize of organization,belongto groupsexhibiting some or all of the hierarchicallevels of authoritydepictedhere.The exceptionis the Lone Wolf (e.g., TheodoreKaczynski)-a terroristactingin isolation.Role or responsibilitywithineach level of authorityis probablydeterminedin partby self-selection. Levels and roles may blurin application.Note thatthe proposed typology of leadersis theoreticalandpreliminary,a heuristiclist of ideal types based on a review of multiple sources. The self-imagined idealist leader (e.g., Menachim Begin, Ulrike Meinhof, Nelson Mandela,andpossibly Usamabin Laden)commitshis life to a goal he imaginesas a moralnecessity,calling for a strategictriumphhe claims to engineeraltruistically.The self-imaginedmessianicleader(e.g., Adolph Hitler,DavidKoresh,ShokoAsahara,andpossibly some Islamicextremistimams)regardshimselfas a guru destinedto fulfill a uniqueplace in historyin which strategictriumphequatesto personalascendancy.Many leaders,especially those promotingviolence against all out-groupmembers(e.g., perhapsYasserArafat, SlobodanMilosevic, variousProvisionalIrishRepublicanArmy (PIRA)leaders,and possibly Usamabin Laden),may be primarilydrivenby ethnic or religious animusthatoverridesidealistic or messianic aims. The entrepreneurial leader(e.g., AbuNidal, "Carlosthe Jackal,"andleadersof the Abu Sayyaf group)may justify his actionsaccordingto one of the othertypes butis primarilycircumscribedby shallow,materialist drives. ent parts in a terroristgroup, any empirical study claiming to characterize"the psychology of terrorists"might be very misleading if it fails to stratifyits findings accordingto level and role. Most importantfor a psychologicalanalysis,it seems reasonablethattheremay be heterogeneity in the temperaments,ideologies, thought processes, and cognitive Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THE TERRORIST 7 capacitiesof terroristswithinpoliticalcategories,hierarchicallevels, androles (Taylor andRyan 1988; Reich 1998). Thus,it is essentialto acknowledgefrom the outsetthat any effort to uncover the "terroristmind" will more likely result in uncovering a spectrumof terroristminds. PSYCHOSOCIAL DATADESCRIBING TERRORISTS Demographicstudiesfromthe 1960s and 1970s constructeda profileof the typical terroristas a well-educatedsingle male in his mid-twentiesfroma middle-classbackground(see Table2). Forinstance,in a 1976 studyof eighteengroups,averageages of membersrangedfrom 23.2 to 31.3. Most identified/convictedterroristscame from middle- or upper-middle-classbackgrounds,and the majorityhad some college education(RussellandMiller 1983). These findingsaresimilarto those froma contemporaneousstudy of 48 Euzkadita Askatasuna(ETA) members(Clark 1983). Handler (1990) investigatedthe relationshipbetweenpolitical orientationand socioeconomic factorsby tabulatingFederalBureauof Investigation(FBI) interviewdata on rightand left-wing terroristsactive in the United States duringthe 1960s and 1970s. He reportedthatwomenrepresenteda muchlargerproportionof left- thanright-wingterrorists(46.2 vs. 11.2 percent),college completion was much more common among left- than right-wing terrorists(67.6 vs. 19.0 percent), blue-collar occupation was morefrequentamongright-thanleft-wingterrorists(74.8 vs. 24.3 percent),andthere was a trendfor both left- andright-wingterroriststo achievelow- to medium-income levels even if they had college education.WeinbergandEubank's(1987) dataon 451 Italian women terroristsalso reveal a predominanceof those in their twenties, althoughthe majoritywereteachersor white-collarworkers.In a rarecontrolledstudy, Ferracuti(1982; FerracutiandBruno 1981; see also Post 2004) comparedItalianRed Brigade terroristswith politically active controls, finding no notable differences in family backgrounds. Thependulumswung in the 1980s with the relativequiescenceof Americanterrorist groups,the decimationof Europeanrevolutionaryanarchist-Marxistgroups, and the risingworldprofileof radicalIslamicterrorists.The typicalPalestinianterroristof thatlaterperiodwas age seventeento twenty-three,came from a largefamily with an impoverishedbackground,and had low educationalachievement(Strentz1988). But the pendulumhas swung again. Middle Easternterroristsin the late 1990s and early twenty-firstcenturycome from a widerdemographicrange,includinguniversitystudents,professionals,marriedmen in theirlate forties, and young women (Rees et al. 2002). For example, the 9/11 pilots included the middle-aged,middle-class urban plannerMohammadAttaandthe well-to-do ZiadJarrah,a manfrom an affluentfamily who attendedChristianschools and enjoyed discos and beer (Carey2002; Laabs andMcDermott2003; Anonymous2004). The most recentdevelopment,the recruitmentof women as suicidebombers,arisesat least in partfromthe fact thatthe al-Aqsa Martyr'sBrigade-associated with YasserArafat'sFatah-is the first seculargroup fighting for Palestinianindependenceand thereforepermits females to participate (Rees et al. 2002; Wilkinson2002). 8 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION TABLE2 ReportedDemographicsof Terrorists Author(s)and Year Russell and Miller (1983) Clark(1983) Weinbergand Eubank(1987) Strentz(1988) Strentz(1988) Handler(1990) Hassan(2001) Pedahzur,Perliger, andWeinberg (2003) Sageman(2004) Subjects Age Social Class 23.2-31.3 > 2/3 middle or uppermiddle class 350 membersof eighteen European,Middle Eastern, South American,and Japanese groups,active 19661976 28 percentlower;30 percent 48 ETA members,active 24 (avg.) middle 1970s 451 Italianwomen terrorists 60 percent Terrorists:35 percentstudents,43 ages 20-29 percentwhite-collarworkersor teachers,and 7 percent"workers";fathersof subgroupof 27: 10 percentblue-collarworkers and 41 percentuppermiddle class Leader. Middle class U.S. domestic terrorists: 1960s and 1970s leftist 25-40; follower, groups(n, NA) 20-25 17-23 1980s Middle Easternter"Unskilledandunemployable" rorists(n, NA) Right wing: 74.8 percentblue161 right-wingand 119 left- NA wing terroristsactive in collar workers,18.3 percent United States, 1960s-1970s white-collarworkers;left wing: 24.3 percentblue-collarworkers, 15 percentwhite-collarworkers 18-38 "Many"middle class "Nearly250" Hamasor Islamic Jihadmembers, 1996-1999 24.5 (avg.) Mean socioeconomic status 80 Palestiniansuicide (SES) = 5.97 (high SES = 1; terrorists low = 10) 102 Salafi Muslim terrorists 25.69 (avg. 18 percentupperclass, 55 percent age of middle class, and 27 percent from SaudiArabia,Egypt, lower class joining) France,Algeria, Morocco, and Indonesia NOTE:NA = not available;ETA= Euzkadita Askatasuna. A poll conducted by the Palestinian Center for Policy and Survey Research (PCPSR)in 2001 among 1,357 adultsin the WestBankandGazatestedthe hypothesis that poverty or low levels of education influence attitudes regarding political violence and found that support for terrorism against Israeli civilians was even more common among professionals than among laborers (43.3 vs. 34.6 percent) and more common among those with secondary education than among illiterate respondents (39.4 vs. 32.3 percent) (Krueger and Maleckova 2002). This is consistent with Sageman's (2004) finding that 94 of 132 (71 percent) of Muslim terrorists had at least some college education, and 57 of 134 (43 percent) were professionals, although his study is Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 9 biased towardleaders. More important,Kruegerand Maleckova(2002) also tested how economic status and educationcomparedwith actual participationin political violence: comparing129 Hezbollahfightersages fifteen to thirty-eightwho died in action between 1982 and 1994 with membersof the generalLebanesepopulationof the sameage range,the povertyratewas similar(28 percentamongfightersvs. 33 percent in the population),but fighterswere significantlymore likely to have attended secondaryschool (33 vs. 23 percent).These findings are not consistentwith theories tying political violence to povertyand lack of education(althougha critiqueof this conclusion is noted laterin the section on sociological theories). Psychologicaldataareeven sparserthansocioeconomicdata,althoughseveralprojects reported"typical"psychosocial characteristicsof terroristsin the 1970s and 1980s. On the basis of unstructuredinterviews,AmericanpsychiatristDavid Hubbard (1971) reportedfive traitsof skyjackers:(1) violent, often alcoholic father;(2) deeply religious mother; (3) sexually shy, timid, and passive; (4) younger sisters toward whomthe terroristactedprotectively;and(5) poor social achievement.On the basis of primarilysecondhandsourcematerialregardinga subsampleof 908 right-wingterrorists in Italy,Ferracuti,andBruno(1981) claimedto haveidentifiedninetypicalcharacteristics:(1) ambivalencetowardauthority,(2) defectiveinsight,(3) adherenceto convention,(4) emotionaldetachmentfrom the consequencesof theiractions, (5) sexual role uncertainties,(6) magical thinking,(7) destructiveness,(8) low education,and (9) adherenceto violent subculturenormsand weaponsfetishes. It is interestingthat these lists, compiled a decade apart,overlapin regardto sexualrole uncertaintiesand probablylow education(if this is a proxyfor poor social achievement).Yetapartfrom this superficialoverlap, the two studies do not suggest common features of backgroundor personality.Neitherof these studiesused controlsor validatedpsychological instruments.Thelargeststudyof thiskindwas thatperformedunderthe auspicesof the WestGermanMinistryof the Interior;this ambitious1980-1983 projectinvolved semistructuredinterviews of 227 left-wing terroristsand 23 right-wing extremists (Jiger, Schmidtchen,and Siillwold 1981). Certaindemographic,life historical, or psychologicalfactorswere reportedwith high frequencyin this studypopulation:25 percentof leftist terroristshad lost one or both parentsby age fourteen,33 percent reportedsevere conflict with parents,and 33 percenthad a history of juvenile court conviction.This studyalso claimedto haveidentifiedtwo patternsof personalitytraits commonto terrorists:an extroverted,stimulus-seeking,dependentpatternand a hostile, suspicious, defensive pattern.This Germanstudy presentsa majorchallenge to some psychology theoriesof terrorismsimplyby recognizingheterogeneouspsychological categories among terrorists.But again, the psychological conclusions were impressionistic,and differentpsychologists on the Germanteam drew differentconclusions (Crenshaw1986). Withoutthe use of valid andreliablebehavioralmeasures andwithouta controlgroup,one cannotconcludethatthe characteristicsidentifiedin the American,Italian,or Germanstudies distinguishterroristsfrom nonterrorists. Since the tragiceventsof September11, 2001, attentionhas shiftedto the psychology of Islamic fundamentalistterrorism.Thereis a dearthof publishedliteraturedescribingpsychological studies of Muslim extremists.Merariand colleagues administered a batteryof standardizedpsychological tests, including some measures of 10 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION cognitivefunction,to Hezbollah,Amal, andsecularpro-SyrianfighterscapturedinfiltratingIsraelfromLebanonin the late 1980s (Ariel Merari,personalcommunication, 2003). Unfortunately,his datahave been classified by the IsraeliDefense Forces and are unavailablefor scholarly scrutinyor attemptedreplication.Post, Sprinzak,and Denny (2003) conductedsemistructuredinterviewswith thirty-fiveincarceratedMiddle Easternextremists,includingtwenty-oneIslamicreligiousterroristsfrom Hamas and its armedwing, Izz a-Din al-Qassam,Islamic Jihad,and Hezbollah, as well as fourteensecularterroristsfrom Fatah.Most had a high school education;some had additionalschooling. (However,the subgroupof suicidebombersamongthe Palestinians was described as ages seventeen to twenty-two, "uneducated,unemployed, unmarried.") Mostcamefromrespectedfamiliesthatsupportedtheiractivism,with 30 percentof the families of religious terroristsand 15 percentof the families of secular terroristsreportingtheir own radical involvement.Peer influence was cited as the majorreasonforjoining a terroristgroup,andjoining increasedsocial standing.Membershipwas describedas being associatedwith a fusionof the young adult'sindividual identitywith the group'scollective identityandgoals. Prisonexperiencewas claimed to strengthengroupcommitmentfor most terroristsof both types. Anger and hatred withoutremorsewere often expressed,but therewas little interestin obtainingweapons of massdestruction.Thisprojectis one of the few to employa directpsychological examinationof recentlyactive terrorists.Unfortunately,the methodof subjectselection, the circumstancesof the interviews,and the method of interviewingwere not describedin this otherwise impressivereport;few specific demographicswere reported,no formalmeasuresof anypsychologicalvariableswere used, andno controls were examined. PsychiatristMarcSageman(2004) compileddatafrompublic sourceson 172 individuals he identifiedas membersof a "globalSalafi mujahedin,"meaningMuslims engagedin terroristacts againstthe "farenemy"in the service of a new Islamicworld order.He includedexpatriateleadersof the EgyptianIslamic Group(EIG),members of the EgyptianIslamic Jihad(EIJ),JemaahIslamiyah,the Moro Islamic Liberation Front,the AlgerianGroupeSalafistepourla Predicationet le Combat(GSPC),and al Qaeda. Sageman excluded terroristsengaged in local jihads, such as Chechnyans, Kashmiris,Afghans,andPalestinians.His sampleis thusbiasedtowardthose involved in transnationalterrorismandtowardthe subgroup,mostly leaders,who have come to publicattention.He identified"somefragmentof childhooddata"in 61 cases. Only 4 hadhistoriessuggestiveof conductdisorder.Only 1 case (HabibZacariasMoussaoui) was suggestive of a childhood trauma.Descriptorsof childhood personalitywere foundfor 69 cases; althoughlonersoutnumberedoutgoingchildren,most descriptors were neutralor positive. One-quarterof the group had historiesof petty crime. Detailedbiographieswere examinedfor 10 cases. Sagemanclaimsthathe found"noevidence of pathologicalnarcissism"and"nopatternof paranoidpersonalitydisorder"in this subgroup,with the exception of possible traits of al Qaeda leader Ayman alZawahiri.AlthoughSageman'sconclusions seem highly plausible,the authoris candid in admittingthe limitationsof this work:his sample is very small, atypical,and uncontrolled,and the authorhad no formal method for confirming these indirect psychiatricimpressions. Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 11 Potentiallyhigh-valuedatawere gatheredoutsidethe academicresearchapparatus by UnitedNations(UN) relief workerNasraHassan,basedon unstructuredinterviews with "nearly250" membersof Hamas or Islamic Jihadconductedin Gaza between 1996 and 1999. She reportsthatthe suicide bombersrangedin age from eighteen to thirty-eight,more thanhalf were refugees, "many"were middleclass, 2 were sons of millionaires,and none were depressed,although"many"reportedthatthey had been beaten or torturedby Israeli forces. Unfortunately,Hassan's lucid and widely cited reportdoes not specify the actualnumberof terroristsubjects,as well as whatproportion of this total subject populationwere intended suicide bombers, failed suicide bombers,or trainers,andoffers no specific demographic,socioeconomic, or psychological data(Hassan2001; Atran2003). (Some of these datawill be incorporatedinto a forthcomingbook [N. Hassan,personalcommunication,2004].) Barber(1999) conducted the most extensive study of psychological factors possibly associated with Islamic political violence. His reportis based on data from the PalestinianFamily Study, a project involving 6,923 ninth-gradestudentsin the West Bank and Gaza. Aggressivity and mood were measured with the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbachand Edelbrock 1987), "familyvalues" were measuredby one question regardingthe importancethat respondentsplaced on getting marriedand having a family, and participationin the intifadaof 1987-1993 was measuredby the yes/no responseto a single question:"Beforethe withdrawalof Israelitroopsfrom the Gaza Stripand Jericho,did you ever distributeleaflets, protectsomeone from Israeli soldiers or police, march or demonstrateagainst the occupation,and throw stones at Israelisoldiers?"A yes answerto this questionwas positivelyassociatedwith depression, aggression,and family values. Unfortunately,this questiondoes not allow discriminationbetween violent andnonviolentpoliticalparticipation,underminingconclusions one might drawfrom this ambitiousstudyregardingthe predictivevalue of psychological factorsfor Islamic insurgentaggression. OVERVIEW OF PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORIES Attemptsto accountfor the behaviorof terroristsfall into two generalcategories: top-downapproachesthatseek the seeds of terrorismin political,social, economic, or even evolutionarycircumstancesand bottom-upapproachesthatexplore the characteristicsof individualsandgroupsthatturnto terrorism(e.g., Wieviorka1993, 2004). These approachesare not mutually exclusive. In fact, approachessuch as rational choice theory and relative deprivation/oppressiontheory combine these points of view, consideringinteractionsbetweencircumstancesandactors.While acknowledging the importanceof top-downanalysesand ultimatecauses, this articlefocuses primarilyon bottom-upapproachesandproximalcauses in substateterrorism.The principal approachesare organizedinto groups for the sake of clarity.However,it will become apparentthatconceptualoverlapexists betweentheorieswithinandbetween groups.It will also become apparentthata particularfundamentalconceptualframework-such as psychoanalysis-may informdiversetheoriesand thatthe same theory may be championedfrom differentconceptualframeworks.For example, group 12 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION theoryhas psychoanalyticand nonpsychoanalyticchampions.Theoriesof terrorism also vary in the extentto which they considerpsychologicaldifferencesbetween terroristsplaying differentroles (e.g., leadersvs. followers), whetherterroristsare regardedas psychologicallyhomogeneousor heterogeneous,and whethersubtypesof terrorismare associatedwith subtypesof terrorists. PSYCHOPATHOLOGICALTHEORY At one end of the spectrumis the popularopinionthatterroristsmustbe insane or psychopathic(Hacker1976;Cooper 1977;Pearce1977;Taylor1988). Herea distinction must be made:modernWesternpsychiatryidentifies adultbehavioraldisorders accordingto a multiaxialclassificationschemein whichAxis I refersto the majorclinical illnesses-those such as schizophreniaor majordepression-while Axis II refers to personalitydisorders-such as antisocialpersonalitydisorder(APD) (American PsychiatricAssociation 2000). APD is the currentterm for a patternof remorseless disregardfor the rightsof othersthatwas calledpsychopathyup until the mid-1950s andsociopathythereafter.Psychosisrefersto a loss of realitytestingobservedprimarily in a subgroupof Axis I disorders(e.g., schizophrenia)butis not expectedin Axis II disorderssuch as APD. Insanityis not a behavioralscience termbut a legal termthat usuallyimpliespsychosis,althoughits definitionis subjectto significantjurisdictional variance(ResnickandNoffsinger 2004). Hence, a psychotic or "insane"personis so mentallydisorderedas to not know rightfrom wrong, while a sociopathknows right from wrong and chooses wrong for selfish reasonswithoutpangs of conscience. In regardto Axis I clinical disordersamongterrorists,very little researchhas been done involving comprehensivepsychiatricexamination,and no properlycontrolled researchis foundin the open literature.However,the conclusion-at leaston the basis of uncontrolledempiricalpsychologicalstudiesof left-wing Germanmilitants,membersof the AlgerianFrontde Lib6rationNationale(FLN),membersof the Provisional IrishRepublicanArmy (PIRA), and Hezbollah-has been thatterroristsdo not usually exhibitwhatwe referto as Axis I or even Axis II psychiatricdisorders(Crenshaw 1981; Jager,Schmidtchen,and Stillwold 1981; Heskin 1984; Merari1998). German psychiatristWilfredRasch(1979) examinedeleven terroristsuspects,includingmembers of the Baader-Meinhofgroup,and reportedon a FederalPolice studyof another fortypersonswantedas terrorists,findingno evidenceof mentalillness in anyrespondent. Post, Sprinzak,and Denny (2003; also see Post and Gold 2002) also found no Axis I disorderson psychiatricevaluationsof twenty-onesecularandfourteenradical IslamicMiddleEasternterrorists.As criminologistFrancoFerracuti(1982) suggested more thantwo decades ago, and as has been supportedby subsequentreports(Reich 1998; Silke 1998; Horgan2003), while terroristgroupsare sometimesled by insane individuals,andwhile a few terroristacts mightbe attributedto unequivocallyinsane persons,terroristsrarelymeet psychiatriccriteriafor insanity. Rather,most of the literatureattributingclinicalmentaldisorderto terroristsspeaks of the remorselesspersonalitytype,psychopathyor sociopathy(Taylor1988). Cooper (1977, 1978), for example, states thatterrorists,like psychopaths,are ruthless"out- Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 13 laws" and "outcasts"who adhereto an anomalousscheme of values out of tune with thatof the rest of society andthatthereis a "nearidentityof this fundamentalcharacteristicin both the psychopathand the terrorist."Pearce (1977) statedthat terrorists were sociopathsacting antisociallydue to "superegolacunae,"meaninggaps in selfmonitoring;he supportshis conclusion partlyon the basis of tattoos found on one terrorist. The claim of sociopathy,advancedwithout evidence from any empirical study, raises the importantquestion of whetherterrorismis usually antisocialor prosocial behavior.It makes a common kind of sense thatindividualswho harminnocentsare antisocial.Those who rejectand attacktheirown society, such as the Germanstudent whojoined the 1970s Red ArmyFactionor the Christian-to-Muslim convertwhojoins a modem radicalIslamic cell, standagainsttheirown and might be regardedas antisocial. Yet severallines of reasoningtend to discreditthe simplisticclaim that antisociality is typical or even common among terrorists.First,extensive evidence supportsthe observationthat,farfrombeing outcasts,terroristsareoften regardedby their in-groupas heroic freedomfighters.As Post (2004) points out, nationalist-separatist terroristsmust be distinguishedfrom revolutionaryterroristsin this regardsince the formerare typically regardedas riskingtheirlives for social welfare,while the latter attack their society of origin. That is, the Basque studentwho joins the ETA, the Chechen"blackwidow"who terrorizesa Moscow theater,or the LiberationTigersof TamilEalem (LTTE)suicide bomberall use terrorismto fight on behalf of their ingroup.The Irishmanwho joins the PIRA or the Middle Easternstudentwho joins an Islamicradicalgroup,dependingon his specific nationandprovince,may enjoy considerablepopularsupportand conscientiously serve his society in a prosocial way. Ironically,therefore,with respect to in-groupsof identity,certaintypes of terrorism oftenrepresentprosocialbehavior.Second,evidenceexists fromthe quantitativeliteraturethatthe actionsof terrorists,even those who fail and die, mightbenefit theirkin and social group (Azam forthcoming).Furtherevidence of the prosocialityof some terroristscomes from the empiricalwork of Italiansociologist Donatelladella Porta (1988): among 1,214 Italianmilitants,351 (45.6 percent)enjoyed personalties with eight or more group membersbeforejoining a terroristorganization.This raises the questionof how large one's groupof identitymust be to considercollaborationprosocial, butit at least suggeststhatrecruitmentofteninvolvesa networkof sharedsocial values. Pedahzur,Perliger,and Weinberg(2003) examinedthis issue from the perspective of Durkheim,who distinguishedaltruisticsuicide-suicide in the service of society-from egoistic andanomicsuicide.Based on the observationthat80 Palestinian suicideterroristsfrom 1973 to 2002 exhibiteda higherrateof religiouseducation, membershipin fundamentalistorganizations,andrepeatterroristacts comparedwith nonsuicidalterrorists,these authorsproposedthat they were probablyacting from altruisticmotives.Indeed,this is the essence of the concept of istishad, selfless death in the serviceof Allah (Post, Sprinzak,andDenny 2003; see also Sageman2004). It is obviously conceptually inadequateto judge antisociality from the perspective of the targetedout-group,yet it is prematureto conclude thatmost membersof ethnic, religious, or national-separatistterroristgroups exhibit prosocialitybased on these limited reports.Some antisocial individualsperhapsuse the moral cover of group 14 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION affiliationto disguise theiraggressiveandremorselessdrives.However,pendingdata to the contrary,it seems plausiblethatmanyterroristsact in a prosocialmanner,both believingthemselvesto be servingsociety andjudgedby theirin-groupto be actingin its interest.(It is a separatequestionto ask whetherthey subjectivelyadoptthe moral positionthatCorrado[1981] labeled"misplacedidealism.")Thus,Ferracuti's(1982) formulationregardingthe relationshipbetween insanityand terrorismmight equally apply to the relationship between sociopathy and terrorism: sociopaths may sometimes be among the terrorists,but terroristsare not, by virtue of their political violence, necessarily sociopaths.Intuitively,one might expect differentpersonality traits among antisocial and prosocial terrorists.This speculation requires further study. RATIONAL CHOICE THEORY If most terroristsdo not meet diagnosticcriteriafor a majormentalillness or for sociopathy,must one conclude thatthey are rational?This raises the questionof the explanatorypower of rationalchoice theory-the theorythatterroristaction derives from a conscious, rational,calculateddecision to takethis particulartype of actionas the optimumstrategyto accomplish a sociopolitical goal (Sandler,Tschirhart,and Cauley 1983; SandlerandLapan1988; Crenshaw1992; Wilson 2000). A distinction shouldbe madebetweenrational-or strategic-choice theoryandotherindividualor group psychological theories of terrorism.The lattertry to explain why people are inclinedtowarda type or style of behavior(e.g., to be a terrorist),while rationalchoice theory, derivedfrom economics, assumes this behavioralproclivity as a given and attemptsto explain how changes in policy-the rules of the "game"that is played betweenterroristsandgovernments-might predictablyalterbehavior.Since rational choice theoryconsidersboth policy and individualbehavioralresponsesto policy, it combines the top-downand bottom-upapproaches. Gametheory,basedon this "assumptionof rationality"in strategicchoice formulations, has beenused to analyzeandpredictpoliticalbehaviorsince the seminalworkof Deutschin the 1950s (Deutsch 1954;DeutschandKrause1962;MilburnandWatman 1981; Machinaet al. 1989). Empiricalsupportfor game theory comes from experimentsin whichvolunteersplay againstrivalsin games such as the prisoner'sdilemma, sometimesto win a payoff such as points,sometimesto avoidcosts suchas loss of face or electric shocks (von NeumannandMorgenstern1947; Deutsch 1954; Borah 1963; RapoportandChammah1968). SandlerandArce (2003) listed six strengthsof modern game theoryfor revealingquantifiablefactorstheoreticallyunderlyingthe behavior of terroristsand targetedgovernments:game theory (1) captures the interdependentnatureof such interactions,(2) helps discoverthe strategicimplicationswhen each side acts accordingto its best guess abouthow the other side thinks, (3) incorporates the impact of threatsand promises from each side, (4) takes advantageof the observationthat"players"tendto maximizegoals subjectto constraints,(5) helps predictoutcomes in bargainingover demands,and (6) acknowledgesthe impact of uncertainty-incomplete information-on all the above.They cite the exampleof the Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 15 shift away from skyjackingsto kidnappingsafterthe installationof metaldetectorsat airportsin 1973 as evidenceof a predictableandrationalresponseto new constraints. PoliticalscientistMarthaCrenshaw(2000) has cautionedthatthe ostensiblegoal of terroristsoften appearsso unlikelyto be achievedby the chosen actionthatit is difficult to supportan overarchingrationalisttheoryof terrorism.Furthermore,the outrageous inhumanityof attackson innocentcivilianschallengesthe commonplaceunderstanding of "rational"behavior. Given questions about incoherent motivations, ghastlymeans,andpoliticalinefficacyof terrorism,some scholarshaveproposedthat the typical terroristis not simply a "rationalactor" in the strict Weberiansense (Brannan,Eslerm, and Anders Strindberg2001). On the other hand, historicalevidence suggests that terrorismis sometimes a practical,low-cost strategy through which subordinategroups leverage their power to successfully achieve their ends (Sandlerand Enders2004). Indeed,modem history is repletewith examplesof successful substatepolitical violence: Irgun'sbombingswere a majorfactorin securing the independenceof EretzIsraelfrom the British;terrorismby the Irish Republican Army(IRA)precipitatedaccommodationsleadingto theIrishFreeState;Shi'ite Muslim terroristsprovidedkey assistancein the ousterof the Shahof Iran;Hezbollah'ssuicide bombingcampaignof 1983-1985 directlyled to theAmerican,French,andIsraeli withdrawalandestablishmentof a Shi'a-controlledsociety in majorpartsof Lebanon; andtheAfricanNationalCongress(ANC) used terrorismas partof its remarkablysuccessful strategyto overthrowthe apartheidgovernmentof South Africa. More recently,al Qaeda'sbrutaltransnationalcampaign,includingthe mass murdersat New York'sWorldTradeCenterin 2001, may have not only rapidlyadvancedUsama bin Laden'sstatedgoal of removingthe large U.S. militarypresencefrom Saudi Arabia but also servedas an extremelypotentrecruitingtool (Laqueur1987; Hoffman 1998, 1999; Whittaker2001). Thus, historicalprecedentssupportmanyterrorists'expectations of success, so the theory of strategicchoice must not be discounted on the grounds that terrorism's goals are uniformly improbable. Game-theoreticalapproachesare also sophisticatedenough to recognize that the "winnings"that satisfy terroristsmay not be theirovertantigovernmentgoals but less obvious goals such as martyrdom,whichmaynot only serveas anend in itself butalso yield unexpectedbenefits to the terrorist'soffspringthatexceed the "opportunitycost" of an educatedlife lost prematurely(Brooks 2002; Azam forthcoming). Moreover, game theory has yielded evidence of counterintuitivebut importantpredictionssuch as the possibility that governmentinvestmentsin deterrencemight waste resources or even produce