Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 1 RUNNING HEAD: CULTURAL BARRIERS AND MENTAL BORDERS Cultural barriers and mental borders: Living in and adapting to foreign cultures facilitates creativity William W. Maddux INSEAD Adam D. Galinsky Northwestern University (currently under review, please do not cite without author permission) Abstract Despite abundant anecdotal evidence that creativity is associated with experience in or exposure to different cultures, there is currently little empirical evidence for this relationship. The authors systematically explored this foreign culture Æ creativity link using a multi-method approach across five studies. Studies 1 and 2 provided initial demonstrations, on both individual and dyadic creativity tasks, that individuals who had spent time spent living abroad were more creative than those who had not. Study 3 demonstrated the causal effect of foreign living experience in a priming paradigm involving participants who had previously lived abroad. Study 4 demonstrated that the degree to which individuals had adapted to different cultures while living abroad mediated the link between foreign living experience and creativity. Finally, Study 5 verified the causal role of adaptation in producing increased creativity. Overall, we find that living in and adapting to different cultures produces the serendipitous benefit of enhanced creative abilities. Implications for our understanding of creative cognition and cultural psychology are discussed. Keywords: Creativity, culture, living abroad, cognition, problem solving, negotiations Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 3 Writers have to have two countries, the one where they belong and the one where they really live. --Gertrude Stein Creativity, which is typically defined as the process of bringing into being something that is both novel and useful (Sawyer, 2006; Sternberg & O’Hara, 1999; c.f. Amabile, 1996), is one of the most intriguing phenomena in the human world. The definition of the word “create” – to cause to exist; to bring into being – implies something profound, almost godlike, which is perhaps the origin of the phrase “divine inspiration.” Indeed, those who create the most vivid paintings, emotive music, or eloquent literature are typically among the most admired and revered individuals in a society. Although hard-work, effort, and training are a significant part of the creative process (e.g. Amabile, 1996; Simonton, 1997; Sawyer, 2006), there is also an insight component that is critical as well, one that seems to work at an unconscious and inaccessible level (Schooler & Mechler, 1995). This moment of discovery is the magical “aha” moment, the point at which an idea seems to leap into consciousness, a moment that is sudden, abstract, and seemingly without logic. Despite the mysterious nature of creative insight, over the past several decades researchers have managed to shed light on many of the psychological factors that are vital to the creative process. Despite this considerable progress, however, one of the most common lay assumptions regarding creativity – the beneficial impact of experiences in foreign countries, such as living abroad – remains largely unstudied. To take one example, living abroad is often seen as a necessary experience for aspiring artists, and there is certainly abundant anecdotal evidence for the idea that creative individuals produce their best known masterworks during or following a stint abroad. This is best exemplified by the thriving community of expatriate Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 4 American writers in Paris in the early 20th century, including Earnest Hemingway, F. Scott Fitzgerald, Gertrude Stein, and Ezra Pound. In addition to writers, many famous painters, (e.g., Pablo Picasso and Paul Gauguin), and composers (e.g., Prokofiev, Rachmaninov, Stravinsky, and Schoenberg) created many of their most admired works while abroad. The novelist Richard Stern notes the importance of living abroad for a creative mindset: “Once I went (abroad) it was extremely exciting for me to become a new personality, to be detached from everything that bound me, noticing everything that was different. That noticing of difference was very important. The languages, even though I was no good at them, were very important. How things were said that were different, the different formulas….So being abroad has been very important.” (quoted in Csikszentmihalyi, 1996). In the current paper, we provide some of the first empirical evidence (c.f., Gurman, 1989) for the idea that experiences in foreign cultures can in fact enhance creative ability. We present a series of five studies involving a multi-method approach to obtain support for the foreign culture Æ creativity link, including correlational and experimental designs, multiple types of creativity tasks, and participant samples of undergraduate and MBA participant samples in the United States and Europe. Psychological variables associated with enhanced creativity To date, research has identified a number of personality and contextual factors related to the creative process. For example, studies on creative personalities have demonstrated that creative people tend to be nonconforming, independent, intrinsically motivated, open to new experiences, and risk-seeking (for reviews, see Simonton, 2000; 2003). Large-scale studies of creative individuals and meta-analyses have found that above-average intelligence, tolerance of ambiguity, energy, self-confidence, and cognitive flexibility are also traits that tend to be found Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 5 in creative people (MacKinnon, 1978; Feist, 1998). In addition to personality factors, a number of contextual factors have been shown to facilitate creativity, and these include motivational, cognitive, and affective variables. Most notably, there is now an abundant literature supporting the notion that individuals who pursue tasks for intrinsic rather than extrinsic purposes show enhanced creativity (Amabile & Gitomer, 1984; Amabile, 1985; Amabile, Hennessey, Grossman, 1986; Conti, Amabile, & Pollack, 1995; Eisenberger and Cameron, 1996; for reviews, see Hennessey & Amabile, 1998; Amabile, 1996). For example, providing external incentives for completing a task, such as monetary compensation, can actually lead not only to decreased motivation to pursue the task, but also to decreased creativity (Amabile, Hennessey, & Grossman, 1986). In addition, research has shown that having a more promotion-oriented regulatory focus rather than a more preventionoriented regulatory focus can lead to enhanced creativity, and these effects have been demonstrated both when regulatory focus is measured or experimentally manipulated (Friedman & Forster, 2001). Moreover, creativity is somewhat self-fulfilling: When individuals or teams are given high task-independence and high levels of creativity are expected within the task, then creative products and solutions tend to result (Gibson & Shalley, 2004). Certain types of cognitive and affective processes can also influence creativity. Visual imagery and creativity training have both been shown to enhance creative thinking (Finke, Ward, & Smith, 1992; Simonton, 1991). The presence of a distant future focus, compared to near future focus, has been shown to lead to more creative negotiation outcomes (Okhuysen, Galinsky, & Uptigrove, 2003) and to enhanced creative insight on individual problem-solving tasks (Förster, Friedman, & Liberman, 2004). Inducing a counterfactual mindset can also enhance insight (Higgins & Chaires, 1980; Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 6 Markman, Lindberg, Kray, & Galinsky, 2007). Finally, at the affective level, creativity seems to flourish when people are in positive or ambiguous affective states rather than negative affective states (Fredrickson, 2001; Amabile, Barsade, Mueller, & Staw, 2005; Ting, 2006), a finding that belies the stereotype of the “starving artist.” The role of cross-cultural experiences and cognitions Directly relevant to the present investigation, empirical evidence also suggests a general relationship between diverse experiences and enhanced creativity. For example, the process of learning multiple languages increases the number of associations between ideas (since the concept-language connections must be made multiple times and in multiple ways), and indeed research has shown that bilinguals tend to be more creative than monolinguals (Nemeth & Kwan, 1987; Simonton, 1999). At the group level, creativity is facilitated within collaborative groups that contain diverse members (Levine & Moreland, 2004; Guimera, Uzzi, Spiro, Nunes Amaral, 2005), and in groups where heterogeneous opinions are allowed expression (Nemeth & Wachtler, 1983; Simonton, 2003). At the sociological level, creativity is found at relatively high rates for individuals who are first- or second-generation immigrants, and for individuals who are ethnically diverse or marginalized (Lambert, Tucker, & d’Anglejan, 1973, Simonton, 1994; 1997; 1999). More broadly, creativity even seems to increase after civilizations open themselves to outside influences, and when geographic areas are politically fragmented and relatively diverse (Simonton, 1994; 1997). Finally, there is also evidence that personality traits that are associated with creativity, such as openness to experience and cognitive flexibility, are related to more positive expatriate adjustment (e.g., Shaffer, Harrison, & Gregersen., 2006; Lievens, Harris, & Van Keer, 2003). Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 7 Given the above work on the relationship between diverse experiences and subsequent creativity, as well as suggestive anecdotal evidence regarding experiences living abroad, we propose that cross-cultural experiences – such as living abroad – may also contribute to enhanced creativity. Because one function of culture is to provide routinized scripts or sets of norms for behaviors that can be used to simplify one’s environment, allowing one to predict, understand, and control events, new cultural experiences expose individuals to a broader range of behavioral and cognitive norms for situations and scripts for solving problems, leading to greater cognitive complexity than those who have had exposure to only one culture and knowledge of a limited set of cultural norms (Tadmor & Tetlock, 2006). In other words, cognitive orientations may become more open and complex when individuals process and integrate new methods of thinking and behaving. Therefore, an individual who has had reflective and transformational foreign cultural experiences should be better able to integrate discrepant ideas in novel ways (Leung, Maddux, Galinsky, Chiu, & Law, 2006). In addition, being abroad may allow people to recognize that the same surface behaviors can have different meanings and thus different implications for social behavior. For example, in some cultures (e.g., China, Jordan), leaving food on one’s plate at a host’s house is an implicit but clear sign of appreciation, implying that the host has provided enough to eat. In other countries (e.g., Indonesia, the United States), the same behavior may often be taken as an insult, a condemnation of the meal. Thus, those with experience living in foreign countries should be more likely to see the same surface behavior (food on the plate, a frown, a bow) as having dynamic functions and multiple possible meanings depending on the cultural context. Overall, then, experiences and ideas that promote the active comparison between one’s home culture Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 8 and the foreign one, and the concomitant behavioral adaptation to new cultural norms, should produce the enduring creative effects of foreign experiences (Leung & Chiu, 2006). Thus, the major aim of the present research was to systematically test the hypothesis that experience in and cognitions about foreign cultures can enhance creativity. We tested this idea in a series of five studies. In Study 1 and Study 2 we measured cross-cultural experience as an individual difference variable and examined whether individuals with experiences living and/or traveling in foreign countries were more creative than those without these experiences. In Study 3 we examined whether the effect of priming (or temporarily activating) cognitions associated with being abroad can also have facilitative effects on subsequent creativity for a sample of individuals who had previously lived abroad. In Study 4 we explored whether adapting to foreign cultures while living abroad mediates the link between experience in foreign cultures and creativity. Finally, in Study 5, we explored the causal role of this important mediating variable – the degree one adapts to a new culture – in the lab. Across these studies, we consistently find that experiences with and contemplations about foreign cultures that involved living in and adapting to other cultures led to greater creativity. Study 1 Study 1 was an initial, correlational study designed as an initial test of our hypothesis. In this study, we measured experience living and traveling abroad as an individual difference and then examined how such experiences were related to participants’ creative abilities. Method Creativity task. The creative task in Study 1 was the Duncker candle problem (see Figure 1). In this scenario, individuals are presented with a picture containing several objects on a table: a candle, a pack of matches, and a box of tacks, all of which are next to a cardboard Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 9 wall. The task is to figure out, using only the objects on the table, how to attach the candle to the wall so that the candle burns properly and does not drip wax on the table or the floor. The correct solution involves the use of the box of tacks as a candleholder – one should empty the box of tacks and then tack it to the wall placing the candle inside. The solution to this problem is considered a measure of insight creativity because it involves the ability to see objects as performing different functions than what is typical (i.e. the box is not just a repository for tacks but can also be used as a stand). In other words, there is a hidden solution to the problem that is inconsistent with pre-existing associations and expectations (Duncker, 1945; Glucksberg & Weisberg, 1966). Participants. Participants were 205 full-time M.B.A. students at a large business school in the United States (127 males, 78 females) who participated as part of an exercise completed prior to a lecture on culture and communication. Procedure. The day before the lecture, participants were emailed and asked to complete an exercise ostensibly related to the lecture the following evening. Participants were instructed to follow a link to a website where a color picture of the Duncker candle problem was presented. The instructions indicated that the task was to try to figure out how to attach the candle to the wall so that no wax would drip on the table or floor when the candle was lit. The instructions explicitly indicated that only the objects on the table could be used to solve the problem. Participants were instructed to type their answer in a text box place below the picture. After participants had written their solution to the problem, participants answered subsequent background questions that assessed their gender, and 1) whether they had lived in a foreign country (i.e. not their native country) previously, and if so, for how long, and, 2) whether they had traveled in a foreign country before, and if so, for how long. These measures of cross- Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 10 cultural experience were our main independent measures. Results and Discussion Percentage of participants with cross-cultural experience. One hundred thirty-five of the 205 participants (66%) indicated they had experience living in a foreign country (M = 39.2 months). Two hundred and two out of the 205 participants (98.5%) said they had traveled in foreign countries previously (M = 13.1 weeks). Creative problem-solving and relationship to cross-cultural experience. Solutions were coded as correct or incorrect. To be considered correct, responses had to include the use of the box of tacks as a candleholder. Overall, 111 of the 205 participants solved the problem correctly (54.1%). A binary linear regression with both experience living and traveling abroad simultaneously entered into the equation demonstrated that the amount of time individuals had spent living abroad significantly predicted creative solutions, β = .073, Wald = 4.24, Exp(B) = 1.076, p = .035. Interestingly, however, time spent traveling abroad showed a marginal, but negative, relationship to creative solutions, β = -.003, Wald = 3.21, p = .073. Thus, the more time individuals had spent living abroad, the more likely they were to come up with a creative solution to the Duncker candle problem; however, this relationship did not hold for time spent traveling abroad, indicating that living abroad may provide a particularly important experience impacting individuals’ chronic creative abilities. Study 2 The goals for Study 2 were twofold. First, we wished to extend the findings from Study 1 by using a very different type of creative context. To this end, we used an interpersonal creativity task – a one-on-one negotiation – where a creative solution was necessary to achieve an acceptable deal. Second, we wanted to control for important personality variables to Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 11 minimize self-selection concerns as much as possible. Self-selection is always a potential alternative explanation for effects based on past experiences and in studies involving correlational designs; for example, personality differences could lead people to both live abroad and be more creative. Thus, in Study 2 we controlled for a variety of personality variables, including the Big Five personality variables (e.g., McCrae & Costa, 1987), gender, and academic performance in order to demonstrate that experience living abroad has a significant and unique impact on creativity over and above important personality variables that might also impact