1 Priming the Evaluation of Intentions of Characters in Television News Piotr Francuz, Anna Szalkowska, Ivan Shyerstuk Department of Experimental Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Poland Online Publication Date: January 26, 2008 Journal of Media Psychology, V 13, No. 1, Winter, 2008 Email: Piotr Francuz at francuz@kul.pl 2 Abstract Television news programs enjoy a high percentage of audiences. For most viewers, events presented on the news are not only a source of knowledge but also a subject for evaluation and commentaries. A typical informational program is built upon series of consecutive stories. Results from numerous studies have shown that the ordering of events in such a program exerts significant impact on the understanding of their content as well as the mood and emotional arousal of the viewers. However, little is known so far about the effect the order of news stories has on the evaluations of intentions of their characters (EICs). The objective of the experiments presented in this article was to establish to what degree the evaluation of the intention of the main character from a preceding TV news story affects that of the main character from a subsequent news story. 3 Introduction Character Intention as the Central Component of the Narrative Schema The narrative schema activated as a consequence of the incoming information enables the structuring of human experiences in the form of a story, i.e., in the categories of human intentions, goals and action planning, and the series of events that mediates actually achieving these goals (Mandler 1984; Bruner 1986; Sarbin 1986). Therefore, the narrative schema is used to interpret received information as a particular history and define the goal and intention of its character. It plays a significant role in understanding the series of events presented in the story. Zwaan, Langston and Graesser (1995) claimed that events and intentional and purposeful actions of characters are the central elements of representation of a story constructed by a subject. Most commonly, the concept of intention is used interchangeably to define the goal, motive of action or needs of the character of the story, or it can be used to describe the type of emotions they experienced. Van Dijk (1985) used the term ‘intention’ with reference to an action that consists of the intentional modification of the state of matters made by the subject for a particular purpose. The intention and the goal are primary characteristics of action. According to the general theory of action, it consists only of the character because both intention and goal are psychic acts. However, they are different with respect to the function they serve: the goal determines the action of the subject because of future consequences of the action; intention, however, anticipates and determines undertaking the action even before the goal is formulated. Therefore, we understand intention as the leading motive of action, the basis of which is values recognized by the character as important. With such conceptualization, the goal of action is secondary to the character’s intention. The process of understanding, which is directed and activated by the narrative schema in which cause-effect or teleological relationships are predominant, is determined mainly by the way in which intentions and goals of the characters of a given story are interpreted. This claim is supported by results from previous classical studies of methods of understanding and remembering social events. By analyzing the structure of stories constructed by people, Bartlett (1932), among others, attempted to find what kind of data pertaining to an event has the most profound impact on understanding. He paid particular attention to the intentions of characters in a story, temporal and spatial parameters of events associated with them, and 4 starting conditions, which to some degree determine the intentions of characters. He found that when subjects differed with respect to understanding an intention, in effect they also perceived the same series of events as different stories. The category of intention is an element guiding the way of understanding events and directing the process of constructing their representations. Information concerning emotions, motives and needs of the main characters of a story has a determining effect on remembering a series of events. For example, the presentation of the goals of characters at the beginning of a story was shown to help people understand and remember the story better than when this information was given at the end (Owens, Bower, Black, 1979; Lichtenstein, Brewer, 1980). A similar effect was found in a study by Abbot, Black and Smith (1985). They found that the more clearly defined the associations were between the goals of characters and described events, the better the subjects recalled the stories presented to them. Also, recognition of single sentences from a previously exposed description of events was quicker and more accurate if prior to their presentation the intentions of characters had been mentioned to the subjects (Abbot, Black, Smith, 1985). The information from the best remembered story includes its main character together with the associated goals and episodes that are an obstacle or help to achieve these goals. However, what is important here is the link between these events and the main intention of the character, in other words, the events making up a cohesive plot of a story (Omanson, 1982). The results of studies have also suggested that intentions of characters conveyed during narration to be the most important are remembered better than their indirect strivings, which are subordinated to the achievement of the main goal. The more these strivings are distant from the main intention, the more difficulties are encountered while recalling them (Black, Bower, 1980). The claim that the process of understanding narration is directed by way of interpreting the main character’s intentions and goals is also supported by the results of an experiment in which subjects were required to recognize facts belonging to a given story. It was found that although certain details appeared to be an obvious consequence of the goal of the character from the narrative, they were incorrectly reported by the subject as coming from the story. Knowing the goal of the main character, the subjects had spontaneously included into the representation of the history a previously heard story, the corresponding facts of which did not actually belong to the presented story (Abbot, Black, Smith 1985). The results found in the studies cited above provide support for the hypothesis claiming that the intention and the goal of the main character of a story are steering elements for the course of the