Adam M. Leventhal*, Lynn P. Rehm

Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25 – 44
The empirical status of melancholia: Implications for psychology
Adam M. Leventhal*, Lynn P. Rehm
Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204-5022, USA
Received 19 April 2004; received in revised form 27 July 2004; accepted 17 September 2004
Abstract
The concept of a subtype of depression with a biological rather than a psychological set of causes has been more
prominent in the psychiatric literature than in the psychological literature on depression. There has been dispute as
to whether research on melancholia supports the distinction of a separate subtype with a distinct symptomatic
profile characterized by marked anhedonia, psychomotor difficulties, excessive guilt or hopelessness, suicidal
features, and appetite and weight disturbances. Research suggests that individuals with melancholic depression are
qualitatively different from those with non-melancholic depression in their symptomatology. Examination of
biological functioning, personality traits, responsiveness to treatment, and suicidality also tend to support the
melancholic–non-melancholic distinction. This paper reviews the status of the melancholia concept and explores
its implications for psychological research and practice.
D 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Melancholia; Psychology; Depression; Endogenous depression
1. Introduction
Depression has been conceptualized as a heterogeneous mixture of conditions and disorders, but
finding meaningful groupings has been a difficult endeavor. The idea of somatogenesis, manifest disease
arising from internal biological dysfunction, is at least as old as Hippocrates’ writings in the 5th century
BC. Melancholia preceded the term depression to describe human despondency. The word is derived
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 7137438600; fax: +1 7137438588.
E-mail address: aleventhal@uh.edu (A.M. Leventhal).
0272-7358/$ - see front matter D 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2004.09.001
26
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
from the Greek term bmelaina choleQ or bblack bile,Q the bodily humor that was identified with black
moods. In recent history, the idea of a somatic genesis for depression has been contrasted with a
psychological genesis, and the terms bendogenousQ versus bexogenousQ or breactiveQ have been used to
contrast these two sources of depression.
Establishing subgroups of patients based on putative etiology has long been a goal of diagnostic
classification. Those with endogenous symptoms should lack precipitating events, although a number
of investigators have failed to find group differences in the incidence of stressful life events between
individuals with endogenous and non-endogenous symptomatic profiles (e.g., Forrest, Frazer, & Priest,
1965; Lafer, Nierenberg, Rosenbaum, & Fava, 1996; Leff, Roatch, & Bunney, 1970; Thomson &
Hendrie, 1972). In fact, Mundt, Reck, and Backenstrass (2000) demonstrated that stressful experiences
often precede onset of melancholic episodes. Findings from other investigations indicate that
depressed individuals with melancholic symptomatology may undergo more adverse experiences than
non-melancholic individuals (Harkness & Monroe, 2002; Willner, Wilkes, & Orwin, 1990). Two
studies have provided support for the idea of fewer precipitating events in depressive episodes with
endogenous symptoms (e.g., Kohn, Zislin, & Agid, 2001; Tomaszewska, Peeslow, Barouche, & Fieve,
1996). Overall, the inconsistencies across investigations demonstrate the difficulty in establishing
groupings based on supposed etiology.
2. The constructs of endogeneity and melancholia
The term endogenous has been used to identify a syndrome of depression identified by symptoms of
pervasive anhedonia, anorexia, weight loss, early morning awakening, diurnal variation of mood,
psychomotor agitation or retardation, guilt, and sometimes psychotic features (Coplov et al., 1986). The
grouping of a separate melancholic subtype of depression was introduced in the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, third edition (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association,
1980). Klein (1974) summarized the rationale for this distinction in patients who show a regular
impairment in the capacity to experience pleasure or to respond effectively to the anticipation of
pleasure. Klein described bneurotic depression,Q as a bchronic emotional or personality disorder related
to low self-esteem, overly severe disappointment reactions, feelings of helplessness, reliance on external
sources of self-esteem, and an irritable, grasping, angry, unhappy, other-blaming, and histrionic attitudeQ
(Klein, 1974, p. 448). He contrasted this with melancholic depression, typified by a loss of interest or
pleasure, anorexia, suicidal preoccupation, fearful perplexity, retardation, or agitation. Klein
characterized melancholia as bendogenomorphicQ depression, that is, having the form or appearance
of endogenous depression. Klein believed that the key feature of true endogenous depression is
persistent and severe anhedonia, which he hypothesized results from inhibition of the brain’s bpleasure
mechanismQ. In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV;
American Psychiatric Association, 1994), melancholia is a specifier for a Major Depressive Episode in
either unipolar or bipolar depression. It requires (A) during the most severe period of the episode that the
patient shows either (1) loss of pleasure in all, or almost all, activities; or (2) lack of reactivity to usually
pleasurable stimuli; and (B) three or more of the following: (1) distinct quality of depressed mood; (2)
depression regularly worse in the morning; (3) early morning awakening; (4) marked psychomotor
retardation or agitation; (5) significant anorexia or weight loss; and (6) excessive or inappropriate guilt.
Loss of interest or lack of reactivity is stressed as the essential feature.
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
27
Evidence supports the existence of qualitatively distinct dimensions of depression, one of which is
identifiable as melancholia that is highly associated with anhedonia. Factor analytic studies have
generally confirmed a melancholic dimension of depression (Biro & Till, 1989; Kiloh & Garside, 1963;
Marcos & Salamero, 1990). Kiloh and Garside (1963) examined personality traits and present depressive
symptoms and reported a two-factor solution with the second factor interpreted as a bipolar melancholic
versus non-melancholic dimension. They listed melancholic clinical features as: early awakening,
depression worse in the morning, distinct quality of depression, retardation, episode duration of 1 year or
less, age 40 or above, depth of depression, failure of concentration, and weight changes. Another
psychometric analysis of 200 diagnostically heterogeneous depressed patients (Biro & Till, 1989) led to
a four-factor solution interpreted to represent dimensions of depression subtypes: melancholic,
psychotic, anxious, and retarded depression. The melancholic factor was weighted with anhedonia,
loss of interest, retardation, reduced efficacy, guilty feelings, diurnal variation, insomnia, and
hopelessness. Marcos and Salamero (1990) reported a three-factor solution and identified the primary
factor as one of melancholia. Clark and Watson (1991) conducted an extensive review of the depression
assessment literature spanning many self-report and clinician ratings of mood, symptom, and syndrome
measures. Their findings confirmed two independent factors in depression: anhedonia and general
distress.
While factor-analytic methods can identify separate dimensions, they cannot detect whether
underlying structure of psychological disorders is taxonic (categorical) or dimensional (continuous).
Examinations using taxometric analysis, a set of statistical techniques able to assess latent structure,
have provided support for a qualitatively discrete melancholic subtype (Ambrosini, Bennett, Cleland,
& Haslam, 2002; Beach & Amir, 2003; Grove et al., 1987; Haslam & Beck, 1994). An analysis of
531 patients with a primary major depressive diagnosis demonstrated that sleep disturbance and loss
of satisfaction, appetite, weight, and libido were indicators of a unique melancholic taxon (Haslam &
Beck, 1994). These results parallel those found by Beach and Amir (2003) in four samples of 984
undergraduates, who demonstrated that in addition to symptoms identified in the Haslam and Beck
(1994) study, preoccupation with health and social anhedonia were good indicators of a categorically
discrete melancholic syndrome. Melancholic features tended to be part of a structurally distinct entity,
whereas cognitive and affective depressive symptoms were more consistent with a dimensional
structure (Beach & Amir, 2003). These findings suggest that melancholia is a constellation of clinical
features and not solely a marker of those who are most severely and chronically depressed. In fact, at
least 50% of the depressed people who score in the upper half of severity range do not have
melancholia (Thase, Hersen, Bellack, Himmelhock, & Kupfer, 1983). Benazzi (2000) demonstrated
no differences in chronicity between melancholic and non-melancholic forms of bipolar II depression.
Taken together, these findings suggest that melancholia may be a naturally occurring phenotype,
qualitatively distinguishable from non-melancholic depression, and not solely an artifact of a
symptom-defined system of disease classification.
Based on a literature review and an empirical examination of 152 inpatients with unipolar major
depression, Zimmerman, Coryell, Pfohl, and Stangl (1986) suggested additional features of the
melancholic construct such as, a greater family history of depression, a lower family history of alcoholism,
a lower family history of antisocial personality, older age, higher scores on symptom severity indexes, less
frequent non-serious suicide attempts, lower prevalence of divorce or marital separations, fewer stressful
life events, lower probability of premorbid personality disorder, better social support, less cognitive
distortion, a higher frequency of neuroendocrine or other biologic abnormalities, a better response to
28
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
somatic therapies, and a poorer response to psychotherapy. Examination of the biological and
psychological factors in melancholia has supported the association with some of these features.
