Seeing Anthropology Cultural Anthropology through Film 4th edition (abridged) by Karl G. Heider Chapter 9 Social Organization and Kinship Societies are organized groups of people. Recall the discussion in Chapter 2, which distinguished between "society" and "culture." The concept of a society centers on patterned relationships, or ties (the social organization, or social structure) between people. The concept of a culture, however, centers on cognition, shared ideas or knowledge (the cultural schemas). Although we (even anthropologists) sometimes use "culture" and "society" interchangeably, they do have importantly different meanings. Think about the term "members." It also means body parts. Members of a club, or social unit, or society, are joined in an organized whole, as are the parts of a living body. Nevertheless, we do not speak of "members of a culture" but of people who "share" a culture. You can be thrown out of a social unit but you cannot be removed from a culture or subculture. In Chapter 2, we saw that cultures are fuzzy categories with no clear boundaries. In contrast, societies and social units generally have precise boundaries. A person either is or is not a member of a social unit. Cultures and societies are similar, however, in that each concept can be used at various levels of inclusiveness. We can use the term "society" for the largest groups, and "social units" for smaller groups (this relationship is comparable to the one between "culture" and "subculture"). Within any society, there are many different social groups, large and small, that are organized according to different sorts of principles and that perform different functions. Some groups constitute embedded series, or units within units with different degrees of inclusiveness. In summary, groups can be defined according to a number of different criteria: 1. Recruitment: How do people become members of a group? By being born into a kinship group (such as a clan), by residence (such as a local group like a neighborhood association), by age (such as Generation X), by choice (such as a club), or by a combination of several criteria? 2. Organization.' Does the group have explicit formal membership, leadership, and functions (such as the United States or a Rotary Club)? Or is it more of a broad category or class of people such as farmers, Irish Americans, or senior citizens? 3. Boundedness: Is the group limited to a certain place, or does it extend across territorial boundaries? 4. Corporateness: Does the group own or control real or intellectual property such as land or ritual knowledge? 5. Function.' What does the group do? Does it wage war, regulate marriage, run an irrigation system, or perform religious ceremonies? The most basic type of group is a kinship group. Anthropologists have long been impressed with the importance of kinship, which can be defined as the various cultural constructions or elaborations of marriage, creating affinal relatives (in-laws), and birth, creating consanguinal (blood) relatives. To summarize: Kinship = Affinity + Consanguinity. Kinship groups are universal, but the patterns and rules that govern kinship ties vary dramatically from culture to culture. The realm of kinship is a prime example of interaction between biology and culture, our "biocultural model." Recall our example of an "unclear family" from Chapter 8, where we saw a complex tangle of consanguinal and affinal ties. Sex and birth are part of our biology. Nevertheless, those relationships are elaborated in myriad ways in different cultures, leading to the "cultural construction of kinship." On the whole, domestic household groups, at the small end of the social unit scale, are kin-based, built around a nuclear family (wife, husband, and child), an extended family (three or more generations), or related families (two siblings and their families). By the time we move up in size to social units that we can call settlements (village, hamlet, or compound cluster), we are no longer dealing with only kin, and we find many unrelated people who for one historical reason or another are linked in that social unit. At the large end of the social unit scale today are nations (for example, Malaysia, Canada, Indonesia), in which kinship ties are of minimal significance. When trying to understand the social structure of a society, you can use three general rules of thumb: The smaller the society (the maximal social unit), the more prominent a role kinship will play in organizing the subunits (the constituent social groups). The larger the maximal social unit, the more different sorts of subunits there will be. The larger the maximal social unit, the more the subunits will be organized on principles other than kinship. DESCENT GROUPS The most important of the kinship groups are descent groups, which contain the people who are united by a direct line of descent from a common ancestor. These groups can be unilineal, in which descent is traced through only one parent (the most widespread form of descent