process of understanding a narrative. Intentions and goals of action, in the structure of a 5 narrative, seem to be the superior categories to which other elements of the structure of a story are subordinated. Priming the Contents in the “Television Stream” Diversity and sequence are probably the most characteristic features of the television message. Every day, from television, a specific stream of information flows that is composed of consecutive program positions (feature films, informational and entertainment programs, commercials, documentaries, etc.). This layout of the daily program is reflected on a smaller scale in television informational programs composed of a sequence of news stories. Results from numerous experimental studies suggest that the order of news stories in an informational program has significant influence on the scope and depth of understanding. The results from more recent studies on sequence of perception of television programs have been interpreted with reference to "contamination" (Kerr, MacCoun, Kramer 1996; Wilson, Brekke 1994). This phenomenon involves transferring positive or negative emotional states that appear while watching a particular program onto at least two other programs preceding or following it. Contamination is a process which takes place automatically, beyond the viewer’s consciousness. Preconscious and Postconscious Priming of Automatic Processes Mental automatic processes can be primed preconsciously or postconsciously (Bargh, 1990; 1996). During preconscious priming, affective responses are evoked by means of the anteceding stimuli exposed below the threshold of perception. As a result of a short-lasting stimulation, the subject is not able to process these stimuli consciously (Murphy, Zajonc, 1994). In studies in which automatic processes are activated at the sub-threshold level, among others, time of exposure and type of antecedent stimulus are manipulated, as well as the time of the break between the exposure of the antecedent and masking stimuli, the type of mask used or the visual field to which the stimuli are exposed. However, during postconscious priming, affective responses are evoked by means of the anteceding stimuli, potentially accessible to conscious processing, although most frequently exceeding the attention span of the subject (Anderson, 1983; Newell, Rosenbloom, 1987; Smith, 1994). The phenomenon of priming automatic cognitive and affective processes is also bound to take place while perceiving a sequence of television programs. However, it is very difficult to decide whether preconscious or postconscious priming occurs while watching television. In 6 the study presented in this article, we assumed that EICs from the antecedent news story can be accessible to consciousness not only potentially, but in actuality. This means we accept the assumption about postconscious priming of evaluations of intentions of a protagonists’ action. The only problem is the fact that in the natural situation of watching television there is usually no possibility of on-line controlling of the viewer’s scope of consciousness. The study procedure was designed for two different sequences of questions concerning the intention of a character from news stories. The question about the intention was asked for the first time immediately after watching the audiovisual material, and a more spontaneous response was expected. The question was asked for the second time after the subject had recalled all details associated with the observed news story (reflected evaluation). It was expected that the answer to the question first in sequence would reveal greater impact on priming than the answer to the second question due to less awareness of reasons for the evaluation. The Assimilation Effect and Contrast Effect The phenomenon of assimilation and contrast can occur as a result of preconscious and postconscious priming. The term “assimilation effect” is understood as the response of the subject to initially neutral target stimuli, in accordance with the emotional valence of the antecedent stimuli. This response is observed as an increase in evaluation of the stimuli when preceded by a stimulus evoking a positive emotional reaction and as a decrease when preceded by one evoking a negative emotional reaction. The contrast effect is defined as a response to the target stimulus, which occurs after the priming stimulus, which is opposite to the valence of the antecedent stimuli. In other words, the contrast effect occurs when the evaluations of the positive antecedent stimuli decrease and the evaluations of the negative antecedent stimuli increase (Bargh 1996, 1997; Zajonc 2000). Contrary to the assimilation effect which seems to be a “natural” affective response to the antecedent stimulus, the identification of factors inducing the contrast effect is still subject to research and discussions (cf. Schwarz, Bless 1992; Schwarz 1995; Wegener, Petty 1997). Taking as a basis the results of experiments in automatic processes, we assumed in our study that evaluations of a character's intention from the immediately antecedent news story would be transferred to the following (test) news story, with preservation of the sign. In other words, it was assumed that the EICs of television news stories would be assimilated. The Semantic Similarity of the Consecutive News Stories 7 In accordance with the models of semantic networks, activation of one notion (source) “spreads” to other notions (targets) constituting its meaning. The more common features are shared by the source notion and target notions, the higher likelihood and the shorter time of their activation. Conversely, the fewer features the two notions have in common, the less likely and the more time-consuming their mutual priming is (Collins, Loftus, 1975; JohnsonLaird 1988; Steyvers, Tenenbaum, 2003). Reasoning in accordance with the lines of the theory of spreading activation, it can be hypothesized that the more similar the two consecutive news stories (the priming and the test news stories) are with respect to their content, the more likely the assimilation effect is and the shorter is the time required for EIC from the test news story. Experiment 1 On the basis of the presented literature review, the following research hypotheses were formulated: H1: The EIC from the test news story will be: (a) higher if the intention from the priming news story is evaluated positively, and (b) lower if the intention from the priming news story is evaluated negatively (the assimilation effect). H2: The EIC from the test news story will require more time if the test news story is not associated semantically with the antecedent news story and will require less time if the priming news story belongs to the same category as the test news story. H3: The EIC from the test news story will be in accordance with the intention of a character from the priming news story to a greater degree (stronger assimilation effect) when it is made immediately after watching the test news story (the spontaneous evaluation) than when it is preceded by the recall of the contents of this news story (the reflected evaluation). Independent Variables In the study, two criteria were used for categorizing the priming news stories: 1. The sign of the intention of a main character from the priming news stories: (a) positive, (b) negative. 