paradoxicalincreasesin threats(Sandlerand Arce 2003). Strategicchoice theorypotentiallyoffers vital insights into the potentialpayoff of terroristversus governmentactions. By uncoveringotherwise cryptic benefits, this approachmayhelp explainotherwiseenigmaticbehaviors.Insofaras humansevolved to function as sophisticatedcalculatorsof risks and benefits, and insofar as groups functioncollectively to actualizethe will of theirmembers,one can makequantitative predictionsregardingthe theoreticalcircumstancesunder which terroristbehavior serves group and individual interests. Such microeconomic analyses may help in calculatingthe likely outcome of differentpolicy options, such as hardeningtargets, calculatingconcessions, or performingretaliatorystrikes(Sandlerand Lapan 1988; 16 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION Lee 1988; Brophy-Baermannand Conybeare1994; Sandierand Arce 2003; Sandier and Enders2004). But the following question remains:what are the limitations,or even potentialpitfalls, of the game-theoreticalapproach? Evidence suggests thatvery few individualswho rationallybelieve thatterrorism may advancetheir cause ever become terrorists(Schbley 2000). This is conceivably relatedto the discoverythat85 percentof WorldWarII infantrymenfacing the enemy failed to pull the triggers of their weapons, despite the urgent rational benefits (Grossman 1995). In other words, even obvious strategicbenefits may not compel humansto violence, an arguablyirrationalresultof modem culture.And some terrorists (e.g., "lonewolf"terroristTheodoreKaczinski)commitviolence dueto unequivocally irrationalmotives (in his case, paranoidschizophrenia).Thus,the rareand idiosyncraticdecision to become a terroristcannotbe explainedby rationalchoice theory. Yetit is inappropriateto criticizethis theorybecauseit fails to explainwhy only a tiny minorityof individualsturnsto terrorism;it does not tryto. It focuses insteadon what membersof this raregroupare likely to do undervariousconditions. Twoothercriticismsof rationalchoice theorymaybe morecompelling.First,rational choice theoryclaims predictivepower for futureevents, extrapolatingboth from laboratoryexperimentsof the behaviorof nonterroristsplayingnonnaturalisticgames and from post hoc analysis of real-worldincidents. But as Wieviorka(1993, 57) observed, this kind of strategicanalysis weighs "questionsof resourcesand power relationships... as if the principlesunderlyingtheiractionshadbeen establishedonce andfor all, andas if the effects of violence were predictableandmeasurable"(see also Wieviorka2004). The uncertaintyof theprinciplesof terrorist-government interaction adds to the uncertaintyof the facts known by the players since, as the early work in game theoryillustrates,a slight change in the "rules"may yield opposite behavioral results (von Neumannand Morgenstern1947; Milburnand Watman1981; Machina engagements et al. 1989). Refinementsin the understandingof terrorist-government based on increasinglysophisticatedevent analysis and classification should reduce this element of uncertaintyand strengthenthe predictivevalidityof this approach. Second, it may be dangerousto assumethata profileof a "typicalplayer"will predict an actualterrorist'sresponses.As Merari(2002, 4) has said, "Ina perfectlyrational system, the basic idea of deterrenceis to deliver a clear,crediblemessage to the opponentthatthe cost of pursuinga certaincourseof behavioroutweighsits benefits. In reality,however,this simple formulararely,if ever, works accordingto expectations."The most likely explanationfor suchunanticipatedconsequencesis simplythat the immenseplasticityandindividualvariabilityof the humancentralnervoussystem often generateidiosyncraticand individualisticresponses that defy predictionsnot only because of incomplete informationheld by the actor but also because of impulsivity,faulty cognition, and emotional processes that overruleadaptivechoices. Writingthe applicablegame-theoreticalequationbecomes ever more challengingas imponderablevariablesareaddedto accommodateindividualemotionalpeculiarities of terrorists,victims, and governments:the lure of bravadoand romanceof risk, the self-destructiveurge for "success"in likely failurewith or withoutthe utility of martyrdom,the Svengali-likeinfluence of charismaticleaderson either side whose fol- Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 17 lowersmarchin maladaptivecolumns,thepowerof rageto betterreason,the blindness of ambition,the illogic of spite, or the frenzy of revengeall may contributeto the stochastic occurrenceof surprisingscenarios.Moreover,the lack of an empiricallyvalidated typology of terroristvariantscomplicateswriting optimumtheoremsfor subtypes of playerswho may exhibit very differentbehavioralproclivities(Bowen et al. 1985; FriedlandandMerari1985;Merari2002). Nonetheless,no behavioraltheoryis expectedto accommodateall examples;the law of largenumbersby itself guarantees some failuresof prediction.It wouldbe sufficientlyvaluableif rationalchoice calculations predicteda higher proportionof terroristbehaviors than did nonquantitative methods or reliably predictedresponses in some subtype of engagements.Merari's (2002) strongclaim thatterroristbehaviors"rarely,if ever"follow such predictionsis the key question.Furtherempiricalwork should be able to resolve thatdebate. I would proposethatrationalchoice analysisis a powerfultool for discoveringtheoreticallyvalidandsurprisinglycounterintuitiveforces thatprobablyinfluenceterrorist and governmentbehaviors.Game theorymay also prove invaluablein predicting likely changes in the base rate (the rate predictedin rationalactor simulations)of behaviorsof an idealizedterroristin responseto concessions or deterrents.However, rationalchoice theoriescannotpredictidiosyncraticresponses.Policy recommendations thatpredictdeterrenceof terroristacts are only as valuableas their capacityto anticipatethe extraordinaryvariabilityand adaptabilityof humans. Moreover,at present,rationalchoice theorydoes not explainwhy a very few individuals, among hundreds of thousands in virtually identical political positions, become terrorists.As Crozier (1960, 9) suggested, "Men do not necessarily rebel merelybecause theirconditionsof life are intolerable:it takes a rebel to rebel."Individualfactorsmustbe at work.Temperamentsvary.Humanfrontallobe corticalplanningbasedon rationalcalculationof costs andbenefitsis foreversubjectto limbictyranny.Passion often trumpsrationality,behaviorsmay deviate significantlyfrom the predictedbase rate,andunderstandingthe mindof the terrorist-with or withoutpredictionof futurebehavior-requires investigationsbeyondtherealmof gametheory. If neitherinsanity/sociopathynorrationalchoice can fully accountfor the genesis of terroristbehaviors,what alternativepsychologicalexplanationsseem most plausible? As Crenshaw(1986, 386) stated,even though terrorismdoes not result from a specific psychopathologicalcondition,thatis not to say that"thepoliticaldecision to join a terroristorganizationis not influenced or, in some cases, even determinedby subconscious or latent psychological motives."In other words, although terrorists rarely exhibit psychological disorders, they may exhibit identifiablepsychological traits or may have been influencedby identifiablesocial factors.Political scientists, sociologists, psychologists,andpsychiatristshave offereddiverseopinionsregarding the degree to which the roots of terroristaggression are innate versus acquired,the productof psychodynamicversus social forces, or the productof individualversus groupforces.The most frequentlycited theoriescan be dividedinto sociological theories,psychoanalyticapproachesto individualpsychology,nonpsychoanalyticpsychological approachesto individualpsychology, and theoriesof groupprocess. 18 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION SOCIOLOGICAL THEORIES SOCIAL LEARNING THEORY Bandura's(1973, 1998) social learningtheoryof aggressionsuggeststhatviolence follows observationandimitationof an aggressivemodel, anda variantof this theory has been invoked to explain terroristbehaviors not as the consequence of innate aggressivitybut of cognitive "reconstrual"of moralimperatives.Teenagersliving in hotbedsof political strifemay directlywitness terroristbehaviorsand seek to imitate themor,even morecommonly,learnfromtheirculture'spublicglorificationof terrorists-for example,the "martyrposters"lining the streetsof Shi'a regionsof Lebanon and Palestinianrefugee camps or the songs celebratingthe exploits of the PIRA (Crenshaw1992;TaylorandQuayle 1994; Kelly andRieber1995). Social learningof the acceptabilityof terroristviolence may also takea didacticform,as in the teaching of an extremistform of jihad in manyPakistaniand Palestinianmadrasas-religious schools for young Muslimboys. Madrasashaveexisted since the time of Muhammad, butthe recentworldwideresurgenceof Islamicfundamentalismhas led to an increase in theirnumbersandpossibly in the violence of theirmessage (Armstrong2000; MarshallandDanizewski2001; Kepel2002; Atran2003). Evidencesuggeststhata minority of prominenttransnationalMuslimterroristswere educatedin madrasas(Sageman 2004; Anonymous2004). This, however,does not exclude the possibility that widespreadeducationof this type influences even nonattendeesvia culturaldiffusion. Terroristdidacticlearningalso occursvia the disseminationof terroristphilosophy andmethodologyin communiquds,audiovisualtapes,compactdisks,books, andWeb sites. The most influential historical example may be the widely translated1969 "Mini-Manual"or "Handbookof Urban Guerilla Warfare"by Brazilian terrorist CarlosMarighella(1971; also see Saper 1988), which, amongotherpracticaladvice, suggests that readerslearn to pilot a plane. The charterof the Islamic Resistance Movement(HarakatAl-MuqawamaAl-Islamiya [Hamas])representsa more recent example; article 15 of this documentemphasizesthe importanceof teachingjihad: "Wemustimprinton the mindsof generationsof Muslimsthatthe Palestinianproblem is a religiousone... I indeedwish to go to warfor the sake of Allah! I will assaultand kill, assault and kill, assaultand kill" (Alexander2002, 57). It seems plausiblethat didacticteachingor social learningmay influence some young people towardterrorism. However,the social learning/cognitiverestructuringmodel fails to explain why only a smallminorityamongthe hundredsof thousandsof studentseducatedforjihad in madrasas,the millions exposed to extremistpublications,and the tens of millions exposed to public glorificationof terroristshave become terrorists.As Taylorand Quayle(1994, 32) put it, "Noteveryonefromthose communities,althoughsubjectto those same or similar influences, becomes a terrorist"(see also Sageman 2004). Therefore,while social learningprobablyhelps animatethe smallminoritywho turns to political violence, this theoryfails to explain why these particularindividualsbecome terrorists.Otherfactorsmust be sought. Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 19 FRUSTRATION-AGGRESSIONHYPOTHESIS This raises the questionof how politically motivatedpeople reachthe point of no returnat which theirpotentialenergyis convertedinto violent action.The frustrationaggression (FA) hypothesis-one outcome of an interdisciplinarycollaborationby political and social scientists at Yale University to better understandthe violence observedin earlytwentieth-centuryEurope-has oftenbeen cited,attributingthe final expressionof the terroristimpulse to desperationin the face of oppression(Dollard et al. 1939;Friedland1992). PoliticalpsychologistJohnChowingDavies (1973, 251) has even stated,"Violenceis always a responseto frustration"(emphasisadded).The FA hypothesisis includedhere as a sociological theory,althoughthe originalintentof Dollardet al. (1939) was also to accountfor individualbehavior;thus, terroristviolence of eithergroupsor individualsmight be explainedby this theory. However,the applicationof this theoryto terrorismstudieshas been criticizedon severalgrounds:millions of people live in frustratingcircumstancesbut neverturnto terrorism,manyterroristsdo notbelong to the desperateclasses whose frustrationthey claim to be expressing,and terrorismdoes not uniformlyappearto be an act of last resortby those who have exhaustedalternateapproaches(Billig 1976; Merariand Friedland1985;Laqueur1987; Friedland1992; SidaniusandPratto1999). The leftist terrorismof 1970s Europe,for example, was primarilyperpetratedby membersof privileged classes, and state-sponsoredterrorismcan hardly be attributedto the oppressionof the governmentby its victims.Frustration,therefore,mayplausiblyplay some partin the genesis of some political violence, but the FA hypothesis is not by itself sufficientto explainterrorism. RELATIVE DEPRIVATIONTHEORY It has also been proposed that economic disparitiescause terrorism.This claim underliesGurr's(1970) theory of relative deprivation-that rebellions come to be when people cannotbearthe misery of theirlot. As Schmid (1983) observed,Gurr's theoryderivesmore from psychoanalysisthanfrom empiricalsociology and is conceptuallyborn of the FA hypothesis.Irrespectiveof these psychiatricroots, multiple writershaveclaimeda sociological linkbetweenpovertyandterrorism(Schmid 1983; Harmon 2000; Hasisi and Pedahzur2000; Krueger and Maleckova 2002). More recently,increasingdifferencesbetween the materialwelfare of the haves and havenotshavebeen postulatedto provokea new eraof politicalviolence thatwill accelerate as globalizationnot only creates new foci of povertybut facilitatescommunication betweenthose who perceivethemselvesto be globalization'svictims (Maya,Lander, and Ungar 2002). One possibility is that either absolutedeprivationor relativeeconomic disparityignites terroristsentiments, especially among members of an oppressedunderclass. The majorEuropeanrevolutionsof the eighteenththroughthe earlytwentiethcenturieswereprobablyprovoked,at least in part,by class disparities.Fromthe Frenchto the Russianrevolutions,have-notsindisputablybecamemajorparticipantsin political 20 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION violence (Zamoyski1999). On the otherhand,as notedabove,the left-wing terrorists of the 1960s to 1970s were not usually impoverished;indeed, they were sometimes accusedof belongingto an idle middleclass thatexpropriatedthe miseryof a different class to serve theirown goals. So, althoughpovertymay play a role in some political violence, relativedeprivationis neithernecessarynor sufficientto explainrevolutionary terrorism.Evidence also exists thatright-wingextremismoccurs independentof economic status.CanettiandPedahzur(2002), for example,reportedthatright-wing extremistsentimentswere unrelatedto socioeconomic variablesamong 1,247 Israeli universitystudents. Kruegerand Maleckova's(2002) previouslycited importantwork with Palestinconclusion. However,their ians does not supporta simple poverty-causes-terrorism analysis is based on socioeconomic background,not on socioeconomic prospects. Giventhe 70 percentadultunemploymentratein Gaza,the gross domesticproductof less than $1,000 throughoutthe PalestinianTerritories,the severelyconstrainedeconomic opportunitiesdespite educationalachievementdue to the unresolvedIsraeliPalestinianconflict,andthe culturalimportanceof the malebreadwinnerrole, it is prematureto ruleout the