3. Biological factors in melancholia
When it was introduced, the dexamethasone suppression test (DST; Carroll, Martin, & Davies, 1968)
was offered as a specific laboratory test and biological marker for the presence of melancholia in
depression. In normals, dexamethasone suppressed cortisol whereas a non-suppression response,
indicative of a pituitary–adrenal hypothalamic dysfunction, was associated with melancholic depression
(Carroll et al., 1981). Although these findings were only correlational and could not demonstrate a causal
relationship, DST results supported the hypothesis that the key component of melancholic depression is
a dysfunction of physiological mechanisms that regulate emotion. Most investigations have found that
35–60% of individuals with melancholia show an abnormal DST response (Giles & Rush, 1982;
Mitchell et al., 1996; Rush et al., 1997).
Other investigators, however, have found no relation between a diagnosis of melancholic depression
and non-suppression DST results (Brown & Shuey, 1980; Mendlewicz, Charles, & Franckson, 1983). A
study evaluating 231 psychiatric inpatients found that DST responses were related to intervening
variables such as stress due to hospital admission, drug withdrawal, suicidal turmoil, and weight loss
(Berger, Pirke, Doerr, Krieg, & von Zerssen, 1984). These variables were found to enhance the rate of
abnormal DST results regardless of diagnostic classification. Klein and Berger (1987) argued that the
DST does not reliably indicate hypercortisolism, a reflection of pituitary–adrenal hypothalamic
dysfunction. Whether or not failure to suppress plasma cortisol is a reliable correlate of melancholia, the
DST may to lack sufficient sensitivity or specificity for an accurate diagnosis of melancholia.
Besides dexamethasone suppression response, other neuroendocrine variables have shown some
ability discriminate melancholic and non-melancholic forms of depression. Thyroid axis dysfunction
may be related to psychomotor abnormalities, weight loss, and sleep disturbances in melancholia (Thase
& Howland, 1995). Basal levels of thyroid stimulating hormone (TSH) and response to thyrotropinreleasing hormone, indices of thyroid axis functioning, have been used as biological measures of
depression (Thase & Howland, 1995). Türlçapar et al. (1999) demonstrated that DSM-IV-diagnosed
melancholic patients have higher cortisol levels and lower levels TSH of than non-melancholic
individuals with depression. In concordance with these findings, Rush et al. (1997) found that
melancholic patients were more likely to have a blunted thyrotropin-releasing hormone stimulation test
(TRH-ST) results and DST non-suppression than non-melancholic depressed patients. Other studies
assessing depression through neuroendocrine measures have indicated that the presence of weight loss
and depressive psychomotor abnormalities may better account for abnormal TSH levels, blunted DST
response, (Staner, Maes, Bouillon, & Linkowski, 1992) and lower melatonin levels (Fontoulakis et al.,
2001) than the constellation of symptoms required for research diagnostic criteria (RDC) and DSM-IV
melancholia diagnoses.
A separate area of research indicates that sleep patterns differ in depressed individuals with and without
melancholia (Kupfer, Foster, Coble, McPartland, & Ulrich, 1978; Rush et al., 1997; Staner et al., 1992;
Simons & Thase, 1992). Individuals with melancholic depression display a reduced REM latency (Rush
et al., 1997) and a higher REM frequency (Coble, Foster, and Kupfer, 1976) than those with nonmelancholic depression. REM latency has demonstrated the greatest sensitivity but lowest specificity in
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
29
the differentiation of melancholic from non-melancholic depression as compared to the DST and the
TRH-ST (Rush et al., 1997). Temporal mood patterns and sleep-related symptoms suggest that circadian
rhythms are shifted in people with melancholia. Vogel, Thurmond, Gibbons, Sloan, Boyd, and Walker
(1975) found that melancholic patients whose circadian rhythms were altered by REM sleep deprivation
improved more than patients with unaffected circadian rhythms deprived of only non-REM sleep. The
same circadian manipulation did not significantly alter symptoms in non-melancholic depressed patients.
In another paper, Vogel, Vogel, McAbee, and Thrumond (1980) proposed that depressive clinical
characteristics represent a damaged sleep cycle boscillatorQ, which results in circadian rhythm
disturbances. They suggested that REM sleep deprivation improved depressive symptoms in melancholia
because it stimulated the oscillator and corrected the circadian rhythm disruption. Other laboratories have
replicated findings that REM sleep deprivation improves depressive symptoms, (Elsenga & van den
Hoofdakker, 1983, 1987; Reynolds et al., 1987). Newly developed software for the real time detection of
REM sleep in conjunction with computer-operated awakening procedures is making this phenomenon
easier to assess and study (Groezinger, Koegel, & Roeschke, 2002).
Studies demonstrating reduced REM latency and the efficacy of sleep deprivation in melancholic
individuals suggest that disrupted circadian rhythms may be a significant feature of melancholic
depression. An interesting corollary to the sleep research findings is found in the time experience of patients
with melancholic depression (Kuhs, Herman, & Tölle 1991; Raamsayer, 1990; Tysk, 1984). When
melancholically depressed inpatients are asked to approximate a 30-s interval, they tend to underestimate
by about 6 s, suggesting that time subjectively passes slowly for them (Kuhs et al., 1991). Misjudgment in
time estimation tasks has been interpreted as evidence of altered binternal clockQ functioning in melancholia
(Tysk, 1984). Raamsayer (1990) investigated temporal discrimination tasks in melancholic patients and
also found results indicative of dysfunctional internal clock regulation. Animal research provides evidence
suggesting that the rate of internal clock functioning depends on the effective level of dopaminergic activity
(Maricq & Church, 1983; Meck, 1983). Thus, internal clock dysregulation in melancholia may be a
consequence of abnormal functioning of dopaminergic systems, a phenomenon which has been
demonstrated in other studies that report dysfunction in the brain’s reward system (BRS) in depression
(Naranjo, Tremblay, & Busto, 2001; Willner, 1997). Future research using neuropharmacological
techniques may clarify the extent of any alterations in dopaminergic functioning present in melancholia
(e.g., Rampello, Nicoletti, & Raffaele, 1991; Thase, Mallinger, McKnight, & Himmelhoch, 1992).
A common finding is that rates of melancholia increase with age leading some to propose that
biological correlates of aging may play a role in producing melancholic symptoms (Thase & Friedman,
1999). For example, levels of serotonin or dopamine, neurotransmitters associated with the
pathophysiology of depression, tend to be inversely related with age (Thase & Howland, 1995). In
addition, inhibitory mechanisms that reduce the effects of stress on brain functioning may decline with
age (Thase & Howland, 1995). Another set of explanations for these findings involves the notion that
various forms of reinforcement may be unattainable or lost in older age due to reduced physical health,
inability to travel, and death of friends and loved ones. Reductions in positive reinforcement may cause a
decline in hedonic tone, which may eventually develop into a more pervasive form of anhedonia.
Whether or not anhedonia is present, it is common for elderly patients with depression to report somatic
symptoms, such as diminished energy, strained effort, and sleep disturbance (Gottfries, 1998; Norris,
Snow-Turek, & Blankenship, 1995). Because sexual activity and appetite changes may not be related to
depression in the elderly (Norris et al., 1995), melancholia assessment of this population should focus
more on anhedonic symptoms and daily variations in mood.
30
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
Animal research has been used to examine models of the etiology of melancholic symptoms and their
neurobiological correlates. Animal models of depression use highly controlled techniques such as
repeated psychostimulant exposures and chronic stress to induce anhedonia and other melancholic
symptoms that can be reversed with antidepressant medication (Leith & Barret, 1975; Phillips & Barr,
1997; Willner, 1997). These methods produce anhedonia by dysregulating the BRS. The BRS is a
complex neural network with active sites along the medial forebrain bundle, particularly in the lateral
hypothalamic, posterior hypothalamic, and ventral tegmental levels (Wise, 1988). The BRS is stimulated
during reward behavior and has been shown to drive hedonic processes through activation of the
mesocorticolimbic dopamine system (Naranjo et al., 2001). Only recently have investigators begun to
examine the BRS in humans with depression. Naranjo et al. (2001) measured the response of subjects
with major depressive disorder (MDD) to d-amphetamine, a pharmacological agent known to activate
the BRS and its dopaminergic network. They found that the MDD subjects reported increases in
subjective reward differently than non-depressed controls. This was interpreted as evidence for BRS
dysfunction in depression. Measures of biological variables, such as DST and TSH response, are thought
to assess the brain’s stress response. BRS alterations may assess the physiological source of anhedonia in
melancholia directly, although current evidence supporting this notion is primarily correlational (e.g.,
Naranjo et al., 2001).
Recently, investigators have utilized neuroimagining techniques to examine regional brain structure
and function in emotionally disordered individuals (see Davidson, 2000; Davidson & Irwin, 1999;
Davidson, Pizzagalli, Nitschke, and Putnum, 2002 for reviews). Davidson et al. (2002) found that
individuals with depression demonstrate structural and functional abnormalities in the prefrontal cortex,
anterior cingulated cortex, hippocampus, amygdala, and other neural sites. A few studies have examined
impact of melancholic depressive symptomatology on brain function (Bench et al., 1992; Galynker et al.,
1998; Pizzagalli et al., 2002). Psychomotor retardation, anhedonia, and flat affect have been associated
with decreased left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity (Bench et al., 1992; Galynker et al., 1998),
whereas comorbid anxiety has been related to more right- than left-sided neural activity (Bruder et al.,
1997). Pizzagalli et al. (2002) examined imaging correlates of depressive symptomatology in
melancholic and non-melancholic depressed individuals. One of the more interesting findings in this
study was that state measures of anxiety demonstrated a more robust correlation with right frontal lobe
activity in melancholic subjects than in non-melancholic subjects. This is consistent with evidence that
melancholia involves more extensive physiological dysregulation than non-melancholic depression.