2. The semantic relationship between the priming and test news stories: (a) the antecedent news story belongs to the same semantic category; so-called court news stories, as the test news story, (b) antecedent news story and test news story do not belong to the same semantic category. 8 The division of the priming according to both independent variables was made on the basis of the evaluations from independent judges. The combination of two double-value variables leads to distinguishing among four groups of subjects. In each group, one of the four priming news stories was exposed, followed by the same test news story. The sign of the intention of a character from the test news story (not preceded by any other news story) was also subject to the independent evaluation of judges. Dependent Variables Two dependent variables were used. 1. The type (sign) of intention of a character from the test narrative. The subjects expressed their EIC from this news twice by answering the question: “What do you think was the intention of the character from the news good or bad?” The first time, the subjects answered this question immediately after watching the test news; the second time it was answered after a series of detailed questions concerning the contents of this news. The first answer was considered an expression of the spontaneous EIC from the narrative, the second was considered as indicative of the reflected evaluation. 2. The time required to evaluate the intention of a character from the test news. Subjects From the John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, 87 students took part in the experiment (4 groups, each composed of 21 or 22 subjects; 63 women and 24 men), with 65 competent judges (51 women and 14 men) from the same environment as the subjects. The age of all subjects who took part in the study ranged from 18 years to 32 years (M = 21.18). On the basis of the results of pilot research, we found that women and men assess the intentions of other people’s behavior in the same way. Thus, we admitted that the phenomenon of intention evaluation is of a universal character and the sex of persons under research has no serious influence on the dependent variable. Only the results of that experiment drew our attention to a possible connection between the sex of the character of the story and the sex of the subject, and that was taken into consideration in experiment 2. Study Materials and Apparatus In the study, four priming news stories and one test news story which had been broadcast on the television “News” program in the spring of 2000 from TVP1 were used. The time of presentation of each news story was approximately 1.5 min. The main characters of 9 two priming news stories were persons whose intentions were evaluated as positive (a woman who saved a handicapped person from a house on fire and a man who on his own took action resulting in finding the murderer of his daughter). The intentions of the other two priming news stories were evaluated as negative (a French politician who embezzled money from the state’s budget and two Satanists who murdered a teenager). The character from the test news story was a man who shot and killed a thief stealing a car radio. The man stood before the court on several instances and, although he was eventually found guilty, the court dismissed him from serving the sentence. Taking into account various circumstances, the decision of our judges, who pre-evaluated the intention of this man on the 20-point scale (from 0 – bad intention to 20 – good intention), was split equally in half (both positive and negative intentions were attributed to the man’s actions; see paragraph 3.5.1; Fig. 1). The news stories whose characters were the father of the murdered daughter, two Satanists and the character from the test news story were categorized as the so-called “court news stories”. The news stories about the heroic act of the woman and about the French politician did not belong to the same semantic category as the test news story. The study was carried out with the use of the Computer Multimedia Questionnaire (v. 2.0). The Questionnaire contained, in order: an exposition of the audiovisual materials in the sequence “a priming news story - the test news story”, a manual instructing how to use the program and two questions concerning the sign of the intention of the character from the test news story intervened by the questions about detailed information from the contents of this news story. The answers and response times were registered by the program. For the study, personal computers with 17’’ color screens and sound cards were utilized. The experiments were carried out in rooms with several computers. Each subject watched and listened (through headphones) to the audiovisual material individually. Results - Exp. 1 Judges' evaluations of the intentions of the characters from the news. After seeing one priming news story or the test news story not preceded by any priming news, competent judges answered the questions concerning the sign of the intention of the character(s) from this news story (Fig. 1). On the basis of the statistical analyses of evaluations made by the judges it was found that: 1. The mean EIC from the test news story confirms its neutrality with respect to both the ends of the 20-point scale (M = 11.63) and the mean EICs from the priming news stories. 10 The intentions of characters from the priming news stories 1 and 3 received more negative mean evaluations, and those from priming news stories 2 and 4 received more positive mean evaluations than the mean evaluation from the test news story. 2. The intuitive EIC from the priming news stories made at the stage of searching for adequate audiovisual materials for the study was supported by the judges’ opinions. The difference between the positively and negatively evaluated intentions of the characters from the news stories was statistically significant (F(1.38) = 55.19; p<0.001). 3. The positive EICs are characterized by a significantly smaller variance than the negative evaluations (B (3) = 29.77; p<0.001). In other words, the judges showed much less agreement in their evaluations of the intentions of the “negative” characters than in that of the “positive” characters. 