creative tendencies. Method Overview. Study 2 involved a dyadic negotiation exercise regarding the sale of a gas station. In this negotiation, a deal based solely on the price of the gas station (the only issue explicitly up for negotiation) was impossible: According to the information in the roles, the buyer’s reservation price (the maximum he/she was authorized to pay) of $500,000 was substantially lower than the seller’s reservation price (the minimum he/she was willing to accept) of $553,000 (the seller needed this amount of money both to finance a two-year boating trip around the world and to have financial security when returning from this journey). In other words, there was a “negative bargaining zone” with regard to finances. However, the underlying interests of the parties were compatible: The buyer was interested in hiring managers to run the station in the future, whereas the seller (whom the buyer was told had been an excellent manager of the station over the past five years) needed to obtain employment after returning from the vacation. Thus, a deal was possible if parties recognized these common underlying interests, and structured a creative solution that circumvented the negative bargaining zone on the sale price of the station. For example, parties Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 12 could agree to a sale price below the seller’s reservation price (e.g. $495,000), but with a stipulation that the seller would work as a manager upon returning from the trip, with the value of the future salary giving sellers a deal that would meet or exceed the interests that motivated his/her reservation price. In this exercise, dyads often reach impasses because they tend to focus only on the sale price of the station. Indeed for most inexperienced negotiators, as well as for many negotiators with even extensive experience, seeking overlap on monetary terms is often seen as the only way to achieve a deal in negotiations. However, in this exercise negotiators can reach an acceptable deal only if they are able to construct a creative solution that looks beyond the sale price of the station. Given that there is a hidden solution in this task that is inconsistent with previous associations and expectations (i.e., negotiators cannot achieve a deal via sale price alone) this task, like the Duncker candle problem in Study 1, is structured as an insight creativity task but within a negotiation context. It is also important to note that such creative solutions were not explicitly suggested in the materials for this particular negotiation exercise: The sale price of the stations was presented as the only issue up for negotiation. Thus, participants had to discover such alternative solutions spontaneously during the course of the negotiation. Participants. Participants were 108 full-time M.B.A. students at a large business school in the United States (72 males, 36 females) who were enrolled in a negotiations class.1 Participants performed the experiment as part of an in-class exercise at the beginning of the academic quarter. Procedure. Participants were randomly assigned to dyads playing the role of either buyer or seller. One week prior to the negotiation, participants were given confidential role Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 13 instructions for the Texoil negotiation (Goldberg, 2000), told to prepare for their roles by themselves, and not to exchange any information with their partner or classmates prior to the negotiation. During the regular class period, participants performed the face-to-face negotiation in separate and isolated rooms, and were given 50 minutes to negotiate a deal. In this study, our main dependent measure of creativity was whether participants were able to negotiate a deal based on the parties’ interests that was allowable within the parameters of each party’s reservation price. An outcome was considered an acceptable and creative deal if the terms involved 1) a sale price not greater than the buyer’s reservation price or less than the seller’s reservation price, and 2) the addition to some type of extra issue(s), such as a job, where the value of the issue(s) could also help the seller meet or exceed his/her reservation price. Outcomes were considered unsuccessful if they 1) involved only the sale price of the station (which indicated a disregard for one of the parties’ reservation prices), or 2) if parties reached an impasse.2 Control and independent variables. In order to control for a number of important individual-difference variables, as a separate exercise one week after the negotiation exercise, participants were given a background information questionnaire that contained questions assessing the major aspects of personality: the “Big Five” personality traits (Costa & McCrae, 1985). The five-factor structure of personality has been replicated in a number of studies (for a review, see Goldberg, 1993) and represents personality at the broadest level of abstraction. Each factor (e.g., extroversion) captures several more specific facets (e.g., sociability), which, in turn, subsume a number of more specific traits and behaviors (e.g., talkative, outgoing). The Big Five include, 1) extroversion (associated with sociability and the tendency to be gregarious); 2) agreeableness (associated with cooperation, trust, and tolerance); 3) emotional Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 14 stability (associated with calmness and lack of worry and insecurity); 4) contientiousness (associated with dependability and responsibility); and 5) openness to experience (associated with creativity and curiosity). By controlling for these personality factors, especially openness to experience which has been shown to be associated with creativity (e.g., McCrae & Costa, 1987; Simonton, 2000; 2003), we wished to demonstrate that experience living abroad experience exerts its own, unique impact on creativity independent of the major personality variables. At the end of the quarter we also collected final class grades as a control for academic and negotiation ability, and recorded each participants’ gender. Finally, we assessed whether participants had lived and traveled abroad (i.e. not in their native country) before, and if so, for how long. These last two variables constituted our main independent variables. Results and Discussion Cross-cultural experience and percentage of deals. Overall, 81 out of 108 participants (75%) indicated they had experience living in a foreign country (M = 38.8 months). In addition, 106 out of 108 participants (98.1%) indicated having traveled in a foreign country or foreign countries (M = 6.14 weeks). Overall, 30 of the 54 dyads (55.6%) discovered a creative and acceptable solution within the parameters of this exercise.3 Variables predicting whether a deal was reached. We then examined which of our independent and control variables predicted whether a successful deal had been struck. Our main analysis consisted of a simultaneous binary logistic regression with the following predictor variables calculated/coded at the dyadic level: participant gender, final class grades, levels of extroversion, emotional stability, agreeableness, contientiousness, and openness to experience, plus total time spent traveling abroad and total amount of time living abroad. Our main dependent variable was whether or not an acceptable deal had been reached. Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 15 Results were consistent with those in Study 1, and are presented in Table 1. The amount of time spent living abroad again significantly predicted whether a deal was reached, even when controlling for a variety of important personality and demographic factors, although as in Study 1 experience traveling abroad did not. Other variables that predicted a deal were openness to experience, which is consistent with previous research (McCrae & Costa, 1987), as did extroversion, which makes sense in the context of this particular negotiation exercise since sharing information about the seller’s planned trip around the world is crucial in structuring an acceptable deal. Interestingly, agreeableness was significantly related to deal-making but in a negative direction, probably because negotiators who care too much about getting along with the other party can lose sight of their own interests and may obtain less beneficial deals than negotiators who keep their own interests in mind. However, although other personality factors predicted the presence of a creative deal, experience living abroad once again had a significant impact on whether a successful creative outcome was reached in this negotiation, and this effect emerged even when controlling for important personality variables. Although this study does not rule out self-selection, it does allow us to have more confidence in the relationship between living abroad and creativity at the chronic, individual-difference level. Study 3 Results from Studies 1-2 demonstrated that experience living abroad reliably predicts creativity on both individual and dyadic creativity tasks. However, because both studies were correlational, rather