Overall, there appears to be some consistent biological factors associated with the presence of
melancholia in depression (e.g., REM latency). While these correlates add to our understanding of
melancholia, many studies that have examined biological factors were cross-sectional which precludes
certain interpretations of the temporal ordering and directional causation of biological dysfunction and
melancholic symptoms. Moreover, none of these factors have developed into definitive biological
markers or practical assessment devices. Consequently, assessment of the melancholia construct remains
at the level of evaluating current behavioral symptomology.
4. Psychological factors in melancholia
A number of assessment techniques have been used to determine whether an individual meets criteria
for melancholic depression. The standard for research is to conduct a semistructured interview, such as
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
31
the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995) to obtain both a
diagnosis of depression and the specifier bwith melancholic features.Q While DSM-IV criteria are the
most frequently used, expanded criteria were proposed in the Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC;
Spitzer, Endicott, & Robins, 1978). RDC lists 10 depressive symptoms to diagnose melancholic
depression very similar to DSM criteria but lacking items on poor reactivity to usually pleasurable
events. Symptoms on the RDC are rated as either present or absent. RDC classification separates
depressed individuals into bdefinite endogenousQ, bprobable or uncertain endogenousQ, and bnonendogenous.Q The RDC category of bprobable endogenousQ is more inclusive than the DSM-IV specifier
bwith melancholic features.Q
In addition to RDC and DSM-IV specifier criteria, empirical scales have also been developed for
identifying items that differentiate melancholic from non-melancholic patients. The Newcastle Index
(Carney, Roth, & Garside, 1965; Gurney, 1971) is a 10-item scale based on multiple regression analyses
of 35 items rated on 116 depressed patients who seemed to fit the melancholic category. The Feinberg–
Carroll Discriminate Index (Feinberg & Carroll, 1982) sums ratings on eight clinical items, each
multiplied by a given weight that allows for a distinction between definite melancholic, probable or
uncertain melancholic, and nonmelancholic. This index has high agreement with clinical diagnoses and
biological correlates of melancholia (Staner et al., 1992) and may be useful to clinicians who wish to
confirm a diagnostic impression of melancholia with a formal assessment procedure.
Melancholia can also be measured as a continuous variable, so that severity can be assessed and
tracked during a course of treatment. The most commonly used clinician rating scale for assessing
severity of depression, the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (Hamilton, 1960), has been adapted to
assess melancholia. Of the 17-item version of the Hamilton, a subset of questions that make up a
Hamilton Endogenomorphy Scale that has been used to asses melancholia in research studies (e.g.,
Thase et al., 1983). The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology-Clinical Ratings (IDS-C; Rush,
Guillon, Basco, Jarret, & Trivedi, 1996) includes items that assess all melancholic and non-melancholic
depressive symptoms listed in the DSM-IV. There is also a Quick-IDS-C (QIDS-C; Rush et al., 2003)
that only takes 5–7 min to complete. However, the QIDS does not measure all melancholic or atypical
features. The Bech–Rafaelsen Melancholia Scale (Bech & Rafaelsen, 1980) is an 11-item clinician rating
measure that lacks items that assess psychomotor agitation or diurnal variation of mood, both of which
appear to be important symptoms associated with melancholia. Overall, clinician ratings that gauge the
severity of melancholia may be useful, especially when used in conjunction with comprehensive
diagnostic tools such as the DSM-IV.
The most frequently used self-report measures of depression give only partial information about
melancholic symptomatology. The BDI (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) is the most
commonly used self-report index of depression. Items tapping increased appetite, weight gain, increased
sleep, psychomotor agitation, psychomotor retardation, problems with concentration, and nonsuicidal
death wishes were not included in the first edition (Zimmerman & Coryell, 1987); however, a factoranalytically derived subscale of the BDI that correlates highly with somatic and vegetative symptoms
reliably distinguished between melancholic and non-melancholic forms of depression (Schotte, Maes,
Cluydts, De Doncker, & Cosyns, 1997). The second edition of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II;
Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) offered questions on atypical symptoms, concentration difficulties, and
psychomotor problems, but still lacks melancholic items on daily mood variation and whether mood
improves after positive events. Similarly, other commonly used self-report measures, the Zung
Depression Inventory (Zung, 1965) and the Center for Epidemiological Studies—Depression Scale
32
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
(Radloff, 1977) do not contain items measuring guilt, diurnal variation of mood, early morning
awakening, and responsiveness of mood. These self-report measures can not substitute for a specific
measure of melancholia, but may be used to screen out non-melancholic individuals who endorse few or
no items assessing anhedonia or somatic and vegetative features. They are also useful for tracking
overall treatment progress.
One self-report scale, The Inventory to Diagnose Depression (IDD; Zimmerman and Coryell, 1987)
was developed to diagnose depression and melancholia according to DSM-III-R criteria. The benefits of
the IDD are its user-friendly language, assessment of subthreshold MDD symptoms (i.e., bless than or
more than two weeksQ), and the five-level severity rating of each symptom. The Inventory of Depressive
Symptomatology-Self Report (IDS-SR; Rush et al., 1996) is much like the IDD but is updated to include
items that assess all melancholic and non-melancholic depressive symptoms listed in the DSM-IV. The
IDS-SR includes a shortened versions, the Quick-IDS-SR (QIDS-SR; Rush et al., 2003), but like the
QIDS-C, the QIDS-SR also does not assess all melancholic symptoms. The IDS-SR uses DSM-IV
wording in its descriptions of symptoms (e.g., bIs your mood variation attributed to the environmentQ and
bMy mood is sad, but this sadness is different from the type of sadness associated with grief or lossQ).
Thus, the language may not be easily understood by respondents. Nonetheless, both the IDS-SR and the
IDD are adequate means of assessing melancholic symptomatology. The Levine–Pilowsky Depression
Questionnaire (LPDQ; Pilowsky, Levine, & Boulton, 1969) is a 57-item test with questions to assess
endogenous and non-endogenous depression. It contains 28 questions that are usually answered yes by
individuals with endogenous depression. However, the LPDQ was developed before DSM-III consensus
on melancholic symptoms, and it omits some consensus criteria and adds some idiosyncratic items.
Anhedonia is traditionally conceptualized as the core symptom of melancholia (Klein, 1974). It may
be pervasive or confined to a specific aspect of experience, such a food and drink palatability, sexual
activity, social interaction, sensory stimulation, or oneTs usual interests and pastimes (Snaith, 1993).
Presence and severity of anhedonia is typically measured in most depression assessment tools through
one or two items. To obtain a more accurate quantitative measure of anhedonia, practitioners and
researchers can use one of three self-report questionnaires: Chapman Physical and Social Anhedonia
Scale (CAS; Chapman, Chapman, & Raulin, 1976), Fawcett–Clark Pleasure Capacity Scale (FCPS;
Fawcett, Clark, & Scheftner, 1983), and Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS; Snaith, Hamilton &
Morely, 1995). All have been shown to correlate with measures of melancholia (Fawcett et al, 1983;
Loas & Boyer, 1996; Snaith, Hamilton, & Morely, 1995).
The CAS asks about past experiences and trait-like anhedonic characteristics (i.e., bThe taste of food
has always been important to meQ). The CAS allows users to assess both social and physical anhedonia
in separate subscales of 40 and 48 items. The SHAPS and FCPS ask about hypothetical responses to
pleasurable stimuli and assess anhedonia as a state, while the CAS examines anhedonia as trait-like. The
SHAPS uses 14 items to measure for differences in hedonic response to social contact, appetite for food,
hobbies and pastimes, and sensory experiences with one or a few items on each category. D’haenen
(1996) pointed out that paper and pencil anhedonia measures are subject to bias according to age, gender,
social, and ethnic group. Nonetheless, using anhedonia measures in conjunction with traditional
melancholia assessment tools such as the Newcastle Index, DSM-IV, or RDC can provide additional
information on the pervasiveness and severity of this symptom. Therapists may potentially plan more
successful behavioral activity schedules by utilizing information gained from anhedonia assessment.
Using these symptoms specific measures may be useful in learning more about the helpfulness of
specific interventions.
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
33
Personality traits have also been associated with melancholic depression, although, due to
methodological limitations, it is unclear whether such traits may be causes or effects of the disorder.
A number of studies report that bautonomousQ personality traits occur frequently in individuals with
melancholia, and bsociotropicQ traits are common in non-melancholically depressed people (Beck, 1983;
Robins & Luten, 1991). Autonomous individuals are sensitive to achievement losses. When they
experience defeat, their clinical features are more likely melancholic. Sociotropic individuals are
sensitive to interpersonal loss and present clinical characteristics related with reactive depression. The
relation of these personality traits to depression type may assist researchers and clinicians in
understanding etiologies of depression from a stress–diathesis point of view. Other studies investigating
personality traits report that patients with melancholic depression have less vulnerable personality styles
and are less neurotic and more obsessive than individuals with non-melancholic depression
(Benjaminsen, 1981; Boyce, Parker, & Hickie, 1990; Charney, Nelson, & Quinlan, 1981). However,
because these findings are primarily correlational, it is difficult to determine whether certain personality
types are risk factors for melancholia.