possibilitythatdiminishedeconomicprospectshavehelpedprovoke Palestinianterrorism(Bennet 2004). Furthermore,nationalist-separatistand many religious fundamentalistterroriststend to enjoy the supportof theircommunities. In such cases, terrorismmay be a prosocial activity ostensibly undertakenon behalf of all classes. If the entirein-group(thatof the politicalactor)faces economic disparitiesrelativeto anout-group(thatof theprivilegedtarget),participationin political violence would not be expectedto be an economic class phenomenonbuta groupof-identityphenomenon.Furtherresearchwill be necessaryto determinethe relationshipbetweenclass of origin,economicexpectations,individualfactors,andterrorism. OPPRESSION THEORY Multipleauthors,fromsociologiststo revolutionaries,contendthatoppressionprovokes political violence (Fanon 1965; Whitaker1972; Schmid 1983). Particularlyin the case of nationalist-separatistor ethnic-sectarianterrorism(e.g., ETA, PIRA, Hamas),actorsoften cite the injusticeof theirtreatmentby governmentsthatrobthem of identity,dignity, security,and freedom as the motive for theirjoining a terrorist group (Crenshaw1986; Taylorand Quayle 1994; Post, Sprinzak,and Denny 2003). Since it is difficultto measureoppressionitself-a sociopoliticalrelationshipsubject to point of view-and since the impact of oppression may be felt subjectivelyto greateror lesser degreesby individualswithina communityat risk,perceivedoppression may be the propercognitive-emotionalvariableto examineas a potentialriskfactor for terrorism.There are innumerablescales and instrumentsfor assessing perceived prejudiceanddiscrimination(e.g., McNeilly et al. 1996; Utsey andPonterotto 1996; Neto 2001; Loo et al. 2001; Murryet al. 2001; Duckittet al. 2002). However, virtuallyall of these are specifically designed to addressthe experience of a single group-in most cases, African Americans. None of them measures the life-andliberty-threateningdominion of one group over another implied by the psychopolitical concept of oppression. In fact, an extensive review of multiple databases Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 21 revealsthatno generalpsychologicalinstrumenthas yet been validatedandpublished for the study of perceivedoppression.As a result, no persuasiveempiricalevidence is availablesupportingthe much-citedhypothesis that oppressionor its perception drivesthe behaviorof terrorists. Evenif perceivedoppressioncouldbe shownto breedterrorism,it wouldneverbe a sufficient explanation.As Silke (2003, 33) said so well, "Veryfew individuals of aggrievedminoritiesgo on to become activeterrorists.The questionhas alwaysbeen, why did these particularindividualsengage in terrorismwhen most of theircompatriots did not?"Sociological theories,like rationalchoice approaches,do not answerthis question. NATIONAL CULTURAL THEORY While many differences are observed between cultures, a specific variablewas claimed to be key by Weinbergand Eubank (1994), who proposed that terrorism expressesitself differentlyin "collectivist"versus"individualist"cultures.According to this theory,in collectivist cultures,a person'sidentityis primarilyderivedfromthe social system, dividingthe world strictlyaccordingto in-groupsand out-groupsand linkingtheirpersonalwell-being to the well-being of theirgroup,while in individualist cultures,identity is derivedfrom personalgoals. Weinbergand Eubankpropose thatcollectivistswould be morelikely thanindividualiststo carryout terroristattacks on out-groups,includingforeigners.Individualistswouldbe less inhibitedin attacking one of theirown. The authorsused psychologicalrankingsof IBM corporateemployees in forty nationson a scale of individualism/collectivism,reportingthatU.S. citizens were the most individualistic(score 91), Israelis were in the middle (54), and ThirdWorldnations tended to be the most collectivist, such as Pakistanat 14 and Columbiaat 13. Comparingthese rankingswith reportsof terroristactivityfrom the Terrorism:Attributesof TerroristEvents 1968-1977"(ITERATE)data"International base (Mickolus 1980), they claim that the data support their prediction that collectivistsaremorelikely to attackforeigners,while individualistsaremorelikely to attackconationalsor membersof otherindividualistcultures.Theyalso claim,without data, that individualists feel morally restrainedfrom attacking innocents, while collectivists have two moralities-one for the in-group,one for the out-group-and would not be morallyinhibitedfrom attackinginnocentsin the out-group. This work might be criticizedon numerousgrounds:the paucityof data thatcultures can be rankedon this collectivist/individualistdimension;the doubt that IBM employees are representativeof their cultures;the failure to addressthe possibility that,withinnations,subculturesexist thatvaryon the presumeddimension(such that terroristsderivefrom a distinctsubculture);the likelihoodthat,regardlessof national culture,individualistsarise who become terrorists;the likelihood thatthe ITERATE databasefor thatdecadecapturedprimarilyleft-wing revolutionarieswho may beara or relidifferentrelationshipto their cultureof origin than do nationalist/separatists gious radicals;andthe factthatno dataareofferedsupportingthe theoryof differential moralinhibition.Nonetheless, setting aside the simplisticconcept of "national"culture,the concept thatdifferencesin group culture,as exploredin culturalanthropol- 22 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION ogy, might influencethe expressionof terrorismand audienceresponsesto terrorism seems worthyof furtherinvestigation. PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORIES In contrastto sociological theoriesthatemphasizefactorsinfluencingthe behavior of an entire group,psychological theories of terroristbehaviorprimarilyemphasize individualfactors.Since the earlytwentiethcentury,a fiercecontroversyhas roiledthe psychiatriccommunity,dividingpsychoanalyticapproachesto the studyof individual psychology, primarilyderivativeof Freudiantheory, from nonpsychoanalyticapproaches (Wallerstein1995). For the purposes of this review, these approachesare consideredseparately. PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOLOGICAL THEORIES OF TERRORISM Psychoanalysisis basedon the propositionthatmuchof mentallife is unconscious, thatpsychologicaldevelopmentproceedsin stagesbasedon infantilesexualfantasies, and thatpsychologicaldistressderivesfrom unresolvedintrapsychicconflict regarding those fantasies(Gabbard2000). The "dynamics"of this theory was literallyderived from nineteenth-centuryconcepts of physics, in which the flow of mentaland libidinalenergy is deterministicallyexpressed,repressed,or discharged.The theory has variants,butthey sharethe notionsthat(1) parenting(as opposedto intrinsictemperament)determinespsychologicaltemperamentandhealth;(2) active,unconscious forces excludeunpleasantthoughtsfromthe consciousness;and(3) relationshipswith others,"objectrelations,"are controlledby unconsciousforces such as projectionthe theory that one irrationallyattributesone's own attitudeto others (Wallerstein 1995; Gabbard2000). Multiplenonscientificassumptionsunderliethe "discoveries" claimed by psychoanalysts,principallythatthe early analysts'impressionisticinterpretationsof classic cases accordingto theirown dynamictheoryconstituteevidence supportingthat theory. Psychoanalyticapproachesto terroristbehaviormay be roughlydividedaccording to theiremphasison identitytheory,narcissismtheory,paranoiatheory,andabsolutist thinking. Identity Theory It has been proposedthatcandidatesfor terrorismare young people lacking selfesteemwho have strongor even desperateneeds to consolidatetheiridentities(Olsson 1988). On the basis of unstructured(andlargelyundocumented)interviewswith Irish andEuropeanterrorists,Taylorand Quayle (1994) reportedthatmanybecamepolitically violent, seeking a sense of purposeand self-worth-"a place in the sun."The theoryof psychologistErikErikson(1959), thatadolescentsreacha stage of identity formationat which ideologies assist in self-definition,was the basis for Bollinger's Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 23 (1981; also see Crenshaw1986) psychoanalyticinterpretationof his interviewswith eight members of Germanterroristgroups: Billinger claimed that overcontrolling parentspreventedthese respondentsfrom developing autonomy,leading to identity crisesthatmadeviolent struggleirresistible.At the extreme,those with identityconfusion are perhapstormentedby a sense of isolation, conceivablyengaging in terrorist violence as an adaptiveresponseto the pain of anomie (Ferracuti1982). Thisperspectiveis consistentwith Freud's(1953-1974) speculationthatthe principle of self-determinationmay be inseparablefrom the impulse for destruction.This view is also reminiscentof the theoriesof psychiatristFrantzFanon(1965), who posited thatviolence againstcolonial oppressionliberatesnot only the body but also the self-identity.MenachemBegin (1977) offered his own confirmationof this mode of thinkingwith his neo-Cartesianaphorism:"Wefight, thereforewe are."Youngpeople turningto politicalviolence in a desperatesearchfor identitymay act alone (e.g., perhapsCharlesBishop, a fifteen-year-oldwho flew a small airplaneinto a bankin early 2002, leaving a suicide note declaring his allegiance with al Qaeda) (Rosenberg, Waddell,and Smalley 2002), yet they may be very eager to join groups-a behavior offeringan instantaneousgraftingof identity.Identity-starved joinersarealso hypothesized to be motivatedby a desire to embracethe intimatetutelage of a charismatic leader-a formof anacliticdevotion(choosing a love objectwho resemblesa parent). To date,no controlledempiricalstudytestingthe applicabilityof this theoryto young terroristshas been published. Narcissism Theory JohnCrayton(1983), EricShaw(1986), RichardPearlstein(1991), andothershave invoked Kohut'sself psychology to explain the sequence that drives young people to terrorism.PsychoanalystHeniz Kohut (1972, 1978; see also Wallerstein1995; Gabbard2000) developedself psychology as a departurefrom the classical ego psychology of Freud.Self psychology emphasizesthe needs thatan infanthas for caring responses to develop normally.Failureof maternalempathyleads to damage to the self-image-so called narcissistic injury-that arrestsdevelopment in one of two ways: persistentinfantile grandiose fantasies or failure to internalizethe idealized image of the parent.Eitherproblempreventsthe developmentof adult identity and morality.Crayton,for example,proposedthatpoliticalexperience,such as the humiliationof subordination,mightproducean adultnarcissisticinjurythatmightreawaken the psychologicaltraitof infantilenarcissism.The resultmightbe a pathologicalexaltationof self (the genesis of the leader),the abandonmentof independenceto merge with the archaicomnipotentfigure (the genesis of the follower), or a combinationof these impulses,as seen in the egotisticalyearningfor glory underthe maskof selflessness. Both of these forms of infantileretreatarehypothesizedto mobilize the expression of the desireto destroythe sourceof the injury(i.e., narcissisticrage).This rageis, in essence, rage againstthe damagedself, projectedonto the targetof the terrorist's animus,as if thetargetwerethe sourceof theintolerablefeelings the terroristhas about himself (Crayton1983; Akhtar1999). Accordingto Risto Fried(1982), the targetor victim is treatedas a "discardableobject,"which psychoanalystRichardPearlstein 24 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION cited as evidencethatterrorismis a "spectacularlyvivid exampleof narcissisticobject manipulation." The theory of terroristnarcissismis consistent with many reportsregardingthe pathologicallydependentpsychology of cult adherents,but it is perhapsmore pertinent that it fits with empirical observationsof both Hubbard(1971) and el Sarraj (2002) that terrorists,far from being the aggressivepsychopathsof public imagination, areoftentimid,emotionallydamagedadolescents-those who havesufferedego injuriessuch as parentalrejectionthatdelay or preventfull achievementof adultidentity-who seem to be in searchof affiliationand meaning.In this respect,narcissism andidentitytheoryoverlap.Potentialsupportfor the importanceof narcissismcomes from GustaveMorf's (1970) clinical examinationsconductedwith prisonersheld as membersof the Frontfor the Liberationof Quebec (FLQ). Morf reportedthat these individualsexhibitednarcissistictraits,wishing to put themselvesat the centerof the universe,but did not fulfill the criteriafor a full-blownnarcissisticpersonalitydisorder.He furtherconcludedthata "permissivesociety"was responsiblefor theirnarcissism. However,he used no standardizedpsychologicalinstruments,reportedno statisticaldata,andused no controlgroup.Like Sageman's(2004) previouslycitedexegesis of ten terroristbiographies,the conclusionsregardingnarcissismareimpressionistic, not empirical.As a result,it remainsundeterminedwhetherthe prevalenceof narcissistic traitsamong terroristsexceeds the prevalencein the general population.And otherauthoritieshaveobjectedthatnarcissismis unlikelyto explainterrorismin even a small numberof groups(Corrado1981; Reich 1998). Again, the intuitivelyplausible scenarioof identitydeficitwith narcissisticragein thedevelopmentalpathto terrorism has yet to be supportedby scientific study. Paranoia Theory George Washington University psychiatrist Jerrold M. Post is unequivocally amongthe principalcontributorsto politicalpsychologicaltheoriesof terrorism.Post (1998, 2004) offers a comprehensive,psychoanalyticallybasedformulationof terrorist behaviors-one thatincludesan explanationfor the terrorist'scapacityfor murder: echoing Kohut(1972, 1978), he positsthatthe salientfeatureof terroristpsychologyis projection,an infantiledefense thatassigns intolerableinternalfeelings to an external object when an individualwho has grown up with a damagedself-concept idealizes the good self and splits out the bad self. This projectionis proposedto be the root of an adult persistenceof the infantilephase that Melanie Klein called the "paranoidschizoid position"(RobinsandPost 1997). While not overtlypsychotic,the paranoid position nonethelessinflamesthe terroristwith suspicionsthatjustify bloody acts of "self-defense"againsthis victims: "the zeal of the torturer,the alacrityof the killer, representshis eagernessto destroythedevaluedanddisownedpartof the self"(Robins and Post 1997, 146). Post's paranoiatheory offers a developmentalmodel that explains not only why only a minorityof individualswith political grievancesturnsto terrorismbutalso why terroristskill those who do not appearto constituteanimminent threat. Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 25 Post (1998, 2004) bases his theoryin parton an interpretationof the findingsof the Germanpsychological team that interviewed250 radicalsfrom the 1970s-mostly left-wing revolutionaries(Jiger, Schmidtchen,and Stillwold 1981; Billinger 1981). Unfortunately,despitethe earnestambitionsof thatmajorstudy,no formalmeasurementsof paranoiawere used, therewas littleeffortto stratifyaccordingto hierarchical level androle, therewereno controls,andextrapolationsfromthis subtypeof terrorists to otherpoliticalcategoriesmaybe inappropriate.It seems plausible,for example,that the studentradicalof the 1970s who adopteda flagrantlyantisocialrevolutionaryideology is more likely to have exhibitedsome kind of psychological atypicalitythanis thetypicalPalestinianextremistor SunniIraqiinsurgentwho chooses behaviorwidely supportedwithin his community.A scientificallyweak but plausiblecriticismof the paranoiatheoryis providedby Sageman's(2004) findingthatnine of ten Muslimterroristbiographiesrevealedno evidence of paranoia.Yet the most importantcriticism of such psychoanalyticaltheories is that it is impossible to test any hypothesisthat attributescovertadultpsychodynamicforces to covertpsychosexualprocessespostulatedto haveoccurreddecadesbefore,in infancy.Paranoiatheory,like narcissismtheory, remains an intriguingalbeit impressionisticpsychoanalyticinterpretationthat might, after controlledresearchusing validatedmeasuresof paranoia,someday be shown to explain some instancesof this very heterogeneousadultbehavior. Absolutist/Apocalyptic Theory HarvardpsychiatristRobert J. Lifton is anotherimportantcontributor.Lifton's (2000) majorrecent contributionis an accountof the Aum Shinrikyocult and other apocalypticgroupsthatenvision mass destructionas a pathtowardreplacingthe corruptworldwith a purenew social order.Apocalypticgroupstypicallyexhibitabsolutist moralpolarization,idealizationof a messianicfigure,and impairedrealitytesting, imaginingvast conspiraciesof evil such as a "worldshadow government"of Jews. Lifton's insights-that absolutist/totalistmoralthinkinghelps motivateterrorismvia its seductive appeal to young adults with weak identities and that terroristsdefend themselves from normal emotional responses to violence through denial, psychic numbing,or isolationof affect-both fit withpsychoanalytictheory.Althoughneither absolutismnor isolationof affectby themselvesoffers an animusbelli or explainsthe specific impulse to harminnocents, it seems plausibleto predictthat irrationalviolence againstthe "other"would be precipitatedwhen pathologicaldefenses lead to black-and-whitethinkingaboutthe out-groupcombinedwithparanoiaaboutin-group annihilation.This is consistentwith the proposalof Devine and Rafalko(1982) to the effect that,paradoxically,terroristsare often uncompromisingmoralistswho see the world in starklypolarterms. Lifton's (2000) absolutistapproachto terrorismrepresentsa compellingcombination of psychoanalyticdevelopmentaltheorywith a theoryof atypicalcognitive style. However,the evidence offeredto supportthis theoryconsists of a subjective,theoryof unstructuredinterviewswith a few individualswho maynotbe driveninterpretation representative,and the postulatedexistentialistdespair,irrationalfantasiesof world- 26 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION wide dominion,andpathologicallydependentgroupbehaviorof apocalypticcults led by messianic leaders seem to characterizeonly a small minorityof terroristactions. One must still explain the majority. The great strengths of psychoanalytic interpretationsof terrorism are their acknowledgmentthatindividualdevelopmentalfactorsbeginningin early childhood probablyinfluence adult behavioralproclivities,their recognitionof the enormous power of the unconsciousto influence conscious thought,and theirobservationthat covertpsychodynamicforces of groupsmay subsumeindividuality.The greatweakness is theirlack of falsifiability.Psychoanalysishas been largely abandonedamong modern psychiatristsprecisely because it rejects the scientific method, asking that adherentsaccept its propositionsas received wisdom. This is not by any means to deny that early childhood,unconsciousprocesses, and group dynamicsmay be key factorsin the genesis of terrorism.However,psychoanalyticclaimsregardingpseudophysicalintrapsychicdynamicstied to presumptivestagesof sexualitycannotbe confirmedaccordingto the modernmethodsof social andbehavioralscience. A less ideological and more empirical psychodynamic model that nonetheless considers the crucialrole of the unconscious-tested, for example,via controlledresearchexamining whether a stratifiedsubgroupof terroristsexhibit elevated scores on validated measures of maternal rejection, self-absorption, or paranoia-might more persuasively demonstratehow developmentaland unconscious processes help drive terrorism. NONPSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOLOGICAL THEORIES OF TERRORISM Cognitive Theories Cognitivecapacity refersto mentalfunctionssuch as memory,attention,concentration,language, and the so-called "executive"functions,includingthe capacityto learnandfollow rules,to anticipateoutcomes,to makesensibleinferences,andto perform accuraterisk-benefitcalculations(Lezak 1995). Many of these mental operations areconductedwithinthe dorsolateralprefrontalcortexof thebrain,a largeneural association region that attends to perceptionof present circumstances,previously learnedassociations, and emotions to calculate and activateadaptiveplans. In contrast,the capacityto restrainimpulses and comportone's behaviorto social expectations depends on the ventromedialcortex, a region that sits just behind the eyes (Gazzaniga2000; Mesulam2000). Cognitivestyle refersto ways of thinking-that is, biases, prejudices,or tendenciesto over- or underemphasizefactorsin decision making. Apartfromreportsof absolutistthinking,littleattentionhas beenpaidto thepossibility thatterrorists,or subtypesof terrorists,exhibitidiosyncrasiesof eithercognitive capacityor cognitive style. Substantialevidence exists thatviolent behavioris influencedby cognitive capacity and/orstyle (Bryantet al. 1984; Kandelet al. 1988; Satterfield1998; Ernstet al. 2003). It has also been proposedthatcognitive style influencesthe aggressivebehav- Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 27 ior of political leaders(Satterfield1998). It is temptingto speculatethatvariationsin either the capacity or style of thoughtmight affect the likelihood that an individual would sympathizewith,join, follow, or lead a terroristgroup.It is also plausiblethat knowledge of typical variationsin cognitive capacityor style might supplementthe rational choice approach to help predict otherwise unaccountablebehaviors in responseto contingenciessuch as interactionswith governments.Unfortunately,this potentiallyrich vein of study has hardlybeen mined. Taylorand Quayle (1994), for example, speculatedthatyoung people joining terroristgroups make a fundamental attributionerror,a cognitivebias inaccuratelyattributingdevious and evil motives to those theyperceiveas oppressors.However,they offer no datasupportingthis reasonable-soundingclaim and no solutionto this potentialproblem,a cognitive factorthat might accountfor certaincomplicationsof conflict resolution.While some classified dataexist regardingcognitivecapacitiesof young terrorists(e.g., Merari1998, 2002), the open literaturedoes not reportneuropsychologicalfindings meaningfullycomparingterroristsor ex-terroristswith matchednonterrorists. Sidanius (1985) conductedone study of potentialimportanceto the question of cognitive style among terrorists:to examine cognitive factors in differenttypes of extremism,he measuredconservatism,cognitiveflexibility,cognitivecomplexity,and intolerance of ambiguity using normed and validated instruments,including the BudnerIntoleranceof AmbiguityScale (Budner1962), in a randomlyselectedsample of 195 Swedishhigh school students.Respondentswere classified as extremeleftists, moderateleftists,moderate,moderaterightists,or extremerightists.Contraryto theories claimingthatextremismis associatedwith cognitive limitations,extremeleftists and moderaterightistshad the highest cognitive complexity;moderateshad the lowest. Consistentwith some assumptionsregardingthe rigidityof right-wingvalues,less cognitive flexibility was associatedwith more sexual repressionand greatergeneral conservatism.Perhapsmost useful for the analysis of terrorism,reduced cognitive flexibility was associatednot only with intoleranceof ambiguity,especially the need for certaintyand uniformity,but also with racismand ethnocentrism.Thus, it seems worthexploringwhetherthose who become ethnic terrorists,drivenby a black-andwhite animus that does not accept the possibility of valued characteristicsamong membersof the out-group,aremorelikely to exhibitthe traitof cognitiveinflexibility. These findingsperhapsmeshwith those of CanettiandPedahzur(2002), who reported that right-wingextremismamong Israelis is associatedwith authoritarianattitudes, xenophobia,and supernaturalbeliefs. Muchfurtherworkwouldbe neededto determinewhethercognitivefactorssuch as inflexibilitymightconceivablyrepresenta generaltraitof terrorists,a predictabletrait of a politicalsubgroupof terrorists,or a traitof leadersthatmightbe identifiedby analysis at a distance.Findingsin this areamayconceivablyhavestrategicimportance.For example, terroristswith diminishedexecutive function will fail to anticipatefuture consequences.As a result,theirresponsesto negotiationor threatmay be less predictable. Those with excessive intoleranceof ambiguityor cognitive inflexibilitymay be less adaptable,unableto appreciatenuance,and more irrationalin bargaining.Political psychologists could potentiallycapitalizeon these factorsto help refine security 28 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION plans, identifyingbehavioralmarkersthatdistinguishterroristswho are more or less likely to follow projectedpaths. Novelty-Seeking Theory Some psychologicaltheoriesattributeterrorismto specific innateaspects of temperament.For example, developmentaltheories might predictthat youngsterswith aggressivetemperamentswould be disproportionatelyattractedto terroristorganizations (Pettit1997). However,no publishedresearchsupportsthis intuitivelyplausible supposition.In fact, a semistructuredpsychologicalanalysisof 227 left-wing German militantsfoundno commonpatternof aggressivity(Jager,Schmidtchen,andSillwold 1981). Anotherpossibilityis thatterrorismis associatedwith the traitof novelty seeking. Terroristplanningandexecutionis indisputablythrillingactionoutsidethe realm of ordinaryexperience,andmanytheoristshaveopinedthatpoliticalviolence may satisfy innate,perhapsgeneticallydeterminedneeds for high-level stimulation,risk, and catharsis(Hacker1983; Kellen 1979;Levine 1999). Possibly consistentwith this proposal, many incarceratedterroristshave expressed their pleasureand excitementat being involved in such thrillingaction (Juergensmeyer2000). IsraeliPrimeMinister YitshakShamir,who spenthis early adulthoodwith the terroristorganizationIrgun, conceivablyhintedat the samewhen he said, "Thatperiodin the undergroundwas the best partof my life" (Marton1996, 53). The "romanceof risk"mayexplainthe special affinity of teenagersfor such behavior:adolescentanxieties, sexual frustration,and developmentalattractionto risk takingmay play a role in a dangerousflirtationwith political violence (Ponton 1997; Levine 1999). This thesis raisestwo issues: first, sensationandnovelty seeking, a normativefeature of adolescentdevelopmentprobablytied to expected changes in neuralactivity (Dahl 2004), may play a role in the naturalhistoryof terroristinvolvement.Second, the personalitytraitof novelty seeking-a measurable,nonnormative,and probably geneticallyinfluencedcharacteristicthatpersistsin certainindividualswell into adulthood (Zuckerman2002)-possibly distinguishesthose who aremorelikely to be terrorismprone.Withregardto the firstpoint,evidencesuggeststhatthetypicaldevelopment of terroristsympathiesperhapsfollows an arc:young adolescentsare plastic in theirpoliticalorientationandopen to indoctrination.Positionshardenin lateradolescence suchthat,as Saper(1988, 26) putit, "oncebelief systems,resentments,anddesperate response tendencies are rigidly instilled . . . they are virtually impossible to modify belatedly."At the far end of the arc, reductionin destructivenessmay occur with maturity:interviewswith many"retired"terroristshaverevealeda mellowingof attitudeconsistentwith the theory that enthusiasmfor terroristaction is primarilya developmentalphenomenonof late adolescenceand early adulthood(Laqueur1987; Levine 1999; Akhtar1999). So the normativedevelopmentalformof novelty seeking probablydoes contributeto terrorism.Withregardto the secondpoint,no studieshave yet been reportedassessing the association between terroristsand adult-persistent nonnormativetraitsof risk taking or novelty seeking. While it seems plausiblethat individualsexhibitingsuch traitsaredisproportionatelyrepresentedamongterrorists, Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 29 pendingsupportiveevidence, it is prematureto concludethatthis atypicalpersonality featurehelps drive terrorism. Humiliation-Revenge Theory Humiliation-and the consequentinternalpressurefor revenge-is anotherpsychological factor that has been hypothesized to drive terrorist violence (Juergensmeyer2000). Revengefor humiliationby an oppressoris, in fact, an ancient culturaltraditionwith direct links to the currentviolence in the Middle East. The oppressionof the early Christians,embodiedin the image of Christon the cross, was partof the inspirationfor the apocalypticmovementin Christianitythatculminatedin the FirstCrusade(Armstrong2001). A cycle of oppressionandhumiliation,followed by violent actionin the nameof liberation,characterizesthe subsequenthistoryof the MiddleEast. PalestinianpsychiatristEyad el Sarraj(2002) has specifically observed thathumiliationis an importantfactormotivatingyoung suicide bombers.Dr. Abdul Aziz Rantisi,the late political leaderof Hamas,confirmedthis notion in a statement published three years before his death via targetedkilling by the Israeli Defense Forces:"Todie in this way is betterthanto die daily in frustrationand humiliation" (Juergensmeyer2000, 187). Several other authoritiesalso propose that humiliation, eitherby parentsin earlychildhoodor by politicaloppressorslaterin life, can provoke terrorism,butno quantitativeresearchhas yet exploredthis hypothesis(Crayton1983; Volkan1997; Stern2003). Whetherconsideredfromthe psychoanalyticpointof view as an inevitabledynamicconsequenceof narcissisticinjuryor from the nonpsychoanalyticpoint of view as a painfulsocial stressor,humiliationseems plausibleas the rootof anurgeto retaliateagainstpoliticalentitiesthatareperceivedto be responsible. The conceptthatfeelings of humiliationor being takenadvantageof gives rise to a passionfor revengeis very familiarin forensicpsychiatryandcriminologyandprobably contributesto many nonpolitical murders(Miller 1993; Brooks, Thomas, and Droppleman 1996; Schlesinger 2000; Meloy 2001). Note that revenge, in itself, should not be regardedas antisocialbehaviorbut as a normaland potentiallyuseful activity.Jurisprudenceformalizesthis featureof social contracts,emphasizingretributionin partas deterrence,andpolitieshaveused vengeancefor misdeedsto maintain theirintegrityat leastsince theTrojanWar.Indeed,revengeis anemotionthatis probably deeply rootedin the adaptiveinstinctto punishtransgressorswho violate the contractsof social species;hence, it is a motivatorthatoften servesnot only the goals of a vengeful individualbut also the goals of his group(Clutton-Brockand Parker1995). In this sense, revengeis often prosocialand sometimes-if the vengeancetaker(e.g., Achilles or suicidebombers)standsto sufferandhis groupto gain-even altruistic.In a recent study combining psychological with functional neuroimagingstudies