than experimental in nature, results should be interpreted with caution as they do not allow firm causal conclusions to be drawn from the data. Although we controlled for a number of important personality factors in Study 2, it is still necessary to provide compelling experimental support for the idea that the experience of living abroad actually Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 16 causes subsequent increases in creativity. To this end, in Study 3 we sought to replicate these effects as closely as possible within an experimental context to provide evidence for the causal effect of living abroad. Specifically, in Study 3 we examined a sample of participants – all of whom had the experience of living abroad – and primed them to think and write about one of several experiences: either, 1) living abroad, 2) traveling abroad, or 3) one of two types of non-foreign experiences. We then assessed the impact of such cognitions on subsequent creativity. We expected that if living abroad actually causes subsequent increases in creativity, then the temporary activation of cognitions and experiences associated with living abroad should temporarily facilitate creative tendencies. We sampled only participants who had lived abroad before to insure that all participants had such experiences available to mentally activate, providing the most direct analog to the actual experiences, and insuring the most possible consistency with Studies 1-2 in which such experience abroad was measured as a chronic, individual difference variable. It is important to note that an abundance of research in cognitive and social psychology has conclusively demonstrated that temporarily activating (or priming) a psychological construct or mindset has the same effects as when the construct is measured as a chronically accessible individual difference (e.g., Bruner, 1957; Higgins, King, & Maven, 1982; Bargh, Lombardi, & Higgins, 1988; Higgins, 1996; see Higgins, 1990, for an empirical and conceptual review). Specifically, Higgins (1990, p. 306) noted that, “chronic individual differences in construct accessibility function like temporary individual differences in construct accessibility.” These temporary accessibility effects have also been found to specifically impact creativity. Directly relevant to the current investigation, Friedman and Forester (2001, p. Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 17 1001) demonstrated that “individual differences in regulatory focus influence creative problem solving in a manner analogous to that of incidental promotion and prevention cues.” In other words, although chronic individual differences in creative ability certainly exist, with some individuals being generally more creative than others, experimental manipulations that approximate these individual differences have also been shown to provide temporary boosts to creativity as well. Given the abundance of previous work on the overlap and similarities between temporary and chronic activation and accessibility of cognitions and mind-sets, we reasoned that activating a mindset involving the active and explicit consideration of the experience of living abroad (versus traveling abroad or other types of experiences) could also enhance subsequent creativity, providing concrete experimental evidence for such cognitions as causing subsequent creativity. Method Participants. Sixty-five undergraduates at a large university in France (29 males, 34 females, 2 gender undisclosed) participated in exchange for a coupon for a free coffee. Participants voluntarily signed up for the study on a website associated with an independent research lab just off the university’s campus. All participants were French nationals and native French speakers. Importantly, the information about the experiment indicated that for participants to be eligible, they needed to have lived abroad (i.e., outside of France) previously. All participants in the final sample had such experience (M = 38.0 months). Experimental conditions. Participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 experimental conditions in a between-subjects design. The cover story indicated that the first experiment was a cognitive generation experiment, and that we were interested in participants’ ability to Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 18 mentally simulate certain events. In the living abroad condition, participants were asked to imagine living in a foreign country, and in particular the types of things that happen, how they feel and behave, and what they think during a particular day living abroad. They were then asked to think and write about this experience for several minutes. In the traveling abroad condition, participants received a similar prime; however, these participants were asked to imagine and write about a day traveling in a foreign country. A third condition involved priming participants with cognitions associated with a non-foreign experience, in particular a day in their life in their hometown. Finally, a control condition involved a prime in which participants were asked to recall and write about what happened the last time they went to the supermarket. Given that results from Studies 1-2, we expected that participants in the living abroad prime condition would show more subsequent creativity than participants in the other three conditions. Creativity task. After participants completed the priming task, instructions indicated that participants should begin the second task. This second task was the Remote Associates Task (Mednick, 1962), a creative test of insight and convergent thinking in which examinees are presented with three words and are asked to come up with an additional word that can logically associate the three words together. (All materials were translated into French from the English originals and back-translated to check for accuracy and logic, however, we provide examples below in the English language equivalent.) As part of the cover story, participants were told that this was a second, separate experiment on cognitive ability. Participants were given two examples: For the triad of words ‘manners-round-tennis’ participants were told that the correct answer was “table” (i.e., table Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 19 manners, round table, table tennis). For the triad of words “playing-credit-report” the correct answer was “card” (i.e., playing card, credit card, report card). Participants were presented with an additional list of 12 more triads, and were instructed to solve as many of these 12 triads as possible. The number of correct responses served as our main dependent measure of creativity. Following the completion of the experiment participants were debriefed, given their coffee coupon, and thanked for their time. Results. A one-way between-subjects Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was conducted as our main analysis, with priming condition as our independent variable and number of correct RAT items as our dependent variable. This analysis revealed a significant effect for priming condition, F(3,64) = 3.15, p = .031, η2 = .134. Pair-wise mean comparisons indicated that participants in the living abroad condition (M = 7.07, SD = 2.84) solved significantly more RAT problems correctly than participants in the other three conditions: the traveling abroad condition, (M = 4.69, SD = 3.46), F(1,30) = 4.35, p = .046, η2 = .130, the hometown condition, (M = 4.79, SD = 2.68), F(1,30) = 5.79, p = .022, η2 = .153, and the control condition, (M = 4.07, SD = 2.09), F(1,30) = 9.46, p = .005, η2 = .252. No other mean differences were significant, p’s > .42 (see Figure 2). Thus, for a sample of individuals who had all lived abroad previously, priming or temporarily making salient the experiences associated with living abroad produced a significant enhancement in subsequent creativity, whereas cognitions associated with other foreign or non-foreign experiences did not temporarily facilitate creativity. Thus, this study provides evidence for the causal effect of experiences living abroad on enhanced creativity. Study 4 Results from Studies 1-3 were consistent in demonstrating that experiences and cognitions associated with living abroad produce enhanced creative abilities. This is likely due Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 20 to the qualitatively and quantitatively different experience that occurs when living abroad versus other types of more cursory foreign or domestic experiences. When living abroad, there are numerous incentives and opportunities for cognitive and behavioral adaptation and change. Accompanying such an experience, it is often necessary to modify one’s thoughts and behavior to fit into and thrive in a new culture; when living abroad there can be little choice for expatriates but to adapt themselves in order to get by. Although obviously individual experiences will vary tremendously, for many tourists, travelers, or temporary visitors, changing one’s actual thinking and behaviors to fit into the new culture is rarely necessary to navigate through a new country; cognitively and