Examinations of life events suggest that stressful experiences often precede the onset of melancholia
(Forrest et al., 1965; Leff et al., 1970; Thomson & Hendrie, 1972). Reports of significant increases in
stressful life events during the pre-episode year (up to 12 months before the onset of the depressive
episode) in people with depression have been found whether or not melancholic symptomatology is
present (Brown, Harris, & Hepworth, 1994; Cornell, Milden, & Shimp, 1985). Although some studies
have found that individuals with melancholia report fewer stressful life events than those with nonmelancholic depression (e.g., Kohn et al., 2001; Tomaszewska et al., 1996), periods of distress have been
shown to predict episodes of melancholia as well (Mundt et al., 2000). Whether or not incidence of
distressing events can differentiate between melancholic versus non-melancholic depressions, it should
be noted that individuals who display melancholic symptomatology may have undergone prior stressful
experiences (Mundt et al., 2000).
Recently, research has examined stress in childhood and its effect on adult depression (e.g., Bifulco,
Brown, Moran, Ball, & Cambell, 1998; Harkness & Monroe, 2002). Harkness and Monroe (2002)
examined a group of depressed 76 women, 31 of whom met RDC for melancholia, and found that
women with melancholia experienced more severe early childhood adversity (i.e., severe physical abuse,
sexual abuse, antipathy, and neglect) than non-melancholic women. Other studies have demonstrated
that less severe forms of childhood adversity, such as a dysfunctional parental marriage, dysfunctional
parenting, and rejection, were more associated with non-melancholic depression (Parker, Gladstone, et
al, 1997; Parker, Kiloh, & Hayword, 1987; Parker, Roussos, et al, 1997). Harkness and Monroe (2002)
hypothesized that severe, prolonged, and uncontrollable stress may cause neuroadaptations that mediate
the development of melancholia in adulthood. As a general rule, clinicians applying psychological
interventions should be cognizant of potential psychosocial precipitants that can occur before the onset
of melancholic depressive episodes.
5. Suicide in melancholic patients
Melancholic patients may have a heightened risk for suicide. Van Praag and Plutchik (1984) studied
suicide attempts, depression type, and depression severity in hospital patients with non-psychotic
depression. Their data showed that violent suicide attempts occurred with greatest frequency in
34
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
individuals with melancholic depression. Goldney, Adam, O’Brien, and Termansen (1981) studied 100
female suicide attempters in four Australian inpatient wards and assessed for melancholia. One-third of
those assessed were classified with melancholia. Another study examined the relation between
suicidality and DST response in 49 newly hospitalized patients and found significantly more of the
suicidal patients had abnormal DSTs (Tagrum, Rosen, & Capodanno, 1983). Five out of six of those who
later made a suicide attempt had abnormal DST results.
A recent review suggested that individuals classified with melancholia may not be at greater risk for
suicide (O’Leary, 1996). It appears that certain melancholic symptoms, such as guilty feelings, loss of
interest, and pervasive anhedonia, may better predict suicidality than RDC or DSM based classification
(Davis, 1998; Van Gastel, Schotte, & Maes, 1997). In the Swedish Lundby Study, over 3500 patients
were monitored over a 25-year period (Hagnell & Rorsman, 1978). The records of 28 subjects who
successfully committed suicide were analyzed and revealed that 10 out of the 14 with a depressive
diagnosis experienced a number of melancholic features (waking up early, extreme guilt, diurnal
rhythm). Fawcett et al. (1990) studied 954 psychiatric patients with affective disorders and found that
severe loss of interest or pleasure to be a predictor of future suicide. Other symptoms that appear to be
related to suicidality are hopelessness, depressed mood, feelings of guilt, loss of interest, and low selfesteem (Van Gastel et al., 1997). Thus, while the data are not uniform in suggesting that melancholia
may be associated with elevated suicide risk, clinicians should be aware of the possibility, especially
when specific symptoms are present such as severe anhedonia and loss of interest, hopelessness, and
inappropriate guilt.
6. Somatic treatment of melancholic depression
There is disagreement about whether melancholic and non-melancholic depressed individuals
respond differently to pharmacotherapy and pill placebos. Because of the large number of studies
with conflicting data, two papers with alternate viewpoints are cited here. Feinberg (1992) suggested
that accurate diagnosis and subtyping of depression is crucial in determining response to treatment.
He cited studies demonstrating that melancholic depressed individuals responded to imipramine but
not placebo, whereas non-melancholic depressed patients responded to active treatment or placebo
(Kiloh, Ball, & Garside, 1961; Rogers & Clay, 1975). Other studies have reported similar findings
(Peselow, Sanfilipo, & Difiglia, 1992; Raskin & Crook, 1976). Feinberg indicated that these results
suggest that individuals with melancholic depression may be less responsive to psychosocial
interventions. These broad statements may not be warranted as studies demonstrating that
melancholic patients do not respond to placebo merely provide evidence that these patients are
less responsive to the psychological effects of pill placebos. In response to Feinberg’s article,
Greenberg, Bornstein, Greenberg, and Fisher (1992) wrote that the studies reviewed by Feinberg
judged the efficacy of antidepressants and placebos without double-blind conditions. Following the
publication of these papers, a double-blind antidepressant treatment study found that individuals with
melancholia are in fact less likely to respond to placebo than non-melancholic patients (Peselow et
al., 1992).
Although research examining melancholic versus non-melancholic differences in treatment response
is useful, it is important that investigators of treatment efficacy extend beyond subtype, e.g., evaluating
symptom-specific improvements within melancholic and non-melancholic patients. Nevertheless, it
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
35
appears that individuals with melancholic depression are less likely to be placebo responders than those
with non-melancholic depression.
It is clear that antidepressant medication is an effective treatment for individuals who meet RDC, or
DSM criteria for melancholia. Angst, Scheidegger, and Stabl (1993) published data from 38 doubleblind and 2 single-blind studies examining moclobemide and other antidepressants versus placebo that
were available for an intent-to-treat meta-analysis of individuals with melancholia. Response rates to
moclobemide were highest in patients with unipolar melancholic (66%) followed by bipolar (57%),
neurotic (52%), and reactive (43%) depression. Studies examining the efficacy of newer antidepressant
medication treatments for melancholia indicate that fluoxetine is less effective than nortryptiline (Roose,
Glassman, & Attia, 1994), venlafaxine (Dierick, 1997; Tzanakaki, Guazzelli, & Nimatoudis, 2000) and
setraline (Flament, Lane, & Zhu, 1999). Nevertheless, fluoxetine has been shown to be superior to
placebo in the treatment of melancholia (Heiligenstein, Tollefson, & Faries, 1993).
Some suggest that melancholic patients respond better to somatic treatment than individuals with nonmelancholic depression (American Psychiatric Association, 1993). However, the clearest predictors of
pharmacological response appear to be biological correlates of melancholia rather than explicit behavioral
symptomatology (Akiskal et al., 1980; Rush et al., 1986, 1989; Svendsen & Christensen, 1981). In fact,
some studies have shown no differences between subtype diagnosis and somatic treatment response
(Coryell & Turner, 1985; Paykel, Hollyman, Freeling, & Sedgwick, 1988; Peselow et al., 1992).
Resistance to pharmacotherapy is rare but does occur in some melancholically depressed individuals
(Puzyznski, Koszewska, & Kalinowski, 1995). Melancholic patients who are resistant to psychosocial or
pharmacological treatment may be treated successfully by ECT (Puzynski et al., 1995; Strober, Rao, &
DeAntonio, 1998). When investigators looked at group differences in depression subtype and temporal
response to ECT, melancholic patients tended to respond to treatment more quickly and more favorably
than non-melancholic subjects (Vlissides & Jenner, 1982). On the whole, ECT and antidepressant
medication are effective treatments for melancholia.
7. Psychosocial treatment for melancholic depression
The widespread clinical belief that melancholia should be treated pharmacologically has not been
challenged until recently. This notion originally stemmed from older conceptualizations of melancholia
as an endogenous disorder. In comparison to the extensive research base on somatic treatments, few
studies have examined the efficacy of psychotherapy for melancholic depression. Prusoff, Weissman,
Klerman, and Rounsville (1980) were the first to evaluate psychosocial treatments in melancholia. They
investigated the effectiveness of short-term interpersonal psychotherapy (IPT) and amitriptyline in a 16week, randomized controlled trial. Study results demonstrated that melancholic depressed individuals
defined by RDC responded well to medication and combined medication and IPT, but failed to respond
to IPT alone. However, a recent analysis of treatment efficacy, using a sample size large enough to detect
smaller effects, suggests that IPT may be helpful for treating melancholia (Sotsky et al., 1991).