in humans,de Quervainet al. (2004) showedthatindividualspunishsocial transgressors even when it is quitecostly to the punisher,andtheyreportedevidencethatthis altruistic behaviorwas drivenby deep subcorticalbrainactivitythatmay have overruledthe more rationalcortex. Knutson(2004) highlights the self-defeating and emotionally driven natureof vengeance demonstratedby this study, stating that these findings 30 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION "chip yet anothersliver from the rationalmodel of economic man."While this science intriguinglyhelps to explainhow revengemight motivateterroristsandperhaps governmentsto commit murderousbehaviors without likely strategicbenefits, no questionnairedataor measurementsof subjectivehumiliation,desire for revenge,or emotionalsatisfactionafterretributionin terroristsor ex-terroristshave yet been published. This plausibletheoryawaitsbettersubstantiation. THEORIES OF GROUP PROCESS Most publishedpsychological theoriesexplain terrorismas the productof group psychology withinidiosyncraticsubculturesthatcoalesce in reactionto circumstances they perceiveas intolerable(Taylorand Ryan 1988; Friedland1992; Hoffman 1998; Merari 1998; Levine 1999; Post 2004; Sageman 2004). Membershipin a terrorist organizationoffersdisciplesa headyliquorof a well-definedpersonalrole, a righteous purpose,the opportunityfor revengefor perceivedhumiliations,andthe liftingof constraintson the expression of otherwise prohibitedbehaviors-freeing the member frompersonalresponsibilityfor attackson out-groups(Hacker1983;TaylorandRyan 1988; Weinbergand Eubank1994; Stern 1999). Groupforces, includingideological indoctrination,repetitive training, and peer pressures,have been hypothesized to influencethe group'sviolence, whetheror not individualmemberswere predisposed to such behavior(Crenshaw1992; Clayton,Barlow,and Ballif-Spanvill1998). This mayoccurbecausecollectiveidentitysubsumesindividualidentity.As Post, Sprinzak, and Denny (2003, 176) put it, "Anoverarchingsense of the collective consumes the individual.This fusion with the groupseems to providethe necessaryjustificationfor theiractionswith an attendantloss of felt responsibility."This descriptionof the submersion of individualityis very reminiscentof Eric Hoffer's (1951, 128) statement that people who plunge into mass movements "arefashioned into incomplete and dependenthumanbeings even when they have withinthemselvesthe makingof selfsufficiententities."Withdrawalfromthe mainstreammay increasethe potencyof collectivethought:basedon a semiquantitativereviewof life historiesof morethan1,500 Italianand Germanmilitants,greaterisolation is associatedwith greaterseparation from social reality (della Porta 1992). The principaldebate among those discussing group versus individualfactors in political violence centerson whethergroup dynamicsare sufficientin and of themselves to turnan averagepersoninto a terroristor whetherindividualhistoryandpersonality must be consideredas well. Sageman, one strong proponentof the group hypothesis,goes so faras to say that"it'sa groupphenomenon.To searchfor individual characteristics . . . will lead you to a dead end" (Rotella 2004, A3). However, Sageman'spsychiatricassessmentsof Islamic mujahedinwere exclusively based on secondarysourcesthatdid not includeanyobjectivebehavioraldata,so his conclusion seems premature.Rasch(1979, 82) observedthatthe dynamicsof living in a terrorist grouptendsto alienateone fromothersbutthat"thestartingpoint andpersonalneeds existing at the time of entryinto the terroristgrouparevery differentfor the different terrorists."This claim of initial psychological heterogeneity followed by group- Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 31 inducedhomogenizationappearssensible,butit requiresempiricalverification.Consistentwiththis theory,Friedland(1992) postulatedthatterrorismis notpurelya group phenomenonbutis obviouslythe resultof an interactionbetweensocial processesand individual dispositions. However, he proposes three conditions under which individualproclivityto violence is a relativelyminorfactorin the group'sterroristturning: (1) deprivationis intense, (2) the group has ideologized its discontent,and (3) the groupis cohesive andclearlydifferentiatedfromthe out-group.He gives the example of the Palestinians,whose specialcircumstancesdriveindividualswith no specialpropensityto violence to undertaketerroristacts. This formulationseems plausibleon its surface.However,one still mustaccountfor the fact that,while most Palestinianssupportsuicidebombing,a very smallminoritydoes it. Furthermore,no publishedstudies supportthe propositionthatthese threeconditionsincreasegroupdynamicsuccess in drivingnonviolentpersonsto politicalviolence. Unless anduntil systematicresearch is conducted making in-depth psychological comparisons between terroristsand matched controls from identical political circumstancesand estimating premembershipandpostmembershipwillingnessto harminnocents,one cannotmeaningfully quantifythe relativeinfluence of individualand groupfactors. LIMITS AND IMPEDIMENTS TO BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE RESEARCH ON TERRORISM PsychiatristWalterReich (1998, 262) has warnedthat"psychologicalaccountsof terrorismare replete with explanationsthat ignore or blur the variety and complexity... a productof loose andweak thinking,a disregardfor the need for evidence, and the habit,unfortunatelyendemicin so manyareasof psychologicaldiscourse,of having a single idea and applying it to everything."Reich's strong caveat against overgeneralizationandreductionismis a vital counterto the potpourriof psychological theoriespromulgatedby terrorismscholars.In this, he supportsCorrado's(1981) criticalreview of the mentaldisordersapproachto politicalterrorism;Corradostates thata terroristpersonalityprobablydoes not exist and thatefforts to psychopathologize this type of aggressionarerootedin biased theory,not in unbiaseddata. Caveats against overgeneralizationand unwarrantedmedicalizationof terrorist behaviorsare logical and important.Yet it seems reasonableto seek a middle ground between the reductionistposition thatproposesa single psychology of terrorismand the nihilist position that denies any explicit psychology of terrorism.That is, until a rigorouseffortis madeto investigatethe nullhypothesisvia the collectionof empirical evidence, it is prematureto conclude that there are no distinguishingpsychological characteristicsamongthetinyminorityof individualswho arewilling to senda terrifying political message to a targetaudienceby attackinginnocentnoncombatants. Whyhas thebehavioralscience communityso farfailedto amassa persuasivebody of evidence in this domain? Multiple practical and theoretical impedimentshave delayedthe scientificpsychological studyof terrorism.Most of the practicalbarriers areobvious.Forexample,terrorismresearchmay involveexpensiveandinconvenient travelto politicallyunstableregions,is potentiallydangerous,andraisesethicalissues 32 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION that may challenge institutionalreview boards (e.g., Wieviorka 1995; Brannan, Eslerm, and Anders Strindberg2001). These issues may explain why journalists, ratherthanacademics,have publisheda substantialproportionof the availableliterature reportingbehavioralobservationsof terrorists.Active terroristsmay have little motivationto cooperatewith behavioralassessment, and inactiveterroristsmay no longerexhibitthe psychology of interest(Reich 1998). Languagebarriers-including the lack of expert translationsof high-qualitypsychological instruments-frustrate collection of data. Authoritiesmay deny scholars access to incarceratedterrorists because of security concerns and the perceptionthat such assessmentsare not pertinentto counterterrorism. A theoreticalissue thatseriouslylimitsthe utilityof interviewswith specificterrorists or groupsis the fact that,contraryto some publishedhypotheses,terrorismis not a unitarybehavior(Crenshaw1986; Laqueur1987; Haroun1999). As a result,theories thatattemptto generalizeandreducethe psychology of terrorismbegin with a premise thatis inconsistentwith the availableobservations,and studiesbasedon such theories will produceresultswith limitedpredictivevalue since they conflatedatafrommixed populations.Classifyingterrorismaccordingto probablehomogeneouspsychological subtypesthatare"atleast descriptive,inclusive,discrete,endowedwith forecastingor prognostic value, policy-generating, possibly etiological, and theoretically grounded,"as advisedby Ferracuti(1982, 132), may be an indispensablepreliminary step to designingresearchprojectsandinterpretingdata(Wilson2000). Moreover,as Crenshaw(1986, 384-85) urged,"theanalysisof terrorismdeals with the intersection of psychological predispositions(which may be derivedas much from priorexperience andsocializationas frompsychologicaltraitsemergingfromearlychildhoodand infancy)andthe externalenvironment."A completeunderstandingof the psychology of terrorism,therefore,will requirethe difficultinvestigationof the dynamicsof that intersection,in concertwith an understandingof the forces of groupdynamicsand a quantitative analyses of events, a challenge demanding an interdisciplinary perspectivebeyond the bordersof parochialregimes. Fundinghas long been a problem,with limitedfederalsupport(Jenkins1983). In 2004, the U.S. Homeland Security Department(DHS) published a Broad Agency Announcementsolicitingproposalsfor a university-basedHomelandSecurityCenter for BehavioralandSocial Aspects of TerrorismandCounter-terrorism, to be fundedat $4 millionperyearfor threeyears(U.S. Departmentof HomelandSecurity2004). On one hand,thisis a historicfirst,the largestgranteverofferedto supportresearchon this vital issue. On the otherhand,the amountinvolvedrepresentsa very smallproportion of the DHS researchbudgetof morethan$500 millionperyear(Brumfiel2003) anda paltrypartof the U.S. defense budget.The announcementemphasizesmodeling "to detect, prevent,preparefor, and respondto terroristactivity at the earliest possible point in time" (U.S. Departmentof HomelandSecurity2004, 8). The above review suggeststhatthat"earliestpointin time"may occurdecadespriorto the formationof a terroristgroup or the planningof an attackand may relateto a combustibleconvergence of historical,political, and psychosocial factors.Psychological researchmay contributemost to long-termnationalsecurityby an open-mindedexplorationof the deep roots of terrorism,with a time horizonthatextendsfarbeyond imminentthreats Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 33 and a conceptualhorizonthatextends beyond the traditionalmission of intelligence services. But the most importantbarriersto scientificresearchon terrorismmay have deeper origins. Terrorism,like a sharkattack,wields tremendouspsychological impact.It is rare but awesome, deriving almost mystical significance by virtue of the suddenness, drama,and outrageousnessof its violence (Zulaikaand Douglass 1996; Mahmood 2001). But terroristsare not bogeymen, and both behavioral scientists and the counterterrorism communitymustbe waryof explainingthe terroristmind-setby prowritespsychoanalyst jecting the stateof mindrequiredto act in this way. "Terrorists," Lloyd DeMause(1986, 419), are"containersinto which one can projectone's unconscious hostility."While this statement again presumes a difficult-to-testpsychodynamictheory,it is legitimateto proposethatsubjectivityconfoundsthe design and the interpretationof terrorismstudies. That is, in both the scholarly and counterterrorismrealms,one mustacknowledgethepossibilitythatterrorismexcites passions thaterode logical discourse,leading to responsesthatarereactiveand enragedrather thanproactiveandanalytical(ZulaikaandDouglass 1996). Justas the terroristadopts absolutistthinkingto justify his indefensiblyimmoralactions, the horrificthreatof terrorismmay perhaps provoke absolutist thinking about terroristsamong some observersand may conceivablylead threatenedgroupsnot only to discountthe value of objectivestudyandprejudgeor misinterpretthe availabledatabutalso to rationalize extralegalsteps and the curtailmentof civil rights in the name of a war on terrorism (Pettit 1997). In his WorldWar I-era essay, "Thoughtsfor the Times on War and Death,"Freud (1953-1974) admonishedthat nation-statessometimes "makeuse of theirintereststo rationalizetheirpassions."This hypothesisitself could be the subject of study. A culturaldividealso separatesbehavioralscientistsfromlaw enforcement,intelliforces occupy an adversarialposition gence, andmilitarypersonnel:counterterrorism and must steel themselves against any sympatheticconsiderationof the terrorist's position. Behavioral scientists, no matterhow much they despise terroristactions, must steel themselvesto adoptthe position of unbiasedobserversand interpretersof behavior(Soskis 1983). While behavioralscientists may recognize markedpsychological heterogeneityandeven prosocialfeaturesof terroriststhatmight be exploited in the developmentof policy, counterterrorismforces and even policy makersmay resist suchconclusionsdue to culturalbias, cognitiveinflexibility,or attributionerror. by isoThis divide unfortunatelymay undercutthe effectivenessof counterterrorism lating practitionersfrom theorists-a separationakin to isolating engineersfrom the discoveriesof physicists. CONCLUSIONS AND PROPOSALS The leadingpsychologicaltheoriesof terrorismincludea broadspectrumof sociological, psychological,andpsychiatricapproaches.Strikingly,virtuallynone of them has been testedin a systematicway. They areoverwhelminglysubjective,speculative, 34 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION TABLE3 PsychologicalVariablesPotentiallyIdentifyingTerroristSubtypes Variable Reality testing Sociality Temperament Cognitivecapacity Cognitivestyle Dominance Classification Nonpsychoticvs. psychotic Prosocialvs. antisocial Typicalvs. atypicalfor culture Atypically aggressive/hostile Novelty seeking Identityseeking Affectively atypical(depression,irritability,anxiety) Vulnerableto charismaticinfluence Sensitiveto perceptionof oppression Sensitiveto humiliation Vengeful Self-destructive Normalvs. impaired Executivefunctionimpairment Impulsecontrolimpairment Typicalvs. atypicalfor culture Intoleranceof ambiguity Low vs. high complexity Leadervs. follower and, in many cases, derivedfrom 1920s-erapsychoanalytichypothesesthat are not amenableto testing. Studentsof terrorismmightjustifiablyconclude from the peerreviewedliteraturethatthe totalnumberof publishedtheoriesexceeds the numberof empiricalstudies-an imbalancethatmay be of morethanacademicimport.Even the smallamountof psychologicalresearchis largelyflawed,rarelyhavingbeen basedon scientificmethodsusingnormedandvalidatedmeasuresof psychologicalstatus,comparingdirectexaminationof individualswith appropriatecontrols,andtestinghypotheses with accepted statisticalmethods. Insofar as policy makersrely on published analysesof the "themindof the terrorist," policies intendedto reducetheriskof terrorism may be basedon invalidpremises.The best solutionis hypothesis-basedresearch andevidence-basedpolicies. Towardthatend, I offer severalpreliminaryconclusions and proposals: behavioris probablyalwaysdetermined of innatefactors, by a combination 1. Terrorist factors,cognitivefactors,temperament, envibiologicalfactors,earlydevelopmental ronmental influences,andgroupdynamics(seeTable3). Thedegreeto whicheachof to a giveneventprobably variesbetweenindividual terrorists, thesefactorscontributes betweenindividualgroups,andbetweentypesof groups.Theoriesthatclaimthepresincenostudieshave dominance of oneof theseinfluencesovertheothersarepremature systematically examinedmorethanone or twoof thesefactors,let aloneempirically themuch-cited examinedonewhilecontrolling fortheothers.Inparticular, claimthat noindividual factorsidentifythoseatriskforbecomingterrorists is basedoncompletely research. inadequate Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 35 A new model is needed,one thataccommodatesthe multiplicityof forces at workto arriveat plausibleandtestableconsilience-that is, a unifiedtheorythatis explanatory across levels of analysis and examples of terroristactivity.One possibility is a neuroeconomic model thatacknowledgesthe ultimateadaptivenatureof this behavior,modified by an empiricallybased psychology identifying the influence of individualand groupdynamics.Terrorismis unequivocallya multiplyandvariablydeterminedsubtype of humanaggression.Recognizing this fact may be the first step towardthe extremely challengingjob of designingresearch,conductingresearch,and interpretingdata. 