behaviorally adapting is much more likely to be necessary if one is actually living abroad. For example, for an American to live effectively in a foreign country like Japan, not only must she master rudimentary customs like bowing, eating with chopsticks, and sleeping on a mat rather than in a bed, she must also understand and use vastly different social norms effectively, such as being modest and self-critical (rather than self-enhancing) when referring to herself, speaking and behaving especially politely to elders and those higher up in societal or organizational hierarchies, and giving priority to the desires of social or organizational groups rather her own individual interests. Thus, we reasoned that adaptation may be the key psychological element that distinguishes the experience of living abroad in terms of its ability to influence creative enhancement. As noted in the introduction, our hypothesis about exposure to foreign cultures enhancing creativity is predicated on the idea that creativity is enhanced when individuals absorb and psychologically integrate new methods of thinking and behaving, and such psychological transformation is unlikely if individuals are insulated from a new culture and need not or cannot adapt themselves. In fact, research has indeed shown that just exposing Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 21 individuals to a new culture is not enough to lead to enhanced creativity: At a minimum, individuals must actively compare multiple cultures and do some cognitive work comparing and exploring cultural differences to get enhanced creativity (Leung & Chiu, 2006.) Thus, the main goal for Study 4 was to obtain evidence for the underlying psychological mechanism responsible for how experience living abroad produces enhanced creativity. To this end, we asked a sample of very diverse sample of individuals to fill out a broad survey assessing their experiences in foreign countries as well as the questions assessing the degree to which they adapted themselves to these new cultures while living abroad. A week later we had them complete the Duncker candle problem, allowing us to examine whether adaptation acts as a mediating variable explaining the link between living abroad and creativity. Method Participants. One hundred thirty-three MBA students (94 males, 39 females) enrolled in an introductory leadership class at a large European business school participated as part of a class exercise during the first week of the academic period. Students in the sample represented 40 different nationalities, and 13 students indicated that they held dual nationalities. The most commonly represented countries were France (16), India (15), the USA (8), the UK (7), Italy (6), and Canada (6), with multiple students coming from China, Greece, Australia, Germany, Japan, Korea, Lebanon, the Netherlands, Spain, Taiwan, Brazil, Russia, Israel, and the Ukraine. Procedure. Prior to the first class of the academic period, participants were asked to complete an online survey concerning their “background experiences” as part of a class exercise. Students were first asked their nationality and the total amount of time they had spent living abroad (i.e. outside their native country), followed by a question assessing which specific countries they had lived in previously; students were provided with a maximum of three Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 22 countries they could list (participants who had never lived abroad before were told to skip this set of questions). Participants were then asked, vis-à-vis each of the foreign countries in which they had lived, the extent to which they adapted themselves to the culture in each foreign country while living there. These responses were then summed to create an index of adaptation which took into account the extent and number of different times participants had adapted to a foreign culture. Subsequent questions assessed the Big Five personality traits, whether or not they were native English speakers (since the majority of participants were not native English speakers), whether or not they were bilingual, and finally their age and gender. At the end of the academic period grades were recorded and used as an additional control variable. One week after the background questionnaire was presented, participants were asked to do a second (ostensibly unrelated) task as part of another class exercise. As in Study 1, students were provided with a link to a website where the Duncker candle problem was presented; students were asked to take a few minutes to try to solve the problem, and a text box was provided for them to type their answers. Answers were scored in the same manner as in Study 1. Results and Discussion Experience living abroad and creative solutions. Overall, 109 out of 131 (83.2%) of students indicated they had lived abroad (i.e. outside their home country) previously. Overall, 57 out of 131 participants (43.5%) solved the Duncker problem correctly. Replicating previous effects, time spent living abroad significantly predicted creative solutions over and above other personality and individual difference variables: The Big Five, gender, age, class grades, bilingualism, whether English was their native language, and number Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 23 of countries they had lived in (β = .009, Wald = 5.49, p = .019) (see Table 2 for complete results). Mediational role of adaptation. We then examined the potential mediating role of adaptation on the living abroad Æ creativity link. We ran a two-step binary logistic regression analysis with the following variables entered on the first step as control variables: The Big Five, gender, age, grades, bilingualism, whether English was the first language, nationality, and time spent living abroad. On the second step we added the adaptation index. Results from this analysis revealed that, as predicted, the extent to which participants had adapted themselves to the foreign countries did in fact emerge as a significant predictor of creativity in this analysis, β = .310, Wald = 8.77, p = .003. In addition, the effect of time spent living abroad became non-significant when entering the above terms (p > .18), suggesting that adaptation did in fact mediate the effect between time abroad and creativity. Thus, we ran additional analyses following Baron and Kenny’s (1986) steps for establishing mediation to insure all necessary paths in the model were significant. All analyses controlled for individual difference variables as above (Big Five, gender, languages spoken, etc.) Figure 3 shows the individual paths in the model and the unstandardized beta weights associated with each path. As noted above, time spent abroad was a significant predictor of creativity. In addition, time abroad was a significant predictor of adaptation, β = .012, t (131) = 3.33, p = .001, and adaptation was a significant predictor of creativity, β = .352, Wald = 12.14, p < .001. However, as noted above, the direct link between time lived abroad and creativity became non-significant (β = .006, Wald = 1.90, p > .18) when entering adaptation into the equation, though the effect of adaptation remained significant, β = .310, Wald = 8.77, p = .003. Thus, adaptation did in fact mediate the direct path between time abroad and creativity because Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 24 the direct effect of time abroad became non-significant when controlling for adaptation, the effect of which remained significant. In addition, a Sobel’s test (Sobel, 1982; Preacher & Leonardelli, 2003) indicated that the mediational effect of adaptation was in fact significant, z = 2.10, p = .035. Study 5 Results from Study 4 suggested that adaptation to new cultures is a critical mediating variable that can explain how experience living abroad leads to subsequent enhanced creativity: The more people adapted to new cultures when living abroad, the more likely they were to find the creative solution to the Duncker candle problem. Although providing evidence for an important underlying psychological mechanism responsible for our effects, this study was again correlational, rather than experimental in nature. Thus, we sought to demonstrate adapting to a new culture as a cause of increased creativity. Specifically, in Study 5 we asked participants (all of whom had lived abroad previously) to think about either adapting to a new culture or their recent experience in a supermarket (control condition) and assessed the impact of such cognitions on subsequent creativity. Consistent with results from earlier studies, we predicted that activating cognitions associated with adapting to a new culture would provide a significant temporary boost in creativity relative to a control prime. Participants. Seventy-nine undergraduates at a large university in France (45 males, 30 females, 4 gender undisclosed) participated in exchange for a coupon for a free coffee. As in Study 3, participants voluntarily signed up for the study on a website associated with an independent research lab just off the university’s campus. The information about the experiment indicated