group), or non-unilineal, in which descent is traced through either or both parents. In the United States, for example, the most common descent pattern is non-unilineal. However, that is far from the norm. Seeing Anthropology – chapter 9 Page 1 In unilineal descent, membership in the descent group is automatically determined at birth by one of two rules of descent. In patrilineal societies, the person is a member of his or her father's group (a patrilineal descent group) or in matrilineal societies, descent is reckoned through the mother (a matrilineal descent group). Unilineal descent groups vary in size, in function, and in how explicitly people can trace their genealogical relationships. Descent groups usually have several attributes. They are generally Exogamous Corporate (Some property is usually owned jointly, in the name of the group as a whole. It may be land, livestock, or sacred objects or even sacred knowledge.) Totemic (Descent groups often are associated in some ritual or symbolic sense with particular animals, birds, or other aspects of nature, an idea we will explore further in the section on totems in Chapter 11.) All manner of other social and religious activities may also be organized by these descent groups. The groups themselves may be egalitarian, or they may be ranked in a hierarchy. We differentiate unilineal descent groups mainly on the basis of size. Lineages, Clans, and Sibs The lineage is the smallest unilineal descent group, in which everyone knows the other members of the groups and how they are related. In societies in which these lineages make up parts, or segments, of larger units, we speak of segmentary lineage systems. Clans and sibs are larger unilineal descent groups in which people believe that they can trace their descent back to some founding ancestor and therefore have a sense of collective identity. However, the line of descent is only assumed and cannot be reconstructed. Clans and sibs are generally exogamous, marriage being forbidden between members of the same group. A distinction is often made between groups that are territorially bounded and own property in the name of the group and are called clans, and groups that are more widely dispersed and have no corporate holdings and are called sibs. Clans or sibs may be made up of clearly recognized lineage segments, as with the Nuer, or may be more internally undifferentiated, as with the Dani, who do not emphasize lineages. Patrilineal Descent There are far more patrilineal societies than matrilineal societies. In patrilineal societies, a person is born into his or her father's group. Property is inherited through the male line, and often family names or titles are passed down through males. Strong patrilineal emphases in naming and inheritance can exist without strongly developed patrilineal descent groups. European societies, for example, have long used patrilineal principles for family names and for inheritance but lacked formal descent groups. As we shall see in Chapter 12, all societies—whether patrilineal or matrilineal—are patriarchal. That is, they vest most of the real power in men. Matrilineal Descent We need to distinguish matrilineal descent, the rule of descent through women (or the principle that a person takes his or her social location from the mother, not the father), from matriarchy, a theoretical society where women, not men, yield the power. In theory, a matrilineal system is just the mirror image of a patrilineal system. In practice, however, because of the dominance of males in every society, this matrilineal system turns out to be not all that different from a patrilineal system. Although an overwhelmingly large majority of unilineal descent group systems are patrilineal, a significant minority are matrilineal. These groups include widely separated cultures such as the Navajo and the Hopi in North America; many sub-Saharan groups; some cultures in India, especially in the southern state of Kerala; and the largest of all, the Minangkabau of West Sumatra, Indonesia (see Krier, 1995; Blackwood, 1995). Non-Unilineal Descent There are ways to constitute descent groups other than through a strictly unilineal rule (be it patrilineal or matrilineal). Some societies form cognatic descent groups by allowing individuals to choose whether to become affiliated with their father's descent group or their mother's descent group. This choice is often tied to residence. A person has relatives in several areas and so is entitled to join any of these groups but chooses one, gaining rights to land and other benefits through that group. Cognatic descent groups, then, are corporate groups that own some valuables in common. Linda Stone draws a useful distinction between cognatic societies and bilateral societies (1997:178). Cognatic societies are organized into cognatic descent groups, and every person has chosen membership in one out of several groups that he or she is eligible to join. Bilateral societies, which are common in Europe and Southeast Asia, also