4. The judges needed approximately the same amount of time for the EICs from all priming news stories, which means that the sign of the intention does not affect the time of its evaluation. Figure 1. Judges' evaluations of the sign of the intention of the characters from the test news story and priming news stories (1-4) 20 POSITIVE 18,70 The evaluation of the intention 17,72 16 12 11,63 8 7,50 6,90 4 ±Std. deviation ±Std. error Mean 0 NEGATIVE test priming 1 priming 2 priming 3 priming 4 NEWS STORY Priming news story The semantic association with the test news story The evaluation of the character’s intention 1 No Negative 2 No Negative 3 Yes Positive 11 4 Yes Positive The effect of the priming news stories on evaluation of the intention of characters from the test news story (hypotheses 1-3). According to hypothesis H1, a direct effect of the sign of the intention of the character from the priming news story was expected on the evaluation of the ambiguous intention of the character from the test news story. This hypothesis was not confirmed. Only a weak interaction was found between the sign of the intention of a character from the priming news stories and the degree of semantic similarity between the priming and test news stories (F(83,1) = 3.10; p<0.082; Fig. 2). Figure 2. The interaction of the sign and the semantic concord of the priming news stories as related to the intention of the character from the test news story.* 13 The intention of the test news story character POSITIVE The control level 12 Priming news story 4 11 Priming news story 1 Priming news story 3 10 9 Priming news story 2 8 NEGATIVE The intention of the priming news story character: negative positive 7 lack of conformity conformity The semantic conformity of the priming and test news stories *The control level corresponds to evaluation of the intention of the character from the experimental news story not preceded by any priming news story. From the point of view of statistical criteria, hypothesis H1 cannot be accepted. However, the two tendencies shown in Fig. 2 deserve a closer look. The first tendency is in accordance with the predictions resulting from this hypothesis. If we accept as the reference 12 point EIC from the test news story when not preceded by any of the priming news stories (the control level), it turned out that irrespective of the semantic similarity between the priming and the test news stories, a negative EIC from the antecedent news story lowered the level of the evaluation from the test news story (priming news stories 1 and 3). Also in accordance with the hypothesis, the subjects slightly increased their EIC from the test news story after watching the semantically similar news stories whose main characters had a clearly positive intention (priming news story 4). The second tendency is of a particular interest. It turned out that if the intention of the character from the antecedent news story was positive, but this news story did not belong to the same semantic category as the following news story, then the intention of the character from the following news story was evaluated negatively (priming news story 2). The contrast effect was observed in this case. The primed positive intention of the character from the antecedent news story, however, induced a decrease in the EIC from the following news story, on condition that both news stories belong to different categories. The results of analysis of the time required to respond to the questions about the intention of the character from the test news story revealed the tendency predicted by hypothesis H2, although it was not statistically significant. After watching the priming news story that did not belong to the same category as the test news story, the time required to evaluate the intention of the character from the test news story was longer than when the antecedent news story had been semantically similar to the test news story. In accordance with hypothesis H3, we had expected that the assimilation effect would be clearer in the case of the evaluation made spontaneously rather than in a reflected way. It turned out, however, that the spontaneous EIC from the experimental news story did not differ from the reflected evaluation made after having answered the questions about the details from contents of this news story. However, it was observed that, irrespective of the type of priming news story, more time was needed to make the spontaneous evaluation than the reflected evaluation (all differences are statistically significant). Discussion 1. The way in which the judges attributed the sign of the intention to the characters from the analyzed news stories reveals an interesting truth about the principles governing our evaluations of the motives of conduct of other people. First of all, we are in much greater agreement when we evaluate others’ behavior positively than when we evaluate it negatively. In the case of evidently negative actions (murder, financial fraud), our evaluations of the intentions of these behaviors are significantly more diverse than in the case of actions 13 evidently positive (the defense of values, of life, the fight for justice). However, it cannot be excluded that the high variance observed with negative evaluations might result from the fact that, for the study, the measurement that was applied for such a subtle phenomenon was too general. Surely, the global evaluation of the intention on the “good – bad” scale should be differentiated at least with respect to the point of view of the evaluator. The evaluation of the intentions of his own actions from the point of view of a Satanist who murders a teenager during a ritual “black mass” is obviously different from the evaluation of his intention made by a person who learns about his bestial act. This remark is directly linked to the basic assumptions of the theory of attribution. According to Heider’s (1958) causal attribution theory, while explaining someone’s behavior we resort to internal (personal) or external (environmental) factors. The personal factors are: intentions, skills and motives. To the environmental factors belong objective obstacles, favorable or unfavorable circumstances, and good luck or bad luck. The two types of causes are mutually exclusive. According to the theory, a person who acts intentionally (internal factors) cannot be directed at the same time by external factors. Jones and Nisbett (1972) proposed a hypothesis concerning a difference in perception of the causes of one’s own and of others’ behaviors. The difference is revealed in explaining behavior by either external or internal factors, depending on adoption of the actor’s (the agent of an action) or an observer’s perspective. In social psychology, this phenomenon is defined as “actor-observer” asymmetry. In accordance with this tendency, as actors we explain our behavior more frequently by resorting to external factors, and as observers of others’ behavior we tend to explain it by internal factors. Nisbett (1973) accounts for the asymmetry of attribution made from the perspectives of an actor and an observer by: differences in the amount and quality of the available information (an actor has much more data concerning determinants of his/her own behavior when compared with an observer), diverse perceptual perspectives (characteristics of a situation are the perceptual figure for an actor whereas for an observer it is the actor who is the perceptual figure) and factors related to language (the first-person or the third-person approach to the explained behavior). 