Alcohol and Development in Youth— A Multidisciplinary Overview Introduction Underage drinking is a complex problem that has plagued society for generations. As the lead Federal agency for supporting and conducting basic and applied research on alcohol problems, the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) has long been at the forefront of efforts to address the broad spectrum of issues related to drinking by youth through projects ranging from studies of alcohol consumption among adolescents to trials of ways to prevent and treat underage drinking. Over the past decade, NIAAA’s research investment in underage drinking has increased steadily, especially during the 5-year National Institutes of Health budget-doubling period that began in 1998. Increased funding during this period allowed NIAAA to support additional studies and undertake its 1998 college drinking initiative, a collaborative enterprise involving researchers and college presidents. The college drinking initiative advanced our understanding of drinking by college students, particularly the heavy episodic alcohol consumption commonly called “binge” drinking, and is leading to ways to treat and prevent alcohol-related problems among college students. It also underscored the fact that although some students begin drinking in college, most begin drinking much earlier—in high school, middle school, and even elementary school. Previously, data from NIAAA’s National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey (NLAES) had shown that alcohol use early in life correlates strongly with the development of alcohol dependence later in life. In March 2002, NIAAA partnered with the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation to launch the Leadership to Keep Children Alcohol Free Initiative. Spearheaded by Governors’ spouses, this initiative focuses on increasing public awareness of alcohol consumption among children ages 9 to 15 and ensuring that policymakers remain fully informed of the seriousness of the childhood drinking issue. The convergence of a number of recent public events and new scientific developments makes this a particularly opportune and appropriate time to intensify our research, evaluation, and outreach efforts to confront this critical public health issue. First, new findings from NIAAAsupported research indicate that the kind of serious drinking problems previously associated with middle adulthood (including what has been called alcoholism) often begin to emerge during adolescence and young adulthood. These findings, and other results that have enhanced our understanding of alcohol consumption during adolescence, have led to a reconceptualization of alcohol dependence. We now believe that our science will advance optimally by investigating alcohol-related problems in a developmental context. In fact, alcohol abuse and dependence are probably best characterized as developmental disorders, with sequelae that play out throughout the life span. Furthermore, neurobiological research suggests that adolescence may be a period of particular vulnerability to the effects of alcohol. In addition, the recent National Research Council and Institute of Medicine report, Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility, underscores the dangers of underage drinking, even when the level of drinking falls short of a diagnosable condition. This report also proposes a strategy to begin to address this issue. Although we clearly must act now, it also is clear that our currently limited understanding of how alcohol influences adolescent development will limit the success of prevention and intervention efforts. For example, adolescents comprise a very diverse population—a 13-year-old and a 17-year-old differ in many ways, both physically and psychologically. Thus, a single approach for preventing underage drinking will likely be less effective than multiple, developmentally appropriate approaches, even if the primary means for reaching all adolescents is via their parents. To truly understand the risk and protective factors for, and consequences of, alcohol consumption during the first decades of life, we must study alcohol consumption as a developmental phenomenon that begins in childhood and continues through adolescence and into young adulthood. This issue of Alcohol Research & Health is a first step in NIAAA’s efforts to bring the developmental perspective to bear upon the problem of underage drinking. In this special issue, we review the many domains that have been shown by research to interface with alcohol consumption by youth and evaluate the latest research findings in each of these domains. Because a developmental perspective has yet to be fully integrated with research on alcohol and youth, any present review of existing research necessarily must remain limited in this regard. For this reason, since May 2004, NIAAA has been consulting with experts in biological and behavioral development to attempt a significant advance in understanding the phenomenon of underage drinking. To this end, scientists from NIAAA’s staff currently are at work with these developmental scientists to produce a “next generation” report that will explicitly address how developmental science can inform both (a) understanding of the origins of underage drinking and (b) strategic planning to reduce the adverse outcomes suffered by our most precious resource, our children. This new report will, for the first time, explicitly consider the issues involved with underage drinking as a function of age and developmental stage. We anticipate this followup report to be available in the spring of 2006. In closing, we wish to enthusiastically acknowledge the 2004 report from the National Research Council and the Institute of Medicine (IOM), Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. There are many areas of overlap between the articles in this issue of Alcohol Research & Health and the IOM report, and some areas of divergence. In part because of the availability of the earlier report and its extensive accompanying background material, the articles included here could be limited to brief tutorials in many relevant areas of science, and could build upon the earlier contributions by increasing coverage of biological and other laboratory findings. We hope that this summary of recent scientific findings will help researchers and policymakers move forward expeditiously to address the critical problem of underage drinking. Vivian B. Faden, Ph.D. Mark Goldman, Ph.D. Co-Leaders, NIAAA Interdisciplinary Team on Underage Drinking Research October 2005 Acknowledgements Outside Advisors Adrian Angold, M.R.C. Psych Duke University Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Box 3454, Brightleaf Square, Suite 22 Durham, NC 22710–3454 Professor Richard Bonnie John S. Battle Professor of Law Director, University of Virginia Institute of Law, Psychiatry and Public Policy University of Virginia School of Law P.O. Box 400405 Charlottesville, VA 22904–4405 Jane Brown, Ph.D. James L. Knight Professor Journalism/Mass Communications 360 Carroll Hall University of North Carolina Chapel Hill, NC 27514 Sandra Brown, Ph.D. Professor of Psychology and Psychiatry Department of Psychology, 0109 University of California, San Diego 9500 Gilman Drive La Jolla, CA 92093–0109 Ronald E. Dahl, M.D. Staunton Professor of Psychiatry and Pediatrics Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic University of Pittsburgh 3811 O’Hara Street Pittsburgh, PA 15213 Thomas J. Dishion, Ph.D. Founder and Director of Research Child and Family Center University of Oregon 195 North 12th Avenue Eugene, OR 97401 John E. Donovan, Ph.D. Department of Psychiatry Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine 3811 O’Hara Street Pittsburgh, PA 15213 Cindy L. Ehlers, Ph.D. Department of Neuropharmacology The Scripps Research Institute 10550 N. Torrey Pines Road CVN–14 La Jolla, CA 92037 Mrs. Kendel Ehrlich First Lady of Maryland Office of the First Lady State House 100 State Circle Annapolis, MD 21401–1925 Ms. Mimi Fleury Community of Concern c/o Georgetown Preparatory School 10900 Rockville Pike North Bethesda, MD 20852 Mrs. Nancy Freudenthal First Lady of Wyoming Office of the First Lady 5001 Central Avenue Cheyenne, WY 82009–4011 Jay Giedd, M.D. National Institutes of Health National Institute of Mental Health 9000 Rockville Pike, MSC 1367 Bethesda, MD 20892–1367 Jennifer Lerner, Ph.D. Department of Social and Decision Sciences Carnegie Mellon University 208 Porter Hall Pittsburgh, PA 15213 Christopher S. Martin, Ph.D. Department of Psychiatry Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine 3811 O’Hara Street Pittsburgh, PA 15213 Ann S. Masten, Ph.D. Director, Institute of Child Development University of Minnesota 51 East River Road Minneapolis, MN 55455–0345 Matthew K. McGue, Ph.D. Psychology Department N218 Elliott Hall University of Minnesota 75 East River Road Minneapolis, MN 55455–0344 Frank A. Middleton, Ph.D. Assistant Professor Departments of Psychiatry and Neuroscience and Physiology Director, Center for Neuropsychiatric Genetics Director, Microarray Core Facility State University of New York Upstate Medical University 750 East Adams Street Syracuse, NY 13210 Stacia A. Murphy President National Council on Alcoholism and Drug Dependence, Inc. 20 Exchange Place, Suite 2902 New York, NY 10005 Daniel Pine, M.D. Chief, Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience and Chief of Child and Adolescent Research Mood and Anxiety Disorders Program National Institute of Mental Health Building 15K, Room 110 Bethesda, MD 20892 John E. Schulenberg, Ph.D. University of Michigan Institute for Social Research P.O. Box 1248 Room 2138 Ann Arbor, MI 48106–1248 Kenneth J. Sher, Ph.D. Professor Department of Psychological Studies University of Missouri 200 South Seventh Street Columbia, MO 65211–0001 Linda Spear, Ph.D. Distinguished Professor of Psychology Chair of Department of Psychology Binghamton University State University of New York P.O. Box 6000 Binghamton, NY 13902–6000 H. Scott Swartzwelder, Ph.D. Professor of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences Senior VA Research Career Scientist Building 16, Room 24 VA Medical Center 508 Fulton Street Durham, NC 27705 Susan F. Tapert, Ph.D. Department of Psychiatry University of California, San Diego VA San Diego Healthcare System 3350 La Jolla Village Drive, 116B San Diego, CA 92161 Michael Windle, Ph.D. Professor of Psychology University of Alabama-Birmingham Center for the Advancement of Youth Health 912 Building, 1530 3rd Avenue, South Birmingham, AL 35294–1200 Robert A. Zucker, Ph.D. Professor of Psychiatry and Psychology Addiction Research Center University of Michigan 400 East Eisenhower Pkwy., Suite 2A Ann Arbor, MI 48108–3318 NIAAA Contributors Charlotte Armstrong Office of Research Translation and Communications Judith Arroyo, Ph.D. Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research Gregory Bloss, M.A. Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research John Bowersox Office of Research Translation and Communications Gayle Boyd, Ph.D. (now at Center for Scientific Review, NIH) Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research Vivian B. Faden, Ph.D. Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research Thomas Gentry, Ph.D. Division of Metabolism and Health Effects David Goldman, M.D. Division of Intramural Clinical and Biological Research Mark Goldman, Ph.D. Office of the Director Roger Hartman, M.L.S. Office of Research Translation and Communications Ralph Hingson, Sc.D., M.P.H. Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research Cherry Lowman, Ph.D. Division of Treatment and Recovery Research Howard Moss, M.D. Office of the Director Patricia Powell, Ph.D. Office of Scientific Affairs Gregory Roa Office of Research Translation and Communications Kathy Salaita, Ph.D. (now at Center for Scientific Review, NIH) Division of Epidemiology and Prevention Research Roger Sorensen, Ph.D. Division of Neuroscience and Behavior Ellen Witt, Ph.D. Division of Neuroscience and Behavior The Scope of the Problem Alcohol is the drug of choice among youth, often with devastating consequences. Alcohol is a leading contributor to injury death, the main cause of death for people under age 21. Drinking early in life also is associated with an increased risk of developing an alcohol use disorder at some time during the life span. Data consistently indicate that rates of drinking and alcohol-related problems are highest among White and American Indian or Alaska Native youth, followed by Hispanic youth, African Americans, and Asians. Prevalence rates of drinking for boys and girls are similar in the younger age groups; among older adolescents, however, more boys than girls engage in frequent and heavy drinking, and boys show higher rates of drinking problems. This article summarizes research on the epidemiology of youth drinking, including the consequences of youthful drinking, risk and protective factors and drinking trajectories, and information on special populations at particular risk for drinking-related problems. Key words: underage drinking; adolescent; survey; AODU (alcohol and other drug use) pattern; binge drinking; AOD (alcohol and other drug) induced risk; risk and protective factors; alcohol and other drug related (AODR) consequences; AODR injury; epidemiology; special populations; children of alcoholics; undergraduate student; military; ethnic group; gender differences OVERVIEW National surveys make it clear that alcohol drinking among youth is both widespread and harmful. Surveys provide data not only on the numbers of middle and high school students who drink but also on how they drink. The data show that when youth drink, they drink heavily in comparison with adults, consuming on average four to five drinks per occasion about five times a month, compared with two to three drinks per occasion about nine times a month for adults. Studies also find that drinking often begins at very young ages; a recent survey found that more than onefourth of 14-year-olds reported drinking within the last year. The negative consequences of underage drinking include a range of physical, academic, and social problems. Perhaps most frightening, alcohol is the leading contributor to injury death, the main cause of death for people under age 21. However, alcohol also plays a powerful role in risky sexual behavior, including unwanted, unintended, and unprotected sexual activity, and sex with multiple partners. Alcohol is associated with academic failure and drug use. Over the longer term, data have shown that drinking early in life is associated with an increased risk of developing an alcohol use disorder at some time during the life span. Although almost all U.S. youth grow up in a culture permeated by alcohol, they are not uniformly at risk for alcohol consumption or its consequences. Epidemiology provides clues to risk and protective factors associated with youth drinking, including family history and genetic vulnerability, comorbid conditions, sociodemographic characteristics, social stressors such as poverty and lack of social support, family characteristics, alcohol availability, temperament, and other individual factors. Epidemiology also provides a profile of how specific populations of young people differ in their drinking patterns. Drinking, including heavy drinking, is common and accepted among college students, with consequences affecting both those who do the drinking and those who do not. Rates of heavy drinking among 18- to 25-year-olds in the military are much higher than among civilians. There is considerable variation between Whites and other ethnic/racial minority youth with respect to drinking, but also significant variation within these populations. Research is needed to determine how national origin, tribal affiliation, acculturation, immigration status, and language all influence drinking patterns among youth. EPIDEMIOLOGY OF UNDERAGE DRINKING Alcohol is the drug of choice among youth. Young people drink too much and at too early an age, thereby creating problems for themselves, for people around them, and for society as a whole. Hence, underage drinking is a leading public health problem in this country. Prevalence and Age of Initiation Nationwide surveys, as well as studies in smaller populations, show that alcohol drinking is widespread among adolescents. For example, 2004 data from Monitoring the Future (MTF), an annual survey of U.S. youth, show that more than three-fourths of 12th graders, nearly two-thirds of 10th graders, and more than two in five 8th graders have consumed alcohol at some point in their lives (Monitoring the Future Web site). And when youth drink, they tend to drink heavily. Underage drinkers consume on average four to five drinks per occasion about five times a month (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration [SAMHSA] 2003). By comparison, adult drinkers ages 26 and older consume on average two to three drinks per occasion about nine times a month. A particularly worrisome aspect of underage drinking is the high prevalence of heavy episodic drinking, defined as drinking five or more drinks in a row in the past 2 weeks. MTF data show that 12 percent of 8th graders, 22 percent of 10th graders, and 28 percent of 12th graders engage in heavy episodic drinking (Johnston et al. 2004). It should come as no surprise, then, that about three-fifths of 12th graders, two-fifths of 10th graders, and one-fifth of 8th graders say they have been drunk (Monitoring the Future Web site). In fact, the highest prevalence of dependence is seen in people ages 18–24. Figure 1 Nationwide surveys, as well as studies in smaller populations, show that drinking is widespread among people under age 21. Studies also indicate that drinking often begins at very young ages. Data from recent surveys show that approximately 10 percent of 9- to 10-year-olds have already started drinking (Donovan et al. 2004), nearly a third of youth begin drinking before age 13 (Grunbaum et al. 2004), and more than one-fourth of 14-year-olds report drinking within the past year (SAMHSA 2003). Other researchers have documented that drinking becomes increasingly common through the teenage years (e.g., O’Malley et al. 1998). In addition, a number of studies have documented that the early onset of alcohol use (usually set at age 13 and younger) as well as the escalation of drinking in adolescence are both risk factors for the development of alcohol-related problems in adulthood (e.g., Gruber et al. 1996; Grant and Dawson 1998; Hawkins et al. 1997; Schulenberg et al. 1996a). These findings clearly are cause for concern, as are recent data suggesting that the age of first use of alcohol is declining (SAMHSA, National Household Survey on Drug Abuse [NHSDA] for years prior to 2000). These data indicate that the average age of first use among young people of all ages was about 16 in 1999, compared with about 17 1/2 in 1965 (SAMHSA 2003). Looking at underage drinkers only, 12- to 18-year-olds who report drinking report that they began doing so between 2 and 3 years earlier, when they were about 9 to 15, respectively (SAMHSA 2003). This is important because, as already noted, initiating alcohol consumption earlier in adolescence or in childhood is a marker for later problems, including heavier use of alcohol and other drugs during adolescence (e.g., Robins and Przybeck 1985; Hawkins et al. 1997) and meeting criteria for an alcohol dependence diagnosis in adulthood (Grant and Dawson 1998). Most of what we know about underage drinking derives from studies of youth ages 12 to 21. To address alcohol- related problems as developmental phenomena, we will need to understand more about what happens before age 12 with regard to alcohol consumption, alcohol awareness, and alcohol expectancies among children who have started to drink and among those who have not. A recent Medline search found a dearth of studies addressing drinking by younger children, and the few existing studies that turned up in this search were conducted among non-U.S. populations. Two national data sets, however, address alcohol use by children in sixth grade or below (typically age 12 and younger), albeit imperfectly and far from comprehensively. One is the Partnership Attitude Tracking Study (PATS), carried out for the Partnership for a Drug-Free America in 1993, and annually from 1995 through 1999. The other is the collection of PRIDE surveys carried out during the academic years 1997–1998 through 2001–2002. PATS data reveal a tripling of alcohol experience between fourth and sixth grade: 9.8 percent of fourth graders, 16.1 percent of fifth graders, and 29.4 percent of sixth graders report trying more than a sip of alcohol (Donovan et al. 2004). PRIDE data show similar rates of use in this population. Despite methodological problems with these data sets, PATS and PRIDE show that a nontrivial level of alcohol consumption occurs among a significant proportion of the 12-and-under population. Consequences Underage drinking can result in a range of adverse short-term and long-term consequences, including: Academic problems Social problems Physical problems such as hangovers or medical illnesses Unwanted, unintended, and unprotected sexual activity Physical and sexual assault Memory problems Increased risk for suicide and homicide Alcohol-related car crashes and other unintentional injuries such as burns, falls, and drownings Death from alcohol poisoning Alterations in brain development that may have consequences reaching far beyond adolescence. Alcohol is a leading contributor to injury death, the main cause of death for people under age 21. Annually, about 5,000 youth under age 21 die from alcohol-related injuries that involve underage drinking. This includes injuries sustained in motor vehicle crashes (about 1,900), homicides (about 1,600), and suicides (about 300), as well as unintentional injuries not related to motor vehicle crashes (National Highway Traffic Safety Administration [NHTSA] 2003; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC] 2004; Smith et al. 1999; Levy et al. 1999; Hingson and Kenkel 2004). Furthermore, the role of alcohol in both fatalities and injuries may be significantly underreported, in part because in many States, alcohol involvement in an injury relieves insurance providers of liability for medical expenses, so health care providers may not ask victims about, or report, alcohol use. Numerous cases of alcohol poisoning, the result of the acute toxic effects of alcohol that can range from gastritis to severe gastrointestinal bleeding to respiratory arrest and death, have been reported in the news media. Although many of these tragedies occur on college campuses, especially striking was the recent report of two 11-year-old boys found dead of alcohol poisoning in a snowy field on the Flathead Indian Reservation in Montana, with blood alcohol concentration (BAC) levels of 0.20 percent and 0.50 percent. Although alcohol poisoning is by no means a major cause of death among youth, reports such as this underscore the tragic influence that hazardous drinking can wield over youth culture. In the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey (NLAES) of people ages 18 and older in the United States, people who reported starting to drink before the age of 15 were four times more likely to also report meeting the criteria for dependence at some point in their lives (Grant and Dawson 1998). This survey also shows that children who