recognize descent through both father and mother but do not have actual organized descent groups. People can inherit through both father and mother and, as we shall see in the next section, kinship terms for cousins, uncles, and aunts do not distinguish the father's side from the mother's side. In bilateral societies, however, one can often recognize a unilateral bias in one realm or another. For example, most Americans follow a patrilineal principle in family names, but there is an unrecognized matrilineal bias in postmarital residence as well as where you eat Thanksgiving dinner. KINSHIP TERMINOLOGY SYSTEMS Among the first really good sets of ethnographic data were kinship terms, the words that people use to talk about their relatives. There were lots of reasons for this development. Kinship terms are easy to discover. People talk about kinship freely. People use kinship terms openly. Diagramming the Kin Universe There are many ways to show kinship ties on paper. Sometimes "family trees" are drawn literally like trees, starting Seeing Anthropology – chapter 9 Page 2 with a single ancestor (the important founder, or apical ancestor) and branching up. Sometimes the diagrams hang down like a branching chandelier. Sometimes the focus is on a single person, the "I," or the speaker, placed in the center with parents and grandparents radiating out in widening concentric circles. Try collecting someone's genealogy. You will quickly find that unless that person has a very standard simple family, in which no one married more than once and there are no stepsisters, half-cousins, or the like, your diagram will quickly get complicated and overloaded. For convenience here, we can just map married couples and the set of children associated with the first marriage. Using this simple diagram, you should be able to map all the relatives. Every culture has a finite and fairly small set of words that label categories of relations. This vocabulary is called their kinship terminology. It includes terms of reference, which are used when speaking about another person ("He is my father.") and terms of address, which are used when speaking to another person ("Hey, Mom!'). Terms may refer to relatives by marriage (affines) or to biological relatives (consanguines), or they may cover both (such as uncle, who may be a father's brother or an aunt's husband). There may be multiple terms, each denoting the same person but with different connotations (pop, pa, dad, daddy, papa, father). Complicating things, kinship terms are often extended metaphorically to non-kin (Uncle Sam, Mother Teresa, Brother Cadfael, Father Christmas). Another problem arises when members of a society refer to someone by using a kin term when no biological ormarriage relationship exists. It may just be an expression of friendship or collective identity. One example is the use of the term brother among African Americans in the United States. It is more an expression of solidarity and shared identity than a definition of a biological relationship. From here, with a large enough piece of paper, you can map any relationships. For example, here is a more complicated family. The first wife of the man (A) died; he and his second wife divorced. What are the English kinship terms for A and B? B and C? C and D? NON-KIN GROUPINGS Kinship is not the only means of grouping people in a society. Age is another dimension along which members of a society are organized. Many of the more complex societies are characterized more by involvement in voluntary organizations, such as churches, corporations, clubs, and the like, than by groupings that are determined at birth. Organization by Age Many societies group people, especially boys, by age cohorts that cut across descent groups. We have seen this pattern among the Nuer. The cohort, called an age set, is formalized with an initiation ceremony in the early teens. Because neighboring Nuer tribes do the same thing, a man traveling away from home can claim ties with the age set that is equivalent to his own. So the Nuer age set members provide social and political support to each other and are allies in conflicts. Western societies include some structures that are generally equivalent to age sets. In Austria, people are labeled by their birth year, rather like a good wine (for example, Jahrgang 1977). In the United States, high school and college classes are identified by the year of their graduation, and they celebrate their collective identity with class reunions every five or ten years. Voluntary Associations Large-scale industrial societies such as the United States are characterized by social groupings that are based not on inherent attributes such as ancestry or date of birth but on the individual's choice. This structure allows for a vast number of social groupings, in contrast to descent groupings, which are more limited. The distinction between automatic and voluntary group membership is not as clear as it seems at first glance. On the one hand, ethnographers who