2. In light of the results from this study, a clear tendency was found indicating that EIC from a news story depends on the intention attributed to a character from the preceding news story. The priming news story not associated semantically with the test news story, presenting a character with positive intentions, displayed the greatest effect on the EIC from the test news story. After watching this priming news story, a clear decrease was observed in 14 the EIC from the test news story, which, contrary to hypothesis H1, exemplified the contrast effect instead of the expected assimilation effect. Schwarz and Bless (1992) and Schwarz (1995), the authors of the inclusion/exclusion model, claimed that the contrast effect occurs when the so-called context clue (in the case of our study it is the intention of a protagonist from the antecedent news story) and the object (the intention of the protagonist from the test news story) belong to different semantic categories. Immediately after watching two consecutive news stories, the viewer has the information from both news stories at one time. However, if events belonging to two different semantic categories are presented, they are likely to become separated from each other. The context clue will be excluded from the semantic category to which the object belongs, and this exclusion will result in attributing the characteristics distinctly contrasted with the priming news story to the test news story. Thus, Schwarz’s model (1995) would account for the occurrence of the contrast effect by resorting to semantic differences between news stories. 3. At the present stage of the analysis, it is difficult to finally decide whether the occurrence of the contrast effect is determined solely by the fact that the priming news story belonged to a semantic category, different from that of the test news story, or also the sign of the intention of its character. Perhaps the observed effect is associated with another factor. In the priming news story, which induced such a distinct contrast effect, the main character was a woman. In the remaining priming news stories and in the test news story the protagonists were men. Perhaps the observed effect is associated with the gender of the characters whose intentions are evaluated? It would also be interesting if men and women differed in their respectively formed evaluations. Experiment 2 This experiment was designed to attempt to explain some of the phenomena observed in the previous study. Better study materials and procedures for measuring dependent variables were used. Based on the theoretical reasons that had originally motivated formulation of the hypothesis for experiment 1 and on those resulting from the attribution theory and the inclusion/exclusion model (Schwarz 1995), the following hypotheses concerning priming of the intentions, and assimilation and contrast effects were put forward for experiment 2: H1. The priming news stories semantically related to the test news story will induce the assimilation effect. The intention of the character from the test news story will be 15 evaluated in accordance with the sign of the intention of a character from the priming news story. H2. The priming news stories semantically unrelated to the test news story will induce the contrast effect. The intention of the character from the test news story will be evaluated inversely to the sign of the intention of a character from the priming news story. H3. The intention of the character from the test news story will be evaluated more quickly if the priming news story is semantically related to it and will require more time if it does not belong to the same category as the experimental news story. Independent Variables In experiment 2, the two independent variables that had also been used in the previous study, namely the sign of the intention of the characters from the priming news stories and the semantic relationship between the priming and the experimental news story, were manipulated. Additionally, two other variables were taken into account: (1) the order of the questions about the EIC from the news story: (a) first the evaluation from the observer’s point of view and then from the actor’s point of view; (b) first from the actor’s perspective and then from the observer’s perspective; and (2) the gender of the subjects. Dependent Variables Similarly to experiment 1, the EIC from the test news story was registered on a 20point scale in terms of “bad” – “good”. This question was answered twice, from the actor’s and observer’s perspectives. Also, the times needed for EICs from the test news story were measured, i.e., the time to respond to the questions about the intention. Subjects 160 students (82 women and 78 men) of the John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin took part in the experiment. They were randomly assigned to four groups, each of 40 persons. Also, 100 judges were employed in the study (57 women and 43 men). In accordance with the suggestion that appeared from the previous research, in experiment 2 we balanced the number of women and men. The age of all subjects ranged from 19 to 40 years (M = 22.23). Study Materials and Apparatus In this experiment, the test news story and two priming news stories with positive characters from the previous experiment were used. Two new priming news stories were 16 added, presenting characters with negative intentions. One of them was semantically related to the test news story (its character was a young man accused of murdering a student in a disco) and the other was not (about an alcoholic abusing his family). As previously, this experiment was conducted with the use of the Computer Multimedia Questionnaire (v. 2.0) which, apart from the instruction and exposition of the audiovisual materials, also contained questions concerning the intention of the character (evaluated from the point of view of both the observer and the character of the news stories). In order to minimize the influence of the order of these two questions, half of the subjects evaluated the intention of the character first from their own point of view and then from the character's; the other half evaluated in the reversed order. The remaining elements of the experimental procedure were the same as in the previous study. Results Judges' evaluations of the intentions of the characters from the news stories. The first stage of the experiment aimed at testing the variance of the evaluations of two new news stories with negative characters in comparison with the analogous news stories from the previous study (Fig. 3). Figure 3. Judges' evaluations of the sign of the intention of the characters from the priming and test news stories from the previous experiment and of two new news stories from the observer’s point of view.