drink at age 14 or younger are much more likely during their lifetimes to sustain unintentional injuries, to get into physical fights, and to become involved in motor vehicle crashes after drinking (Hingson et al. 2000, 2001, 2002). TEXT BOX Consequences of Underage Drinking: Mortality From Alcohol-Related Injuries Annually, about 5,000 people under age 21 die from alcohol-related injuries involving underage drinking, including: Motor vehicle crashes – 1,900 Homicides – 1,600 Suicides – 300 SOURCE: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration 2003; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2004; Smith et al. 1999; Levy et al. 1999; Hingson and Kenkel 2004. All statistics are approximate. END OF TEXT BOX Similarly, other survey data indicate that the younger children and adolescents are when they start to drink, the more likely they are to engage in behaviors that can harm themselves and others (Grunbaum et al. 2004). Those who start to drink before age 13, for example, are nine times more likely to binge1 drink frequently (five or more drinks on an occasion at least six times per month) as high school students than those who begin drinking later. ( 1 SAMHSA’s definitions of binge and heavy drinking: binge drinkers report that they had consumed five or more drinks on the same occasion at least once in the past 30 days; heavy drinkers report that they had consumed five or more drinks on the same occasion on at least 5 different days in the past 30 days (SAMHSA 2003). The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism’s (NIAAA’s) definition of binge drinking: a “binge” is a pattern of drinking alcohol that brings blood alcohol concentration (BAC) to 0.08 grams percent or above. For the typical adult, this pattern corresponds to consuming five or more drinks (men), or four or more drinks (women), in about 2 hours. Binge drinking is clearly dangerous for the drinker and for society. [The NIAAA National Advisory Council approved this definition of binge drinking on February 5, 2004.]) And compared with nondrinkers, a greater proportion of frequent binge drinkers (nearly 1 million high school students nationwide) engaged in other risky behavior in the past 30 days (Grunbaum et al. 2004), including carrying a gun (22 percent vs. 3 percent), using marijuana (73 percent vs. 7 percent), using cocaine (26 percent vs. 0 percent), and having sex with six or more partners (31 percent vs. 4 percent). In addition, these youth were more likely than abstainers to earn grades that are mostly Ds or Fs in school (15 percent vs. 5 percent), be injured in a fight (13 percent vs. 2 percent), or be injured in a suicide attempt (9 percent vs. 1 percent). The extent to which alcohol use per se makes these other outcomes more likely is yet to be determined. However, the longitudinal evidence is very strong that the risk factors predicting earlier alcohol use also are strong predictors of virtually all of these other consequences (Biglan et al. 2004; Caspi et al. 1997). Risk Trajectories and Drinking Trajectories Not only do youth begin drinking at different ages but their trajectories of risk also vary considerably, even before alcohol use has begun. Recent work following high-risk populations of children from preschool onward has shown major differences in the trajectories of externalizing and internalizing risk from preschool to early adolescence. These varied as a function of initial level of risk in early childhood, the child’s age, and the level of familial risk. Particularly for the externalizing trajectory, children who started out at very high levels of individual and familial risk became indistinguishable from those at lower levels during the early school years, but as these youth moved into early adolescence, the differences reemerged and became amplified, with the highest-risk children increasing the greatest amount (Zucker et al. 2003). Conversely, the externalizing behavior of children at the lowest level of initial risk who were exposed to the lowest level of familial risk changed the least, although even they increased in level of externalizing behavior as they moved into adolescence. Similarly, the drinking patterns and practices youth adopt as they grow into young adults—their drinking trajectories—also vary considerably once they start to drink. No single trajectory describes the course of alcohol use for all or even most young people. Research findings provide strong evidence for wide developmental variation in drinking patterns in the population. For example, Steinman and Schulenberg (2003) identified six common trajectories among early and middle adolescents: abstinence, rare use, high school onset, early but nonescalating use, early and gradually escalating use, and consistently high use. In another study, Schulenberg and colleagues (1996b) identified six trajectories of heavy drinking among young people ages 18 to 24: chronic heavy drinkers, decreased, increased, fling (i.e., low, high, low), rare, and never. In addition, alcohol abuse treatment (Chung et al. 2003) and other experiences may influence drinking trajectories. Studying the developmental trajectories of drinking behavior and how various risk and protective factors influence those trajectories is critical to understanding the complexity of underage drinking. Figure 2 Alcohol use by youth is an international phenomenon. The 2003 European School Survey Project on Alcohol and Other Drugs (ESPAD) surveyed 15-year-olds in 35 European countries where legal drinking ages are lower (typically ages 16–18) than in the United States. The ESPAD questions were similar to those used with 10th graders in the U.S. Monitoring the Future study. In all European countries except the predominantly Moslem nation of Turkey, a greater percentage of 15-yearolds drank alcohol than in the United States; and in more than three-quarters of the countries, a greater percentage reported drinking to intoxication in the previous year than in the United States. SOURCE: http://www.espad.org/reports.html SPECIAL POPULATIONS OF YOUNG PEOPLE Children of Alcoholics Children of alcoholics (COAs) are between 4 and 10 times as likely to become alcoholics themselves as children from families that have no adults with alcoholism (Russell 1990). COAs are at elevated risk for earlier onset of drinking (Donovan et al. 2004) and earlier progression into drinking problems (Grant and Dawson 1998). Some of the elevated risk is attributable to exposure and socialization effects found in alcoholic households, some to genetically transmitted differences in response to alcohol that make the drinking more pleasurable and/or less aversive, and some is attributable to elevated transmission of risky temperamental and behavioral traits that lead COAs, more than other youth, into increased contact with earlier-drinking and heavierdrinking peers. From a public health standpoint, according to NLAES data (Grant 2000), approximately 9.7 million children age 17 or younger, or 15 percent of the child population in that age range, were living in households with one or more adults classified as having an alcohol abuse or dependence diagnosis during the past year. Approximately 70 percent of these children were biological, foster, adopted, or stepchildren. That is, 6.8 million children meet the formal definition of COA, although not all are exposed to the same level of risk for use, problem use, and alcohol use disorder (AUD). Given that these figures concern past-year exposure to at least one alcoholic adult, from the perspective of socialization risk, they only reflect acute exposure. Other data from NLAES provide estimates of the number of children living in a household with an adult who had abused or been dependent on alcohol at some point; the figure is 43 percent of the under-18 population, or somewhat less than half of all children. Given the size of this group, any approach to risk identification will be extremely complex. A second important consideration is that COA status is heavily used as a proxy for “alcoholism risk” on the one hand and socialization risk on the other, but the COA designation more precisely is a proxy for multiple causal inputs, not all of which may be present in the individual case. Thus, being a COA implies elevated genetic risk, although the alcoholic genetic diatheses may not have been passed on to a particular child. One may be a COA without being undercontrolled, having an attention deficit hyperactivity disorder diagnosis, or other problems known to be associated with increased risk of alcohol dependence. Socialization risk involves exposure, but given the high divorce rates found in this population, evaluating the level of socialization risk is complex, involving both the quantification of the length of the exposure and the identification of the developmental period during which the socialization took place. Vulnerability is greater during some developmental periods than others (Fuller et al. 2003). In addition, a substantial amount of marital assortment occurs in alcoholic families (Schuckit et al. 2002). When assortment is present, risk exposure is multiplied, and COA effects become a function of genetic risk(s), individual parent risk, and the synergistic risk created by marital interaction (Fuller et al. 2003). SIDEBAR Risk and Protection Although almost all U.S. youth grow up in a culture permeated by alcohol, they are not uniformly at risk for alcohol consumption or its consequences. Much research has addressed the risk and protective factors associated with youth drinking. These factors include but are not limited to family history and genetic vulnerability, comorbid conditions and their developmental antecedents, sociodemographic characteristics, social stressors such as poverty and lack of social support, family characteristics, alcohol availability, temperament, and other individual factors. Some of the most consistently documented epidemiologic findings regarding the association of alcohol consumption and other factors are presented in the following brief overview. Data from general population surveys of youth, as well as data from smaller, more localized studies, consistently indicate that rates of drinking and alcohol-related problems are highest among White and American Indian or Alaska Native youth, followed by Hispanic youth, African Americans, and Asians. Likewise, studies uniformly indicate that alcohol consumption generally increases as a person’s age increases. Prevalence rates of drinking for boys and girls are similar in the younger age groups; among older adolescents, however, more boys than girls engage in frequent and heavy drinking, and boys show higher rates of drinking problems. Other common findings are a strong association between conduct problems and earlier alcohol consumption, and youth with a family history of alcohol problems are at much greater risk both for problem use and later alcohol use disorders. Studies also show that underage drinkers generally possess more than one risk factor and exhibit clusters of problem behaviors. The scientific literature on risk and protective factors for underage drinking reveals important conceptual as well as methodological issues. For example, many such risk factors have been identified solely on the basis of their association with drinking and its consequences. This association is not sufficient evidence, however, to prove that something actually increases risk for, or protection from, underage drinking. Some scientists, therefore, advocate a stricter definition of risk/protective factors (e.g., Donovan 2004). The term “risk factor,” for example, would apply only to variables for which there is a statistically significant link to the onset of adolescent alcohol use as well as evidence that any such variable was present prior to the onset of drinking (Donovan 2004). Furthermore, “alcohol consumption” encompasses not just one but numerous phenomena. Finally, it is important to be aware that many of the risk factors predicting early drinking are not drinking variables but instead are more nonspecific characteristics, such as externalizing and internalizing problems, that are identifiable much earlier than the first drinking experience but represent high-risk pathways into earlier use (NIAAA 2000; Zucker and Wong 2005). We therefore need to more precisely define those risk/protective factors that apply to the initiation of drinking, to the escalation of drinking, to risky drinking, and to other aspects of consumption. References Donovan, J.E. Adolescent alcohol initiation: A review of psychosocial risk factors. Journal of Adolescent Health 35:529.e7–18, 2004. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). Alcohol involvement over the life course. In: Tenth Special Report to the U.S. Congress on Alcohol and Health: Highlights From Current Research. Bethesda, MD: Dept. of Health and Human Services, NIAAA, 2000. pp. 28–53. Available online at: http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/10report/intro.pdf. Zucker, R.A., and Wong, M.M. Prevention for children of alcoholics and other high risk groups. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 299–320. PMID: 15789872 END OF SIDEBAR Third, the potential for indirect socialization effects also is higher in COAs than in other children. Parental psychopathology has been documented as a risk factor for poorer parental monitoring (Chilcoat et al. 1996), which in turn leads to a higher probability of involvement with a deviant peer group, including earlier exposure to alcohol and other drugs. Fourth, COA risk is not simply risk for the development of AUD (Zucker and Wong 2005). Given what is known about the elevated comorbidities found among offspring of alcoholics, this designator also is a marker of elevated risk for behavioral and cognitive deficits. These include attention deficit disorder, behavioral undercontrol/conduct disorder, delinquency, lower IQ, poor school performance, low self-esteem, and other problems (Noll et al. 1992; Nigg et al. 1998; Poon et al. 2000; Sher 1991; West and Prinz 1987). Furthermore, the evidence strongly implicates some of these non-alcohol-specific characteristics as causal to both problem alcohol use and elevated risk for AUD (Caspi et al. 1997; Donovan and Jessor 1985; Nigg et al. 1998). These factors implicate the COA population as an important component of the underage drinking population. For the same reasons, however, it is essential to determine which components of that risk composite are the strongest mediators of the underage drinking outcome. College Students College students are a highly visible group of underage drinkers among whom alcohol consumption is commonplace. Indeed, many college students accept alcohol use as a normal part of student life. Studies consistently indicate that about four in five college students drink alcohol; about two in five engage in episodic heavy consumption, often called bingeing (five or more drinks in a row for men and four or more in a row for women; generally asked with respect to the past 2 weeks or past 30 days, depending on the survey); and about one in five engages in frequent episodic heavy consumption (bingeing three or more times in the past 2 weeks) (NIAAA 2002). The consequences of drinking among college students include academic problems, social problems, legal problems, involvement in physical and/or sexual assault or risky sex, and even death. An estimated 1,700 college students between the ages of 18 and 24 die each year from alcohol-related unintentional injuries including motor vehicle crashes (Hingson et al. 2005). Another 599,000 students are unintentionally injured while under the influence of alcohol, 696,000 are assaulted by other students who have been drinking, and 97,000 are victims of alcoholrelated sexual assault or date rape (Hingson et al. 2005). A striking number of college students also report having experienced alcohol-induced memory blackouts. One recent study indicated that among nonabstaining college students, 40 percent reported experiencing a blackout within the past year, and 9.4 percent reported having a blackout within the past 2 weeks (White et al. 2002). This could relate to students’ tendency to be unaware of standard drink volumes and to overpour drinks, thus underestimating their consumption (White et al. 2003). It is not known if younger drinkers are more susceptible to the memory-impairing effects of alcohol, but one study in humans showed that a dose of alcohol resulting in a BAC in the range of 80 mg/dl significantly disrupted learning in people in their early twenties but had little effect on people in their late twenties (Acheson et al. 1998). Drinking in college varies from campus to campus and from person to person. Levels and patterns of consumption are associated with individual, intracampus, and intercampus factors. For example, athletes and members of fraternities and sororities are among the heaviest drinkers on most campuses, and students in the Northeast and on campuses where athletics and Greek organizations are prominent tend to drink more than their counterparts at other institutions (NIAAA 2002). Underage and Youthful Drinking Among Military Personnel The Department of Defense (DOD) conducts periodic surveys to assess alcohol use and other health-related behaviors among military personnel. Approximately 193,000 of 1.4 million active duty military personnel are between the ages of 17 and 20. These surveys, therefore, provide important information about underage drinking in an important subset of young people. According to the 2002 DOD survey (the most recent one for which results have been released): 33.3 percent of military personnel age 20 and younger are “abstainers” (drink once a year or less). 15.7 percent are “infrequent/light” drinkers (one to four drinks per typical occasion, one to three times per month). 10.4 percent are “moderate” drinkers (one drink per typical drinking occasion at least once a week, or two to four drinks per typical drinking occasion two to three times per month, or five or more drinks per typical drinking occasion once a month or less). 14.4 percent are “moderate/heavy” drinkers (two to four drinks per typical drinking occasion at least once a week or five or more drinks per typical drinking occasion two to three times per month). 26.1 percent are “heavy” drinkers (five or more drinks per typical drinking occasion at least once a week). A comparison of data from the 2002 DOD survey with data from the 2001 NHSDA (in which heavy alcohol use is defined as five or more drinks on one occasion on 5 or more days in the past 30 days) indicates that rates of heavy drinking among 18- to 25-year-olds in the military are higher than for civilians of the same age (32.2 percent vs. 17.8 percent for men and 8.1 percent vs. 5.5 percent for women). The surveys conducted by the DOD also indicate that substantial numbers of youth in the military experience negative consequences from drinking. The 2002 data show that, during the 12 months prior to the survey, more than one-fifth of the most junior enlisted personnel (who typically are between the ages of 17 and 20) experienced serious consequences as a result of drinking or a drinking-related illness, including military punishment, alcohol-related arrest, and the need for detoxification, and that more than one-fourth experienced a productivity loss because of alcohol use. DOD investigators classified more than one-fifth of survey participants as alcohol “dependent” based on the number of days during the previous 12 months that they reported (1) withdrawal symptoms, (2) inability to recall things that happened while drinking, (3) inability to stop drinking before becoming drunk, or (4) morning drinking. Minority Youth According to national surveys, there is considerable variation between Whites and ethnic/racial minorities with respect to alcohol consumption. Minority youth generally start drinking at older ages than their White non-Hispanic counterparts. A greater difference also exists in levels of drinking between male and female minority youth and a greater percentage of minority youth abstain or drink very little (Vega et al. 1993). Although a “typical” pattern of underage drinking could never be attributed to any specific minority group, it is useful to compare minority groups to identify potential risk and protective factors that may be operating to produce some of the observed differences in drinking practices. With a burgeoning minority population, it is also essential to better understand these factors to help design and implement the most effective prevention and intervention programs. Data from a recent nationwide survey reveal that about three-fourths of White, American Indian, and Hispanic high school seniors used alcohol in the past year. More than 6 percent of American Indian and 5 percent of Mexican and Cuban American seniors report daily drinking, compared with 1 percent to 3.8 percent for all other groups (Wallace et al. 2002). The limited data on Hispanic and American Indian adults suggest that, among those who drink, there is a tendency toward high average intake per drinking day. Youth survey data suggest that some students establish a pattern of heavy drinking by their senior year of high school. About 60 percent of African American and about 57 percent of Asian American high school seniors report having used alcohol in the past 12 months, and about 32.5 percent in both groups report having used it in the past 30 days. However, there appears to be a “cross-over” effect for African Americans. That is, even though they use less alcohol as youths than their non-Hispanic White counterparts, rates of heavy and problem drinking among African American adults, especially males, are higher than for non-Hispanic Whites. Studies of race and ethnicity should be conducted with sufficiently large and diverse samples to allow investigators to assess variations in drinking by national origin or tribal affiliation, acculturation, immigration status, and language. Significant variation exists among Hispanics and among American Indians. Recent evidence indicates that members of some American Indian groups are more likely to abstain than are people in the general U.S. population. Like their Mexican American counterparts, American Indian drinkers, however, consume more alcohol per drinking occasion (Beals et al. 2003). In addition, although Asian Americans often are considered the “model minority,” with low rates of alcohol use, most current literature does not include data from rapidly growing at-risk Asian groups such as Southeast Asians, Koreans, and Filipinos, or groups believed to have higher rates of alcohol use, such as Native Hawaiians and other Pacific Islanders (Zane and Sasao 1992). Although there is clear evidence of genetic variability in alcohol metabolism, we have yet to fully understand the interplay of genetic and environmental variables. For example, the inability to metabolize alcohol efficiently, deemed a protective factor in a subset of the Asian population because of the unpleasant effects of drinking, often results in facial flushing. Highlighting the complexity of the interplay between genetic and environmental variables is the observation that Asian American drinking often increases with level of acculturation in spite of the flushing response (Sue et al. 1979). REFERENCES Acheson, S.K.; Stein, R.M.; and Swartzwelder, H.S. Impairment of semantic and figural memory by acute ethanol: Age-dependent effects. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 22:1437–1442, 1998. PMID: 9802525 Beals, J.; Spicer, P.; Mitchell, C.M.; et al. and the AI-SUPERPFP Team. Racial disparities in alcohol use: Comparison of 2 American Indian reservation populations with national data. American Journal of Public Health 93:1683–1685, 2003. PMID: 14534221 Biglan, A.; Brennan, P.A.; Foster, S.L.; et al. Helping Adolescents at Risk: Prevention of Multiple Problem Behaviors. New York: Guilford, 2004. Caspi, A.; Begg, D.; Dickson, N.; et al. Personality differences predict health-risk behaviors in young adulthood: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73(5):1052–1063, 1997. PMID: 9364760 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) National Center for Injury Prevention and Control (NCIPC). Web-Based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting System (WISQARS), 2004. Available online at: http://www.cdc.gov/ncipc/wisqars/default.htm. Chilcoat, H.D.; Breslau, N.; and Anthony, J.C. Potential barriers to parent monitoring: Social disadvantage, marital status, and maternal psychiatric disorder. Journal of the American Medical Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 35:1673–1682, 1996. PMID: 8973075 Chung, T.; Martin, C.S.; Grella, C.E.; et al. Course of alcohol problems in treated adolescents. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27:253–261, 2003. PMID: 12605074 Donovan, J.E. Adolescent alcohol initiation: A review of psychosocial risk factors. Journal of Adolescent Health 35:529.e718, 2004. PMID: 15581536 Donovan, J.E., and Jessor, R. Structure of problem behavior in adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 53:890–904, 1985. PMID: 4086689 Donovan, J.E.; Leech, S.L.; Zucker, R.A.; et al. Really underage drinkers: Alcohol use among elementary students. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 28:341–349, 2004. PMID: 15112942 Fuller, B.E.; Chermack, S.T.; Cruise, K.A.; et al. Predictors of aggression across three generations among sons of alcoholics: Relationships involving grandparental and parental alcoholism, child aggression, marital aggression and parenting practices. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 64:472–483, 2003. PMID: 12921189 Grant, B.F. Estimates of U.S. children exposed to alcohol abuse and dependence in the family. American Journal of Public Health 90:112–115, 2000. PMID: 10630147 Grant, B.F., and Dawson, D.A. Age at onset of drug use and its association with DSM–IV drug abuse and dependence: Results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. Journal of Substance Abuse 10:163–173, 1998. PMID: 9854701 Gruber, E.; DiClemente, R.J.; Anderson, M.M.; and Lodico, M. Early drinking onset and its association with alcohol use and problem behavior in late adolescence. Preventive Medicine 25:293–300, 1996. PMID: 8781007 Grunbaum, J.A.; Kann, L.; Kinchen, S.; et al. Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2003. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report Surveillance Summary, May 21;53:1–96, 2004. Erratum in MMWR, 2004 June 25;53:536. Erratum MMWR Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 2005 June 24; 54(24):608. PMID: 15152182 Hawkins, J.D.; Graham, J.W.; Maguin, E.; et al. Exploring the effects of age of alcohol use initiation and psychosocial risk factors on subsequent alcohol misuse. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 58:280–290, 1997. PMID: 9130220 Hingson, R., and Kenkel, D. Social, health, and economic consequences of underage drinking. In: National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. pp. 351–382. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Hingson, R.W.; Heeren, T.; Jamanka, A.; and Howland, J. Age of drinking onset and unintentional injury involvement after drinking. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association 284:1527– 1533, 2000. PMID: 11000646 Hingson, R.; Heeren, T.; and Zakocs, R. Age of drinking onset and involvement in physical fights after drinking. Pediatrics 108:872–877, 2001. PMID: 11581438 Hingson, R.; Heeren, T.; Levenson, S.; et al. Age of drinking onset, driving after drinking, and involvement in alcohol-related motor vehicle crashes. Accident Analysis and Prevention 34:85– 92, 2002. PMID: 11789578 Hingson, R.W.; Heeren, T.; Winter, M.; and Wechsler, H. Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students age 18-24: Changes from 1998 to 2001. Annual Review of Public Health 26:259–279, 2005. PMID: 15760289 Johnston, L.D.; O’Malley, P.M.; and Bachman, J.G. Monitoring the Future, National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2002. Volume I: Secondary School Students. NIH Pub. No. 03–5375. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2003. Available online at: http://monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/monographs/vol1_2002.pdf . Johnston, L.D.; O’Malley, P.M.; Bachman, J.G.; and Schulenberg, J.E. Monitoring the Future, National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2003. Volume I: Secondary School Students. NIH Pub. No. 04–5507. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2004. Available online at: http://monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/monographs/vol1_2003.pdf . Johnston, L.D.; O’Malley, P.M.; Bachman, J.G.; and Schulenberg, J.E. Monitoring the Future, National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2004. Volume I: Secondary School Students. NIH Pub. No. 05–5727. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2004. Available online at: http://monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/monographs/vol1_2004.pdf. Levy, D.T.; Miller, T.R.; and Cox, K.C. Costs of Underage Drinking. Washington, DC: U.S. Dept. of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, 1999. Available online at: http://www.udetc.org/documents/costunderagedrinking.pdf. Monitoring the Future Web site. http://www.monitoringthefuture.org/data/data.html. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA). Traffic Safety Facts 2002: Alcohol. DOT Pub. No. HS–809–606. Washington, DC: NHTSA, National Center for Statistics & Analysis, 2003. Available online at: http://www-nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/pdf/nrd-30/NCSA/TSF2002/2002alcfacts.pdf. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). Drinking in the United States: Main Findings from the 1992 National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey (NLAES). NIH Pub. No. 99–3519. U.S. Alcohol Epidemiologic Data Reference Manual, Vol. 6. Rockville, MD: NIAAA, 1998. Available online at: http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/Nlaesdrm.pdf. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). Alcohol involvement over the life course. In: Tenth Special Report to the U.S. Congress on Alcohol and Health: Highlights From Current Research. Bethesda, MD: Dept. of Health and Human Services, NIAAA, 2000. pp. 28–53. Available online at: http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/10report/intro.pdf . National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). High-Risk Drinking in College: What We Know and What We Need To Learn. Final Report of the Panel on Contexts and Consequences, Task Force of the National Advisory Council on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Bethesda, MD: Dept. of Health and Human Services, 2002. Available online at: http://www.collegedrinkingprevention.gov/Reports/Panel01/Panel01_TOC.aspx. Nigg, J.T.; Hinshaw, S.P.; Carte, E.T.; and Treuting, J.J. Neuropsychological correlates of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Explainable by comorbid disruptive behavior or reading problems? Journal of Abnormal Psychology 107:468–480, 1998. PMID: 9715582 Noll, R.B.; Zucker, R.A.; Fitzgerald, H.E.; and Curtis, W.J. Cognitive and motoric functioning of sons of alcoholic fathers and controls: The early childhood years. Developmental Psychology 28:665–675, 1992. O’Malley, P.M.; Johnston, L.D.; and Bachman, J.G. Alcohol use among adolescents. Alcohol Health & Research World 22(2):85–93, 1998. PMID: 15706782 Poon, E.; Ellis, D.A.; Fitzgerald, H.E.; and Zucker, R.A. Intellectual, cognitive, and academic performance among sons of alcoholics, during the early school years: Differences related to subtypes of familial alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:1020–1027, 2000. PMID: 10924005 Robins, L.N., and Przybeck, T.R. Age of onset of drug use as factor in drug and other disorders. In: Jones, C.L., and Battjes, R.J., eds. Etiology of Drug Abuse: Implications for Prevention. NIDA Research Monograph No. 56. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse, 1985. pp. 178– 192. Available online at: http://www.drugabuse.gov/pdf/monographs/56.pdf. Russell, M. Prevalence of alcoholism among children of alcoholics. In: Windle, M., and Searles, J.S., eds. Children of Alcoholics: Critical Perspectives. New York: Guilford, 1990. pp. 9–38. Schuckit, M.A.; Smith, T.L.; Eng, M.Y.; and Kunovac, J. Women who marry men with alcohol-use disorders. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26:1336–1343, 2002. PMID: 12351927 Schulenberg, J.E.; Wadsworth, K.N.; O’Malley, P.M.; and Bachman, J.G. Adolescent risk factors for binge drinking during the transition to young adulthood: Variable- and pattern-centered approaches to change. Developmental Psychology 32:659–674, 1996a. Schulenberg J.; O’Malley, P.M.; Bachman, J.G.; et al. Getting drunk and growing up: Trajectories of frequent binge drinking during the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 57:289–304, 1996b. PMID: 8709588 Sher, K.J. Children of Alcoholics: A Critical Appraisal of Theory and Research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1991. Smith, G.S.; Branas, C.C.; and Miller, T.R. Fatal nontraffic injuries involving alcohol: A metaanalysis. Annals of Emergency Medicine 33:659–668, 1999. PMID: 10339681 Steinman, K.J., and Schulenberg, J. A pattern-centered approach to evaluating substance use prevention programs. In: Damon, W.; Peck, S.C.; and Roeser, R.W.; eds. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, Vol. 101: Person-Centered Approaches to Studying Development in Context. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 2003. pp. 87–98. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA). Results from the 2002 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings. NHSDA Series H–22, DHHS Pub. No. SMA 03–3836. Rockville, MD: SAMHSA, Office of Applied Studies, 2003. Available online at: http://www.oas.samhsa.gov/nhsda/2k2nsduh/Results/2k2Results.htm. Sue, S.; Zane, N.; and Ito, J. Alcohol drinking patterns among Asian and Caucasian Americans. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 10:41–56, 1979. Vega, W.A.; Zimmerman, R.S.; Warheit, G.J.; et al. Risk factors for early adolescent drug use in four ethnic and racial groups. American Journal of Public Health 83:185–189, 1993. PMID: 8427320 Wallace, J.M., Jr.; Bachman, J.G.; O’Malley, P.M.; et al. Tobacco, alcohol, and illicit drug use: Racial and ethnic differences among U.S. high school seniors. Public Health Reports 117(Suppl. 1):S67–S75, 2002. PMID: 12435829 West, M.O., and Prinz, R.J. Parental alcoholism and childhood psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin 102:204–218, 1987. PMID: 3310059 White, A.M.; Jamieson-Drake, D.W.; and Swartzwelder, H.S. Prevalence and correlates of alcohol-induced blackouts among college students: Results of an e-mail survey. Journal of American College Health 51:117–119; 122–131, 2002. PMID: 12638993 White, A.M.; Kraus, C.L.; McCracken, L.A.; and Swartzwelder, H.S. Do college students drink more than they think? Use of a free-pour paradigm to determine how college students define standard drinks. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27:1750–1756, 2003. PMID: 14634490 Zane, N., and Sasao, I. Research on drug abuse among Asian Pacific Americans. Drugs and Society 6:181–209, 1992. Zucker, R.A., and Wong, M.M. Prevention for children of alcoholics and other high-risk groups. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 299–320. PMID: 15789872 Zucker, R.A.; Wong, M.M.; Puttler, L.I.; and Fitzgerald, H.E. Resilience and vulnerability among sons of alcoholics: Relationship to developmental outcomes between early childhood and adolescence. In: Luthar, S.S., ed. Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2003. pp. 76–103. Developmental Issues in Underage Drinking Research To better understand underage drinking and how it can be prevented, research is being conducted in a wide variety of disciplines—focusing on aspects such as risk and protective factors, biological processes underlying human development, and the impact of socioenvironmental and pharmacologic influences on these mechanisms. This article examines underage drinking from a developmental perspective, which seeks to identify critical developmental periods during which interventions may be especially useful. These critical periods can provide key opportunities to redirect the course of development and alter the life course trajectory of the individual. Key words: underage drinking; adolescence; growth and development; biological maturation; psychological development; brain; cognitive development; risk factors; protective factors; social adjustment; peer group; gender differences; intervention; prevention; statistical modeling OVERVIEW A mix of many different kinds of factors underlies the development of problem drinking in adolescents. For this reason, research focusing on any one area is likely to miss the complex interactions that shape how an adolescent will respond to the availability of alcohol. Research that takes a developmental perspective seeks to provide an understanding of behavior in the context of the changes that take place during human maturation. The developmental perspective assumes interactions that not only are complex but that change over time. The speed and timing of development are not uniform. The biologically based ability of a person to regulate mood as well as outwardly directed behavior, for example, changes during adolescence as the brain matures. The progress of these changes can affect how well an adolescent handles the tasks of adolescence—achieving autonomy and taking on more adult roles—or whether problems arise from a mismatch of development and social pressures. Girls and boys differ not only in the pace of physical maturation but also in how they respond to the resulting social experiences for which physical changes serve as stimuli. In ways that are different for boys and girls, the attachments children form with peers or older teens can influence their risk of involvement in potentially harmful behavior. One of the challenges of this research is to develop theories that then can be tested using statistical models to determine how well they predict how the complex array of social, cultural, environmental, and biological factors interact to increase or reduce the risk of underage drinking. THE DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVE In the effort to understand underage drinking, research in many disciplines has contributed valuable information on risk and protective factors, biological processes underlying human development, and the impact of socioenvironmental and pharmacologic influences on these mechanisms. To date, much of this diverse work has been aligned with specific disciplinary paradigms. Problematic involvement with alcohol is multicausal, however; studies conducted within any single research discipline lack the breadth required for a comprehensive approach to the elucidation of risk and protective factors and to develop improved interventions. Genetic and socioenvironmental factors act together through biological mechanisms to generate the complexities of behavior, including early-age problematic involvement with alcohol. The developmental perspective is a life course approach to understanding behavioral problems such as underage drinking and its consequences. This perspective has evolved from relatively recent advances in the fields of developmental psychopathology, human brain development, and behavior genetics. It is set in the context of the chronology of human maturation and the multiple social and cultural systems that interact with the developing human. Like systems biology, it posits complex multidirectional and reciprocal interactions that change over time. Viewed in this way, development encompasses not only the roots of risk and resilience in maturational pathways and developmental stages but also the modulation of behavior by present circumstances (Sroufe and Rutter 1984). The developmental perspective can inform the development of strategies and opportunities to prevent adverse, health- compromising drinking outcomes. It also can shape the content and process of therapeutic interventions. CHARACTERISTICS OF DEVELOPMENTAL RESEARCH Developmental research is, by nature, longitudinal. There are multiple possible starting points and developmental pathways to a problematic or positive adolescent outcome. Young people who are vulnerable as preadolescents can acquire positive, health-promoting low-risk behaviors upon reaching adolescence. Others who are low risk as preadolescents can have substantial problematic involvement with alcohol in later adolescence. Cross-sectional strategies may not reveal the complex interactions of multiple causal factors with biological, cognitive, affective, psychological, and social maturational milestones in determining relevant positive and negative outcomes. This is particularly true given the variation within the population in the timing and achievement of these milestones. A key assumption underlying developmental research is that, although maturation is progressive, it is not uniform in speed or timing. There are periods of rapid transition, reorganization, and spurts of growth (i.e., saltation), alternating with periods of quiescence and consolidation (i.e., stasis). Rapid transitions may be critical developmental periods during which the social or cultural environment most strongly influences the biology and behavior of the developing human, leading to either an adverse or positive outcome (reviewed in Greenough et al. 1987; Cicchetti and Tucker 1994). Critical developmental periods may provide key opportunities to redirect the course of development and alter the life course trajectory of the person (Masten 2004). Timing healthpromoting interventions in terms of critical developmental transitions could enhance efficacy. SOCIAL CONTEXT Social context seems to be particularly important in understanding and modifying human developmental trajectories. During adolescence, parental influences continue to be important, but there is a progressive increase in the influence of peers. Perhaps reflective of this altered balance, Brook and colleagues (1990) noted that family-directed alcohol and other drug abuse prevention efforts are generally more effective for children, whereas interventions involving peers are more effective for adolescents. SELF-REGULATION Self-regulation refers to the organism’s ability to monitor and modulate internal states. In humans, it includes both the ability to modulate affect and level of arousal and the neurocognitive executive capacities to engage in goal-directed behavior. These executive cognitive capacities include the regulation of attention, planning, organization, concept formation, abstract reasoning, cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring, motor programming, and motor control (Stuss and Benson 1984). Self-regulatory behavioral capacities are refined during early adolescence as neurobiological maturation progresses and frontal brain regions mature. In parallel, early adolescence is characterized by the emergence of a social drive to establish an adult role, behave autonomously, and engage in adult decisionmaking. This social drive for autonomy and adult status is further stimulated by the mass media’s shaping of perceived social norms for adolescents. For some adolescents, however, there may be a mismatch between the drive to assume an adult social role and the adolescents’ biologically mediated capacity to regulate internal mood states and outwardly directed behavior. Research indicates that this mismatch between social aspirations and self- regulatory abilities increases the adolescent’s vulnerability to a variety of adverse behavioral outcomes, including problematic involvement with alcohol and other drugs (Dahl 2004). To mitigate problems arising from this mismatch in cognitive-emotional abilities and social pressures, the balanced support, monitoring, and modeling of social roles by influential adults (known as social scaffolding) can help guide and protect the adolescent through this vulnerable period. PUBERTAL TIMING, SOCIAL FACTORS, AND GENDER HETEROGENEITY Boys and girls differ significantly in the onset, tempo, and phenomenology of physical sexual maturation. Puberty’s physical changes serve as stimuli for dynamic changes in the social experience of maturing children. These social experiences require significant psychosocial adaptation and are different for boys and girls, particularly when it comes to romantic and peer affiliations. Early physical maturation can lead to attachments to older boyfriends or girlfriends and exposure to social pressures and high-risk situations that younger adolescents may not be able to manage. Having older boyfriends and girlfriends is associated with early sexual activity, engagement in risky sexual practices, and enhanced risk for sexually transmitted diseases and unwanted pregnancy (Vanoss et al. 2000; Flick 1986). For adolescent girls specifically, having an older or adult boyfriend raises the risk for underage use of alcohol and other drugs and the adoption of delinquent behaviors (Castillo Mezzich 1999). For boys, same-gender peers tend to provide more of a vector for initiation into alcohol and other drug use as well as delinquency (Kandel 1978; Dishion et al. 1994; Elliot and Menard 1996; Sampson and Laub 1993; Fergusson and Horwood 1996, 1999; Hawkins et al. 1992). Thus, boys and girls may follow different developmental trajectories, may have a different set of maturational vulnerabilities, and may have a different profile of adverse outcomes. Consequently, developmental underage drinking research needs to explore the roles of gender differences in risk and resilience. MULTICAUSALITY, INTERDISCIPLINARY RESEARCH, AND QUANTITATIVE MODELING Multiple causal influences, from molecules to the media, interact in complicated ways over time to influence underage drinking behavior and outcomes. Building empirical models that capture this complexity can be challenging. Constructing developmental models requires repeated measurement of social, cultural, environmental, and biological factors influencing each other across time. Interdisciplinary expertise is typically required to collect and integrate these diverse types of data. Statistical models also must be developed to validly test a developmental theory— to see if it accurately reflects what happens in the real world. Although quantitative methods such as growth curve analysis—an approach to studying change over time—can address some research questions, methods have yet to be developed to deal with more complex theoretical models relevant to underage drinking and adolescent alcohol misuse (Curran and Willoughby 2003). REFERENCES Brook, J.; Whiteman, M.; Gordon, A.; et al. The psychosocial etiology of adolescent drug use: A family interaction approach. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs 116:113–267, 1990. Castillo Mezzich, A.; Giancola, P.R.; Lu, S.Y.K.S.; et al. Adolescent females with a substance use disorder: Affiliations with adult male sexual partners. American Journal of Addictions 8:190–200, 1999. PMID: 10506900. Cicchetti, D., and Tucker, D. Development and self-regulatory structures of the mind. Development and Psychopathology 6:533–549, 1994. Curran, P.J., and Willoughby, M.T. Implications of latent trajectory models for the study of developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology 15:581–612, 2003. PMID: 14582933. Dahl, R.E. Adolescent brain development: A period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Keynote address. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021:1–22, 2004. PMID: 15251869 Dishion, T.J.; Duncan, T.E.; Eddy, J.M.; et al. The world of parents and peers: Coercive exchanges and children’s social adaptation. Social Development 3:255–268, 1994. Elliot, D.S., and Menard, S. Delinquent friends and delinquent behaviour: Temporal and developmental patterns. In: Hawkins, D., ed. Delinquency and Crime: Current Theories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996. pp. 28–67. Fergusson, D.M., and Horwood, L.J. The role of adolescent peer affiliations in the continuity between childhood behavioral adjustment and juvenile offending. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 24:205–221, 1996. PMID: 8743245 Fergusson, D.M., and Horwood, L.J. Prospective childhood predictors of deviant peer affiliations in adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 40:581–592, 1999. PMID: 10357164 Flick, L.H. Paths to adolescent parenthood: Implications for prevention. Public Health Reports 101:132–147, 1986. Greenough, W.T.; Black, J.E.; and Wallace, C.S. Experience and brain development. Child Development 58:539–559, 1987. PMID: 3038480 Hawkins, J.D.; Catalano, R.F.; and Miller, J.Y. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: Implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychological Bulletin 112:64–105, 1992. PMID: 1529040 Kandel, D. Homophily, selection and socialization in adolescent friendships. American Journal of Sociology 84:427–436, 1978. Masten, A.S. Regulatory processes, risk, and resilience in adolescent development. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021:310–319, 2004. PMID: 15251901 Sampson, R.J., and Laub, J.H. Crime in the Making: Pathways and Turning Points Through Life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1993. Sroufe, L.A., and Rutter, M. The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development 55:17–29, 1984. PMID: 6705619 Stuss, D.T., and Benson, D.F. Neuropsychological studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin 95:3–28, 1984. PMID: 6544432 Vanoss, M.B.; Coyle, K.K.; Gomez, C.A.; et al. Older boyfriends and girlfriends increase risk of sexual initiation in young adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health 27:409–418, 2000. PMID: 11090743 The Effects of Alcohol on Physiological Processes and Biological Development Adolescence is a period of rapid growth and physical change; a central question is whether consuming alcohol during this stage can disrupt development in ways that have long-term consequences. In general, the existing evidence suggests that adolescents rarely exhibit the more severe chronic disorders associated with alcohol dependence such as liver cirrhosis, hepatitis, gastritis, and pancreatitis. Adolescents who drink heavily, however, may experience some adverse effects on the liver, bone, growth, and endocrine development. Evidence also is mounting, at least in animal models, that early alcohol use may have detrimental effects on the developing brain, perhaps leading to problems with cognition later in life. This article summarizes the physiological effects of alcohol on adolescents, including a look at the long-term behavioral and physiological consequences of early drinking. Key words: underage drinking; binge drinking; AODU (alcohol and other drug use); adolescence; growth and development; puberty; physiological AODE (alcohol and other drug effects); psychological AODE; chronic AODE; brain; liver; bone; reproductive system; sexual maturation; long-term AOD (alcohol and other drug) use; animal studies OVERVIEW The damage that long-term heavy alcohol consumption can do to the health of adults is well documented. Some research suggests that, even over the shorter time frame of adolescence, drinking alcohol can harm the liver, bones, endocrine system, and brain, and interfere with growth. Adolescence is a period of rapid growth and physical change; a central question is whether consuming alcohol during this stage can disrupt development in ways that have longterm consequences. Liver disease is a common consequence of heavy drinking. More severe alcohol-related liver disease typically reflects years of heavy alcohol use. However, elevated liver enzymes that are markers of harm have been found in adolescents with alcohol use disorders and in overweight adolescents who consume more modest amounts of alcohol. During puberty, accelerating cascades of growth factors and sex hormones set off sexual maturation, growth in stature and muscle mass, and bone development. Studies in humans have found that alcohol can lower the levels of growth and sex hormones in both adolescent boys and girls. In animals, alcohol has been found to disrupt the interaction between the brain, the pituitary gland (which regulates secretion of sex hormones), and the ovaries, as well as systems within the ovaries that are involved in regulating sex hormones. In adolescent male animals, both short- and long-term alcohol administration suppresses testosterone; alcohol use also alters growth hormone levels, the effects of which differ with age. Studies on alcohol and adolescent bone development are limited. In studies of male and female rats, chronic alcohol consumption (an alcohol diet) for the length of adolescence was found to stunt limb growth. One study found that feeding female rats alcohol in a way that mimics binge drinking resulted in either increases in bone length and density or in no change with more frequent bingeing. In human adolescent males but not females, studies have found that alcohol consumption decreases bone density. The brain also is changing during adolescence. Adolescents tend to drink larger quantities on each drinking occasion than adults; this may in part be because adolescents are less sensitive to some of the unpleasant effects of intoxication. However, research suggests that adolescents may be more sensitive to some of alcohol’s harmful effects on brain function. Studies in rats found that alcohol impairs the ability of adolescent animals more than adult animals to learn a task that requires spatial memory. Research also suggests a mechanism for this effect; in adolescents more than adults, alcohol inhibits the process in which, with repeated experience, nerve impulses travel more easily across the gap between nerve cells (i.e., neurons) involved in the task being learned. The reasons for these differences in sensitivity to alcohol remain unclear. Research also has found differences in the effects of bingelike drinking in adolescents compared with adults. Normally, as people age from adolescence to adulthood, they become more sensitive to alcohol’s effects on motor coordination. In one study, however, adolescent rats exposed to intermittent alcohol never developed this increased sensitivity. Other studies in both human subjects and animals suggest that the adolescent brain may be more vulnerable than the adult brain to chronic alcohol abuse. Young people who reported beginning to drink at age 14 or younger also were four times more likely to report meeting the criteria for alcohol dependence at some point in their lives than were those who began drinking after age 21. Although it is possible that early alcohol use may be a marker for those who are at risk for alcohol disorders, an important question is whether early alcohol exposure may alter neurodevelopment in a way that increases risk of later abuse. Research in rats has found that prenatal or early postnatal exposure to alcohol results in a greater preference for the odor and consumption of alcohol later in life. Social experiences associated with youthful drinking also may influence drinking later in life. Additional research is needed to resolve the question of whether and how early alcohol exposure might contribute to drinking problems years down the road. ALCOHOL’S EFFECTS ON THE LIVER, THE NEUROENDOCRINE SYSTEM, AND BONE The medical consequences of chronic alcohol abuse and dependence have been well documented in adults. They include liver disease, lung disease, compromised immune function, endocrine disorders, and brain changes. Investigations of the health problems associated with adolescent alcohol abuse are sparse and rely mainly on self-report (see Clark et al. 2001; Aarons et al. 1999; Brown and Tapert 2004). In general, the existing evidence suggests that adolescents rarely exhibit the more severe chronic disorders associated with alcohol dependence, such as liver cirrhosis, hepatitis, gastritis, and pancreatitis. However, more research is needed to determine whether severe alcohol-induced organ damage is strictly a cumulative process that begins in adolescence and culminates in adulthood as a result of long-term chronic heavy drinking or whether serious alcohol-related health problems can emerge during the teenage years. The few studies available indicate that adolescents who drink heavily experience adverse effects on the liver, bones, growth, and endocrine development, as summarized below. The effects of chronic alcohol consumption on the adolescent brain are discussed in the section “Long-Term Behavioral and Physiological Conse quences of Early Drinking.” Liver Effects Elevated liver enzymes have been found in some adolescents who drink alcohol. Clark and colleagues (2001) found that adolescent alcohol use disorders were associated with higher gamma-glutamyl transpeptidase (GGT) and alanine amino transferase (ALT). Moreover, young drinkers who also are overweight or obese exhibit elevated levels of serum ALT with even modest amounts of alcohol intake (Strauss et al. 2000). Growth and Endocrine Effects In general, there has been a gradual decline in the onset of female puberty over the last century, at least when puberty is defined by age at menarche (Tanner 1989). Whether initiation of female puberty is continuing to decline and at what rate are the subjects of some debate (Lee et al. 2001; Herman-Giddens et al. 1997). Much less information exists on pubertal development in males because of the greater difficulty in assessing developmental milestones. However, a recent study comparing data from two national surveys, one conducted between 1988 and 1994 and the other between 1963 and 1970, found that American boys from the later generation had earlier onset of some pubertal stages as measured by standard Tanner staging (Herman-Giddens et al. 2001; Karpati et al. 2002). Perhaps not surprisingly, early puberty—especially among girls—is associated with early use of alcohol, tobacco, and other drugs (Wilson et al. 1994; Dick et al. 2000). In addition, alcohol use in early maturing adolescents has implications for normal growth and neuroendocrine development. In both males and females, puberty is a period of activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis. Pulsatile secretion of gonadotrophin-releasing hormone (GnRH) from the hypothalamus stimulates pituitary secretion of follicle-stimulating hormone (FSH) and luteinizing hormone (LH) pulses, followed by marked increases in gonadal sex steroid output (estrogen and testosterone), which in turn increases growth hormone (GH) and insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF1) production (see Mauras et al. 1996). Data from several studies suggest that both androgens and estrogens stimulate GH production, but that estrogen controls the feedback mechanism of GH production during puberty even in males (Mauras et al. 1996; Dees et al. 2001). The increase in these hormones not only promotes maturation of the gonads but also affects growth, muscle mass, and mineralization of the skeleton. Thus, alcohol consumed during rapid development (i.e., prior to or during puberty) has the potential to disrupt normal growth and endocrine development through its effects on the hypothalamus, the pituitary gland, and the various target organs such as the ovaries and testes. Most human and animal research on alcohol and endocrine development has been conducted in females, but the limited data on both genders suggest that alcohol can have substantial effects on neuroendocrine function (see Dees et al. 2001; Emanuele et al. 1998; Emanuele et al. 2002a,b). Human studies have found that alcohol ingestion can lower estrogen levels in adolescent girls (Block et al. 1993) and lower both LH and testosterone levels in midpubertal boys (Diamond et al. 1986; Frias et al. 2000a). In both genders, acute alcohol intoxication produces a decrease in GH levels without significant change in either IGF-1 or insulin-like growth factor binding protein-3 (IGFBP3) (Frias et al. 2000b). In female rats, alcohol has been shown to suppress the secretion of specific female reproductive hormones, thereby delaying the onset of puberty (see Dees et al. 2001 and Emanuele et al). Dees and colleagues (2000) found that immature female rhesus macaques exposed daily to alcohol (2 g/kg via nasogastric tube) exhibit lower levels of GH, FSH, LH, estradiol (E 2), and IGF1 (but not FSH or Leptin) compared with control subjects. Moreover, even though there was no effect on age of menarche in these animals, the interval between subsequent menstruations was lengthened, thereby interfering with the development of regular monthly cycles. Additional studies in rats have found that alcohol interferes with intraovarian systems, including IGF-1 and IGF-1 receptors; the nitric oxide (NO) system (Dees et al. 2001; Srivastava et al. 2001a), and the steroidogenic acute regulatory protein (StAR) (Srivastava et al. 2001b), all of which combine to decrease estradiol secretion. Thus, alcohol not only disrupts the interaction between the brain, pituitary gland, and ovaries, it also directly impairs the regulatory systems within the ovaries (see Dees et al. 2001 for review). In male rats, both acute and chronic alcohol exposure during adolescence results in a reversible suppression of serum testosterone (Little et al. 1992; Cicero et al. 1990; Tentler et al. 1997; Emanuele et al. 1998, 1999a,b; Steiner et al. 1997). Evidence exists for involvement at the hypothalamic, pituitary, and gonadal levels, although the testes appear to be the prime target of alcohol’s actions (Emanuele et al. 1999a). Furthermore, GH levels are affected by acute and chronic alcohol exposure in male adolescent rats, whereas IGF-1, growth hormone releasing factor (GRF), and GRF mRNA content are variable, depending on the type of administration (Steiner et al. 1997; Tentler et al. 1997). Thus, the data so far indicate that females who consume alcohol during early adolescence may be at risk for adverse effects on maturation of the reproductive system. Although in males the long-term effects of alcohol on reproductive function are unclear, the fact that GH as well as testosterone and/or estrogen levels are altered by alcohol in both genders may have serious implications for normal development because these hormones play a critical role in organ maturation during this stage of development. Bone Density and Growth Effects Only a handful of studies have examined the effects of adolescent drinking on bone development, with the most informative data thus far coming from animal research. Male rats chronically fed an alcohol liquid diet for 60 days encompassing the adolescent period (postnatal days 35 to 90) display limb length reduction and reduced metaphyseal and cortical bone growth in the limbs (Wezeman et al. 1999). These skeletal effects may be mediated through a reduction in osteoblast formation, which is associated with a decline in testosterone but not IGF-1. In addition, with abstinence, normal bone metabolism is not completely restored. Similarly, in female rats, Sampson and colleagues (Sampson et al. 1996; Sampson and Spears 1999) found that chronic alcohol consumption (4 weeks on an ethanol liquid diet) produces decreased limb length and reductions in cortical and cancellous bone, which are not fully reversed following cessation of drinking. Interestingly, female adolescent animals administered a binge model of drinking (i.e., 5 percent alcohol by gavage for either 2 or 5 consecutive days per week) show increased bone length, weight, and density, or no change, respectively (Sampson et al. 1999). Human studies indicate an inverse relationship between alcohol consumption and bone mineral density in adolescent males, but not females (Fehily et al. 1992; Neville et al. 2002; Elgan et al. 2002; Fujita et al. 1999). However, more studies are needed in humans and animals to get a clearer picture of alcohol’s effects on bone growth in adolescents, particularly with respect to dose and pattern of consumption. A Snapshot of Findings on Alcohol’s Physiological Effects in Adolescent Humans & Animals Findings Study On the Liver Levels of enzymes that indicate liver damage are higher in adolescents with alcohol use disorders Clark et al. 2001 And in obese adolescents who drink more moderate amounts. Strauss et al. 2000 In humans On the Endocrine System In humans In rats Drinking alcohol can lower estrogen levels in adolescent girls. Block et al. 1993 Drinking alcohol can lower luteinizing hormone and testosterone levels in adolescent boys. Diamond et al. 1986; Frias et al. 2000a In both sexes, acute intoxication reduces levels of growth hormones. Frias et al. 2000b In female rats, ingesting alcohol during adolescence Dees et al. 2001 is associated with adverse effects on maturation of the reproductive system. In rhesus macaques Alcohol suppresses the secretion of certain female reproductive hormones, delaying the start of puberty. Emanuelle et al. 2001, 2002 Alcohol not only disrupts the interaction between the brain, pituitary gland, and ovaries, but also impairs regulatory systems within the ovaries. Dees et al. 2001 In male rats, alcohol consumption alters growth hormone and testosterone levels, which may have serious consequences for normal development. Little et al. 1992; Cicero et al. 1990; Tentler et al. 1997; Emanuelle et al. 1998, 1999a, 1999b; Steiner et al. 1997 In immature female monkeys, daily exposure to alcohol lowered levels of female hormones and affected the development of regular monthly cycles. Dees et al. 2000 On Bone Density In humans Increased alcohol consumption is associated with lowered bone mineral density in adolescent males but not females. Fehily et al. 1992; Neville et al. 2002; Elgan et al. 2002; Fujita et al. 1999 In adolescent female rats, chronic alcohol consumption produced shorter limb lengths and reductions in bone growth, neither of which was fully reversed with abstinence. Sampson et al. 1996; Sampson and Spears 1999 In adolescent male rats, chronic alcohol ingestion was associated with shorter limb length and reduced bone growth, which are not fully reversed with abstinence. Wezeman et al. 1999 In rats On the Brain In humans In rats A history of alcohol abuse or dependence in adolescents was associated with reduced hippocampal volumes De Bellis et al. 2000 And with subtle white-matter microstructure abnormalities in the corpus callosum. Tapert et al. 2003 Chronic intermittent exposure to high alcohol doses (i.e., bingeing) results in long-lasting changes in memory in adolescent rats White et al. 2000 And to more damage to the frontal-anterior cortical regions of the brain than are produced in adult rats. Crews et al. 2000 Prolonged alcohol exposure during adolescence produces: Neurophysiological changes in the response to alcohol challenge and in the tolerance to alcohol’s sedative effects; Enhanced expression of withdrawal behaviors; and Long-lasting neurophysiological effects in the cortex and hippocampus. Slawecki et al. 2001; Slawecki 2002; Slawecki and Roth 2004 LONG-TERM BEHAVIORAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES OF EARLY DRINKING Although increased tolerance to alcohol’s sedative effects may enable greater intake in adolescents, repeated exposure to alcohol may produce increased sensitivity to alcohol’s harmful effects. Studies in rats show that ethanol-induced inhibition of synaptic potentials mediated by Nmethyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) and long-term potentiation (LTP) is greater in adolescents than in adults (Swartzwelder et al. 1995a,b; see White and Swartzwelder 2005 for review). Initially, the developmental sensitivity of NMDA currents to alcohol was observed in the hippocampus, but more recently this effect was found outside the hippocampus in pyramidal cells in the posterior cingulate cortex (Li et al. 2002). Behaviorally, adolescent rats show greater impairment than adults in acquisition of a spatial memory task after acute ethanol exposure (Markwiese et al. 1998) in support of greater LTP sensitivity to alcohol in adolescents. Behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms for the ontogenetic differences in alcohol tolerance and sensitivity are unclear, as is the relationship between differential sensitivity to ethanol and onset of alcohol abuse and alcoholism. Binge alcohol exposure (i.e., chronic intermittent exposure to high alcohol doses) in rats during adolescence produces long-lasting changes in memory function (White et al. 2000) and interferes with the normal development of sensitivity to alcohol-induced motor impairments (White et al. 2002). In addition, prolonged alcohol exposure during adolescence, but not adulthood, produces alterations in neurophysiological response to ethanol challenge, tolerance to the sedative effects of ethanol, enhanced expression of withdrawal-related behavior, and long-lasting neurophysiological changes in the cortex and hippocampus in rats (Slawecki et al. 2001; Slawecki 2002; Slawecki and Roth 2004). Further more, chronic ethanol treatment in rats may lead to increased NMDA-mediated neurotoxicity, which could be exacerbated by repeated withdrawals (Hunt 1993). Consistent with this hypothesis is the finding that severity of alcohol and drug withdrawal symptoms may be a powerful marker of neuropsychological impairments in detoxified older human adolescents and young adults (Brown et al. 2000; Tapert and Brown 1999; Tapert et al. 2002). Moreover, one recent study found reduced hippocampal volumes in human adolescents with a history of alcohol abuse/dependence disorder (De Bellis et al. 2000), and another preliminary investigation of alcohol-abusing teenagers observed subtle white-matter microstructure abnormalities in the corpus callosum (Tapert et al. 2003), which may be a precursor of more severe damage produced by long-term chronic drinking (Pfefferbaum and Sullivan 2002). Juvenile rats exposed to heavy bingelike episodes of ethanol have greater damage than adults in frontal-anterior cortical regions, including the olfactory frontal cortex, anterior perirhinal, and piriform cortex (Crews et al. 2000). Thus, the immature brain may be more susceptible to binge ethanol-induced neurotoxicity, although the mechanisms are