A small body of literature has addressed the effectiveness of Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CBT) for
treating melancholic depression. Cappeliez (2000) used RDC to assess melancholia in a sample of 21
elderly participants, six of whom were classified as melancholic. In this study, two of the six participants
reached non-depressed status at the end of CBT. Gallager and Thompson (1983) and Thompson and
Gallager (1984) also reported that around one-third of elderly depressed individuals with melancholic
36
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
symptoms improved after CBT. Nevertheless, interpretations of these studies are limited because of
small sample size (N=15) and restricted age of participants (mean age of 67 years). Thase, Simons,
Cahalane, and McGeary (1991) assessed 38 melancholically depressed individuals prior to treatment and
administered a 16-week, 20-session outpatient protocol of Beck’s CBT (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery,
1979). At post treatment, 75% of the participants did not meet response criteria for depression as
measured by Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, Beck Depression Inventory, and the Global Assessment
Scale. Other investigations have found cognitive therapy effective for individuals with melancholic
depression (Blackburn, Bishop, Glen, Whalley, & Christie, 1981; Kovacs, Rush, Beck, & Hollon, 1981;
Teasdale, Fennell, Hibbert, & Amies, 1984). It is difficult to compare these findings with those found in
larger studies because of small sample sizes and other methodological differences. In addition, most of
these studies included patients who satisfied less restrictive RDC criteria of probable endogenous, which
limited the generalizability of these investigations to patients with more extensive melancholic
symptoms.
The National Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program
examined depression subtype as a patient predictor of response to four treatment conditions:
imipramine+clinical management, placebo+clinical management, Cognitive Therapy, and IPT (Sotsky
et al., 1991). The large sample size permitted sufficient power to detect the effects of psychotherapy in
melancholic patients. In their analysis, melancholia was an overall predictor of lower depression severity
at termination across all conditions. Additionally, the analysis indicated that melancholic patients treated
with IPT improved more than those treated with Cognitive Therapy. A separate investigation examined
maintenance data following psychosocial and pharmacological treatment for the prevention of relapse in
melancholia (Frank, Kupfer, Hamer, Grochocinski, & McEachran, 1992). In this study, 52 patients who
received IPT-maintenance therapy without active medication did not fare very well. On the whole, it
appears that interpersonal psychotherapy approaches may be somewhat helpful in the treatment of active
melancholia, but may be less effective in preventing relapse. One study examining follow-up data in
RDC classified melancholic patients treated with CBT employed a single group analysis of biological
predictors of treatment response (Simons & Thase, 1992). Fourteen percent of those who remitted
following treatment were categorized as relapsers and tended to have longer REM latency values than
those who remained well.
To organize this literature, Thase and Friedman (1999) published an informative review on the
effectiveness of psychotherapy for melancholia and severe depressive states. They hypothesized that IPT
may be better suited for melancholically depressed patients because it avoids unnecessary strain to the
therapeutic alliance. For instance, if a melancholic client fails to engage in scheduled activities or
complete assignments, cognitive interventions to overcome such obstacles may further de-emotionalize
the relationship. Addressing noncompliance from an interpersonal perspective may be more likely to
keep the alliance intact.
Pervasive anhedonia in melancholia may present particular challenges to psychotherapeutic
approaches. Young, Weinberger, and Beck (2001) recommend that behavioral techniques are
especially bnecessary for those more severely depressed patients who are passive, anhedonic, socially
withdrawn and unable to concentrate for extended periods of timeQ (p. 281). Behavioral approaches to
therapy emphasize encouraging the patient to engage in activities that were formerly pleasurable, to
restore positive mood. In that anhedonia is characterized as a biologically based inability to
experience pleasure and by a lack of reactivity to positive stimuli, it may be that at least some
anhedonic patients may not respond to behavioral activation interventions. No studies to date have
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
37
looked at the specific responses of melancholic patients to behavioral activation interventions, nor has
anyone specifically assessed anhedonia as treatment progresses. For activation methods to be
effective, it may require extra attention to the anhedonia. For example, in session exercises with
pleasurable sensations or practice of pleasurable experiences in imagination may be helpful in cueing
anhedonic patients to pleasurable stimuli. Homework exercises that involve rating pleasurable events
involved in daily experiences may also help the person be more aware of small changes in pleasure
and mood.
Cognitive approaches to therapy involve exploring automatic thoughts associated with depressed
moods recorded by patients between sessions. With poor affective reactivity to emotionally-laden events,
patients may not be experiencing situational downswings in mood in response to negative interpretations
of events or situational upswings in mood in reaction to more positive interpretations. They may instead
remain apathetic. This may be problematic because some experts assert that for cognitive techniques to
work, the patient must be able to examine their negative thoughts while experiencing negative mood
(Beck, et al., 1979). Melancholic patients may be less likely to experience noticeable mood shifts and
less likely to be able to re-experience the emotion in the therapy session. In session exercises, recalling
the events in imagery may help to reproduce and identify feelings and accompanying thoughts.
McCullough (2003) recommends several other techniques that can be used to target the cognitive
components of inactivity, anhedonia, and lack of reactivity to environmental events. Because
melancholic patients may believe that they will remain miserable and unhappy no matter what happens
to them or what they do, discussing the consequences of behavior in response to specific environmental
events may be helpful. In addition, in session arrangements of contingencies may help patients recognize
the effects of their behavior and develop causal thought about their interpersonal experiences, potentially
helping to enhance emotional responsitivity. Periodic monitoring of melancholia and especially using
anhedonia scales described in the present paper could be helpful in assessing the success of these
behavioral or cognitive interventions.
8. Summary and conclusions
The proportion of cases with melancholia in an outpatient setting is much lower than nonmelancholic forms of depression (Thase & Friedman, 1999). Although it is an uncommon problem,
practitioners should be aware that individuals with melancholic symptomatology may require separate
assessment tools and the consideration of different treatment options. Unfortunately, there is still
debate over what features define melancholia. The DSM-IV specifier bwith melancholic featuresQ and
RDC of bdefinite endogenousQ are the most researched and restrictive definitions of melancholia. Yet
there is still uncertainty as to what some of the criteria in these diagnoses mean (Carrol, 1984;
Parker, Gladstone, et al., 1997, Parker, Roussos, et al., 1997). For example, how distinct is bdistinct
qualityQ of mood? Empirical analysis has lead to the suggestion that this criterion be removed from
the DSM-IV diagnosis of melancholia until its definition be improved (Parker, Gladstone, et al.,
1997; Parker, Roussos, et al., 1997). Some investigators have tried to avoid vague symptom criteria
altogether by utilizing a sign-based measure of melancholia (Parker et al., 1994). In addition to
disagreement over what defines the syndrome, there is also debate as to whether melancholia is a
true phenotype. While it is possible that melancholia is solely a consequence of diagnostic
categorization or a cluster of symptoms present in chronic and severe cases of depression, empirical
38
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
evidence tends to support the phenotype model (Ambrosini et al., 2002; Beach & Amir, 2003;
Benazzi, 2000; Grove et al., 1987; Haslam & Beck, 1994; Thase et al., 1983). Melancholia is not
always present in severe and chronic forms of depression (Benazzi, 2000; Thase et al., 1983).
Moreover, taxometric analyses have isolated specific clinical features that define a melancholic
syndrome that is qualitatively distinct from non-melancholic symptomatology (Ambrosini et al.,
2002; Beach & Amir, 2003; Grove et al., 1987; Haslam & Beck, 1994), supporting a unique
melancholic phenotype. Generally, patients fitting this syndrome tend to be older and demonstrate
severe anhedonia, psychomotor abnormalities, and neurovegetative features. Assessment of
melancholic symptoms as defined by the DSM-IV can be performed via clinician ratings (e.g.,
Newcastle Index) or self-report (e.g., IDS-SR).
The current review indicates that there is empirical support for a melancholic subtype that
qualitatively is distinct from other forms of depression. Individuals with melancholia are different
than other depressed individuals in their patterns of symptomatology, psychological and biological
factors, and treatment response. Although endogenous and melancholic depression share the same
constellation of symptoms, findings from empirical research suggests that there is a clear
distinction between these two constructs. Individuals described as melancholic can experience
severe life stress, which may play a role in the etiology of their disorder (Forrest et al., 1965;
Harkness & Monroe, 2002; Lafer et al., 1996; Leff et al., 1970; Mundt et al., 2000; Thomson &
Hendrie 1972; Willner et al., 1990, but see Kohn et al., 2001; Tomaszewska et al., 1996),
whereas endogenous depression by definition lacks any clear environmental precipitants. Future
research of the interrelation between genetic and environmental factors may better elucidate the
epigenetics involved in the development of melancholia. Biological dysfunction may also
influence melancholic symptomatology. While much evidence is correlational, measures of basal
neuroendocrine levels and neuroendocrine response and REM sleep data provide evidence for a
pathophysiology of melancholia. Thus, endogenous and melancholic forms of depression may
both include internal biological dysfunction; however, episodes of melancholia appear to be
initiated by life stress in some circumstances. The future of biological assessment of depression
appears to be directed towards identifying the association between regional brain activity and
specific symptomatology (Davidson, 2000; Stahl, Zhang, & Damatarca, 2003). Other empirical
efforts may focus on the relationship between biological indicators and environmental stress.