2. Terroristsarepsychologicallyextremelyheterogeneous.Whateverhis statedgoals and groupof identity,every terrorist,like everyperson,is motivatedby his own complex of psychosocial experiences and traits.Plausible psychological variablesand classes of behaviorare summarizedin Table3. 3. Terroristsexhibiting differentpsychological subtypes probablyconform to different behavioralproclivities.It is plausiblebutyet to be proventhatdifferenttypes of terrorism disproportionatelyattractindividualswith specific temperaments.Futureresearch should attemptto determinethe most likely psychological types among terroristsin groupswith differentpolitical orientations,as well as the relationshipbetweenpsychological types, individualroles in the group, and typical responses to constraints.For example,the psychology,morality,andresponseto bargainingamongterroristswho are primarilyprosocialin theirorientationmay proveto be dramaticallydifferentfromthat of antisocialterrorists.Leadersand followers tend to be psychologically distinct.Because leadershiptends to requireat least moderatecognitive capacity,assumptionsof rationalitypossibly apply better to leaders than to followers. Those with diminished executivefunctionmay be less predictable.Those with subnormalcognitive flexibility may be less adaptableand more irrationalin bargaining.Those with atypicaltemperaments-who aredrivenby an excessive need for self-affirmation,hatred,vengefulness, or self-destructiveness-may behave more erratically.Improvedmodeling of markers of psychological subtypesmay enhancethe predictionof terroristbehaviors. 4. Acceptingthatterroristsareheterogeneous,fourtraitsmaypossiblybe characteristicsof "typical"terroristswho lead or follow in substategroups: a. High affective valence regardingan ideological issue b. A personalstake-such as stronglyperceivedoppression,humiliation,or persecution;anextraordinaryneed for identity,glory,or vengeance;or a drivefor expression of intrinsic aggressivity-that distinguisheshim or her from the vast majorityof those who fulfill characteristica c. Low cognitiveflexibility,low tolerancefor ambiguity,andelevatedtendencytoward attributionerror d. A capacityto suppressboth instinctiveandlearnedmoralconstraintsagainstharming innocents, whether due to intrinsic or acquired factors, individual or group forces-probably influencedby a, b, and c Thesefourcharacteristicsseem plausiblebasedon the abovesummaryof research.They are testablehypothesesproposedfor furtherstudy. 5. It seems plausiblethatthe cultureof origindifferentiates,to some degree,expectedindividual and group dynamics. However, group theory would predict that the internal psychodynamicsof a terroristgroupis influencedas muchby the specific personalityof its leaderand the temperamentsof its followers as accordingto any systematicdiffervs. religious). ence accordingto politically types (e.g., nationalist/separatist 6. The currentthrustof strategicchoice studiesfocuses on predictingthe behaviorof committedterrorists.Forthepurposesof long-termsecuritypolicy formulation,anincreased emphasisshouldbe placedon earlyprevention,thatis, on the analysisof the interaction between those psychological, cultural,economic, and political factors that influence uncommittedbut impressionableyoung people to turntowardterrorism. 36 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION 7. A balance must be achieved between the benefits of secrecy and the urgentneed to advanceknowledgein this field. Restrictedaccess to datawill slow scholarlyprogress with unknownconsequencesto nationalandinternationalsecurity.A reviewof the ultimateimpactof this issue at the highestlevels of securitypolicy may be requiredto optimize this balanceand overcomepotentiallycounterproductivebarriers. 8. Scholarsmust be willing to attemptresearchthatbringsthem into directcontactwith active terrorists,recentlyactive terrorists,or those at risk for becomingterrorists.Noncoercive recruitment,voluntaryparticipation,and informedconsent are essential. 9. A majorinvestmentis requiredto advancethe field of the behavioralandsocial aspects of terrorism.Meaningfulresearchis likely to be interdisciplinary, empirical,controlled, ethical, conductedacross levels of analysis, anddirectedat root causes andmodifiable riskfactorsalong the entirechain of causalityfromhistoricalforces to childhoodinfluences to the momentof a terroristact. Since the best expertsin anydisciplineareinevitably scatteredgeographically,ratherthan dependingon a single center of excellence, fundingcommensuratewith the magnitudeof the threatshouldbe availableon a competitivebasis to seriousscholarswhereverthey workthroughindependentscience supporterssuch as the NationalScience Foundationor the Departmentof Defense. The problem is to ask questions the answers to which are most likely to make a difference for security, to prioritize research within the remarkable spectrum of possible investigations, and to develop practical projects. For example, is the carrot of perceived concern for victims of disenfranchisement or the stick of high-altitude bombing a better investment in reducing the psychological forces nurturing the next generation of potential terrorists? What observable behavioral traits distinguish terrorist groups or leaders who would be likely to back away from aggression if their grievances were addressed by negotiation, as opposed to traits distinguishing groups that can only be deterred by force? Is the social influence of fundamentalist madrasas associated with a measurable increase in the likelihood of adult terrorist behavior? If so, could support for alternative, culturally valued education help impressionable young people find more productive foci for their high emotional energy? Do economic prospects and a sense of personal hope reduce the lure of terrorism? If so, what socioeconomic or psychological factors modify that association, and what cost-benefit formula is applicable? Do psychological traits of leaders of target nations drive policies that mitigate or exacerbate the threat? Answers to these and similar questions may be part of the key to avoiding catastrophic violence in the twenty-first century. REFERENCES Achenbach,T.M., andC. Edelbrock.1987.Manualfortheyouthself-reportandprofile.Burlington:University of Vermont,Departmentof Psychiatry. Akhtar,S. 1999. The psychodynamicdimensionof terrorism.PsychiatricAnnals 29:350-5. Alexander,Y. 2002. Palestinianreligiousterrorism:Hamasand IslamicJihad.Ardsley,NY: Transnational Publishers. AmericanPsychiatricAssociation.2000. Diagnosticandstatisticalmanualof mentaldisorders.4th ed., text revision.Washington,DC: AmericanPsychiatricAssociation. Anonymous. 2004. The 9/11 Commissionreport: Final report of the national commissionon terrorist attacksupon the UnitedStates.New York:Norton. Armstrong,K. 2000. Islam. New York:The ModernLibrary. Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 37 2001. Holy war: The Crusadesand their impacton today'sworld.New York:Anchor. ---. Atran,S. 2003. Genesis of suicide terrorism.Science 299:1534-9. Azam, J.-P.Forthcoming.Suicide-bombingas inter-generational investment.Public Choice. Badey,T.J. 1998.Defininginternationalterrorism:A pragmaticapproach.Terrorismand Political Violence 10:90-107. Bandura,A. 1973. Aggression:A social learninganalysis. New York:PrenticeHall. . 1998.Mechanismsof moraldisengagement.In Originsof terrorism:Psychologies,ideologies, theologies, states of mind,edited by W. Reich, 161-92. Washington,DC: WoodrowWilson CenterPress. Barber,B. K. 1999. Politicalviolence, family relations,andPalestinianyouthfunctioning.Journalof Adolescent Research 14:206-30. Begin, M. 1977. The revolt:Storyof the Irgun.Jerusalem:Steimatzky'sAgency. Bennet, J. 2004. In chaos, Palestiniansstrugglefor way out. New YorkTimes,July 15, 1, 10-11. Billig, M. 1976. Social psychologyand intergrouprelations.London:Academic Press. Billinger, L. 1981. Die entwicklung zu terroristischemhandeln als psychosozialerprozess: begegnungen mit beteiligten. In Analyzen zum terrorismus 2: Lebenslaufanalysen, edited by H. Jiiger, G. Schmidtchen,and L. Sillwold. Darmstadt,Germany:DeutscherVerlag. Borah,L. A. 1963. The effects of threatin bargaining:Criticalandexperimentalanalysis.JournalofAbnormal and Social Psychology66:37-44. Bowen, R., Y. Alexander,R. Cox, M. Crenshaw,andD. Rapoport.1985. Colby College conferencereport. Terrorism:An InternationalJournal8:79-112. Towardsan indeBrannan,D. W., P. E Eslerm,andN. T. AndersStrindberg.2001. Talkingto "terrorists": pendentanalyticframeworkfor the studyof violent substateactivism.Studiesin Conflictand Terrorism 24:3-24. Brooks,A., S. Thomas,andP.Droppleman.1996.Fromfrustrationto redfury:A descriptionof work-related angerin male registerednurses.NursingForum31:4-15. Brooks,D. 2002. The cultureof martyrdom:How suicidebombingbecamenotjust a meansbutan end. The AtlanticMonthly289:18-20. Brophy-Baermann, B., and J. A. C. Conybeare.1994. Retaliatingagainstterrorism:Rationalexpectations and the optimalityof rules versusdiscretions.AmericanJournalof Political Science 38:196-210. Brumfiel,G. 2003. Researchmiredin homelandsecuritydelays.Nature424:986. Bryant,E. T., M. L. Scott,C. J. Golden,andC. D. Tori.1984. Neuropsychologicaldeficits,learningdisability, and violent behavior.Journalof Consultingand Clinical Psychology52:323-4. Budner,S. 1962. Intoleranceof ambiguityas a personalityvariable.Journalof Personality30:29-50. Canetti,D., andA. Pedahzur.2002. The effects of contextualandpsychologicalvariableson extremerightwing sentiments.Social Behaviorand Personality30:317-34. Carey,B. 2002. Methodwithoutmadness?Los Angeles Times,July 7, 1. Clark,R. 1983. Patternsin the lives of ETAmembers.Terrorism6:423-54. Clayton,C. J., S. H. Barlow,andB. Ballif-Spanvill.1998.Principlesof groupviolence witha focus on terrorism. In Collectiveviolence, edited by H. V. Hall and L. C. Whitaker,277-311. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Clutton-Brock,T. H., and G. A. Parker.1995. Punishmentin animalsocieties. Nature373:209-16. Cooper,H. H. A. 1977.Whatis a terrorist:A psychologicalperspective.LegalMedicalQuarterly1:16-32. ---. 1978. Psychopathas terrorist:A psychological perspective.Legal Medical Quarterly2:253-62. Corrado,R. R. 1981. A critiqueof the mentaldisorderperspectiveof politicalterrorism.InternationalJournal of Law and Psychiatry4:293-310. Crayton,J. W. 1983. Terrorismandthe psychology of the self. In Perspectiveson terrorism,editedby L. Z. Freedmanand Y. Alexander,33-41. Wilmington,DE: ScholarlyResources,Inc. Crenshaw,M. 1981. The causes of terrorism.ComparativePolitics 13:379-99. . 1986.Thepsychologyof politicalterrorism.InPoliticalpsychology,editedby M. G. Hermann,379413. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass. . 1992. How terroriststhink:Whatpsychologycan contributeto understandingterrorism.In Terrorism: Roots, impact,responses,edited by L. Howard.New York:Praeger. . 2000. Thepsychologyof terrorism:An agendaforthe 21st century.PoliticalPsychology21:405-20. 38 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION Crozier,B. 1960. The rebels:A studyof post-war insurrections.London:Chatto& Windus. Dahl,R. E. 2004. Adolescentbraindevelopment:A periodof vulnerabilitiesandopportunities.Annalsof the New YorkAcademyof Sciences 1021:1-22. Davies, J. C. 1973.Aggression,violence, revolutionandwar.InHandbookofpoliticalpsychology,editedby J. N. Knutsen,234-60. San Francisco:Jossey-Bass. della Porta,D. 1988. Recruitmentprocesses in clandestinepolitical organizations:Italianleft-wing terrorism. InternationalSocial MovementResearch 1:155-69. . 1992. Political socializationin left-wing undergroundorganizations:Biographiesof Italianand ---Germanmilitants.InternationalSocial MovementResearch4:259-90. DeMause, L. 1986. The real targetwasn't terrorism.JournalofPsychohistory 13:413-26. de Quervain,D. J.-E, U. Fischbacher,V.Treyer,M. Schellhammer,U. Schnyder,A. Buck,andE. Fehr.2004. The neuralbasis for altruisticpunishment.Science 305:1254-8. Deutsch, K. W. 1954. Game theory and politics. CanadianJournal of Economics and Political Science 20:76-83. Deutsch,K. W., andR. M. Krause.1962. Studiesof interpersonalbargaining.Journalof ConflictResolution 6:52-76. Devine, P.E., andR. J. Rafalko.1982. On terror.Annalsofthe AmericanAcademyof Political & Social Science 463:39-53. Dollard,J., L. W. Doob, N. E. Miller,W. Mowrer,andR. R. Sears. 1939. Frustrationand aggression.New Haven,CT:Yale UniversityPress. Duckitt,J., C. Wagner,I. du Plessis, andI. Birum.2002. The psychologicalbases of ideology andprejudice: Testinga dual process model. Journalof Personalityand Social Psychology83:75-93. el Sarraj,E. 2002. [Televisedinterview].CBSNews, April 4. Enders,W.,andT. Sandler.2000. Is transnationalterrorismbecomingmorethreatening?A time seriesinvestigation.Journalof ConflictResolution44:307-32. Erikson,E. 1959. Identityand the life cycle, 171. PsychologicalIssues Monograph1. New York:International UniversityPress. Ernst,M., S. J. Grant,E. D. London,C. S. Contorreggi,A. S. Kimes, andL. Spurgeon.2003. Decision making in adolescentswithbehaviordisordersandadultswith substanceabuse.AmericanJournalof Psychiatry 160:33-40. Fanon,F. 1965. The wretchedof the earth. New York:Pelican. Reprintof Les damnesde la terre (Paris, 1961). Ferracuti,F. 1982.A sociopsychiatricinterpretation of terrorism.Annalsof theAmericanAcademyof Political and Social Science 463:129-40. Ferracuti,E, andE Bruno.1981. Psychiatricaspectsof terrorismin Italy.In Themad,the badand thedifferent: Essays in honor of Simon Dinitz, edited by I. L. Barak-Glantzand C. R. Huff. Lexington,MA: LexingtonBooks. Freud,S. 1953-1974. Thoughtsfor the times on waranddeath.In Standardeditionof the completepsychological worksof SigmundFreud,vol. 14, edited by J. Strachey.London:Hogarth. Fried,R. 1982. Thepsychologyof the terrorist.In Terrorismandbeyond:An internationalconferenceon terrorismand low-level conflict, edited by B. M. Jenkins.SantaMonica, CA: RAND. Friedland,N. 1992. Becoming a terrorist:Social and individualantecedents.In Terrorism:Roots, impact, responses,edited by L. Howard.New York:Praeger. Friedland,N., andA. Merari.1985. Thepsychologicalimpactof terrorism:A double-edgedsword.Political Psychology6:591-604. Gabbard,G. 0. 