unknown. Because teenagers are likely to engage in binge drinking, it is important to study the effects of chronic binge patterns of ethanol exposure on brain structure, neurochemistry, and cognitive functioning. Care must be taken in extrapolating from the described animal studies to the bingedrinking adolescent. Because binge drinking does not usually entail withdrawal, it is important to distinguish between damage caused by the alcohol itself and that caused by repeated withdrawals. In addition, primate models may be a better choice for studying the long-term consequences of alcohol exposure because of primates’ prolonged adolescent period, which allows extensive manipulation of different types and lengths of exposure. These models, coupled with new neuroanatomical and neuroimaging techniques, offer a unique opportunity to study the brain changes associated with adolescent drinking and determine whether adolescent brains are able to recover more easily because of greater plasticity. Early Exposure as a Predictor of Later Alcohol Abuse Early exposure to alcohol—at or before age 14—is strongly associated with later alcohol abuse and dependence (Grant and Dawson 1998). Two possible explanations for this effect are obvious. First, early alcohol use may simply be a marker for later alcohol abuse rather than a causative factor. A good deal of evidence indicates that at least one behavioral factor, behavioral undercontrol, is measurable very early in life and is a consistently robust predictor of earlier alcohol use as well as of elevated risk for later alcohol use disorder (NIAAA 2000; Zucker and Wong 2005; Caspi et al. 1996). Second, it is possible that alcohol exposure during adolescence actually may alter neurodevelopmental processes in such a way that the likelihood of later abuse is increased. For example, alcohol use could promote rewiring or alter normal maturation and pruning within the nervous system. Ample evidence exists that exposing rats to low or moderate doses of alcohol during the prenatal or early postnatal period yields a greater preference for ethanol’s odor and its consumption later in life (Abate et al. 2000; Honey and Galef 2003; see Molina et al. 1999 and Spear and Molina 2001 for reviews). The young rat’s response to alcohol also is mediated by social factors such as maternal interactions and/or nursing from an intoxicated dam (e.g., Hunt et al. 2001; Pepino et al. 2001, 2002; Spear and Molina 2001). Recent evidence shows that prior nursing experience from an ethanol-intoxicated dam heightens ethanol consumption in infant and adolescent rats (Ponce et al. 2004; Pepino et al. 2004). In contrast, relatively few reports using animal models to study the effects of adolescent alcohol exposure on later alcohol consumption exist, and the results are conflicting (see Spear and Varlinskaya 2005). Yet, as is the case with younger animals, social experiences associated with adolescent drinking may influence future drinking behaviors (Hunt et al. 2001; Varlinskaya and Spear 2002). More studies are needed, however, to explore whether a causal relationship between early chronic exposure to alcohol and later alcohol problems exists, as well as to discover the underlying mechanisms for this effect. Nonhuman primates, because of their extended adolescent period, offer a good opportunity to study the effects of early exposure to alcohol. REFERENCES Aarons, G.A.; Brown, S.A.; Coe, M.T.; et al. Adolescent alcohol and drug abuse and health. Journal of Adolescent Health 24:412–421, 1999. PMID: 10401969 Abate, P.; Pepino, M.Y.; Dominguez, H.D.; et al. Fetal associative learning mediated through maternal alcohol intoxication. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:39–47, 2000. PMID: 10656191 Block, G.D.; Yamamoto, M.E.; Mallick, E.; and Styche, A. Effects on pubertal hormones by ethanol abuse in adolescents. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 17:505, 1993. Brown, S.A., and Tapert, S.F. Health consequences of adolescent alcohol involvement. In: NRC and IOM. Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. pp. 383–401. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Brown, S.A.; Tapert, S.F.; Granholm, E.; and Dellis, D.C. Neurocognitive functioning of adolescents: Effects of protracted alcohol use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:164–171, 2000. PMID: 10698367 Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T.E.; Newman, D.L.; and Silva, E.P.A. Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 53:1033–1039, 1996. PMID: 8911226 Cicero, T.J.; Adams, M.L.; O’Connor, L.; et al. Influence of chronic alcohol administration on representative indices of puberty and sexual maturation in male rats and the development of their progeny. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 255:707–715, 1990. PMID: 2243349 Clark, D.B.; Lynch, K.G.; Donovan, J.E.; and Block, G.D. Health problems in adolescents with alcohol use disorders: Self-report, liver injury, and physical examination findings and correlates. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25:1350–1359, 2001. PMID: 11584156 Crews, F.T.; Braun, C.J.; Hoplight, B.; et al. Binge ethanol consumption causes differential brain damage in young adolescent rats compared with adult rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:1712–1723, 2000. PMID: 11104119 De Bellis, M.D.; Clark, D.B.; Beers, S.R.; et al. Hippocampal volume in adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 157:737–744, 2000. PMID: 10784466 Dees, W.L.; Dissen, G.A.; Hiney, J.K.; et al. Alcohol ingestion inhibits the increased secretion of puberty-related hormones in the developing female rhesus monkey. Endocrinology 141:1325– 1331, 2000. PMID: 10746635 Dees, W.L.; Srivastava, V.K.; and Hiney, J.K. Alcohol and female puberty: The role of intraovarian systems. Alcohol Research & Health 25(4):271–275, 2001. PMID: 11910704 Diamond, F., Jr.; Ringenberg, L.; MacDonald, D.; et al. Effects of drug and alcohol abuse upon pituitary-testicular function in adolescent males. Journal of Adolescent Health Care 7:28–33, 1986. PMID: 2935515 Dick, D.M.; Rose, R.J.; Viken, R.J.; and Kaprio, J. Pubertal timing and substance use: Associations between and within families across late adolescence. Developmental Psychology 36:180–189, 2000. PMID: 10749075 Elgan, C.; Dykes, A.K.; and Samsioe, G. Bone mineral density and lifestyle among female students aged 16–24 years. Gynecological Endocrinology 16:91– 98, 2002. PMID: 12012629 Emanuele, M.A.; LaPaglia, N.; Steiner, J.; et al. Reversal of ethanol-induced testosterone suppression in peripubertal male rats by opiate blockade. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 22:1199–1204, 1998. PMID: 9756033 Emanuele, M.A.; Wezeman, F.; and Emanuele, N.V. Alcohol’s effects on female reproductive function. Alcohol Research & Health 26(4):274–281, 2002a. PMID: 12875037 Emanuele, N.; Ren, J.; LaPaglia, N.; et al. EtOH disrupts female mammalian puberty: Age and opiate dependence. Endocrine 18:247–254, 2002b. PMID: 12450316 Emanuele, N.V.; LaPaglia, N.; Vogl, W.; et al. Impact and reversibility of chronic ethanol feeding on the reproductive axis in the peripubertal male rat. Endocrine 11:277–284, 1999a. PMID: 10786824 Emanuele, N.V.; Lapaglia, N.; Steiner, J.; et al. Reversal of chronic ethanol-induced testosterone suppression in peripubertal male rats by opiate blockade. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 23:60–66, 1999b. PMID: 10029204 Fehily, A.M.; Coles, R.J.; Evans, W.D.; and Elwood, P.C. Factors affecting bone density in young adults. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 56:579–586, 1992. PMID: 1503072 Frias, J.; Rodriguez, R.; Torres, J.M.; et al. Effects of acute alcohol intoxication on pituitarygonadal axis hormones, pituitary-adrenal axis hormones, β-endorphin and prolactin in human adolescents of both sexes. Life Sciences 67:1081–1086, 2000a. PMID: 10954041 Frias, J.; Torres, J.M.; Rodriguez, R.; et al. Effects of acute alcohol intoxication on growth axis in human adolescents of both sexes. Life Sciences 67:2691–2697, 2000b. PMID: 11105985 Fujita, Y.; Katsumata, K.; Unno, A.; et al. Factors affecting peak bone density in Japanese women. Calcified Tissue International 64:107–111, 1999. PMID: 9914316 Grant, B.F., and Dawson, D.A. Age at onset of alcohol use and its association with DSM–IV alcohol abuse and dependence: Results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. Journal of Substance Abuse 9:103–110, 1998. PMID: 9494942 Herman-Giddens, M.E.; Slora, E.J.; Wasserman, R.C.; et al. Secondary sexual characteristics and menses in young girls seen in office practice: A study from the Pediatric Research in Office Settings Network. Pediatrics 99:505–512, 1997. PMID: 9093289 Herman-Giddens, M.E.; Wang, L.; and Koch, G. Secondary sexual characteristics in boys: Estimates from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey III, 1988–1994. Archives of Pediatric & Adolescent Medicine 155:1022–1028, 2001. PMID: 11529804 Honey, P.L., and Galef, B.G., Jr. Ethanol consumption by rat dams during gestation, lactation and weaning increases ethanol consumption by their adolescent young. Developmental Psychobiology 42:252– 260, 2003. PMID: 12621651 Hunt, W.A. Are binge drinkers more at risk of developing brain damage? Alcohol 10:559–561, 1993. PMID: 8123218 Hunt, P.S.; Holloway, J.L.; and Scordalakes, E.M. Social interaction with an intoxicated sibling can result in increased intake of ethanol by periadolescent rats. Developmental Psychobiology 38:101–109, 2001. PMID: 11223802 Karpati, A.M.; Rubin, C.H.; Kieszak, S.M.; et al. Stature and pubertal stage assessment in American boys: The 1988–1994 Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Journal of Adolescent Health 30:205–212, 2002. PMID: 11869928 Lee, P.A.; Guo, S.S.; and Kulin, H.E. Age of puberty: Data from the United States of America. APMIS (Acta Pathologica, Microbiologica, et Immunologica Scandinavica) 109:81–88, 2001. PMID: 11398998 Li, Q.; Wilson, W.A.; and Swartzwelder, H.S. Differential effect of ethanol on NMDA EPSCs in pyramidal cells in the posterior cingulate cortex of juvenile and adult rats. Journal of Neurophysiology 87:705–711, 2002. PMID: 11826039 Little, P.J.; Adams, M.L.; and Cicero, T.J. Effects of alcohol on the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis in the developing male rat. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 263:1056–1061, 1992. PMID: 1469619 Markwiese, B.J.; Acheson, S.K.; Levin, E.D.; et al. Differential effects of ethanol on memory in adolescent and adult rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 22:416–421, 1998. PMID: 9581648 Mauras, N.; Rogol, A.D.; Haymond, M.W.; and Veldhuis, J.D. Sex steroids, growth hormone, insulin-like growth factor-1: Neuroendocrine and metabolic regulation in puberty. Hormone Research 45:74–80, 1996. PMID: 8742123 Molina, J.C.; Dominguez, H.D.; Lopez, M.F.; et al. The role of fetal and infantile experience with alcohol in later recognition and acceptance patterns of the drug. In: Hannigan, J.; Goodlett, C.; Spear, L.; Spear, N., eds. Alcohol and Alcoholism: Brain and Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1999, pp. 199–227. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). Alcohol involvement over the life course. In: Tenth Special Report to the U.S. Congress on Alcohol and Health: Highlights from Current Research. Bethesda, MD: Dept. of Health and Human Services, NIAAA, 2000. pp. 28–53. Available online at: http://pubs.niaaa.nih.gov/publications/10report/intro.pdf . Neville, C.E.; Murray, L.J.; Boreham, C.A.G.; et al. Relationship between physical activity and bone mineral status in young adults: The Northern Ireland Young Hearts Project. Bone 30:792– 798, 2002. PMID: 11996922 Pepino, M.Y.; Spear, N.E.; and Molina, J.C. Nursing experiences with an alcohol-intoxicated rat dam counteract appetitive conditioned responses toward alcohol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25:18–24, 2001. PMID: 11198710 Pepino, M.Y.; Abate, P.; Spear, N.E.; and Molina, J.C. Disruption of maternal behavior by alcohol intoxication in the lactating rat: A behavioral and metabolic analysis. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26:1205–1214, 2002. PMID: 12198395 Pepino, M.Y.; Abate, P.; Spear, N.E.; and Molina, J.C. Heightened ethanol intake in infant and adolescent rats after nursing experiences with an ethanol-intoxicated dam. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 28:895–905, 2004. PMID: 15201632 Pfefferbaum, A., and Sullivan, E.V. Micro structural but not macrostructural disruption of white matter in women with chronic alcoholism. Neuroimage 15:708–718, 2002. PMID: 11848714 Ponce, L.F.; Pautassi, R.M.; Spear, N.E.; and Molina, J.C. Nursing from an ethanol-intoxicated dam induces short- and long-term disruptions in motor performance and enhances later selfadministration of the drug. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 28:1039–1050, 2004. PMID: 15252290 Sampson, H.W., and Spears, H. Osteopenia due to chronic alcohol consumption by young actively growing rats is not completely reversible. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 23: 324–327, 1999. PMID: 10069563 Sampson, H.W.; Perks, N.; Champney, T.H.; and Defee, B., 2nd. Alcohol consumption inhibits bone growth and development in young actively growing rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 20:1375–1384, 1996. PMID: 8947313 Sampson, H.W.; Gallager, S.; Lange, J.; et al. Binge drinking and bone metabolism in a young actively growing rat model. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 23:1228–1231, 1999. PMID: 10443990 Slawecki, C.J. Altered EEG responses to ethanol in adult rats exposed to ethanol during adolescence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26:246–254, 2002. PMID: 11964565 Slawecki, C.J., and Roth, J. Comparison of the onset of hypoactivity and anxiety-like behavior during alcohol withdrawal in adolescent and adult rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 28:598–607, 2004. PMID: 15100611 Slawecki, C.J.; Betancourt, M.; Cole, M.; and Ehlers, C.L. Periadolescent alcohol exposure has lasting effects on adult neurophysiological function in rats. Developmental Brain Research 128:63–72, 2001. PMID: 11356263 Spear, L.P., and Varlinskaya, E.I. Adolescence: Alcohol sensitivity, tolerance, and intake. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 143–159. PMID: 15789864 Spear, N.E., and Molina, J.C. Consequences of early exposure to alcohol: How animal studies reveal later patterns of use and abuse in humans. In: Carroll, M.E., and Overmier, J.B., eds. Animal Research and Human Health: Advancing Human Welfare through Behavioral Science. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2001. pp. 85–99. Srivastava, V.K.; Hiney, J.K.; Dearth, R.K.; and Dees, W.L. Effects of alcohol on intraovarian insulin-like growth factor-1 and nitric oxide systems in prepubertal female rats. Recent Research Developments in Endocrinology 2(part 1):213–221, 2001a. Srivastava, V.K.; Hiney, J.K.; Dearth, R.K.; and Dees, W.L. Acute effects of ethanol on steroidogenic acute regulatory protein (StAR) in the prepubertal rat ovary. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25:1500–1505, 2001b. PMID: 11696671 Steiner, J.C.; LaPaglia, N.; Hansen, M.; et al. Effect of chronic ethanol on reproductive and growth hormones in the peripubertal male rat. Journal of Endocrinology 154:363–370, 1997. PMID: 9291847 Strauss, R.S.; Barlow, S.E.; and Dietz, W.H. Prevalence of abnormal serum aminotransferase values in overweight and obese adolescents. Journal of Pediatrics 136:727–733, 2000. PMID: 10839867 Swartzwelder, H.S.; Wilson, W.A.; and Tayyeb, M.I. Age-dependent inhibition of long-term potentiation by ethanol in immature versus mature hippocampus. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 19:1480–1485, 1995a. PMID: 8749814 Swartzwelder, H.S.; Wilson, W.A.; and Tayyeb, M.I. Differential sensitivity of NMDA receptormediated synaptic potentials to ethanol in immature versus mature hippocampus. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 19:320–323, 1995b. PMID: 7625564 Tanner, J.M. Foetus into Man: Physical Growth From Conception to Maturity. Ware, Great Britain: Castlemead Publications, 1989. Tapert, S.F., and Brown, S.A. Neuropsychological correlates of adolescent substance abuse: Four-year outcomes. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 5:481–493, 1999. PMID: 10561928 Tapert, S.F.; Granholm, E.; Leedy, N.G.; and Brown, S.A. Substance use and withdrawal: Neuropsychological functioning over 8 years in youth. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 8:873–883, 2002. PMID: 12405538 Tapert, S.F.; Theilmann, R.J.; Schweinsburg, A.D.; et al. Reduced fractional anisotropy in the splenium of adolescents with alcohol use disorder. Proceedings of the International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 11:8217, 2003. Tentler, J.J.; LaPaglia, N.; Steiner, J.; et al. Ethanol, growth hormone and testosterone in peripubertal rats. Journal of Endocrinology 152:477–487, 1997. PMID: 9071969 Varlinskaya, E.I., and Spear, L.P. Acute effects of ethanol on social behavior of adolescent and adult rats: Role of familiarity of the test situation. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26:1502–1511, 2002. PMID: 12394283 Wezeman, F.H.; Emanuele, M.A.; Emanuele, N.V.; et al. Chronic alcohol consumption during male rat adolescence impairs skeletal development through effects on osteoblast gene expression, bone mineral density, and bone strength. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 23:1534–1542, 1999. PMID: 10512321 White, A.M., and Swartzwelder, H.S. Age-related effects of alcohol on memory and memoryrelated brain function in adolescents and adults. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 161–176. PMID: 15789865 White, A.M.; Ghia, A.J.; Levin, E.D.; and Swartzwelder, H.S. Binge pattern ethanol exposure in adolescent and adult rats: Differential impact on subsequent responsiveness to ethanol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:1251–1256, 2000. PMID: 10968665 White, A.M.; Truesdale, M.C.; Bae, J.G.; et al. Differential effects of ethanol on motor coordination in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 73:673–677, 2002. PMID: 12151043 Wilson, D.M.; Killen, J.D.; Hayward, C.; et al. Timing and rate of sexual maturation and the onset of cigarette and alcohol use among teenage girls. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine 148:789–795, 1994. PMID: 8044254 Zucker, R.A., and Wong, M.M. Prevention for children of alcoholics and other high risk groups. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 299– 320. PMID: 15789872 Genetics, Pharmacokinetics, and Neurobiology of Adolescent Alcohol Use Complex behaviors such as the initiation and use of alcohol result from an intricate interplay between genes and environment. Genes shape physiological and behavioral responses to alcohol that can influence the likelihood that a young person will begin using alcohol and that he or she will progress to problem drinking. Youthful alcohol use also can have an impact on unfolding developmental patterns, and for some, early use becomes the entry point for pathways that lead to problems with alcohol. This article first describes research on genes that may be involved in the development of alcohol problems and how genetic factors may contribute to adolescent alcohol use. It then examines how the changes that occur during adolescent development—in alcohol metabolism, in the brain, and in the endocrine and stress response systems—may affect how a young person experiences alcohol and the likelihood that he or she will develop alcohol use problems. Key words: adolescent; AOD (alcohol and other drug) use initiation; alcohol abuse; alcohol dependence; AOD sensitivity; use initiation; risk factors; protective factors; genetic risk and protective factors; heredity factors; genetics and heredity; environmental factors; heredity vs. environmental factors; human study; twin study; animal study; animal model; puberty; biological development; psychological development; cognitive development; endocrine system; hormones; stress; ethanol metabolism; alcohol metabolism; pharmacokinetics OVERVIEW Many studies have focused on how physiological and neurobiological responses to alcohol— such as sensitivity to alcohol, change in its rewarding effects, craving, tolerance and withdrawal— factor into the development of alcohol use and alcohol use disorders. The majority of studies to identify genes and neurobiological mechanisms that may contribute to alcohol use and alcohol use disorders in humans have been done with adults. Recent data suggest, however, that the highest prevalence of alcohol dependence in the general population occurs in people ages 18– 24, and it is not yet clear to what extent genes involved in the onset of alcohol problems in adults play a similar role in youth. A central goal of research is to understand the genetic and environmental factors, and the interplay between them, that contribute to the development of alcohol abuse and dependence in adolescents. Human genetic research related to alcohol use has involved studies with twins or with families that have a high prevalence of alcohol-dependent individuals. This work has identified regions of chromosomes that are associated with an altered risk of developing alcohol dependence, and in some cases, individual genes or candidate genes. Analysis of the role these genes and gene regions play in alcohol use is difficult for a number of reasons. As with other complex genetic diseases, multiple genetic factors may contribute to the risk of developing alcohol dependence, but no one factor is associated with a large percent of risk. Different risk factors may be active in different individuals. In the case of alcohol use, studies of both adults and adolescents suggest that the relative contributions of genes and environment change at different stages of problematic drinking; for example, genes have a strong influence over the development of problem use, whereas environment seems to play a greater role in the initiation of alcohol use. Research using animal models also added to our understanding of the genes that may contribute to alcohol abuse and/or dependence. Animal studies have helped to identify genes involved in the effects of alcohol as well as those involved in the pathways affecting sensitivity to acute alcohol exposure, reward, craving, and withdrawal. Many of the genes that have emerged from this research have roles in other behaviors as well. For example, serotonin has been implicated in alcohol consumption in conjunction with its role in anxiety, which, in some individuals, is particularly manifest during adolescence. Further studies will assess the relative roles of various signaling pathways and their component genes in the development of alcohol-related behavior, and determine which are most influential in adolescents. Superimposed on genetically shaped aspects of physiology are developmental changes that can measurably affect both the response to alcohol and the chances that someone will drink heavily. Some research, for example, suggests modest changes during adolescence in the pharmacokinetics of alcohol—how it is absorbed, distributed, and eliminated. Gender differences in these processes emerge during puberty and affect blood alcohol levels after drinking. Animal studies suggest that sensitivity to alcohol is different