that for participants to be eligible, they needed to have lived abroad (i.e., outside of France) previously. All participants in the final sample had such experience (M = Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 25 66.6 months). Procedure. As in Study 3, participants were asked to fill out two questionnaires that were (ostensibly) part of two separate experiments: a cognitive generation experiment (which constituted the priming task), and a cognitive ability task (which constituted our test of creative thinking). Experimental conditions. Participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 2 experimental conditions. The cover story indicated that the first experiment was a cognitive generation experiment, and that we were interested in participants’ ability to mentally simulate certain events. In the adapt condition, participants were asked to imagine adapting themselves to a foreign culture, and in particular they were asked to imagine and write about the types of things that would happen, how they would feel and behave, and what they would think during a particular day adapting themselves to a foreign culture. In the control condition, participants were asked to think and write about their last trip to the supermarket. As in Study 3, participants were then asked to complete a second, ostensibly unrelated experiment on cognitive ability, which consisted of 12 items of the RAT. We predicted that participants in the adapt-prime condition would show more creativity than participants in the control condition. Following the completion of the experiment participants were debriefed, given their coffee coupon, and thanked for their time. Results Correct solutions and experimental prime. As predicted, results from a one-way ANOVA showed that participants who had lived abroad previously and were primed to think and write about adapting to a foreign culture solved more RAT problems correctly (M = 5.95, SD = 3.20) than participants who had lived abroad before but were primed with a control prime Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 26 (M = 3.94, SD = 3.33), F(1,78) = 4.24, p = .043, η2 = .052 (see Figure 4). Thus, for a sample of individuals who had all lived abroad previously, priming such participants with cognitions associated with adapting to a foreign culture produced a significant enhancement in subsequent creativity, demonstrating the causal effect of adaptation to new cultures in producing enhanced creativity. General Discussion Across five studies, the present research presented evidence that the critical experiences of living abroad and adapting oneself to a new culture facilitate creativity. This relationship was demonstrated in correlational and experimental studies, across multiple measures of creativity, and across an array of participant samples, including multiple MBA and undergraduate samples in both the United States and Europe. In the first two studies, we demonstrated that individuals who had lived abroad were more creative than those who had not. These studies showed that the more time participants spent living abroad, the more likely they were to find a creative and correct solution to the Duncker candle problem (Study 1) and the more likely they were to find a hidden but necessary solution to an interpersonal negotiation task (Study 2). Importantly, living abroad rather than traveling abroad proved to be the critical type of foreign experience that facilitated creativity, and this effect occurred independently of a number of personality variables and other possible confounding factors. A subsequent experimental study (Study 3) provided evidence that mentally accessing the experience of living abroad in a priming paradigm acts as a causal mechanism in producing enhanced creativity. In this study we primed individuals (all of whom had lived abroad previously) to imagine and write about a day living abroad versus other types of foreign and non-foreign experiences. Importantly, results indicated creativity was enhanced Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 27 only following the activation of cognitions associated with living abroad. Thus, these initial studies demonstrated that it is something about the experience and thought processes involved in actually living in a foreign country that is critical for the creative process. Study 4 investigated adaptation as a possible mediating mechanism of this phenomenon, and demonstrated that the degree to which individuals indicated they had adapted themselves to the local culture while living abroad was a critical variable mediating the foreign culture living Æ creativity link. This finding is consistent with previous work on diverse experiences facilitating creativity, and suggests that a certain amount of mental and behavioral transformation on the part of expatriates is necessary to derive the benefit of enhanced creativity. It also suggests one reason why living abroad seems to be a particularly critical type of foreign experience: If adapting oneself to take on new behaviors and thoughts is critical for creativity to be significantly enhanced, such experiences are much more likely to have the opportunity to occur when individuals are living but not merely traveling in or visiting a new country. When actually living abroad, people have the time and depth of exposure to a new culture to produce adaptation and psychological integration and assimilation. Thus, more cursory exposures to foreign cultures, in the absence of understanding and integrating newness into one’s self, should provide less creative benefit than more transformational cross-cultural experiences. Finally, Study 5 primed cognitions associated with adapting to a new culture, and found that such a mindset produced reliable enhancements in creative tendencies. It is important to emphasize that the consistency of our results across a variety of methodologies speaks to the robust nature how experiences in and exposure to foreign cultures can enhance creativity. Across all studies, consistent effects were observed regardless of whether such experiences living abroad were chronically accessible (when measured as Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 28 individual differences in living experiences) or when made temporarily accessible (via experimental manipulations). The current research also employed multiple operationalizations of creativity, including both intrapersonal and interpersonal tasks, as well as both insight and convergent creative tasks, demonstrating the robustness of the culture-creativity association. Regardless of how these variables were instantiated, experiences and cognitions associated with living abroad or adapting to new cultures led to more creativity. Overall, our results suggest that that to gain a tangible creative benefit, it is important not only to live abroad, but also to make concrete adaptations to such new cultures; our data suggest that cursory exposures via traveling or living in the absence of real cognitive and behavioral change are less likely to impact creativity. Thus, our data suggest that it is the specific approach individuals take during their foreign culture experiences that matter. A person who lives abroad but remains separate from its citizens and sheltered from its customs will likely not experience a boost in creativity. Similarly, travelers who actively reflect on the differences and similarities between their own and the foreign culture, perhaps by engaging local people and participating in activities that give an insider experience of the culture, will be more likely to walk away more creative. Although future research is needed to fully explore these possibilities, we believe it is clear that reflection about and adaptation to a foreign culture is critical. However, we do not mean our results to suggest that more cursory cross-cultural experiences are without value. By contrast, we believe that such experiences are very often important and of high value, particularly depending on the experiences individuals have and/or the degree to which they experience behavioral or cognitive change. For example, interpersonal contact with individuals from different ethnic or cultural backgrounds has long been known to Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 29 decrease racial prejudice (e.g., Allport, 1954). In addition, there are numerous examples of high-profile individuals having life-changing experiences while traveling or during short visits in a foreign country. The late Boris Yeltsin indicated that his beliefs in the value of the communist system died forever following a visit to a Houston supermarket, where he was astounded at the wide selection of foods available to any American, and appalled that the Soviets had no such access to basic necessities. Malcolm X similarly had his views on racial prejudice in the United States transformed during a pilgrimage to Mecca, where he was stunned to see Muslims of all nationalities and ethnic backgrounds living and worshipping in harmony. Thus, we believe that our results simply point to the conclusion that opportunities and incentives matter: The more opportunity and incentives people have for and adapting their thinking and behavior to a new culture, the more likely creativity is to be enhanced, and such experiences are likely to occur more consistently when individuals have the time and depth of exposure that comes with deeper and more immersive experiences such as living abroad. Overall, our results are highly consistent with the idea that certain types of exposure to heterogeneous environments can train individuals to recognize that there are multiple perspectives from which to think about or approach various behaviors and tasks. Living abroad and adapting oneself to new cultures may thus be one type of important heterogeneous experience that can spark cognitive complexity, conceptual expansion, broadening of attention, accessibility of alternative mindsets, and creativity (Leung et al., 2006; Tadnor & Tetlock, 2006). Realizing that there are alternative methods of approaching tasks and situations in different cultures may ultimately foster creative thinking in the same way that exposure to other heterogeneous environments fosters creativity. Our findings may also be part of a larger process of becoming culturally intelligent, i.e. the ability to make sense of and blend into Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 30 unfamiliar cultural contexts (Earley & Ang, 2003; Earley & Mosakowski, 2004). Although some individuals may be naturally more adept at blending in to new cultural environments, the ability to adapt oneself to new environments may not only help people to become increasingly culturally intelligent, but the mental processes involved in becoming more multiculturally proficient may have the beneficial side effect of enhancing creativity as well. Future research should explicitly explore this possibility of a link between cultural intelligence and creative abilities. The current data have clear ramifications for individuals and organizations that value creativity. Given that living abroad seems to enhance individuals chronic ability to find creative solutions to problems, it may behoove organizations to hire individuals with experience living abroad, or to send employees on transfers or sabbaticals to foreign branches if creativity is particularly valued in a given firm. For individuals who wish to enhance their own creativity, particularly at the chronic level, our results suggest it would be advantageous to consider study-abroad programs in school, or to seek job assignments in foreign countries if the opportunities present themselves. Limitations and Directions for Future Research Although the current results were consistent across several studies, a number of open questions remain to be explored. First, it would be interesting to look at creative enhancement for individuals who had lived in different cultural areas but within the same country. Within a country like the United States, for example, very different cultural norms exist within different regions, such as the Midwest, the Deep South, and the East and West coasts. It is possible that individuals who were raised in the southern United States within a ‘culture of honor’ (Cohen & Nisbett, 1996) but who later live in more northern areas of the US may also benefit creatively Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 31 from being exposed to different cultural traditions, even if they have never left their native country. It would also behoove future research to examine the specific elements involved in adaptation to new cultures, such as learning of cultural norms and values, amount of social interaction, language acquisition, and media exposure, to name a few, to see which of these variable might be critical to the adaptation, and by extension, creativity process. In addition, future research should consider employing a longitudinal study in which individuals’ creative tendencies are assessed before and after a stint living abroad; such a design would provide particularly compelling evidence the relationship between living abroad and creativity. In addition, the current results suggest that certain types of transformational experiences, such as living abroad, may have a strong or profound enough affect on individuals to impact variables – such as creativity – that are typically thought of as relatively stable personality traits. In fact, our results imply that researchers interested in personality and individual differences should consider the possibility that living abroad and other types of transformational experiences might, at some level, be able to impact even relatively stable individual difference variables. For example, might living abroad also cause individuals to subsequently become more open to new experiences? Might putting an introvert into an extrovert-dominated industry (i.e., sales) cause him/her to become more extroverted over time? Although results from the present research are only suggestive as to the extent to which personality traits may be altered via unique experiences, it may behoove future research to consider such possibilities more explicitly going forward. Finally, the current research suggests that a relatively deep and immersive foreign experience might actually serve to change not only one’s cognitive orientation towards the social world in general, but perhaps even toward one’s own sense of self. Anecdotally at least, Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 32 people returning from a foreign sojourn often speak of the experience as “life-changing” and such individuals claim to have a fundamentally different, deeper, and clearer understanding of themselves following such an experience. It is possible that individuals’ self-concepts and core values and attitudes may be fundamentally altered following a stint abroad. Thus, it would be interesting for future research to explore this possibility empirically to determine whether, in addition to one’s psychological orientation toward the outside world changing following foreign experiences, one’s orientation towards and concept of the self is altered as well. Conclusion Although travel may indeed broaden the mind, the current studies suggest that deeper experiences such as living abroad are critical for producing enduring cognitive changes, such as enhanced creativity. Living in a different country may lead to the realization that every form – from gestures to vocal tones to a simple smile – can convey different meanings and have different functions depending on the cultural context. Those critical months or years of turning bewilderment into understanding may instill not only the ability “think outside the box”, but also the capacity to realize that the box is more than a simple square, more than its simple form, but is a repository of many functions and creative possibilities. Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 33 References Amabile, T. M., and J. Gitomer (1984). Children's artistic creativity: Effects of choice in task materials. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 209-215. Amabile, T. M. (1985). Motivation and creativity: Effects of motivational orientation on creative writers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48, 393-399. Amabile, T. M., B. A. Hennessey, and B. S. Grossman. (1986). Social influences on creativity: The effects of contracted-for reward." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 14-23. Amabile, T. M., Sigal G. Barsade, J. S. Mueller, and B. M. Staw. (2005). Affect and creativity at work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50(3), 367-403. Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A. D. (in press). Power, optimism, and the proclivity for risk. European Journal of Social Psychology. Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 123-152. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity. New York, HarperCollins, 1996. Conti, R., T. M. Amabile, and S. Pollack. "Enhancing Intrinsic Motivation, Learning, and Creativity." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1107-1116. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58, 5 (No. 270). Earley, P.C. & Ang, S. (2003). Cultural intelligence: Individual interactions across cultures. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Earley, P.C., & Mosakowski, E. (2004). Cultural intelligence. Harvard Business Review, October, Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 34 2004. Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental effects of reward: Reality or myth? American Psychologist, 51, 1153-1166. Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fredrickson, B.L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218-226. Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1001-1013. Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H, & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453-466. Galinsky, A.D., & Moskowitz, G.B. (2000). Counterfactuals as behavioral primes: Priming the simulation heuristic and considerations of alternatives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 384-409. Glucksberg, S., & Weisberg, W. R. (1966). Verbal behavior and problem solving: Effects of labeling in a functional fixedness problem. The Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 659-664. Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48, 26-34. Goldberg, S. (2000). Texoil. In J. M. Brett (ed.) Negotiation and Decision Making Exercises. Evanston IL: Dispute Resolution Research Center, Northwestern University CD. Gilson, L. L., & Shalley, C. E. (2004). A little creativity goes a long way: An examination of teams' engagement in creative processes. Journal of Management, 30, 453-470. Guimera, R., Uzzi, B., Spiro, J., & Nunes Amaral, L.A. (2005). Team assembly mechanisms Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 35 determine collaboration network structure and team performance. Science, 308, 697-702. Gurman, E.B. (1989). Travel abroad: A way to increase creativity? Educational Research Quarterly, 13(3), 12-15. Hennessey, B. A., and T. M. Amabile. "Reward, intrinsic motivation, and creativity." American Psychologist 53(6), 674-675. Higgins, E. T., Bargh, J. A., & Lombardi, W. (1985). Nature of priming effects on categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 11, 59-69. Higgins, E. T., King, G. A., & Mavin, G. H. (1982). Individual construct accessibility and subjective impressions and recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 3547. Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133-168). New York: Guilford Press. Higgins, E.T. (1990). Personality, social psychology and person-situation relations: Standards and knowledge activation as a common language. In Pervin, L.A. (ed.) Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. Higgins, E.T., & Chaires, W.M. (1980). Accessibility of interrelational constructs: Implications for stimulus encoding and creativity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 348-361. John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press. Levine, J.M., & Moreland, R.L. (2004). Collaborations: The social context of theory Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 36 development. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(2), 164-172. Leung, K.Y., Maddux, W.W., Galinsky, A.D., & Chiu, C.Y. (2006). Multicultural experiences and creative processes. Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois, ChampaignUrbana. Leung, A. K-y. & Chiu, C-y. (2006) Multicultural Experience, Idea Receptiveness, and Creativity. Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign. Lievens, F., Harris, M. M.; Van Keer, E. (2003). Predicting cross-cultural training performance: The validity of personality, cognitive ability, and dimensions measured by an assessment center and a behavior description interview. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 476489. Markman, K. D., Lindberg, M. J., Kray, L. J., Galinsky, A. D. (2007). Implications of counterfactual structure for creative generation and analytical problem solving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 33, 312-324. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 52, 81-90. Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220-232. Nemeth, C. J., & Kwan, J. (1987). Minority influence, divergent thinking and detection of correct solutions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 788-799. Okhuysen, G. A., Galinsky, A. D., & Uptigrove, T. A. (2003). Saving the worst for last: The effect of time horizon on the efficiency of negotiating benefits and burdens. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 269-279. Schooler, J.W., & Melcher, J. (1995). The ineffability of insight. In Smith, S.M., Ward, T.B., & Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 37 Finke, R.A. The creative cognition approach, pp. 97-133. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Shalley, C. E., & Perry-Smith, J. E. (2001). Effects of social-psychological factors on creative performance: The role of informational and controlling expected evaluation and modeling experience. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 84, 1-22. Shaffer, M. A., Harrison, D. A., Gregersen, H. (2006). You Can Take It With You: Individual Differences and Expatriate Effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(1), 109-125. Simonton, D. K. (1991). Career landmarks in science: Individual differences and interdisciplinary contrasts. Developmental Psychology, 27, 119-130. Simonton, D. K. (1994). Greatness: Who makes history and why. New York: Guilford Press. Simonton, D. K. (1997). Foreign influence and national achievement: The impact of open milieus on Japanese civilization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 86-94. Simonton, D.K. (1999). Origins of Genius: Darwinian Perspectives on Creativity. New York: Oxford University Press. Simonton, D.K. (2000). Creativity: Cognitive, personal, developmental, and social aspects. American Psychologist, 55(1), 151-158. Sternberg, R.J., & O’Hara, L.A. (1999). Creativity and Intelligence. In Sternberg, R.J. (ed.) Handbook of Creativity. New York: Cambridge. Tadmor, C.T., & Telock, P. E. (2006). Biculturalism: A model of the effects of second-culture exposure on acculturation and integrative complexity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37(2), 173 - 190. Ward, C., Bochner, S., & Furnham, A. (2001). The psychology of culture shock. (2nd edition). East Sussex, UK: Routledge. Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 38 Authors’ Note This research was supported by a grant from the INSEAD Research and Development committee to William W. Maddux., and portions of this research were conducted at the INSEAD Social Science Research Center in Paris, France. This research was also supported by grants from the Dispute Resolution Research Center at the Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University. We thank Bernadette Park, CY Chiu, Marilynn Brewer, Angela Leung, Dan Ames, Serena Chen, and Don Moore for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this article. Thanks also to Liselott Petersen, Hajo Adam, Vanessa Hsieh, Cecile Adam, and Katie Dover-Taylor for their help with data collection, and Caroline Leygue for translation assistance. Address correspondence on this article to: William W. Maddux, INSEAD, Organisational Behaviour Area, Boulevard de Constance, 77305 Fontainebleau, France. E-mail: william.maddux@insead.edu. Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 39 Table 1: Personality/demographic predictors of whether a deal was reached in negotiation, Study 2. Variables are coded/calculated at the dyadic level. Personality/ Demographic Variables β SE Wald Statistic p value Gender 0.077 0.727 0.011 .915 Class Grades -0.281 0.704 0.159 .690 Agreeableness -0.963 0.363 7.061 .008 Openness to Experience 0.217 0.105 4.292 .038 Contientiousness -0.146 0.180 0.655 .418 Emotional Stability Extraversion 0.167 0.236 0.499 .480 0.538 0.200 7.222 .007 Time spent traveling abroad 0.037 0.053 0.484 .486 Time spent living abroad 0.012 0.006 3.967 .046 Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 40 Table 2: Personality/demographic predictors of correct solution to Duncker candle problem, Study 4. Personality/ Demographic Variables Gender -0.046 0.482 0.009 0.923 Age 0.060 0.091 0.433 0.510 0.042 0.110 0.144 0.705 -0.028 0.107 0.068 0.795 -0.178 0.151 1.387 0.239 0.188 0.178 1.108 0.292 -0.080 0.112 0.501 0.479 -1.304 0.713 3.344 0.067 0.165 0.541 0.093 0.760 0.150 0.130 1.330 0.249 0.099 0.216 0.210 0.647 0.009 0.004 5.491 0.019 Extroversion Emotional Stability Openness to Experience Agreeableness Contientiousness Bilingual β SE Wald Statistic P value st English 1 Language Number Countries Lived Grades Time Lived Abroad Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 41 Figure Captions Figure 1: Duncker candle problem, Studies 1 & 4. Figure 2. Mean number of correct responses on the Remote Associates Test (out of 12 total) as a function of experimental condition, Study 3. Figure 3: Mediational analyses, Study 4. Numbers represent unstandardized beta weights. (* Analysis is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed); **Analysis is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed); ***Analysis is significant at the 0.001 level (2-tailed).) Figure 4: Correct responses on the RAT as a function of experimental condition, Study 5. Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 42 Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 43 RAT Correct Responses 8 7 6 5 4 3 living abroad prime travel abroad prime hometown prime control prime Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 44 .009*/.006(ns) Time Lived Abroad Creativity .012** .352***/.310** Degree to Which Adapted While Abroad Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 45 RAT correct answers 7 6 5 4 3 adapt prime control prime Cultural Barriers and Mental Borders 46 Footnotes 1 There was no overlap in participant samples in Study 1 and Study 2. 2 We considered the actual terms of the deal of less importance than the presence of a creative deal itself. 3 Out of the 24 dyads that did not reach a successful and acceptable deal, 10 dyads reached an agreement on only the sale price of the station, while 14 reached an impasse.