2000. Psychoanalysis.In Kaplanand Sadock'scomprehensivetextbookof psychiatry,7th ed., edited by B. J. Sadockand V. A. Sadock, 563-607. Philadelphia:LippincottWilliams& Wilkins. Gazzaniga,M. S., ed. 2000. Thenew cognitiveneurosciences.2nd ed. Cambridge,MA: MIT Press. Grossman,D. 1995. On killing:Thepsychological cost of learningto kill in war and society.Boston:Little, Brown. Gunaratna,G. 2002. InsideAl Qaeda: Global networkof terror.New York:ColumbiaUniversityPress. Gurr,T. 1970. Whymen rebel. Princeton,NJ: PrincetonUniversityPress. Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THE TERRORIST 39 Hacker,F. J. 1976. Crusaders,criminals,crazies: Terrorand terrorismin our time.New York:Norton. ---. 1983. Dialectic interrelationshipsof personaland political factors in terrorism.In Perspectives on terrorism,edited by L. Z. Freedmanand Y. Alexander, 19-31. Wilmington, DE: Scholarly Resources, Inc. Handler,J. S. 1990. Socioeconomicprofile of an Americanterrorist:1960s and 1970s. Terrorism13:195213. Harmon,C. C. 2000. Terrorismtoday.London:FrankCass. Haroun,A. M. 1999. Psychiatricaspects of terrorism.PsychiatricAnnals 29:335-6. Hasisi, B., andA. Pedahzur.2000. State,policy,andpoliticalviolence: Arabsin the Jewishstate. Civil Wars 3:64-84. Hassan,N. 2001. An arsenalof believers:Talkingto the "humanbombs."TheNew Yorker77:36. Heskin,K. 1984.Thepsychologyof terrorismin Ireland.In Terrorismin Ireland,editedby Y.Alexanderand A. O'Day. New York:St. Martin's. Hoffer,E. 1951. Thetruebeliever:Thoughtson thenatureof massmovements.New York:HarperPerennial. Hoffman,B. 1998. Inside terrorism.New York:ColumbiaUniversityPress. . 1999.Themindof theterrorist:Perspectivesfromsocialpsychology.PsychiatricAnnals29:337-40. Horgan,J. 2003. The searchforthe terroristpersonality.In Terrorists,victimsandsociety,editedby A. Silke, 3-27. Chichester,UK: Wiley. Hubbard,D. G. 1971. Theskyjacker:His flights offantasy. New York:Macmillan. Jiger, H., G. Schmidtchen,andL. Sillwold, eds. 1981. Analyzenzum Terrorismus2: Lebenslaufanalysen. Darmstadt,Germany:DeutscherVerlag. Jenkins,B. M. 1982. Statementsaboutterrorism.Annals of the AmericanAcademyof Political and Social Science 463:11-23. ----. 1983. Researchin terrorism:Areas of consensus,areasof ignorance.In Terrorism:Interdisciplinaryperspectives,editedby B. Eichelman,D. Soskis, andW. Reid, 153-77. Washington,DC: American PsychiatricAssociation. Juergensmeyer,M. 2000. Terrorin the mindof God. Berkeley:Universityof CaliforniaPress. Kandel, E., S. A. Mednick, L. Kirgegaard-Sorenson,B. Hutchings, J. Knop, R. Rosenberg, and F. Schulsinger.1988. IQ as a protectivefactorfor subjectsatriskfor antisocialbehavior.Journalof Consulting and Clinical Psychology56:224-6. Kellen,K. 1979. Terrorists-whatare theylike?How someterroristsdescribetheirworld andactions. Santa Monica, CA: RAND. Kelly, R. J., and R. W. Rieber. 1995. Psychosocial impacts of terrorismand organized crime: The counterfinalityof the practico-inert.Journalof Social Distress and the Homeless 4:265-86. Kepel, G. 2002. Jihad: Thetrail of political Islam. Cambridge,MA: Belknap. Knutson,B. 2004. Sweet revenge?Science 305:1246-7. Kohut,H. 1972. Thoughtson narcissismand narcissisticrage. PsychoanalyticStudyof the Child 27:360400. . 1978. Thesearchfor the self. New York:InternationalUniversitiesPress. Krueger,A. B., andJ. Maleckova.2002. Education,poverty,politicalviolence, andterrorism:Is therea connection?WorkingPaperNo. w9074, NationalBureauof EconomicResearch.Availableat http://papers. nber.org/papers/w9074 Laabs,D., andT.McDermott.2003. Preludeto 9/11:A hijacker'slove, lies. LosAngelesTimes,January27, 1. Laqueur,W. 1987. Theage of terrorism.Boston: Little, Brown. . 1999. Thenew terrorism:Fanaticismand the armsof mass destruction.New York:OxfordUniversity Press. AmericanEconomicReview78:22-6. Lee, D. R. 1988.Freeridingandpaidridingin thefightagainstterrorism. Levine,S. 1999.Youthsin terroristicgroups,gangsandcults:The allure,the animus,andthe alienation.PsychiatricAnnals 29:342-9. Lezak, M. D. 1995. Neuropsychologicalassessment.3rd ed. New York:OxfordUniversityPress. Lifton,R. J. 2000. Destroyingthe worldto save it: AumShinrikyoand the new global terrorism.New York: Holt. 40 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION Loo, C. M., J. A. Fairbank,R. M. Scurfield,L. O. Ruch,D. W.King,andL. J. Adams.2001. Measuringexposureto racism:Developmentandvalidationof a Race-RelatedStressorScale (RRSS) for Asian American Vietnamveterans.PsychologicalAssessment13:503-20. Machina,M. J., R. Hogarth,K. MacCrimmon,J. Roberts,A. Roth, P. Slovic, R. Thaler,and J. Ferejohn. 1989. The neuralbasis for altruisticpunishment.In Leading edges in social and behavioralscience, editedby R. D. Luce,N. J. Smelser,andD. R. Gerstein,237-59. New York:RussellSageFoundation. Mahmood,C. K. 2001. Terrorism,myth, and the power of ethnographicpraxis.Journalof Contemporary Ethnography30:520-45. Marighella,C. 1971. For the liberationof Brazil.London:Cox and Wyman. Marshall,T., and J. Danizewski. 2001. Pakistan'sMuslim schools offer a darkview of U.S. Los Angeles Times,September9, Al. Marton,K. 1996.A death in Jerusalem.New York:Arcade. Maya,M. L., L. E. Lander,andM. Ungar.2002. Economics,violence, andprotestin Venezuela:A previewof the global future?In Violenceand politics: Globalization'sparadox, edited by K. Worcester,S. A. Bermanzhon,and M. Ungar,184-208. New York:RoutledgeKeganPaul. McNeilly, M. D., N. B. Anderson,C. A. Armstead,R. Clark,M. Corbett,E. L. Robinson,C. F. Pieper,and E. M. Lepisto. 1996.PerceivedRacismScale:A multidimensionalassessmentof the experienceof white racismamong AfricanAmericans.Ethnicityand Disease 6:154-66. Meloy,J. R. 2001. Communicatedthreatsandviolence towardpublicandprivatetargets:Discerningdifferences among those who stalk and attack.Journalof ForensicSciences 46:1211-13. Merari,A. 1998.Thereadinessto kill anddie: Suicidalterrorismin the MiddleEast.In Originsof terrorism: Psychologies, ideologies, theologies, states of mind, edited by W. Reich, 192-207. Washington,DC: WoodrowWilson CenterPress. . 2002. Deterringfear: Governmentresponses to terroristattacks.HarvardInternationalReview 23:4. Merari,A., andN. Friedland.1985. Social psychologicalaspectsof politicalterrorism.AppliedSocial PsychologyAnnual6:185-205. Mesulam,M.-M. 2000. Behavioralneuroanatomy.In Principlesof behavioraland cognitiveneurology,2nd ed., edited by M.-M. Mesualm, 1-120. Oxford,UK: OxfordUniversityPress. Mickolus, E. 1980. International terrorism:Attributes of terrorist events 1968-1977 Westport, CT: Greenwood. Milburn,T. W., and K. H. Watman.1981. On the natureof threat:A social psychological analysis. New York:Praeger. Miller, W. H. 1993. Humiliationand other essays on honor,social discomfort,and violence. Ithaca,NY: CornellUniversityPress. Morf, G. 1970. Terrorin Quebec: Case studies of the FLQ.Toronto:ClarkeIrwin. Murry,V. M., P. A. Brown,G. H. Brody,C. E. Cutrona,andR. L. Simons. 2001. Racialdiscriminationas a moderatorof the linksamongstress,maternalpsychologicalfunctioning,andfamilyrelationships.Journal of Marriageand the Family63:915-26. Neto, F. 2001. Satisfactionwith life among adolescentsfrom immigrantfamilies in Portugal.Journal of Youthand Adolescence30:53-67. Olsson, P. A. 1988. The terroristand the terrorized:Some psychoanalyticconsideration.Journal of Psychohistory16:47-60. Pearce,K. I. 1977. Police negotiations.CanadianPsychiatricAssociationJournal22:171-4. Pearlstein,R. M. 1991. Themindof thepolitical terrorist.Wilmington,DE: SR Books. Pedahzur,A., A. Perliger,andL. Weinberg.2003. Altruismandfatalism:The characteristicsof Palestinian suicide terrorists.DeviantBehavior24:405-23. Pettit,G. S. 1997. The developmentalcourse of violence and aggression:Mechanismsof family and peer influence.PsychiatricClinics of NorthAmerica20:283-99. Ponton,L. E. 1997. The romanceof risk: Whyteenagersdo the things theydo. New York:Basic Books. Post, J. M. 1998. Terroristpsycho-logic:Terroristbehavioras a productof psychologicalforces. In Origins of terrorism:Psychologies, ideologies, theologies,states of mind,editedby W. Reich, 25-40. Washington, DC: WoodrowWilson CenterPress. Victoroff/ THEMIND OF THETERRORIST 41 . 2004. Leaders and theirfollowers in a dangerousworld: Thepsychology of political behavior. Ithaca,NY: CornellUniversityPress. Post,J. M., andS. N. Gold. 2002. Thepsychologyof theterrorist:An interviewwithJerroldM. Post.Journal of TraumaPractice 1:83-100. Post,J. M., E. Sprinzak,andL. M. Denny.2003. Theterroristsin theirown words:Interviewswith thirty-five incarceratedMiddle Easternterrorists.Terrorismand Political Violence15:171-84. Rapoport,A., andA. M. Chammah.1968.Prisoner'sdilemma.AnnArbor:Universityof MichiganPress. Rasch,W. 1979. Psychologicaldimensionsof politicalterrorismin the FederalRepublicof Germany.InternationalJournalof Law and Psychiatry2:79-85. Rees, M., M. August, G. Baghdadi,J. Hamad,A. Klein, S. MacLeod,and N. Mustafa.2002. Why suicide bombingis now all the rage. Time,April 15, 33-9. Reich, W. 1998. Understandingterroristbehavior:The limits andopportunitiesof psychologicalinquiry.In Originsof terrorism:Psychologies, ideologies, theologies,states of mind,editedby W. Reich, 261-79. Washington,DC: WoodrowWilson CenterPress. Resnick,P.J., andS. Noffsinger.2004. Competencyto standtrialandthe insanitydefense.In TheAmerican PsychiatricPublishingtextbookofforensicpsychiatry,editedby R. I. Simon andL. H. Gold. Washington, DC: AmericanPsychiatricPublishing. Robins,R. S., andJ. M. Post. 1997. Politicalparanoia: Thepsychopoliticsof hatred.New Haven,CT:Yale UniversityPress. Rosenberg,D., L. Waddell,and S. Smalley. 2002. A troubledteenager'stragic flight plan. Newsweek, January21. Rotella,S. 2004. Social bondspullMuslimyouthtojihad,expertsays.LosAngelesTimes,October3, A3. Russell, C. A., and B. H. Miller. 1983. Profile of a terrorist.In Perspectiveson terrorism,edited by L. Z. Freedmanand Y. Alexander,45-60. Wilmington,DE: ScholarlyResources,Inc. Sageman,M. 2004. Understandingterrornetworks.Philadelphia:Universityof PennsylvaniaPress. Sandler,T., and D. G. Arce. 2003. Terrorismand game theory.Simulation& Gaming34:319-37. Sandler,T., andW.Enders.2004. Transnationalterrorism:An economicanalysis.Unpublishedmanuscript, School of InternationalRelations,Universityof SouthernCalifornia,Los Angeles. Sandler,T., and H. E. Lapan. 1988. The calculus of dissent: An analysis of terrorists'choice of targets. Synthese76:245-61. Sandler,T., J. T. Tschirhart,and J. Cauley. 1983. A theoreticalanalysis of transnationalterrorism.American Political Science Review77:36-54. Saper,B. 1988. On learningterrorism.Terrorism11:13-27. Satterfield,J. M. 1998. Cognitive-affectivestatespredictmilitaryandpoliticalaggressionandrisktaking:A contentanalysisof Churchill,Hitler,Roosevelt, and Stalin.Journalof ConflictResolution42:667-90. Schbley,A. H. 2000. TornbetweenGod,family,andmoney:Thechangingprofileof Lebanon'sreligiousterrorists.Studies in Conflict& Terrorism23:175-96. Schlesinger,L. B. 2000. Familicide,depressionand catathymicprocess. Journalof ForensicSciences 45: 200-3. Schmid,A. 1983. Political terrorism:A researchguide to the concepts,theories,databasesand literature. Witha bibliographyby the authorand a worlddirectoryof "terrorist"organizationsbyA. J. Jongman. Amsterdam:NorthHolland. Schultz, R. 1980. Conceptualizingpolitical terrorism-a typology. In Internationalterrorism:Current researchandfuture directions,edited by A. D. Buckley and D. D. Olson, 9-15. Wayne,NJ: Avery. Shaw, E. D. 1986. Political terrorists:Dangersof diagnosis and an alternativeto the psychopathological model. InternationalJournalof Law and Psychiatry8:359-68. Sidanius,J. 1985.Cognitivefunctioningandsociopoliticalideologyrevisited.PoliticalPsychology6:637-61. Sidanius,J., andE Pratto.1999.Social dominance:An intergrouptheoryof social hierarchyandoppression. Cambridge,UK: CambridgeUniversityPress. Silke, A. P. 1998.Cheshire-catlogic: Therecurringthemeof terroristabnormalityin psychologicalresearch. Psychology,Crimeand Law 4:51-69. ---. 2003. Becominga terrorist.In Terrorists,victimsand society,editedby A. Silke, 29-53. Chichester, UK: Wiley. - 42 JOURNALOF CONFLICTRESOLUTION Soskis, D. 1983. Law enforcementandpsychiatry:Forgingthe workingalliance.In Terrorism:Interdisciplinaryperspectives,editedby B. Eichelman,D. Soskis, andW.Reid, 129-45.Washington,DC: American PsychiatricAssociation. Stern,J. 1999. The ultimateterrorists.Cambridge,MA: HarvardUniversityPress. 2003. Terrorin the name of God: Whyreligiousmilitantskill. New York:Ecco. ----. Strentz,T. 1988.A terroristpsychosocialprofile:Pastandpresent.FBILawEnforcementBulletin57:13-9. Taylor,M. 1988. Theterrorist.London:Brassey's. Taylor,M., and E. Quayle. 1994. Terroristlives. London:Brassey's. Taylor,M., and H. Ryan. 1988. Fanaticism,political suicide and terrorism.Terrorism11:91-111. U.S. Departmentof HomelandSecurity.2004. HomelandSecurityCenters(HS Centers)ProgramBroad Agencyannouncement:HomelandSecurityCenterfor Behavioraland Social Aspectsof Terrorismand Counter-terrorism. Washington,DC: UniversityPrograms,Office of Researchand Development,Science and Technology. Utsey, S. 0., and J. G. Ponterotto.1996. Developmentand validationof the Index of Race-RelatedStress (IRRS).Journalof CounselingPsychology43:490-501. Volkan,V.D. 1997.Blood lines: Fromethnicpride to ethnicterrorism.New York:Farrar,Straus,& Giroux. von Neumann,J., and 0. Morgenstern.1947. Theoryof games and economicbehavior.2nd ed. Princeton, NJ: PrincetonUniversityPress. Wallerstein,R. S. 1995. Thetalkingcures: Thepsychoanalysesandpsychotherapies.New Haven,CT:Yale UniversityPress. Wardlaw,G. 1989. Political terrorism,theory,tactics, and countermeasures.Cambridge,UK: Cambridge UniversityPress. Weinberg,L., and W. L. Eubank. 1987. Italian women terrorists.Terrorism:An InternationalJournal 9:241-62. -. 1994. Culturaldifferencesin the behaviorof terrorists.Terrorismand Political Violence6:1-28. Whitaker,B. 1972. Thefourth world: Victimsof groupoppression:Eight reportsfrom thefield workof the minorityrightsgroup.New York:Schocken. Whittaker,D. J. 2001. The terrorismreader.London:RoutledgeKeganPaul. Wieviorka,M. 1993. Themakingof terrorism.Translatedby D. G. White. Chicago:Universityof Chicago Press. ---. 1995. Terrorismin the context of academic research. In Terrorismin context, edited by M. Crenshaw,597-606. UniversityPark:PennsylvaniaState UniversityPress. ---. 2004. Preface.In Themakingof terrorism.Translatedby D. G. White.Reprint,Chicago:University of Chicago Press. Los Angeles Times,December31, 1. Wilkinson,T. 2002. End of the line for a "martyr." Wilson, M. A. 2000. Towarda model of terroristbehaviorin hostage-takingincidents.Journalof Conflict Resolution44:403-24. Zamoyski, A. 1999. Holy madness: Romantics,patriots, and revolutionaries,1776-1871. New York: Viking. Zuckerman,M. 2002. Genetics of sensation seeking. In Molecular genetics and the humanpersonality, edited by J. Benjamin,R. P. Ebstein, and R. H. Belmaker.Washington,DC: AmericanPsychiatric Publishing. Zulaika,J., and W. A. Douglass. 1996. Terrorand taboo: Thefollies, fables, andfaces of terrorism.New York:RoutledgeKeganPaul.