among adolescents than it is in adults. For example, adolescent rats are less sensitive to the unpleasant effects of intoxication, such as sedation, loss of coordination, and hangover effects, and they consume higher levels of alcohol than do older animals. Possible underlying reasons for the lack of sensitivity include the developmental immaturity of neurotransmitter receptor systems. Dramatic hormonal changes take place during puberty, affecting growth and sexual development, and the body’s stress response systems. These hormonal changes also may affect sensitivity to alcohol. For example, animal research suggests that hormones involved in the response to stress may interact with neurotransmitters in the brain that are associated with the sensation of reward to facilitate drinking. Clearly, development adds a layer of complexity to understanding the reciprocal interactions between genes and environment, alcohol metabolism, and the neurobiology of the response to alcohol. GENETICS OF ADOLESCENT ALCOHOL USE AND ALCOHOL USE DISORDERS Accumulating research indicates that complex behaviors result from the interplay between genes and environment over developmental time. Alcohol use is a prime example of a complex behavior in which gene expression and environment/ context reciprocally influence one another. For example, during adolescence, biological and physiological changes may promote risk-taking behavior, thereby influencing the way people decide to spend their time. The environments that a person selects may foster the use of alcohol, which in turn may result in acute physiological reactions that have the potential to trigger long-term biological changes. These changes then may affect the person’s more immediate behavior as well as move unfolding developmental pathways toward adverse outcomes, including psychopathology (e.g., anxiety disorders). In this way, youthful patterns of alcohol initiation and escalation of use can become entry points for pathways that ultimately lead to abuse and dependence. However, not all young people experience the same outcome(s) from what may appear to be similar patterns of adolescent alcohol use. Studying such complexity is difficult, and adding to the difficulty of understanding these complex pathways at the biological level is the recognition that, unlike other drugs of abuse, alcohol does not appear to act through a specific receptor. Instead, alcohol modulates the function of multiple neurotransmitter systems and voltage-gated ion channels. At the macro level, pathways that have been associated with the development of alcohol problems in general, and alcohol dependence more specifically, manifest themselves in the form of multiple behavioral and physiological characteristics, including disinhibition/ impulsivity, anxiety, variations in the intensity of response to alcohol, and several independent psychiatric disorders and alcohol-metabolizing patterns. These characteristics can influence the development of alcohol dependence through alterations in pathways that determine the rewarding effects of alcohol, tolerance to some of its intoxicating effects, pathologic effects on the brain, and the development of withdrawal symptoms (Koob and Le Moal 1997). Genetically conferred characteristics contribute to the degree and/or rate of development of these changes (Crabbe 2001). Attempts have been made to categorize people based on the degree and rate that they develop dependence and to differentiate between the genetic contributions to various categories of alcohol dependence (Cloninger et al. 1981; Babor et al. 1992). Because the development of alcohol use, abuse, and alcoholism occurs on a continuum, and because recent data show that the highest prevalence of alcohol dependence in the population occurs between the ages of 18 and 25, this review focuses on efforts to identify genes contributing to those pathways that appear in childhood and adolescence (Grant et al. 2004). It must be emphasized, however, that many of the studies identifying heritable components which may contribute to alcohol abuse or dependence have been carried out in adults. The extent of their role, if any, in the development of alcohol problems in youth, therefore, remains to be determined. Additional genetic contributors to alcohol problems may be identified by future investigations, and some of these may be more specific to risk in children and adolescents. Finding genes that contribute to the development of alcohol abuse and dependence in humans may be simplified by focusing on endophenotypes (i.e., intermediary connections between the manifestation of dependence and its biological underpinnings). To this end, human genetic studies have focused on families that have a high prevalence of members with alcohol dependence, on twin studies, and on studies assessing potential markers and/or contributors to risk. By performing genetic analyses on such populations, researchers have identified specific regions on individual chromosomes that correlate with risk for alcohol dependence (e.g., Reich et al. 1998; Long et al. 1998; Foroud et al. 2000). In some cases, individual candidate genes have been associated with these regions. The goal now is to further refine those regions for which a specific gene has not yet been identified as responsible for the observed phenotype and to determine respective contributions of candidate genes to alcohol dependence. Research on how the identified genes interact with other genes and gene products and with the environment to result in alcohol dependence also is important. To date, functional polymorphisms in the alcohol-metabolizing enzymes alcohol dehydrogenase and mitochondrial aldehyde dehydrogenase have been the most thoroughly documented for providing protective effects in specific populations (reviewed in Oroszi and Goldman 2004). In addition, the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA) has identified specific genes within identified regions that affect risk for alcoholism (reviewed in Edenberg and Kranzler 2005). These include gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptor subunits GABRA2 and GABRG3 and the muscarinic cholinergic receptor. GABRA2 has been independently confirmed (reviewed in Edenberg and Kranzler 2005). Other promising candidates that have been implicated and are under investigation include the serotonin transporter 5-HTT (reviewed in Oroszi and Goldman 2004); specific alleles of the neurotransmitter dopamine (reviewed in Bowirrat and Oscar-Berman 2005); catechol-Omethyltransferase (COMT) (reviewed in Oroszi and Goldman 2004); neuropeptide Y (NPY) (reviewed in Oroszi and Goldman 2004; Mottagui-Tabar et al. 2005); and the m opioid receptor (OPRM) (reviewed in Oroszi and Goldman 2004; Edenberg and Kranzler 2005; Bart et al. 2005). Animal Studies In addition to studies with humans, studies using animal models, such as worms, flies, and rodents, permit researchers to model, in less complex systems, individual biological and behavioral components that may factor into alcohol problems, including dependence. The goal of this work is the convergence of findings from human and animal model studies to facilitate the design of pharmacological agents that can reduce, prevent, or ameliorate alcohol problems, including dependence and associated consequences. In model organisms, several basic approaches have been used to uncover genetic influences. One approach is to generate inbred strains of animals (usually rodents) that voluntarily consume large amounts of alcohol and those which do not, and then perform genetic analyses to determine the gene(s) underlying this behavior (reviewed in Crabbe et al. 1994). For example, a well-studied rodent model of alcoholism uses rats selectively bred for increased alcohol consumption (i.e., alcohol preferring [P] rats and nonpreferring [NP] rats). P rats, in addition to voluntarily consuming approximately 10 times more alcohol than NP rats, also selfadminister alcohol and are willing to work for alcohol (reviewed in Crabbe et al. 1994). Because these characteristics are not typical of most rats, they indicate that, compared with NP rats, P rats possess a genetically determined difference in the neural substrate for determining the rewarding value of alcohol. These and other high-drinking strains of rodents are being used to identify chromosomal locations that correlate with the observed drinking behaviors and then to further refine this correlation to a single gene (e.g., for review in rats, see Bice et al. 1998; Carr et al. 2003; in mice, see Belknap and Atkins 2001). Although perhaps less complex than human genetic mapping, such research strategies remain complicated. And such searches are made even more difficult by the pleiotropic nature of genes (i.e., genes have multiple influences and sites of action) and their complex interactions. Transgenic mice (i.e., mice that lack a specific gene [knockout mice] or overexpress a gene product in a specific cell, tissue, or region) also have been used to assess the role of individual genes. Examples of candidate genes that have been identified or confirmed using transgenic mice include neuropeptide Y (reviewed in Thiele et al. 2003), protein kinase A (Thiele et al. 1998), and the cannabinoid receptor1 (CB1; Wang et al. 2003). More recently, gene mapping has been combined with gene profiling (i.e., analysis and comparison of gene expression patterns to identify candidate genes within chromosomal regions). Recently, studies combining these lines of investigation have identified α-synuclein as a potential contributor to alcohol preference in inbred preferring (or iP) rats (Liang et al. 2003). Another major approach using model organisms exploits the ability to generate mutations randomly across the entire genome of an organism (e.g., in worms or fruit flies) and then to screen these mutations for specific phenotypes or behaviors following alcohol administration. Because the basic behavioral responses to acute alcohol exposure are similar among humans, rodents, and flies, identification of mutant animals that are either more or less sensitive to alcohol exposure has been an area of active research. Studies in animals and humans suggest that reduced sensitivity to alcohol in an individual predicts development of alcoholism (Crabbe et al. 1994; Schuckit 1999). Mutated flies that show an increased sensitivity to the acute effects of ethanol have been generated. Analyses of the gene mutations present in these flies have implicated the cyclic AMP (cAMP) signal transduction pathway in the regulation of acute ethanol sensitivity (Moore et al. 1998). The requirement for proper cAMP signaling then was mapped to a small group of neurosecretory cells in the fly brain, which led to the identification of a cluster of cells in this location that produces insulinlike peptides (DILPs) (Brogiolo et al. 2001; Rulifson et al. 2002). Within these specific cells, inhibition of protein kinase A (PKA), a downstream component of the cAMP signal transduction pathway, increased ethanol sensitivity. In addition, flies that have a mutation which causes a reduction in insulin receptor activity show increased ethanol sensitivity (Corl et al. 2005). These results suggest a role for the insulin- signaling pathway in regulating behavioral responses to alcohol. In apparent contradiction, flies with a mutation in pka-RII, one of the PKA regulatory subunits, showed reduced sensitivity to ethanol (Park et al. 2000). Similar to the pka-RII-deficient flies, mice lacking the regulatory subunit of PKA showed increased voluntary alcohol consumption and were less sensitive to alcohol’s sedative effects (Thiele et al. 2000). However, mice with reduced levels of Ga’s, the adenylyl cyclase–stimulating G-protein, show increased ethanol sensitivity and reduced voluntary consumption (Wand et al. 2001). As a group, these results illustrate the complex nature of the regulation of ethanol sensitivity by the cAMP signal transduction pathway in a variety of cell types. Similar lines of investigation in flies and mice also have implicated neuropeptide Y (NPY) in mediating sensitivity to ethanol sedation and in modulating alcohol consumption (reviewed in Thiele et al. 2003; Wen et al. 2005). As mentioned previously, NPY also has been identified in human studies. In addition to sensitivity to acute alcohol administration, other factors such as reward, craving, and withdrawal also contribute to alcohol dependence. Both corticotropinreleasing factor (CRF) (reviewed in Valdez and Koob 2004) and NPY may play a role in maintenance of heavy drinking despite serious negative consequences. In addition, the dopamine pathway has been intensively studied to determine its role in the reward pathway (reviewed in Koob and Le Moal 1997). More recent studies also suggest a role for the endocannabinoids in the rewarding effect of ethanol (Wang et al. 2003). Studies analyzing mutagenized worms have uncovered a role for the calcium-activated large conductance BK potassium channel in ethanol sensitivity (Davies et al. 2003). A primary function of this channel is to repolarize active neurons, so activation in the presence of ethanol would inhibit neuronal activity. Alcohol use is a complex behavior that emerges from the interplay of genes and environment in the context of development. Adolescence The recognition that the prevalence of alcohol dependence is highest in those ages 18 to 24 focuses attention on identifying the genetic contribution to alcohol initiation in childhood and adolescence; adolescent developmental pathways to alcohol dependence; and adolescent vulnerabilities to the consequences of alcohol abuse. Very early starters (those who initiate alcohol use before age 12) often have several co-occurring problems that may include various externalizing and internalizing behaviors. Ongoing research is attempting to determine the order of causation and the potential underlying mechanisms that may be responsible for multiple problems as well as to identify early markers that might indicate some of those in need of early intervention. For example, what Cloninger (1987) classified as Type II alcoholism (characterized as male-limited, early onset, and occurring in sensation-seeking people and people with impaired impulse control) historically has been attributed to serotonergic dysfunction (Linnoila et al. 1994). Variation in the serotonin transporter gene promoter (5HTTLPR) in humans has been associated with Type II alcoholism as well as with other neuropsychiatric diseases (Hallikainen et al. 1999; Saunder et al. 1998). Work in nonhuman primates has confirmed this relationship between excessive alcohol consumption and serotonin system genes in macaques; and, in addition, it has integrated research on the effects of early environmental stressors. In particular, this work has shown how environmental factors can influence gene expression to produce different patterns of behavior (see also Barr et al. 2004 for review). The gene that encodes the enzyme monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) also influences synaptic concentrations of serotonin. Among adolescent and young adult male rhesus macaques, studies suggest that MAOA gene promoter variation may confer risk for alcohol dependence. Given that this gene resides on the X chromosome (males only have a single copy and females have two), males may be particularly susceptible to alterations in its expression. In humans, a polymorphism in the transcriptional control region of this gene has been associated with antisocial behavior in alcohol-dependent males (Samochowiec et al. 1999) and with impulsivity, hostility, and a lifetime history of aggression in a community sample of men (Manuck et al. 2000). Environmental factors and genetic variations may result in similar phenotypes by affecting levels of specific gene products. For example, nonhuman primates that are removed from their parents at birth and reared with age-matched peers exhibit higher levels of anxiety and deficits in impulse control and are prone to violently aggressive behaviors. These animals also consume significantly higher volumes of alcohol and are more likely to drink to intoxication than are those reared with their parents under baseline nonstressful conditions (Higley et al. 1991). When stress is induced in mother-reared monkeys, however, they increase their alcohol consumption to that of their peerreared counterparts. If peer-reared monkeys are given the serotonin reuptake inhibitor sertraline, alcohol consumption as well as anxiety and aggression are decreased (Higley and Linnoila 1997a). The level of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) concentrations in the cerebrospinal fluid predicts subjects’ response to sertaline, suggesting that a deficit in serotonin underlies these behaviors (see also Barr et al. 2004 for review). Consistent with the findings from adult twin studies, problematic use of alcohol in adolescence has been found to be more heritable than are initiation and more limited use. In a study specifically addressing adolescents, Rhee and colleagues (2003) applied a logistic regression model to data collected from sibling/twin/adopted adolescents ages 12–19. Their analysis resulted in the following conclusions: Alcohol initiation arises from genetic and from shared (common, family) and nonshared (uniquely experienced) environmental contributions. Alcohol use is minimally influenced by genetics but rather arises largely from environmental influences (shared, nonshared, and twin-shared experiences). Problem use of alcohol has a high heritability component and is significantly influenced by nonshared environmental influences (peer experiences, accessibility), but the contribution of shared environment (family) is small (Rhee et al. 2003). Whether the specific genes underlying the stages in the progression from initiation to problem alcohol use are different, and whether the influence of specific genes and their expression vary with age and/or context remains to be determined. These findings are consistent with those from other studies of adolescents. Rose and colleagues (2001) also reported that the strength of genetic influence on adolescent drinking behavior appears to increase from modest levels in midadolescence to moderate levels by late adolescence. And these types of findings are not unique to alcohol use; in late adolescence, genetic influences on problem drinking appear to overlap extensively with genetic influences on other indicators of disinhibited behavior (Krueger et al. 2002; Young et al. 2000). Although this latter research suggests that abusive drinking in late adolescence may be driven substantially by inherited differences in a general disposition to undercontrolled behavior, it does not rule out the influence of alcohol-specific genetic effects (i.e., genetic influences on alcohol sensitivity), or the impact of contextual factors. Indeed, mounting evidence exists that genetic influences on complex behavioral outcomes such as drinking behavior reflect a complex interplay between inherited and environmental factors (Rutter and Silberg 2002), the implications of which are only beginning to be explored for models of adolescent drinking (Rose et al. 2001). ADOLESCENCE AND IN VIVO ALCOHOL PHARMACOKINETICS Adolescence is associated with profound physiological changes that almost certainly have an impact on in vivo alcohol pharmacokinetics. There is scant direct evidence in humans of any differences between the pharmacokinetics of alcohol in adolescents compared with adults, and for good reason—no one would expose young people to alcohol for research purposes. Although data relevant to this point in humans is lacking, the ontogeny (i.e., development over the life span of an individual) of ethanol metabolism has been examined in laboratory animals. (Note, however, that what may be called “adolescence” in animals may not precisely correspond to the same developmental period in humans, especially when differences in the timing of adolescence in human males and females are considered.) These studies suggest general ontogenetic increases in alcohol dehydrogenase activity (Raiha et al. 1967; Lad et al. 1984), ethanol elimination rates (see Kelly et al. 1987), and the rate of ethanol metabolism (Silveri and Spear 2000). An exception to this pattern sometimes has been seen during adolescence, with adolescent animals occasionally reported to exhibit slightly higher levels of ethanol metabolism than more mature animals (Hollstedt et al. 1977; Brasser and Spear 2002). Significant elevations in ethanol metabolism during adolescence are not always evident (e.g., Kelly et al. 1987; Silveri and Spear 2000), however, and are insufficient to account for the attenuated sensitivity to certain ethanol effects seen in adolescent animals relative to their more mature counterparts (see Little et al. 1996; Silveri and Spear 2000). What can be expected in human adolescents with respect to specific aspects of pharmacokinetics as they relate to alcohol effects is based on inference from animal studies and from research on adults, elements of which are described in the following sections. Distribution Volume One feature of puberty is the appearance of gender differentiation in body fat. Girls have increased fat as a percentage of body weight (BWt). Because ethanol is soluble in water, the distribution volume for ethanol (VD, water space /BWt) is decreased in girls. Thus, girls experience higher blood alcohol concentrations (BACs) when they receive an ethanol dose that is proportionate to BWt (g ethanol /Kg BWt). In contrast, boys typically gain muscle mass and lose fat, increasing VD and thus reducing the BAC reached after they receive an ethanol dose proportionate to body weight (NIAAA 1993). Elimination of Alcohol Women reportedly metabolize alcohol faster than do men, a difference that probably becomes evident over the period of pubertal development. Similarly, the reported variation in alcohol elimination associated with the menstrual period among women presumably develops over the same age range. Whether the gender difference is an effect of changes in males or females (or both) should be determinable in animals (NIAAA 1993). Absorption Rate and Bioavailability Both the rate of alcohol absorption and bioavailability are largely influenced by prandial state (the quantity—and perhaps quality—of food recently ingested). Gender differences in this interaction, particularly as they may affect first-pass metabolism (the metabolism of alcohol in the stomach and its first passage through the liver), have been suggested by some but not all studies in adults. Nothing is known about pre- vs. post-pubertal changes (NIAAA 1993). NEUROBIOLOGICAL MECHANISMS OF ADOLESCENT ALCOHOL ABUSE AND DEPENDENCE Over the past 10 years, basic human and animal research has generated important new knowledge in the following areas: (1) identification of neurobiological and behavioral risk factors for alcohol abuse and dependence; (2) determination of the consequences of acute and chronic heavy drinking during adolescence on brain and behavioral maturation; (3) understanding of the neuropharmacological, neuroanatomical, hormonal, and behavioral mechanisms underlying the variable response to alcohol across developmental stages; and (4) assessment of the contribution of early alcohol exposure (during juvenile and adolescent periods) to excessive drinking and abnormal cognitive and social functioning in adulthood. Below is a summary of the current research findings