Individual Differences in Attachment This activity will help you to: Understand and recall ways in which attachments differ based on the work of Ainsworth and Bell Understand and recall the research on which this knowledge is based Develop evaluation skills Individual Differences in Attachment When you read psychological theories it would seem that everyone is the same. However, if you look around, it’s fairly obvious that everyone is very different. In the context of research for example, Schaffer and Emerson found that some babies like cuddling whereas others avoid physical contact. Also, in the same study, they found that after 18months, some infants (13%) only attached to one person, whereas others (31%) had made multiple attachments to 5 or more people. These are INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES. All normal children form an attachment to a caregiver (even those which have been abused or neglected demonstrate attachment behaviour), however, the main individual differences lie in the degree to which a child is attached. Mary Ainsworth devised an assessment technique called the Strange Situation Classification in order to investigate how attachments may vary between children. What you need to do… Working individually or in pairs, use your textbook or any other sources of information available fill in the APFCE of Ainsworth and Bell’s (1970) Strange Situation Study: Aims Procedures To investigate individual variation in infant attachments, and in particular differences between secure and insecure attachments. Hoped the strange situation test would be a valid and reliable measure of attachments. The process was tested on infants aged 12-18 months in America. Lasts for just over 20 mins in a laboratory, using controlled observation. Consists of eight episodes/stages: STAGE PEOPLE PRESENT PROCEDURE 1 (30 sec) Mother, infant, researcher Researcher introduces mother and infant to room and leaves 2 (3 mins) Mother, infant Mother passive while baby explores 3 (3 mins) Mother, infant, stranger Stranger enters and after a while talks to mother and then infant. Mother leaves 4 (3 mins) Stranger, infant Stranger talks and plays with infant 5 (3 mins) Mother, infant Stranger leaves, mother returns to comfort infant and then leaves 6 (3 mins) Infant Infant is alone in the room 7 (3 mins) Stranger, infant Stranger returns and tries to interact with infant 8 (3 mins) Mother, infant Mother returns and interacts with infant, and stranger leaves Findings Infants explored the room more enthusiastically when mother present than when stranger was present. Considerable individual differences in reunion behaviour: 15% - anxious-avoidant – Ignored mother, showing indifference. Few signs of stress when mother left room. Responded to stranger in similar way as to mother. 70% - securely attached – Played happily when mother was present. Became upset when mother left, and play was seriously disrupted. Wanted immediate comfort when mother returned. Treated mother and stranger very differently. 15% - anxious-resistant – Fussy and wary even when mother present. Became distressed when mother left, and wanted immediate comfort when she returned. However, they also showed anger towards mother and resisted contact. Conclusions The strange situation regarded as a good measure of individual differences in attachment. Secure attachment is the preferred and most popular attachment type. Study showed how sensitivity of mother to infant played a part in type of attachment the infant displayed. Mothers who displayed sensitivity towards their infant were most likely to have a securely attached infant; and mothers who displayed insensitivity towards their infant were most likely to experience an insecure attachment with their infant. Criticisms The strange situation procedure has been widely used in attachment research with infants, and has been adapted for studies of children and even adults Validity concerns the extent to which something is true. What does the Strange situation actually measure? Is it something about the infant (child is a secure or insecure type) or is it one particular r’ship? It has been found by Main & Weston (1981) that children behaved differently depending on which parent they were tested with, which indicates that it is the r’ship that determines the response to the S.S rather than attachment being due to the infant’s temperament. = classification of an attachment type may not be valid as what is being measured is 1 r’ship rather than something within the individual If you agree with idea of monotropy, the 1 r’ship the infant has with it’s primary caregiver is all that matters for emotional dev’t. The fact that an infant responds differently with someone other than the primary caregiver tells us something about that r’ship, but the attachment type is related to the 1 special r’ship Validity of classification - questioned by Main and Solomon (1986) who put forward the fourth type of disorganised attachment. This is where no consistent behaviour was shown by the infant. Could be seen as ethnocentric – culturally biased – because it was carried out in America. Assumes that norms/values/behaviour is the same across all cultures. Lack of external validity – artificial situation (in a lab) which may distort behaviour. Ethical considerations – Is this method of deliberately exposing children to stress unethical? Does the end justify the means? Questions 1) What are the strengths of using the experimental approach used by Ainsworth and Bell? Controlled, lacks bias, replicable 2) What factors determine infant’s attachment style Attachments Secure (70%) Type B Children given a positive working model Carer who is emotionally available, sensitive and supportive Avoidant (20%) Type A Resistant (10%) Type C Children have a working model of themselves as unacceptable and unworthy Children have a negative welf-image and exaggerate their emotional responses to gain attention Carer who is rejecting Carer who is inconsistent