Currently, research indicates that biological abnormalities and melancholic features in depression
predict a poor response to psychotherapy (Frank et al., 1992; Thase, Dubé, & Bower, 1996).
Does this mean that depressed patients with melancholia do not respond well to psychosocial
interventions because of their underlying biological dysfunction? This question may remain
unanswered until newer research techniques can better uncover the physiological bases of
emotional disorders.
Current research literature indicates that somatic treatments are generally effective for melancholic
and non-melancholic depression. The empirical status of psychosocial interventions are less conclusive.
Findings from smaller studies support the efficacy of psychosocial treatments for melancholia
(Blackburn et al., 1981; Kovacs et al., 1981; Teasdale et al., 1984; Thase et al., 1991; Sotsky et al.,
1991), but larger studies with more conservative inclusion criteria have indicated that some forms of
psychotherapy may be ineffective (Frank et al., 1992; Prusoff et al., 1980; Sotsky et al., 1991). Given the
impact of psychosocial stressors on the course of melancholia, the combination of psychosocial and
somatic techniques may be the most favorable option for melancholia treatment.
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
39
References
Akiskal, H. S., Rosenthal, T. L., Haykay, R. F., Lemmi, H., Rosenthal, R. H., & Scott-Strauss, A. (1980). Characterological
depressions: Clinical and sleep EEG findings separating dysthymias from character spectrum disorders. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 37, 777 – 783.
Ambrosini, P. J., Bennett, D. S., Cleland, C. M., & Haslam, N. (2002). Taxonicity of adolescent melancholia: A categorical or
dimensional construct? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 36, 247 – 256.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC7
Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1993). Practice guidelines for major depressive disorder in adults. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 150, 1 – 26.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC7
Author.
Angst, J., Scheidegger, P., & Stabl, M. (1993). Efficacy of moclobemide in different patient groups: Results of new subscales of
the Hamilton depression rating scale. Clinical Neuropharmacology, 16(Suppl. 2), S55 – S62.
Beach, S. R. H., & Amir, N. (2003). Is depression taxonic, dimensional or both? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112,
228 – 236.
Bech, P., & Rafaelsen, O. J. (1980). The use of rating scales exemplified by a comparison of the Hamilton and Bech–Rafaelsen
Melancholia Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 62(s 285), 128 – 131.
Beck, A. T. (1983). Cognitive therapy of depression: New perspectives. In P. J. Clayton, & J. E. Barrett (Eds.), Treatment of
depression: Old controversies and new approaches (pp. 265 – 290). New York7 Raven Press.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. G., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York7 Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX7
Psychological.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 38, 1039 – 1046.
Benazzi, F. (2000). Bipolar II depression with melancholic features. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 12, 29 – 33.
Bench, C. J., Friston, K. J., Brown, R. G., Scott, L. C., Frackowiak, R. S., & Dolan, R. J. (1992). The anatomy of melancholia:
Focal abnormalities of cerebral blood flow in major depression. Psychological Medicine, 22, 607 – 615.
Benjaminsen, S. (1981). Primary non-endogenous depression and features attributed to reactive depression. Journal of Affective
Disorders, 3, 245 – 259.
Berger, M., Pirke, K. M., Doerr, P., Krieg, J. C., & von Zerssen, D. (1984). The limited utility of the dexamethasone suppression
test for the diagnostic process in psychiatry. British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 372 – 382.
Bifulco, A. T., Brown, G. W., Moran, P., Ball, C., & Cambell, C. (1998). Predicting depression in women: The role of past and
present vulnerability. Psychological Medicine, 28, 39 – 50.
Biro, M., & Till, E. (1989). Factor analytic study of depressive disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 369 – 373.
Blackburn, I. M., Bishop, S., Glen, A. I. M., Whalley, L. J., & Christie, J. E. (1981). The efficacy of cognitive therapy in
depression. A treatment trial using cognitive therapy, pharmacotherapy, each alone and in combination. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 139, 181 – 191.
Boyce, P., Parker, G., & Hickie, I. (1990). Personality differences between patients with remitted melancholic and nonmelancholic depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147(11), 1476 – 1483.
Brown, G. W., Harris, T. l. O., & Hepworth, C. (1994). Life events and endogenous depression: A puzzle reexamined. Archives
of General Psychiatry, 51, 525 – 534.
Brown, W. A., & Shuey, I. (1980). Response to dexamethasone and subtype of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37,
747 – 751.
Bruder, G. E., Fong, R., Tenke, C. E., Leite, P., Towey, J. P., Stewart, J. E., et al. (1997). Regional brain asymmetries in major
depression with or without an anxiety disorder: A quantitative electroencephalographic study. Biological Psychiatry, 41,
939 – 948.
Carney, M. W. P., Roth, M., & Garside, R. (1965). The diagnosis of depressive syndromes and the prediction of ECT response.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 659 – 674.
Carrol, B. J. (1984). Problems with diagnostic criteria for depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 45, 14 – 18.
40
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
Carroll, B. J., Feinberg, M., Greden, J. F., Tarika, J., Albala, A. A., Haskett, N. M., et al. (1981). A specific laboratory test for
the diagnosis of melancholia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 15 – 22.
Carroll, B. J., Martin, F. I., & Davies, B. (1968). Resistance to suppression by dexamethasone of plasma 11-O.H.C.S. levels in
severe depressive illness. British Medical Journal, 3, 285 – 287.
Cappeliez, P. (2000). Presentation of depression and response to group cognitive therapy with older patients. Journal of Clinical
Geropsychology, 6, 165 – 174.
Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P., & Raulin, M. L. (1976). Scales for physical and social anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 85, 374 – 382.
Charney, D., Nelson, J. C., & Quinlan, D. M. (1981). Personality traits and disorder in depression. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 138, 1601 – 1604.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic
implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316 – 336.
Coble, P., Foster, F. G., & Kupfer, D. J. (1976). Electroencephalographic sleep diagnosis of primary depression. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 33, 1124 – 1127.
Coplov, D.L, Rubin, R. T., Mander, A. J., Sashidharan, S. P., Whitehouse, A. M., Blackburn, I., et al. (1986). DSM-III
Melancholia: Do the criteria accurately and reliably distinguish endogenous pattern depression? Journal of Affective
Disorders, 10, 191 – 202.
Cornell, D. G., Milden, R. S., & Shimp, S. (1985). Stressful life events associated with endogenous depression. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 173, 470 – 476.
Coryell, W., & Turner, R. (1985). Outcome with desipramine therapy in subtypes of nonpsychotic depression. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 9, 149 – 154.
Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathology, and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. American
Psychologist, 55, 1196 – 1214.
Davidson, R. J., & Irwin, W. (1999). The functional anatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3,
11 – 21.
Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., & Putnam, K. (2002). Depression: Perspectives from affective neuroscience.
Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 545 – 574.
Davis, A. T. (1998). Depression and attempted suicide: A hospital based study of 437 patients. In R. J. Kosky, & H. S.
Eshkevari (Eds.), Suicide prevention: The global context (pp. 157 – 161). Adelade, Australia7 U Adelade.
D’haenen, H. (1996). Measurement of anhedonia. European Psychiatry, 55, 247 – 250.
Dierick, M. (1997). A review of the efficacy and tolerability of venlafaxine. European Psychiatry, 12(Suppl. 4), 307s – 313s.
Elsenga, S., & van den Hoofdakker, R. H. (1983). Clinical effects of sleep deprivation and clomipramine in endogenous
depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17, 361 – 374.
Elsenga, S., & van den Hoofdakker, R. H. (1987). Response to total sleep deprivation in endogenous depression. Journal of
Psychiatric Research, 21, 157 – 161.
Fawcett, J., Clark, D. C., & Scheftner, W. A. (1983). Assessing anhedonia in psychiatric patients: The pleasure scale. Archives
of General Psychiatry, 40, 79 – 84.
Fawcett, J., Scheftner, W. A., Fogg, L., Clarck, D. C., Young, M. A., Hedeker, D., et al. (1990). Time-related predictors of
suicide in major affective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1189 – 1194.
Feinberg, M. (1992). Comment: Subtypes of depression and response to treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 60, 670 – 674.
Feinberg, M., & Carroll, B. J. (1982). Separations of subtypes of depression using discriminant analysis. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 140, 384 – 391.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., Williams, J. B. W. (1995). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I and II
Disorders, version 2.0. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute: Biometrics Research.
Flament, M. F., Lane, R. M., & Zhu, R. (1999). Predictors of an acute antidepressant response to fluoxetine and sertraline.
International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 14, 259 – 275.
Fontoulakis, K. N., Karamouzis, M., Iacovides, A., Nimatoudis, J., Diakogiannis, J., Kaprinis, J., et al. (2001). Morning and
evening plasma melatonin and dexamethasone suppression test in patients with nonseasonal major depressive disorder from
Northern Greece (Latitude 40–41.58). Neuropsychobiology, 44, 113 – 117.