describe kin-based societies inevitably come across cases in which people manipulate or even break the rules, even as they are ostensibly agreeing with them. On the other hand, in the United States, many of our "voluntary" social groups are those of our parents. Churches, schools, and clubs all have a degree of family continuity. Even though they are technically voluntary, we somehow wind up in our paternal or maternal groups. One of the most ambitious fieldwork studies of such voluntary associations in the United States was conducted by two psychologists, Roger G. Barker and Herbert F. Wright. They and their research team exhaustively cataloged all of the "behavior settings" in which the residents of a small Midwestern town interacted: The Presbyterian worship services, the high school basketball games, and the post office, for example, persist year after year with their unique configurations of behavior, despite constant changes in the persons involved. These persisting, extra-individual behavior phenomena we have called the standing behavior patterns. (Barker and Wright, 1955:7) Of the 585 behavior settings identified, many revealed ongoing voluntary associations in the town, from formal (Boy Scouts) to informal (high school kids at the drug store soda fountain). An analysis of kinship groups in the town would have given only an incomplete picture of social activities there. In fact, most activities were organized through voluntary associations. Seeing Anthropology – chapter 9 Page 3 SEEING ANTHROPOLOGY Social Organization and The Nuer Filmmakers: Hilary Harris, George Breidenbaugh, and Robert Gardner The Nuer, shot in 1968 in Ethiopia, is surely one of the most beautiful of our films. Hilary Harris, who shot most of it, was already famous for his New York dance films, and his sensitivity to Nuer movement shows clearly. Robert Gardner, who had made the Dani film Dead Birds (Chapter 3) seven years earlier, visited Harris in the field for a couple of weeks with a synchronous sound camera and shot the interview sequences. The short clip begins as a Nuer man describes the importance of cattle in Nuer life. We see scenes of domestic activity: milking, cooking, and fetching water from a river. A group of men try to work out a problem: one man's wife has not borne him a son, so he wants to divorce her and have the bride wealth cattle returned. More domestic scenes follow, and then the men of an age set gather to drink beer together. Setup Questions 1. How does the film use emic, or native, statements to augment the etic narration? 2. What various uses do the Nuer make of cattle and cattle products? 3. What does the divorce case suggest about cattle and social organization? CHAPTER SUMMARY Social organization refers to the ways in which a society is subdivided into constituent subgroups or smaller social units. In organizing themselves, all societies use principles based on the biological facts of procreation: affinity, the (marriage) links between a man and a woman, and consanguinity, the "blood" link of descent connecting parents and children. Unilineal descent groups are formed by the rule that a person is by birth a member of the father's group (patrilineal descent) or of the mother's group (matrilineal descent). These groups are usually exogamous, corporate, and totemic. Small unilineal descent groups, in which everyone knows everyone and the links are common knowledge, are called lineages; larger groups, in which the exact relations may be no longer remembered, are called clans and sibs. Some societies use a principle of non-unilineal descent, forming cognatic descent groups where people use either patrilineal or matrilineal links to claim membership in a particular group. Most kinship terms are like uncle, lumping several different sorts of people together in a single category. This choice is not an arbitrary one, as the sets of kinship terms in a culture give clues about the structure of the interpersonal relationships. The terminology tends to lump together people who are alike in culturally salient ways and split apart those who are different in culturally important ways. Common non-kinship criteria for forming social units are age (age sets) and voluntary associations (such as clubs). KEY TERMS affines (affinal) age sets clans consanguines (consanguinal) descent groups genealogical method kinship kinship terms lineage matriarchy matrilineal descent non-unilineal descent patriarchal patrilineal descent sibs social organization social structure societies totemic unilineal descent voluntary associations QUESTIONS TO THINK ABOUT 1. Why are patrilineal systems so much more common than matrilineal systems? 2. In large-scale societies, kinship principles seem less important than in small-scale tribal societies. What does this fact suggest about the advantages and disadvantages of kinship organizations? 3. Can you identify changes in (ideas about) social organization between older and younger generations today? Seeing Anthropology – chapter 9 Page 4