* Intention of the character from the observer's point of view 17 [+] 20 ±Standard deviation ±Standard error Mean 18,50 16,40 16 12 8 9,43 5,80 3,70 4,50 4 1,50 0 [-] Test news story T1-s T1-n T2-s T3-s T3-n T4-s news story *T1 and T3 – news stories with a negative character; T2 and T4 – news stories with a positive character; T1 and T2 – news stories semantically similar to the test news story; T3 and T4 – news stories semantically different from the test news story; -s – a news story used in experiment 1; -n – a new news story. Taking into account the EICs from the television news stories made from the point of view of the observer, it turned out that the mean values of the EICs from the new news stories (T1-n and T3-n) were more polarized, and intragroup variances were lower than in the news stories used in the previous study. Thus, in experiment 2, two new news stories with negative characters were used. As far as the remaining news stories are concerned, the obtained results were comparable with those obtained in experiment 1. Evaluations of the intentions of the characters from the news stories from the perspectives of actors and observers. The judges evaluated the intentions of the characters from the priming and test news stories from two perspectives: that of the actor and that of the observer. The EICs from all news stories made without a defined point of view (that is, made by the judges in the same way as in experiment 1) are also presented in Fig. 4. Figure 4. Mean evaluations of the intentions of the characters from the test and priming news stories obtained when taking into account the perspective of the evaluator or without a defined point of view. 18 20 18 The ev aluation of the intention: f rom the character's point of v iew The evaluation of the intention without a def ined point of v iew 16 f rom the observ er's point of v iew 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 test news story priming 2-s priming 4-s priming 1-s priming 3-s News story On the basis of the obtained data, the following conclusion can be drawn: As a result of taking into account two perspectives, the actor's and the observer's, the EICs from the news stories were differentiated. The characters with bad intentions and the character from the test news story were evaluated more highly from their own point of view than from the observer’s. Statistically significant differences were demonstrated in the case of the test news story (t = -3.98; p<0.001) and the priming news stories with negative characters: the news story unrelated semantically to the test news story (T1: t = -4.83; p<0.001) and the news story semantically related to the test news story (T3: t = -2.41; p<0.03). In the case of the positive characters, no statistically significant differences in the evaluation of their intentions from the point of view of the observer or actor were found. The influence of EIC from the priming news story on that from the test news story. According to hypotheses H1 and H2, two effects – assimilation and contrast – were expected, depending on whether the contents of the priming and the test news stories were similar (the assimilation effect, H1) or different (the contrast effect, H2). Making a distinction between two perspectives of the EIC, the following tendencies were found on the basis of the obtained data: 1. Taking into consideration the semantic association between the priming and the test news stories, the contrast effect occurred only in the case of the news story unrelated thematically with the test news story (the observer's perspective: t(78) = 2.06; p<0.04; the actor's perspective: t(78) = 2.96; p<0.004); see Fig. 5). This finding unequivocally confirms 19 hypothesis H2. Most probably, the more distinct contrast effect observed in experiment 2 relative to that from the previous experiment was due to both modified procedure of the evaluation of the intention and less ambiguous audiovisual material, as evaluated by judges. Figure 5. Evaluation of the intention of the character from the test news story preceded by priming news stories carried out from his own point of view; the control level is indicative of the evaluation of the intention of the character from 15,0 14,5 POSITIVE Priming news story 1 14,0 13,5 13,0 12,5 The control level 12,0 11,5 t(78) = 2.96; p<.004 The intention of the test news story character from his point of view the test news story not preceded by any priming news story. Priming news story 4 Priming news story 3 11,0 10,5 Priming news story 2 10,0 9,5 9,0 8,5 NEGATIVE The intention of the priming news story character: negative positive 8,0 lack of conformity conformity The semantic conformity of the priming and test news stories 2. The obtained data do not allow us to fully confirm hypothesis H1 concerning the assimilation effect. Although the result of the interaction depicted in Fig. 5 in a way confirms these predictions (F(1,156) = 6.28; p<0.013), the assimilation effect induced by the semantically similar news story was significantly weaker than the contrast effect induced by the news story unrelated semantically. In experiment 2, the independent variables were also controlled for which hypotheses had not been formulated. These were: (1) the order of asking the question about the intention of the character, first from the observer’s point of view and then from the character’s perspective or in the reversed order; and (2) gender. 3. On the basis of the obtained data, it was found that the order of asking these questions significantly modified the level of the evaluation of the intention, if the question was asked first from the observer’s point of view and only then from the actor’s point of view (F(1,152) = 32.07; p<0.001). This means that the intention of the character was perceived 20 more favorably if evaluated first from the observer’s perspective and later from the actor’s point of view. This effect did not occur with the reversed order of questions. This finding was observed in all groups irrespective of the type of the priming news story. 