Forrest, A. D., Frazer, R. H., & Priest, R. G. (1965). Environmental factors in depressive illness. British Journal of Psychiatry,
111, 243 – 253.
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
41
Frank, E., Kupfer, D. J., Hamer, T., Grochocinski, V. J., & McEachran, A. B. (1992). Maintenance treatment and
psychobiologic correlates of endogenous subtypes. Journal of Affective Disorders, 25, 181 – 190.
Gallager, D. E., & Thompson, L. W. (1983). Effectiveness of psychotherapy for both endogenous and non-endogenous
depression in older adult outpatients. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 707 – 712.
Galynker, I. I., Cai, J., Ongseng, F., Finestone, H., Dutta, E., & Serseni, D. (1998). Hypofrontality and negative symptoms in
major depressive disorder. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 39, 608 – 612.
Giles, D. E., & Rush, A. J. (1982). Relationship of dysfunctional attitudes and dexamethasone response in endogenous and nonendogenous depression. Biological Psychiatry, 17, 1303 – 1314.
Goldney, R. D., Adam, K. S., O’Brien, J. C., & Termansen, P. (1981). Depression in young women who have attempted suicide:
An international replication study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 3, 327 – 337.
Gottfries, C. G. (1998). Is there a difference between elderly and younger patients with regard to the symptomatology and
aetiology of depression? International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 13(Suppl. 5), S13 – S18.
Greenberg, R. P., Bornstein, R. F., Greenberg, M. D., & Fisher, S. (1992). As for the kings: A reply with regard to depression
subtypes and antidepressant response. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 675 – 677.
Groezinger, M., Koegel, P., & Roeschke, J. (2002). Effects of REM sleep awakenings and related wakening paradigms on the
ultradian sleep cycle and the symptoms in depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 36(5), 299 – 308.
Grove, W. M., Andreasen, N. C., Young, M., Endicott, J., Keller, M. B., Hirschfeld, R. M. A., et al. (1987). Isolation and
characterization of a nuclear depressive syndrome. Psychological Medicine, 17, 471 – 484.
Gurney, C. (1971). Diagnostic scales for effective disorders. Proceedings of the Fifth World Conference of Psychiatry, Mexico
City (p. 330).
Hagnell, O., & Rorsman, B. (1978). Suicide and endogenous depression with somatic symptoms in the Lundby study.
Neuropsychobiology, 4, 180 – 187.
Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 23, 56 – 62.
Harkness, K. L., & Monroe, S. M. (2002). Childhood adversity and the endogenous versus non-endogenous distinction in
women with major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 387 – 393.
Haslam, N., & Beck, A. T. (1994). Subtyping major depression: A taxometric analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103,
686 – 692.
Heiligenstein, J. H., Tollefson, G. D., & Faries, D. E. (1993). A double-blind trial of fluoxetine, 20 mg, and placebo in outpatients with DSM-III-R major depression and melancholia. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, Special Issue:
Affective Disorders: Current and Future Perspective, 8, 247 – 251.
Kiloh, L. G., Ball, J. R. B., & Garside, R. F. (1961). Prognostic factors in treatment of depressive states with imipramine. British
Medical Journal, 1, 1225 – 1227.
Kiloh, L. G., & Garside, R. F. (1963). The independence of neurotic depression and endogenous depression. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 109, 451 – 463.
Klein, D. F. (1974). Endogenomorphic depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31, 447 – 454.
Klein, H. E., & Berger, M. (1987). The pitfalls of the dexamethasone supression test: A biological marker of endogenous
depression? Human Psychopharmacology, 2, 85 – 103.
Kohn, Y., Zislin, J., & Agid, O. (2001). Increased prevalence of negative life events in subtypes of major depressive disorder.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42, 57 – 63.
Kovacs, M., Rush, A. J., Beck, A. J., & Hollon, S. D. (1981). Depressed outpatients treated with cognitive therapy or
pharmacotherapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 33 – 39.
Kuhs, H., Hermann, W., Kammer, K., & Tölle, R. (1991). Time estimation and the experience of time in endogenous depression
(melancholia): An experimental investigation. Psychopathology, 24, 7 – 11.
Kupfer, D. J., Foster, F. G., Coble, P., McPartland, R. J., & Ulrich, R. F. (1978). The application of EEG sleep for the differential
diagnosis of affective disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 69 – 74.
Lafer, B., Nierenberg, A. A., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (1996). Outpatients with DSM-III-R versus DSM-IV melancholic depression.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37, 37 – 39.
Leith, N. J., & Barrett, R. J. (1976). Amphetamine and the reward system: Evidence for tolerance and post-drug depression.
Psychopharmacologia, 46, 19 – 25.
Leff, J. P., Roatch, J. F., & Bunney Jr., W. E. (1970). Environment factors preceding the onset of severe depressions. Psychiatry,
33, 293 – 311.
Loas, G., & Boyer, P. (1996). Anhedonia in endogenomorphic depression. Psychiatry Research, 60, 57 – 65.
42
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
Marcos, T., & Salamero, M. (1990). Factor study of the Hamilton rating scale for depression and the Bech Melancholia scale.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 82, 178 – 181.
Maricq, A. V., & Church, R. M. (1983). The differential effects of haloperidol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the
rat. Psychopharmacology, 79, 10 – 15.
McCullough, J. P. (2003). Treatment for chronic depression using cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 59, 833 – 846.
Meck, W. H. (1983). Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. Journal of Experimental
Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 171 – 201.
Mendlewicz, J., Charles, G., & Franckson, J. M. (1983). The dexamethasone suppression test in affective disorder: Relationship
to clinical and genetic subgroups. British Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 464 – 470.
Mitchell, P., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Parker, G., Hickie, I., Wilhelm, K., Brodaty, H., et al. (1996). Depressive psychomotor
disturbance, cortisol, and dexamethasone. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 941 – 950.
Mundt, C., Reck, C., & Backenstrass, M. (2000). Reconfirming the role of life events for the timing of depressive episodes. A
two-year prospective follow-up study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 59, 23 – 30.
Naranjo, C. A., Tremblay, L. K., & Busto, U. E. (2001). The role of the brain reward system in depression. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 25, 781 – 823.
Norris, M. P., Snow-Turek, A. L., & Blankenship, L. (1995). Somatic depressive symptoms in the elderly: Contribution or
confound? Journal of Clinical Geropsychology, 1, 5 – 17.
OTLeary, D. (1996). The endogenous subtype and naturalistic course in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 41, 117 – 123.
Parker, G., Gladstone, G., Wilhelm, G., Mitchell, P., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., & Austin, M. P. (1997). Dysfunctional parenting:
Over-representation in non-melancholic depression and capacity of such specificity to refine sub-typing measures.
Psychiatry Research, 73, 57 – 71.
Parker, G., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Wilhelm, K., Hickie, I., Brodaty, H., Boyce, P., et al. (1994). Defining melancholia: Properties
of a refined sign-based measure. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 316 – 326.
Parker, G., Kiloh, L., & Hayward, L. (1987). Parental representations of neurotic and endogenous depressives. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 13, 75 – 82.
Parker, G., Roussos, J., Eyers, K., Wilhem, K., Mitchell, P., & Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. (1997). How distinct is ddistinct qualityT of
mood? Psychological Medicine, 27, 445 – 453.
Paykel, E. S., Hollyman, J. A., Freeling, P., & Sedgwick, P. (1988). Predictors of therapeutic benefit from amitriptyline in mild
depression: A general practice placebo-controlled trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 16, 24 – 59.
Peselow, E. D., Sanfilipo, M. P., & Difiglia, C. (1992). Melancholic/endogenous depression and response to somatic treatment
and placebo. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 1324 – 1334.
Phillips, A. G., & Barr, A. M. (1997). Effects of chronic mild stress on motivation for sucrose: Mixed messages.
Psychopharmacology, 134, 361 – 362.
Pilowsky, I., Levine, S., & Boulton, D. M. (1969). The classification of depression by numerical taxonomy. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 115, 937 – 945.
Pizzagalli, D. A., Nitschke, J. B., Oakes, T. R., Hendrick, A. M., Horras, K. A., Larson, C. L., et al. (2002). Brain electrical
tomography in depression: The importance of symptom severity, anxiety, and melancholic features. Biological Psychiatry,
52, 83 – 85.
Prusoff, B. A., Weissman, M. M., Klerman, G. L., & Rounsville, B. J. (1980). Research diagnostic criteria subtypes of
depression. Their role as predictors of differential response to psychotherapy and drug treatment. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 37, 796 – 801.
Puzynski, S., Koszewska, I., & Kalinowski, A. (1995). Drug-resistance in endogenous depression: I. Refractoriness to
antidepressants in patients with depression firstly admitted to a psychiatric clinic. Psychiatria Polska, 29(3), 95 – 103.
Raamsayer, T. (1990). Temporal discrimination in schizophrenic and affective disorders: Evidence for a dopamine-dependent
internal clock. International Journal of Neuroscience, 53, 111 – 120.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied
Psychological Measurement, 1, 385 – 401.
Rampello, L., Nicoletti, G., & Raffaele, R. (1991). Dopaminergic hypothesis for retarded depression: A symptom profile for
predicting therapeutical responses. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 84, 552 – 554.
Raskin, A., & Crook, T. A. (1976). The endogenous-neurotic distinction as a predictor of response to antidepressant drugs.
Psychological Medicine, 6, 59 – 70.
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
43
Reynolds III, C. F., Kupfer, D. J., Hoch, C. C., Stack, J. A., Houck, P. A., & Berman, S. R. (1987). Sleep deprivation effects in
older endogenous depressed patients. Psychiatry Research, 21, 95 – 109.
Robins, C. J., & Luten, A. G. (1991). Sociotropy and autonomy: Differential patterns of clinical presentation in unipolar
depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 74 – 77.
Rogers, S. C., & Clay, P. M. (1975). A statistical review of controlled trials of impipramine and placebo in the treatment of
depressive illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 127, 599 – 603.
Roose, S. P., Glassman, A. H., & Attia, E. (1994). Comparative efficacy of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and tricyclics
in the treatment of melancholia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 15, 1735 – 1739.
Rush, A. J., Erman, M. K., Giles, D. E., Schlesser, M. A., Carpenter, G., Vasavada, N., et al. (1986). Polysomnographic findings
in recently drug-free and clinically remitted patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 878 – 884.
Rush, A. J., Giles, D. E., Jarrett, R. B., Feldman-Koffler, F., Debus, J. R., Weissenburger, J., et al. (1989). Reduced REM
latency predicts response to tricyclic medication in depressed outpatients. Biological Psychiatry, 26, 61 – 72.
Rush, A. J., Giles, D. E., Schlesser, M. A., Orsulak, P. J., Weissenburger, J. E., Fulton, C. L., et al. (1997). Dexamethasone
response, thyrotropin-releasing hormone stimulation, rapid eye movement latency, and subtypes of depression. Biological
Psychiatry, 41, 915 – 928.
Rush, A. J., Gullion, C. M., Basco, M. R., Jarrett, R. B., & Trivedi, M. H. (1996). The inventory of depressive symptomatology.
Psychological Medicine, 26, 477 – 486.
Rush, A. J., Trivedi, M. H., Ibrahim, H. M., Carmody, T. J., Arnow, B., Klein, D. N., et al. (2003). The 16-item Quick Inventory
of Depressive Symptomatology (QIDS), clinician rating (QIDS-C), and self-report (QIDS-SR): A psychometric evaluation
in patients with chronic major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 573 – 583.
Schotte, C. K., Maes, M., Cluydts, R., De Doncker, D., & Cosyns, P. (1997). Construct validity of the Beck depression
inventory in a depressive population. Journal Affective Disorders, 46, 115 – 125.
Simons, A.D., & Thase, M.E. (1992). Biological markers, treatment outcome, and 1-year follow-up in endogenous depression:
Electroencephalographic sleep studies and response to cognitive theraphy. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 60,
392 – 401.
Snaith, P. (1993). Anhedonia: A neglected symptom of psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 23, 957 – 966.
Snaith, R. P., Hamilton, M., & Morley, S. (1995). A scale for the assessment of the hedonic tone: The Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure
Scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 99 – 103.
Sotsky, S. M., Glass, D. R., Shea, M. T., Pilkonis, P. A., Collins, J. F., Elkin, E., et al. (1991). Patient predictors of response to
psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy: Findings of the NIMH Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 997 – 1008.
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., & Robins, E. (1978). Research diagnostic criteria: Rationale and reliability. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 35, 773 – 782.
Stahl, S. M., Zhang, L., & Damatarca, C. (2003). Brain circuits determine destiny in depression: A novel approach to the
psychopharmacology of wakefulness, fatigue, and executive dysfunction in major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry, 64, 6 – 17.
Staner, L., Maes, M., Bouillon, E., & Linkowski, P. (1992). Biological correlates of the Newcastle Scale in depressive illness: A
multivariate approach. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 85, 345 – 350.
Strober, M., Rao, U., & DeAntonio, M. (1998). Effects of electroconvulsive therapy in adolescents with severe endogenous
depression resistant to pharmacotherapy. Biological Psychiatry, 43, 335 – 338.
Svendsen, K., & Christensen, P. G. (1981). Duration of REM sleep latency as a predictor of effect of antidepressant therapy: A
preliminary report. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 64, 238 – 243.
Targum, S. D., Rosen, L., & Capodanno, A. E. (1983). The dexamethasone suppression test in suicidal patients with unipolar
depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 877 – 879.
Teasdale, J. D., Fennell, M. J. V., Hibbert, G. A., & Amies, P. L. (1984). Cognitive therapy for major depressive disorder in
primary care. British Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 400 – 406.
Thase, M. E., & Friedman, E. S. (1999). Is psychotherapy an effective treatment for melancholia and other severe depressive
states? Journal of Affective Disorders, 54, 1 – 19.
Thase, M. E., Dubè, S., & Bowler, K. (1996). Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical activity and response to cognitive
behavior therapy in unmedicated, hospitalized depressed patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 886 – 891.
Thase, M. E., Hersen, M., Bellack, A. S., Himmelhock, J. M., & Kupfer, D. J. (1983). Validation of a Hamilton subscale for
endogenomorphic depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 5, 267 – 268.
44
A.M. Leventhal, L.P. Rehm / Clinical Psychology Review 25 (2005) 25–44
Thase, M. E., & Howland, R. H. (1995). Biological processes in depression: An updated review and integration. In E. E.
Beckham, & W. R. Leber (Eds.), Handbook of depression. New York7 The Guilford Press.
Thase, M. E., Mallinger, A. G., McKnight, D., & Himmelhoch, J. M. (1992). Treatment of imipramine resistant recurrent
depression: IV. A double-blind crossover study of tranylcypromine in anergic bipolar depression. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 151, 500 – 505.
Thase, M. E., Simons, A. D., Cahalane, J. F., & McGeary, J. (1991). Cognitive behavior therapy of endogenous depression: Part
1: An outpatient clinical replication series. Behavior Therapy, 22, 457 – 467.
Thompson, L. W., & Gallager, D. (1984). Efficacy of psychotherapy in the treatment of later-life depression. Advances in
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6, 127 – 139.
Thomson, K. C., & Hendrie, H. C. (1972). Environmental stress in primary depressive illness. Archives of General Psychiatry,
26, 130 – 132.
Tomaszewska, W., Peeslow, E. D., Barouche, F., & Fieve, R. R. (1996). Antecedent life events, social supports and response to
antidepressants in depressed patients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 94, 352 – 357.
Türlçapar, M. H., Akdemir, A., örsel, S. D., Demirergi, N., Sirin, A., Kiliç, E. Z., et al. (1999). The validity of diagnosis of
melancholic depression according to different diagnostic systems. Journal of Affective Disorders, 54, 101 – 107.
Tysk, L. (1984). Time perception and affective disorders. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 58, 455 – 464.
Tzanakaki, M., Guazzelli, M., & Nimatoudis, I. (2000). Increased remission rates with venlafaxine compared with fluoxetine in
hospitalized patients with major depression and melancholia. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 15, 29 – 34.
Van Gastel, A., Schotte, C., & Maes, M. (1997). The prediction of suicidal intent in depressed patients. Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavica, 96, 254 – 259.
Van Praag, H. M., & Plutchik, R. (1984). Depression type and depression severity in relation to risk of violent suicide attempt.
Psychiatry Research, 12, 333 – 338.
Vlissides, D. N., & Jenner, F. A. (1982). The response of endogenously and reactively depressed patients to electroconvulsive
therapy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 239 – 242.
Vogel, G. W., Thurmond, A. J., Gibbons, P., Sloan, K., Bod, M., & Walker, W. (1975). REM sleep reduction on depression
syndromes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 765 – 777.
Vogel, G. W., Vogel, F., McAbee, R. S., & Thurmond, A. J. (1980). Improvement of depression by REM sleep deprivation: New
findings and a theory. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 247 – 253.
Young, J. E., Weinberger, A., & Beck, A. T. (2001). Cognitive therapy for depression. In D. H. Barlow (Ed.), Clinical handbook
of psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual (pp. 264 – 308). New York7 Guilford.
Willner, P. (1997). Validity, reliability, and utility of the chronic mild stress model of depression: A 10-year review and
evaluation. Psychopharmacology, 134, 319 – 329.
Willner, P., Wilkes, M., & Orwin, A. (1990). Attributional style and perceived stress in endogenous and reactive depression.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 18, 281 – 287.
Wise, R. A. (1988). Drug activation of brain reward pathways. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 51, 13 – 22.
Zimmerman, M., & Coryell, W. (1987). The inventory to diagnose depression (IDD): A self-report scale to diagnose major
depressive disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 55 – 59.
Zimmerman, M., Coryell, W., Pfohl, B. M., & Stangl, D. (1986). The validity of four definitions of endogenous depression: II.
Clinical, demographic, familial, and psychosocial correlates. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 234 – 244.
Zung, W. W. K. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 63 – 70.