4. Introduction of a new variable of the order of the questions about the intention of the character caused a decrease in intragroup variances and made the assimilation effect discussed in point 3 considerably more distinct (the level of significance for this interaction increased from p<0.013 to p<0.001). 5. Hypotheses H1 and H2, concerning the assimilation and contrast effects as related to the semantic association between the priming and experimental news stories, were ultimately verified after taking into account the subjects’ gender. The effects predicted by these hypotheses were fully confirmed in the case of both the observer's (F(1,152) = 6.72; p<0.01) and the character's (F(1,152) = 5.39; p<0.02) perspectives, but only with respect to men (Fig. 6). In terms of high variations inside the groups, no serious statistical differences between women and men were found in terms of major effects (there was no gender effect). The influence of sex on the effect of assimilation and contrast came to be seen only in terms of interaction with the remaining two replaceable independent values. Figure 6. The interaction of the following variables: the sign of the intention of a character from the priming news stories and the semantic association between the priming and test news story relative to the evaluation of the intention of the character from the test news story from the observer’s perspective. 21 The intention of the character from the observer's point of view 13,0 The intention of the priming news story character: negative positive 12,5 C 12,0 11,5 A 11,0 10,5 10,0 9,5 9,0 D Control level (competent judges) 8,5 8,0 7,5 7,0 Semantics non-confirm B confirm Semantics non-confirm Femals confirm Males 6. In accordance with hypothesis H3, differences in the time required for EIC from the test news story were expected to depend on the semantic similarity between the test news story and the priming news story. The obtained data did not confirm this hypothesis. Discussion The objective of the studies presented above was to identify factors responsible for the phenomenon of priming EICs from television news stories. Two independent experiments were carried out. In the light of the obtained data, one can claim that the intention of a character from a news story is dependent on the type of story presented previously. This process takes place under conditions of limited consciousness of the EICs from the preceding news story. In accordance with the hypotheses verified in experiment 1, the negative EIC from the priming news story was found to slightly decrease the EIC from the test news story and, after watching the semantically similar news story whose character had a clearly positive intention, the EIC from the following news story increased. These tendencies illustrate the assimilation effect. In experiment 2, the impact of the semantic similarity between the priming and the test news stories on the EIC from the test news story was clearly displayed. The EIC from the test news story was influenced most by the news story unrelated semantically, presenting a female character with a positive intention. In this case, the contrast effect occurred; that is, the EIC from the following news story decreased significantly when compared with the control group. 22 This result supports theoretical assumptions of the inclusion/exclusion model (cf. Schwarz, Bless 1992; Schwarz 1995). Taking into consideration the falsified hypotheses concerning the time required to evaluate the intention of the character from the test news story preceded by the priming news story belonging versus not belonging to the same semantic category, it should be concluded that the theoretical bases for Collins and Loftus’s model (1975) of spreading activation do not find support with respect to such complex stimuli as television news stories, and especially the intentions of their characters. These results indicate the need for further research in the area of experimental social psychology and psychology of gender differences. The modifications introduced into experiment 2, consisting of reformulation of the questions about the intention of the character, turned out to be very fruitful. First of all, differentiating the questions with respect to the perspective of the evaluator allowed us to eliminate a significant source of the uncontrolled variance that was observed in the scores of experiment 1. Controlling the order of the questions about the intention of the character from the test news story from the point of view of the observer and actor also proved a successful solution. This finding supports the claim that we are able to adopt someone else's perspective only when we have previously determined our own attitude toward this character. Therefore, the attribution theory should be recognized as highly valuable in accounting for the EICs from television news stories. The most striking result in this study was finding that the hypotheses concerning the assimilation and contrast effects were unambiguously confirmed, but only in males. The effects were revealed when the subjects evaluated the intention of the character from the test news story both from their own and from the protagonist’s perspectives. To account for this finding, we should point to some diverse correction mechanism for men and women who are engaged in the process of priming and evaluating intentions of characters from consecutive news stories. Reports from numerous studies support the occurrence of gender differences in many spheres. Among other things, they suggest a marked difference between men and women in social sensitivity and empathy. Studies of Blum (1997) provided compelling evidence that women demonstrate a significantly better ability than men to decode non-verbal and affective information or to interpret visual and auditory clues about others (Blum 1997; Goleman 1997; Moir, Jessel, 1991). Women are more willing than men to listen to interlocutors and spontaneously adopt their point of view. However, this does not mean that men are deprived of these kinds of empathic skills. 23 Gender also determines the diverse thinking characteristics of men and women, and the schemata of information processing associated with them. Activated schemata influence interpretation of events, especially in ambiguous situations or unequivocal interpretations (e.g., the EIC from the test news story). In this case, gender differences affect the process of causal attribution. Convincing reports have suggested that this process is much more complex in women than in men (Heider 1958). It is probable that while evaluating the intention of a character from a news story, women concentrate on the process of monitoring numerous social contexts in which they perceive another person and, in this way, their resistance to effects of priming increases. There is ample evidence confirming the occurrence of gender-related communication problems, the source of which is diversity in the ways of “reading” the other’s intentions. In the context of the issues of automatic processes and priming of intentions, the result obtained in our study provides inspiration for further scientific investigations. From a practical point of view, the conclusions from this study can be a valuable source of knowledge for authors of informational programs on public television. They should be particularly aware of the effects brought about by the order in which they put together broadcast news stories. Another practical implication from this study is knowledge showing that women and men display a diverse manner of understanding others’ behaviors. This knowledge is very important in everyday life. In the process of improving our mutual communication, we should be aware of the existing differences between men and women with regard to perceiving the surrounding world, especially the intentions of other people’s behaviors. We should also realize that every day we meet people who are under the influence of the evaluations made during “just-passed” events and that they can transfer these evaluations onto subsequent events. Unfortunately, they often do it subconsciously and, what is worse, not always appropriately. Although not all predictions contained in the verified hypotheses were confirmed, they did put forward new questions and suggested an interesting research perspective. Communicating and understanding intentions of human behaviors still remains a puzzle, although it is very probable that the path suggested by this form of study is more and more heuristically fruitful: it brings more questions than answers. References 24 Abbott, V., Black, J., Smith, F. (1985). The representation of scripts in memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 4-19. Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bargh, J. A. (1990). Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of thought and behavior. In: E. T. Higgins, R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition, Vol. 2 (pp. 93-130). New York: Guilford. Bargh, J. A. (1996). Principles of automaticity. In: E. T. Higgins, A. Kruglansky (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 169-183). New York: Guilford. Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In: R. S. Wyer Jr. (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition, Vol. 10 (pp. 1-61). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Bartlett, F. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. London: Cambridge University Press. Black, J., Bower, G. (1980). Story understanding as problem solving. Poetics, 9, 223-250. Blum, D. (1997). Sex on the brain: The biological differences between men and women. New York: Viking. Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Collins, A. M., Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407-428. Goleman, D. (1997). Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ. New York: Bantam Books Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. Moir, A., Jessel, D. (1991). Brain sex: The real difference between men and women. New York: Carol Publishing Group. Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1988). How is meaning mentally represented? Versus. Quaderni di studi semiotici, 44/45, 99-118. Jones, E. E., Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. New York: General Learning Press. Kerr, N. L., MacCoun, R. J., Kramer, G. (1996). Bias in judgment: comparing individuals and groups. Psychological Review, 103, 687-719. Lichtenstein, H., Brewer, W. (1980). Memory for goal directed events. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 412-445. 25 Mandler, J. (1984). Stories, scripts and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA Publishers. Murphy, S. T., Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 723-739. Newell, A., Rosenbloom, P. S. (1981). Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice. In: J. R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 1-55). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Nisbett, R. E., Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings. New York: McGraw-Hill. Omanson, R. (1982). The relation between centrality and story category variability. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 12-23. Owens, J., Bower, G., Black, J. (1979). The „soap opera” effect in story memory. Memory and Cognition, 7, 31-45. Sarbin, T. (Ed.) (1986). Narrative psychology. The storied nature of human conduct. New York: Praeger Press. Schwarz, N. (1995). Social cognition: Information accessibility and use in social judgments. In: E. E. Smith, D. N. Osherson (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science Vol. 3 (pp. 345-376). Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. Schwarz, N., Bless, H. (1992). Constructing reality and its alternatives: An inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation and constant effects in social judgments. In: L. L. Martin. A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 217-245). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Smith, E. R. (1994). Procedural knowledge and processing strategies in social cognition. In: R. S. Wyer, T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition Vol. 1 (pp. 99-152). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Steyvers, M., Tenenbaum, J. B. (2003). The large-scale structure of semantic networks: statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science, 29, 41-78. Van Dijk, Teun A. (Ed.) (1985). Discourse and communication: New approaches to the analysis of mass media discourse and communication. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E. (1997). The flexible correction model: The role of naive theories of bias in bias correction. In: M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology Vol. 29 (pp. 141-208). San Diego: Academic Press. 26 Wilson, T. D., Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluation. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 117-142. Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Feeling and thinking: Closing the debate over the independence of affect. In: J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 31-58). New York: Cambridge University Press. Zwaan, R., Langston, M., Graesser, A. (1995). The construction of situation models in narrative comprehension: An event-indexing model. Psychological Science, 6, 292297. Inquiries should be sent to the first author, Piotr Francuz at francuz@kul.pl