on the neurobiological mechanisms involved in adolescent drinking. Predisposition to Alcoholism Neurobehavioral research in human adolescents has largely been limited to studies of neural risk markers in children with a positive family history of alcoholism. These investigations suggest that there are subtle heritable neurocognitive and neurophysiological abnormalities in children of recovering alcoholics which could be early indicators of risk for alcoholism (see Tapert and Schweinsburg 2005 for a review). The most common finding is reduced P3 amplitude of the event-related potential in children with familial alcoholism (Begleiter et al. 1984; Hill and Steinhauer 1993). More recently, other neural risk factors that predate the onset of heavy drinking are being considered in at-risk youth, such as sleep electroencephalographic abnormalities (Dahl et al. 2003) and changes in brain structure (Hill et al. 2001) and function (Schweinsburg et al. 2004). For example, it was found that youths with dense family histories of alcoholism show reduced right amygdala volumes, which correlate with P3 amplitudes (Hill et al. 2001). More importantly, the neurophysiological and neuroanatomical abnormalities may be most pronounced during the prepubertal and adolescent years. This latter finding underscores the importance of considering developmental phases when attempting to identify early risk markers for alcoholism. Taken together, these studies indicate that subtle neural abnormalities may underlie the heritable aspects of alcohol use disorders (AUDs) (Begleiter and Porjesz 1999; Pihl and Peterson 1996). However, some studies suggest that family history of AUDs primarily affects brain functioning in people who also show conduct disorder, antisocial personality disorder, sensation seeking, behavioral undercontrol, difficult temperament, or poor impulse control (Bauer and Hesselbrock 1999a,b; Schuckit 1998; Schuckit and Smith 1997; Tarter et al. 1985). Understanding these brain characteristics helps us to appreciate the brain abnormalities that may be produced by personal alcohol involvement as opposed to features that are attributable to predrinking risk factors. As discussed in the genetics section, animal models have been used to study heritable factors that contribute to alcoholism. The selectively bred alcohol-preferring (P) and high alcohol drinking (HAD) lines of rats are particularly good models for studying the neural mechanisms of early onset drinking because they readily consume alcohol in the postnatal weaning stage and attain adult levels of intake by adolescence. Even as early as adolescence, innate differences are observed in the P and HAD lines in several neurobiological markers that have been associated with a genetic susceptibility to high alcohol drinking (McKinzie et al. 1998, 2002; Strother et al. 2003; see McBride et al. 2005 for review). Further, nonhuman primates with low levels of the serotonin metabolite 5-HIAA have been used to model key aspects of adolescent behavior, such as impulsiveness and aggressiveness, tolerance to alcohol’s effects on initial exposure to alcohol, and the ability to drink excessive amounts of alcohol (Higley et al. 1996). Increased availability of serotonin transporters and low platelet monoamine oxidase activity also are thought to be traitlike markers in nonhuman primates associated with alcohol sensitivity and increased alcohol consumption (Heinz et al. 2003; Fahlke et al. 2002). This pattern of behavioral and biochemical markers is similar to that predisposing to early onset alcoholism in humans and is influenced by genotype–environment interactions. For example, recent studies in rhesus monkeys found that serotonin transporter genotype influences cerebrospinal fluid 5-HIAA levels as well as alcohol sensitivity, preference, and consumption, but only in animals exposed to early life stress (Bennett et al. 2002; Barr et al. 2003). Thus, it is important to understand the relationships among environmental factors, genetic backgrounds, and neurobiological markers in predisposing an individual to alcoholism. Ontogeny of Initial Tolerance and Sensitivity to Alcohol Adolescent rats consume higher absolute levels of alcohol than do older animals as a result of multiple factors. One is that adolescents are less sensitive than adult animals to the aversive effects of acute intoxication (e.g., sedation, ataxia, social impairment, and acute withdrawal/ hangover effects) (Little et al. 1996; Silveri and Spear 1998; White et al. 2002; Doremus et al. 2003; Varlinskaya and Spear 2004; for review, see Spear 2000 and Spear and Varlinskaya 2005). Another is their greater sensitivity to alcohol-induced social facilitation and stimulation of alcohol intake by social experiences (Hunt et al. 2001; Varlinskaya et al. 2001; Varlinskaya and Spear 2002; for review, see Spear 2000 and Spear and Varlinskaya 2005). The neural basis for the developmental differences in initial response to alcohol remains speculative. Recent evidence suggests, however, that the relative resistance of adolescents to the sedative effects of alcohol is related in part to both accelerated development of acute tolerance (Silveri and Spear 1998; Swartzwelder et al. 1998) and developmental immaturity of the GABA (Silveri and Spear 2002; Li et al. 2003) and/or NMDA (Silveri and Spear 2004) receptor systems. The available data on the consequences of longer-term adaptations (i.e., rapid and chronic tolerance) to alcohol’s effects in adolescents are inconsistent, with studies indicating more tolerance in adolescents than adults, similar levels of tolerance, or the appearance of sensitization rather than tolerance after repeated adolescent exposures (see Spear and Varlinskaya 2005). Stress, Hormones, Adolescence, and Alcohol Abuse Late childhood and adolescence are periods marked by dramatic sexual and psychosocial development. Between the ages of 5 and 9, adrenarche occurs, resulting in increased secretion of many adrenal steroids (cortisol, androstenedione, dehydroepiandosterone). Adrenal androgens in humans are associated with auxiliary and pubic hair growth and a slight increase in bone and skeletal growth. This is followed by maturation of the reproductive system, also referred to as “gonadarche,” which is characterized by increased activity of gonadotropins and the sex steroids (estradiol in females and testosterone in males). The hypothalamic-pituitary- adrenal axis response to stress also undergoes development during the pubertal period (e.g., Goldman et al. 1973; Sapolsky et al. 1985). Increased life stressors associated with sexual and social maturation, together with hormonally induced mood and behavior changes, could contribute to increased consumption of alcohol during the adolescent period (Tschann et al. 1994). In adult humans and animals, the relationships among stress, drinking, and underlying neuroendocrine or neurochemical mechanisms are complex. However, basic animal research suggests that stress-induced changes in glucocorticoids (i.e., corticosterone) may interact with neurotransmitters in the mesolimbic reward system to facilitate drinking. In adolescents, the interaction between stress and drinking is even more complicated because the neural systems involved in modulating alcohol reward and/or stress are undergoing development (see Spear 2000 for review). In a few studies of adolescent nonhuman primates, it has been shown that under conditions of social separation stress, subjects double their rates of alcohol consumption (Higley and Linnoila 1997b; Fahlke et al. 2000). In these studies, individual differences in stress-induced drinking are attributed to anxietylike behaviors mediated by ontogenetic changes in cortisol and corticotropin levels or to poor impulse control and impaired social competence associated with reduced serotonin functioning (a traitlike marker present in infancy). In rats, findings indicate that adolescent animals exhibit an attenuated corticosterone response to alcohol challenge compared with adults, and that gender differences in this response begin to emerge at adolescence (Ogilvie and Rivier 1996; Silveri and Spear 2004; for review, see Spear 2000). If elevations in corticosterone contribute to the rewarding effects of alcohol, as indicated in adult animals, then adolescents may need to increase their levels of alcohol to attain the reinforcing value reached by more mature animals at lower levels. At this point, however, any hypothesized interactions between stress-induced changes in hormones and reward-related neurotransmitters, and their impact on adolescent drinking, remain tentative. Gonadal hormones influence many aspects of brain development and behavior outside the realm of reproductive functions via their actions on receptors located throughout the brain (see Wang et al. 2002; McEwen 2002 for reviews). Alcohol’s disruptive effects on pubertal hormone secretion may interfere with hormonally mediated developmental processes and result in negative behavioral outcomes. For example, alcohol-induced increases in testosterone are related to augmented aggression in male hamsters following chronic alcohol exposure during adolescence (Ferris et al. 1998). Evidence from adult female nonhuman primates indicates that sensitivity to the subjective effects of ethanol changes during different phases of the menstrual cycle as a result of alterations in endogenous levels of ovarian-derived hormones (Grant et al. 1997). However, virtually nothing is known about adolescent females’ sensitivity to the subjective or aggressive effects of alcohol or the correlation of these effects with cyclical hormonal changes. Given that adolescence is a time when hormonal and brain systems are still developing in humans and animals, research on the relationships among life stressors, affective states, and hormonal/neurotransmitter interactions may be critical to understanding the onset, maintenance, and consequences of adolescent drinking. REFERENCES Babor, T.F.; Hofmann, M.; DelBoca, F.K.; et al. Types of alcoholics, I. Evidence for an empirically derived typology based on indicators of vulnerability and severity. Archives of General Psychiatry 49:599–608, 1992. PMID: 1637250 Barr, C.S.; Newman, T.K.; Becker, M.L.; et al. Serotonin transporter gene variation is associated with alcohol sensitivity in rhesus macaques exposed to early-life stress. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27:812–817, 2003. PMID: 12766626 Barr, C.S.; Schwandt, M.L; Newman, T.K.; and Higley, J.D. The use of adolescent nonhuman primates to model human alcohol intake: Neurobiological, genetic, and psychological variables. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021:221–233, 2004. PMID: 15251892 Bart, G.; Schluger, J.H.; Borg, L.; et al. Nalmefene induced elevation in serum prolactin in normal human volunteers: Partial kappa opioid agonist activity? Neuropsychopharmacology 2005. (Epub ahead of print) PMID: 15988468 Bauer, L.O., and Hesselbrock, V.M. P300 decrements in teenagers with conduct problems: Implications for substance abuse risk and brain development. Biological Psychiatry 46:263–272, 1999a. PMID: 10418702 Bauer, L.O., and Hesselbrock, V.M. Subtypes of family history and conduct disorder: Effects on P300 during the Stroop Test. Neuropsychopharmacology 21:51–62, 1999b. PMID: 10379519 Begleiter, H., and Porjesz, B. What is inherited predisposition toward alcoholism? A proposed model. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 23:1125–1135, 1999. PMID: 10443977 Begleiter, H.; Porjesz, B.; Bihari, B.; and Kissin, B. Event-related brain potentials in boys at risk for alcoholism. Science 255:1493–1496, 1984. PMID: 6474187 Belknap, J.K., and Atkins, A.L. The replicability of QTLs for murine alcohol preference drinking behavior across eight independent studies. Mammalian Genome 12:893–899, 2001. PMID: 11707775 Bennett, A.J.; Lesch, K.P.; Heils, A.; et al. Early experience and serotonin transporter gene variation interact to influence primate CNS function. Molecular Psychiatry 7:118–122, 2002. PMID: 11803458 Bice, P.; Fouroud, T.; Bo, R.; et al. Genomic screen for QTLs underlying alcohol consumption in the P and NP rat lines. Mammalian Genome 9:949–955, 1998. PMID: 9880658 Bowirrat, A., and Oscar-Berman, M. Relationship between dopaminergic neurotransmission, alcoholism, and Reward Deficiency syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 132B:29–37, 2005. PMID: 15457501 Brasser, S.M., and Spear, N.E. Physiological and behavioral effects of acute ethanol hangover in juvenile, adolescent, and adult rats. Behavioral Neuroscience 116:305–320, 2002. PMID: 11996316 Brogiolo, W.; Stocker, H.; Ikeya, T.; et al. An evolutionarily conserved function of the Drosophila insulin receptor and insulin-like peptide in growth control. Current Biology: CB 11:213–221, 2001. PMID: 11250149 Carr, L.G.; Habegger, K.; Spence, J.; et al. Analysis of quantitative trait loci contributing to alcohol preference in HAD1/LAD1 and HAD2/LAD2 rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27:1710–1717, 2003. PMID: 14634485 Cloninger, C.R. Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism. Science 236:410–416, 1987. PMID: 2882604 Cloninger, C.R.; Bohman, M.; and Sigvardsson, S. Inheritance of alcohol abuse: Cross-fostering analysis of adopted men. Archives of General Psychiatry 38:861–868, 1981. PMID: 7259422 Corl, A.B.; Rodan, A.R.; and Heberlein, U. Insulin signaling in the nervous system regulates ethanol intoxication in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature Neuroscience 8:18–19, 2005. PMID: 15592467 Crabbe, J.C. Use of genetic analyses to refine phenotypes related to alcohol tolerance and dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25:288–292, 2001. PMID: 11236845 Crabbe, J.C.; Belknap, J.K.; and Buck, K.J. Genetic animal models of alcohol and drug abuse. Science 264:1715–1723, 1994. PMID: 8209252 Dahl, R.E.; Williamson, D.E.; Bertocci, M.A.; et al. Spectral analyses of sleep EEG in depressed offspring of fathers with or without a positive history of alcohol abuse or dependence: A pilot study. Alcohol 30:193–200, 2003. PMID: 13679113 Davies, A.G.; Pierce-Shimomura, J.T.; Kim, H.; et al. A central role of the BK potassium channel in behavioral responses to ethanol in C. elegans. Cell 115:655–666, 2003. PMID: 14675531 Doremus, T.L.; Brunell, S.C.; Varlinskaya, E.I.; and Spear, L.P. Anxiogenic effects during withdrawal from acute ethanol in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 75:411–418, 2003. PMID: 12873633 Edenberg, H.J., and Kranzler, H.R. The contribution of genetics to addiction therapy approaches. Pharmacology & Therapeutics 2005. 108:86–93, 2005. PMID: 16026844 Fahlke, C.; Lorenz, J.G.; Long, J.; et al. Rearing experiences and stress-induced plasma cortisol as early risk factors for excessive alcohol consumption in nonhuman primates. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:644–650, 2000. PMID: 10832905 Fahlke, C.; Garpenstrand, H.; Oreland, L.; et al. Platelet monoamine oxidase activity in a nonhuman primate model of type 2 excessive alcohol consumption. American Journal of Psychiatry 159:2107, 2002. PMID: 12450967 Ferris, C.F.; Shtiegman, K.; and King, J.A. Voluntary ethanol consumption in male adolescent hamsters increases testosterone and aggression. Physiology & Behavior 63:739–744, 1998. PMID: 9617993 Foroud, T.; Edenberg, H.J.; Goate, A.; et al. Alcoholism susceptibility loci: Confirmation studies in a replicate sample and further mapping. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:933– 945, 2000. PMID: 10923994 Goldman, L.; Winget, C.; Hollingshead, G.W.; and Levine, S. Postweaning development of negative feedback in the pituitary-adrenal system of the rat. Neuroendocrinology 12:199–211, 1973. PMID: 4353346 Grant, K.A.; Azarov, A.; Shively, C.A.; and Purdy, R.H. Discriminative stimulus effects of ethanol and 3 alpha-hydroxy-5 alpha-pregnan-20-one in relation to menstrual cycle phase in cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Psychopharmacology 130:59–68, 1997. PMID: 9089848 Grant, B.F.; Dawson, D.A.; Stinson, F.S.; et al. The 12-month prevalence and trends in DSM–IV alcohol abuse and dependence: United States, 1991–1992 and 2001–2002. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 74:223–234, 2004. PMID: 15194200 Hallikainen, T.; Saito, T.; Lachman, H.M.; et al. Association between low activity serotonin transporter promoter genotype and early onset alcoholism with habitual impulsive violent behavior. Molecular Psychiatry 4:385–388, 1999. PMID: 10483057 Heinz, A.; Jones, D.W.; Gorey, J.G.; et al. Serotonin transporter availability correlates with alcohol intake in non-human primates. Molecular Psychiatry 8:231–234, 2003. PMID: 12610654 Higley, J.D., and Linnoila, M. Low central nervous system serotonergic activity is traitlike and correlates with impulsive behavior. A nonhuman primate model investigating genetic and environmental influences on neurotransmission. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 836:39–56, 1997a. PMID: 9616793 Higley, J.D., and Linnoila, M. A nonhuman primate model of excessive alcohol intake: Personality and neurobiological parallels of type I- and type II-like alcoholism. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 13: Alcoholism and Violence: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Psychology, Family Issues. New York: Plenum, 1997b. pp. 191–219. PMID: 9122496 Higley, J.D.; Hasert, M.F.; Suomi, S.J.; and Linnoila, M. Nonhuman primate model of alcohol abuse: Effects of early experience, personality, and stress on alcohol consumption. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 88:7261–7265, 1991. PMID: 1871131 Higley, J.D.; Suomi, S.J.; and Linnoila, M. A nonhuman primate model of type II alcoholism? Part 2. Diminished social competence and excessive aggression correlates with low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentrations. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 20:643–650, 1996. PMID: 8800379 Hill, S.Y., and Steinhauer, S.R. Assessment of prepubertal and postpubertal boys and girls at risk for developing alcoholism with P300 from a visual discrimination task. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 54:350–358, 1993. PMID: 8487544 Hill, S.Y.; De Bellis, M.D.; Keshavan, M.S.; et al. Right amygdala volume in adolescent and young adult offspring from families at high risk for developing alcoholism. Biological Psychiatry 49:894– 905, 2001. PMID: 11377407 Hollstedt, C.; Olsson, O.; and Rydberg, U. The effect of alcohol on the developing organism: Genetical, teratological and physiological aspects. Medical Biology 55:1–14, 1977. PMID: 321891 Hunt, P.S.; Holloway, J.L.; and Scordalakes, E.M. Social interaction with an intoxicated sibling can result in increased intake of ethanol by periadolescent rats. Developmental Psychobiology 38:101–109, 2001. PMID: 11223802 Kelly, S.J.; Bonthius, D.J.; and West, J.R. Developmental changes in alcohol pharmacokinetics in rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 11:281–286, 1987. PMID: 3307494 Koob, G.F., and Le Moal, M. Drug abuse: Hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science 278:52– 58, 1997. PMID: 9311926 Krueger, R.F.; Hicks, B.M.; Patrick, C.J.; et al. Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and personality: Modeling the externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 111:411–424, 2002. PMID: 12150417 Lad, P.J.; Schenk, D.B.; and Leffert, H.L. Inhibitory monoclonal antibodies against rat liver alcohol dehydrogenase. Archives of Biochemistry and Biophysics 235:589–595, 1984. PMID: 6393880 Li, Q.; Wilson, W.A.; and Swartzwelder, H.S. Developmental differences in the sensitivity of hippocampal GABAA receptor-mediated IPSCS to ethanol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27:2017–2022, 2003. PMID: 14691391 Liang, T.; Spence, J.; Liu, L.; et al. Alpha-synuclein maps to a quantitative trait locus for alcohol preference and is differentially expressed in alcohol-preferring and -nonpreferring rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100:4690– 4695, 2003. PMID: 12665621 Linnoila, M.; Virkkunen, M.; George, T.; et al. Serotonin, violent behavior and alcohol. Experientia (Suppl. 71):155–163, 1994. PMID: 7518265 Little, P.J.; Kuhn, C.M.; Wilson, W.A.; and Swartzwelder, H.S. Differential effects of ethanol in adolescent and adult rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 20:1346–1351, 1996. PMID: 8947309 Long, J.C.; Knowler, W.C.; Hanson, R.L.; et al. Evidence for genetic linkage to alcohol dependence on chromosomes 4 and 11 from an autosome-wide scan in an American Indian population. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics 81:216– 221, 1998. PMID: 9603607 Manuck, S.B.; Flory, J.C.; Ferrell, R.E.; et al. A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity. Psychiatry Research 95:9–23, 2000. PMID: 10904119 McBride, W.J.; Bell, R.L.; Rodd, Z.A.; et al. Adolescent alcohol drinking and its long-range consequences: Studies with animal models. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 123–142. PMID: 15789863 McEwen, B. Estrogen actions throughout the brain. Recent Progress in Hormone Research 57:357–584, 2002. PMID: 12017552 McKinzie, D.L.; Nowak, K.L.; Murphy, J.M.; et al. Development of alcohol drinking behavior in rat lines selectively bred for divergent alcohol preference. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 22:1584–1590, 1998. PMID: 9802545 McKinzie, D.L.; McBride, W.J.; Murphy, J.M.; et al. Effects of amphetamine on locomotor activity in adult and juvenile alcohol-preferring and -nonpreferring rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 71:29–36, 2002. PMID: 11812505 Moore, M.S.; DeZazzo, J.; Luk, A.Y.; et al. Ethanol intoxication in Drosophila: Genetic and pharmacological evidence for regulation by the cAMP signaling pathway. Cell 93:997–1007, 1998. PMID: 9635429 Mottagui-Tabar, S.; Prince, J.A.; Wahlestedt, C.; et al. A novel single nucleotide polymorphism of the neuropeptide Y (NPY) gene associated with alcohol dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 29:702–707, 2005. PMID: 15897713 National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Biochemical effects of alcohol metabolism. In: Eighth Special Report to the U.S. Congress on Alcohol and Health. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health, 1993. pp. 148–164. Ogilvie, K., and Rivier, C. Gender difference in alcohol-evoked hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity in the rat: Ontogeny and role of neonatal steroids. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 20:255–261, 1996. PMID:8730215 Oroszi, G., and Goldman, D. Alcoholism: Genes and mechanisms. Pharmacogenomics 5:1037– 1048, 2004. PMID: 15584875 Park, S.K.; Sedore, S.A.; Cronmiller, C.; and Hirsh J. PKA-RII-deficient Drosophila are viable but show developmental, circadian and drug response phenotypes. Journal of Biological Chemistry 275:20588–20596, 2000. PMID: 10781603 Pihl, R.O., and Peterson, J. Characteristics and putative mechanisms in boys at risk for drug abuse and aggression. In: Craig, T.G., and Ferris, F., eds. Understanding aggressive behavior in children. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 794:238–252, 1996. PMID: 8853606 Raiha, N.C.; Koskinen, M.; and Pikkarainen, P. Developmental changes in alcoholdehydrogenase activity in rat and guinea-pig liver. Biochemical Journal 103:623–626, 1967. PMID: 6069164 Reich, T.; Edenberg, H.J.; Goate, A.; et al. Genome-wide search for genes affecting the risk for alcohol dependence. American Journal of Medical Genetics 81:207–215, 1998. PMID: 9603606 Rhee, S.H.; Hewitt, J.K.; Young, S.E.; et al. Genetic and environmental influences on substance initiation, use, and problem use in adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry 60:1256–1264, 2003. PMID: 14662558 Rose, R.J.; Dick, D.M.; Viken, R.J.; and Kaprio, J. Gene-environment interaction in patterns of adolescent drinking: Regional residency moderates longitudinal influences on alcohol use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25:637–643, 2001. PMID: 11371711 Rulifson, E.J.; Kim, S.K.; and Nusse, R. Ablation of insulin-producing neurons in flies: Growth and diabetic phenotypes. Science 296:1118–1120, 2002. PMID: 12004130 Rutter, M., and Silberg, J. Gene-environment interplay in relation to emotional and behavioral disturbance. Annual Review of Psychology 53:463–490, 2002. PMID: 11752493 Samochowiec, J.; Lesch, K.P.; Rottmann, M.; et al. Association of a regulatory polymorphism in the promoter region of the monoamine oxidase A gene with antisocial alcoholism. Psychiatry Research 86:67–72, 1999. PMID: 10359483 Sapolsky, R.M.; Meaney, M.J.; and McEwen, B.S. The development of the glucocorticoid receptor system in the rat limbic brain. III. Negative-feedback regulation. Developmental Brain Research 18:169–173, 1985. PMID: 3986611 Saunder, T.; Harms, H.; Dufeu, P.; et al. Serotonin transporter gene variants in alcoholdependent subjects with dissocial personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry 43:908–912, 1998. PMID: 9627746 Schuckit, M.A. Biological, psychological and environmental predictors of the alcoholism risk: A longitudinal study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 59:485–494, 1998. PMID: 9718100 Schuckit, M.A. New findings in the genetics of alcoholism. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association 281:1875–1876, 1999. PMID: 10349877 Schuckit, M.A., and Smith, T.L. Assessing the risk for alcoholism among sons of alcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 58:141–145, 1997. PMID: 9065891 Schweinsburg, A.D.; Paulus, M.P.; Barlett, V.C.; et al. An fMRI study of response inhibition in youths with a family history of alcoholism. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021:391–394, 2004. PMID: 15251915 Silveri, M.M., and Spear, L.P. Decreased sensitivity to the hypnotic effects of ethanol early in ontogeny. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 22:670–676, 1998. PMID: 9622449 Silveri, M.M., and Spear, L.P. Ontogeny of ethanol elimination and ethanol-induced hypothermia. Alcohol 20:45–53, 2000. PMID: 10680716 Silveri, M.M., and Spear, L.P. The effects of NMDA and GABAA pharmacological manipulations on ethanol sensitivity in immature and mature animals. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26:449–456, 2002. PMID: 11981119 Silveri, M.M., and Spear, L.P. Characterizing the ontogeny of ethanol-associated increases in corticosterone. Alcohol 32:145–155, 2004. PMID: 15163565 Spear, L.P. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 24:417–463, 2000. PMID: 10817843 Spear, L.P., and Varlinskaya, E.I. Adolescence: Alcohol sensitivity, tolerance, and intake. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 143–159. PMID: 15789864 Strother, W.N.; Lumeng, L.; Li, T.K.; and McBride, W.J. Regional CNS densities of serotonin 1A and dopamine D2 receptors in periadolescent alcohol-preferring P and alcohol-nonpreferring NP rat pups. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 74:335–342, 2003. PMID: 12479952 Swartzwelder, H.S.; Richardson, R.C.; Markwiese-Foerch, B.; et al. Developmental differences in the acquisition of tolerance to ethanol. Alcohol 15:311–314, 1998. PMID: 9590516 Tapert, S.F., and Schweinsburg, A.D. The human adolescent brain and alcohol use disorders. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 177–197. PMID: 15789866 Tarter, R.E.; Alterman, A.I.; and Edwards, K.L. Vulnerability to alcoholism in men: A behaviorgenetic perspective. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 46:329–356, 1985. PMID: 4033133 Thiele, T.E.; March, D.J.; Ste. Marie, L.; et al. Ethanol consumption and resistance are inversely related to neuropeptide Y levels. Nature 396:366–369, 1998. PMID: 9845072 Thiele, T.E.; Willis, B.; Stadler, J.; et al. High ethanol consumption and low sensitivity to ethanolinduced sedation in protein kinase A-mutant mice. Journal of Neuroscience 20:RC75, 2000. PMID: 10783399 Thiele, T.E.; Navarro, M.; Sparta, D.R.; et al. Alcoholism and obesity: Overlapping neuropeptide pathways? Neuropeptides 37:321–337, 2003. PMID: 14698675 Tschann, J.M.; Adler, N.E.; Irwin, C.E., Jr.; et al. Initiation of substance use early in early adolescence: The roles of pubertal timing and emotional distress. Health Psychology 13:326– 333, 1994. PMID: 7957011 Valdez, G.R., and Koob, G.F. Allostasis and dysregulation of corticotropin-releasing factor and neuropeptide Y systems: Implications for the development of alcoholism. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 79:671–689, 2004. PMID: 15582675 Varlinskaya, E.I., and Spear, L.P. Acute effects of ethanol on social behavior of adolescent and adult rats: Role of familiarity of the test situation. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26:1502–1511, 2002. PMID: 12394283 Varlinskaya, E.I., and Spear, L.P. Acute ethanol withdrawal (hangover) and social behavior in adolescent and adult male and female Sprague-Dawley rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 28:40–50, 2004. PMID: 14745301 Varlinskaya, E.I.; Spear, L.P.; and Spear, N.E. Acute effects of ethanol on behavior of adolescent rats: Role of social context. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25:377–385, 2001. PMID: 11290848 Wand, G.; Levine, M.; Zweifel, L.; et al. The cAMP-protein kinase A signal transduction pathway modulates ethanol consumption and sedative effects of ethanol. Journal of Neuroscience 21:5297–5303, 2001. PMID: 1148605 Wang, L.; Andersson, S.; Warner, M.; and Gustafsson, J.A. Estrogen actions in the brain. Sci STKE [electronic resource]: Signal Transduction Knowledge Environment 138:PE29, 2002. PMID: 12084905 Wang, L.; Liu, J.; Harvey-White, J.; et al. Endocannabinoid signaling via cannabinoid receptor 1 is involved in ethanol preference and its age-dependent decline in mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100:1393–1398, 2003. PMID: 12538878 Wen, T.; Parrish, C.A.; Xu, D.; et al. Drosophila neuropeptide F and its receptor, NPFR1, define a signaling pathway that acutely modulates alcohol sensitivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102:2141–2146, 2005. PMID: 15677721 White, A.M.; Truesdale, M.C.; Bae, J.G.; et al. Differential effects of ethanol on motor coordination in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 73:673–677, 2002. PMID: 12151043 Young, S.E.; Stallings, M.C.; Corley, R.P.; et al. Genetic and environmental influences on behavioral disinhibition. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics 96:684–695, 2000. PMID: 11054778 Psychosocial Processes and Mechanisms of Risk and Protection Psychosocial research on adolescent drinking includes studies of personality and the impact of particular personality traits on drinking risk, expectancies (that is, the effects someone expects after drinking alcohol), and cognitive development. Although studies involving adolescents have not identified specific sets of personality traits that uniquely predict alcohol use, some traits have been shown to be associated with heavy alcohol use and alcohol use disorders. These traits include disinhibition or poor self-regulation, impulsiveness and aggression, novelty-seeking, and negative affectivity. Externalizing behaviors in childhood and early adolescence have been found to predict alcohol use disorders in early adulthood, as have certain internalizing behaviors. This article examines the theories and psychosocial processes thought to underlie underage drinking. Key words: underage drinking; adolescent; cause of AODU (alcohol and other drug use); risk factors; protective factors; AOD expectancies; predictive factor; AOD use behavior; personality theory of AODU; psychosocial environment; personality trait; negative emotionality; positive emotionality OVERVIEW The interactions among alcohol-related genes, biological development, and environment play out in the psychological processes underlying adolescent decisions to drink or to abstain from drinking. Psychosocial research on adolescent drinking encompasses studies of personality and the impact of particular personality traits on drinking risk, expectancies (the effects someone expects from drinking alcohol), and cognitive development. As is true for adults, studies involving adolescents have repeatedly failed to find specific sets of personality traits that uniquely predict alcohol use. In addition, adolescence is a period of change, and personality is not as stable as it will be in adulthood. Nonetheless, some personality traits have been shown to be associated with heavy alcohol use and alcohol use disorders in adolescents. These traits include disinhibition or poor self-regulation, impulsiveness and aggression, and novelty-seeking. Longitudinal studies have found that externalizing behaviors in childhood and early adolescence predict alcohol use disorders in early adulthood. Negative emotionality—depression and anxiety—also have been found to predict alcohol problems. Adolescents in this case may use drinking as a coping strategy. Expectancies about the effects of alcohol are measurable in children before they ever begin to drink. Alcohol-related expectancies influence how early a child will begin to drink and how much she or he will drink at that point. Research suggests that people who have expectancies of more positive experiences from drinking tend to drink more than others and are at highest risk for excessive drinking. Research is looking into the neural processes underlying expectancies and exactly how they drive behavior. An almost universal theme whenever adolescent drinking is addressed relates to how adolescents think and make decisions about the world around them. Despite much literature suggesting that adolescents have not yet reached full maturity in their cognitive processing, when called upon to make reasoned decisions using abstract processes, they generally do as well as adults. Differences in decisionmaking appear between adults and adolescents in situations that may have social or emotional overtones. Like adults, adolescents may vary their judgments based on social context, but the contexts that encourage such decisionmaking differ for adults and adolescents. With this in mind, adolescent thinking and decisionmaking may be best understood as fully developed for the purpose for which they evolved: to deal with the tremendous transitions that humans face at this stage of life. The goal for research is how to integrate this emerging understanding of adolescence with the need to reduce adverse outcomes. Many factors play a part in the development of adolescent drinking. Comprehensive theories on (or models of) the development of adolescent drinking create a framework for understanding and testing ideas about how multiple factors interact to lead to problems with alcohol. One of the goals of NIAAA’s underage drinking initiative is to stimulate the synthesis and testing of new and comprehensive models for adolescent drinking within a developmental framework. PERSONALITY AND ALCOHOL PROBLEMS IN YOUTH Although the relationship between personality and alcohol use disorders (AUDs) has been extensively studied in older adolescents and adults (Sher et al. 1999), far less research has been conducted with respect to personality and alcohol involvement earlier in adolescence. Indeed, personality in adolescence in general is much less developed as a research area than it is in adults. This is probably due in part to the tendency among researchers interested in individual differences in infancy and childhood to focus on temperamental traits that are thought to represent very basic tendencies in a person’s response to the environment. These temperamental traits are highly heritable and are usually assessed by parents or other adults. In contrast, researchers focusing on adults tend to look at more complex traits obtained through self-reports. Thus, the period of adolescence (especially early adolescence) sits at the junction of research traditions on childhood temperament and adult personality, which may be why this age has not received a greater amount of attention and development. Numerous personality traits have been described in the literature, but research suggests that most of these can be subsumed by a handful of higher order traits. Researchers disagree on the number of these higher order traits, but current influential models are usually defined as either “Big Three” (Eysenck 1990; Tellegen 1985) or “Big Five” (e.g., Costa and McCrae 1992; Digman 1989, 1990; Goldberg 1990, 1992). Big Three approaches typically describe their factors as representing: (1) negative emotionality or neuroticism, (2) positive emotionality or sociability or extraversion, and (3) impulsivity or behavioral undercontrol or (lack of) constraint. Both negative emotionality/neuroticism and positive emotionality/sociability/extraversion have their counterparts in Big Five approaches, but impulsivity or behavioral undercontrol or (lack of) constraint appears primarily to be reflected in the Big Five trait of (or lack of) conscientiousness as well as in the Big Five traits of (low) agreeableness and neuroticism. In addition, a higher order trait referring to either “openness to experience” or “intellect” (depending upon the personality system) also emerges. Research in older adolescents and adults strongly suggests that traits related to behavioral undercontrol are the most strongly associated with both alcohol use and alcohol use disorders, whereas traits associated with negative emotionality are somewhat less important (Sher et al. 1999). Existing research suggests that these basic findings generalize to younger adolescents, but the evidence base at present is somewhat underdeveloped (Caspi et al. 2005). SIDEBAR Cognitive Development and Adolescent Decisionmaking An almost universal theme whenever adolescent drinking is addressed relates to how adolescents think and make decisions about the world around them. As perhaps a reflection of this emphasis, the recent National Research Council and Institute of Medicine report on underage drinking includes two separate background papers on this topic (Halpern-Felsher and Biehl 2004; Jacobs 2004), which provide a foundation for this overview. The classic conclusion in this domain is that adolescents have not yet achieved full maturity of their cognitive processing and that they are more likely than adults to make risky decisions. A large body of literature exists containing many variations on the theoretical underpinnings for this conclusion. We will not attempt to review this material in any depth for this article, but a few ideas that have been central to this conclusion are worth noting. One is that the transition to the Piagetian concept of “formal operations” in thinking style has not yet taken place. Another view is that adolescents are very egocentric and feel invulnerable to harm because of their perceived uniqueness (Elkind 1967, 1978). Still another view is that adolescents use rational (reason-based) thinking in fewer situations than adults and depend more on intuitive processing that involves cognitive heuristics and judgment biases (Agnoli 1991; Barrouillet et al. 2002; Davidson 1995; Jacobs and Potenza 1991). Social considerations are a potentially important factor; that is, adolescents are understood to be very interested in their social standing among their peers and therefore are more vulnerable to decisionmaking that relies heavily on what other adolescents are doing. Related to this notion is that personal identity is less well established in adolescence, with the result that young people are more influenced by what they perceive others around them to be doing. A more recent version of this theme is based on neurological development: the neural substrate for emotional behavior develops in advance of the more frontal, rational decisionmaking portion of the brain (Luna and Sweeney 2004). Other versions of this theme are more specific to the alcohol field. For example, a recurrent view is that young people systematically overestimate the frequency and quantity of drinking being undertaken by their peers (DeJong 2002; Aas and Klepp 1992; Beck and Treiman 1996). Although there may be aspects of the above views that warrant further consideration, our understanding of the general domain of cognitive development and risk taking also is evolving in a way that offers an entirely different perspective on these issues. First of all, adolescent decisionmaking generally has not been found to be inferior to that of adults. When called upon to make reasoned decisions using abstract processes, adolescents generally do as well as adults. In many instances, however, both adolescents and adults perform poorly; unless reasoned thinking is somehow explicitly required in a particular circumstance, both adolescents and adults will use the more intuitive style of problem solving, leading to a generally equal rate of errors. It is evident, therefore, that we cannot entirely attribute the apparent riskiness of adolescence to fundamental differences in problem solving between adolescents and adults. Where differences do appear, however, is in real-world situations that may have social and/or emotional overtones. In these situations, adolescents often do not make the same choices as adults. When adolescent decisionmaking is analyzed closely for underlying processes, the results suggest that adolescents do not use appropriate base rate information about peers but instead use social heuristics or “rules of thumb” rather than actual counts of behavioral frequencies to vary their judgments based on particular contexts (e.g., to use different judgment rules when they are with social peers with high standing in the peer group, etc.) (Gardner and Steinberg 2005; Steinberg 2004). It is important to appreciate that adults also make decisions and judgments based on these biases. It is just that the particular content areas that accentuate biased decisionmaking may be different (e.g., appearance vs. role performance), as may the contexts in which these biases most often occur. And in some domains, decisionmaking actually may become more biased (i.e., based on social heuristics) as children mature into adolescence. It is clearly not the case, therefore, that humans become more “rational” with age in a linear fashion. Hence, although it is clearly appropriate to apply cognitive development and decisionmaking models to the understanding of adolescent drinking, these models should be informed by the emerging picture of adolescent development. For example, it might be best to broaden our conceptualization of adolescent thinking and not assume that adolescent thinking is deficient in some sense relative to adult decisionmaking, only to arrive at “full” levels of cognition with adulthood. Instead, adolescent thinking and decisionmaking may be better understood as fully developed for the purpose for which they evolved; that is, to deal with the tremendous transitions that humans face at this stage of life. How to integrate this emerging understanding of adolescence with the need to reduce adverse outcomes from drinking is a critical question. References Aas, H., and Klepp, K.I. Adolescents’ alcohol use related to perceived norms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 33:315–325, 1992. PMID: 1287824 Agnoli, F. Development of judgmental heuristics and logical reasoning: Training counteracts the representativeness heuristic. Cognitive Development 6:195–217, 1991. Barrouillet, P.; Markovits, H.; and Quinn, S. Developmental and content effects in reasoning with causal conditionals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 81:235–248, 2002. PMID: 11884089 Beck, K.H., and Treiman, K.A. The relationship of social context of drinking, perceived social norms, and parental influence to various drinking patterns of adolescents. Addictive Behaviors 21:633–644, 1996. PMID: 8876762 Davidson, D. The representativeness heuristic and conjunction fallacy effect in children’s decision-making. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 41:328–346, 1995. DeJong, W. The role of mass media campaigns in reducing high-risk drinking among college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol (Suppl. 14):182–192, 2002. PMID: 12022724 Elkind, D. Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development 38:1025–1034, 1967. PMID: 5583052 Elkind, D. Understanding the young adolescent. Adolescence 13:127–134, 1978. Gardner, M., and Steinberg, L. Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology 41:625–635, 2005. PMID: 16060809 Halpern-Felsher, B.L., and Biehl, M. Developmental and environmental influences on underage drinking: A general overview. In: National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. pp. 402–416. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Jacobs, J.E. Perceptions of risk and social judgments: Biases and motivational factors. In: National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. pp. 417–436. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Jacobs, J.E., and Potenza, M.T. The use of judgment heuristics to make social and object decisions: A developmental perspective. Child Development 62:166–178, 1991. Luna, B., and Sweeney, J.A. The emergence of collaborative brain function: fMRI studies of the development of response inhibition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021:269– 309, 2004. PMID: 15251900 Steinberg, L. Risk taking in adolescence: What changes, and why? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1021:51–58, 2004. PMID: 15251873 END OF SIDEBAR Stability of Adolescent Personality Because adolescence is a period of dramatic physical, social, and interpersonal change, it is reasonable to believe that the structure and stability of adolescent personality differs in important ways from the structure and stability of adult personality. Much of the current research on adolescent personality focuses on the rank order stability of various personality traits from adolescence through adulthood. Surprisingly, amidst a preponderance of significant life changes, the rank order stability of adolescent personality remains remarkably high. Specifically, correlations across time among Big Five personality traits range from .47 to .51 throughout adolescence (Roberts and DelVecchio 2000; Shiner 2005). Importantly, however, these relatively high correlations are far from unity. Moreover, personality stability appears to increase throughout adulthood (with peak correlations after age 50), suggesting that personality during adolescence is relatively kinetic and unsettled. Other studies of adolescent personality focus on differing levels of various traits in adult and adolescent populations (Caspi et al. 2005). These studies typically find that traits related to agreeableness, conscientiousness, dominance, and openness to experience increase from adolescence to middle adulthood, whereas traits related to neuroticism and sociability decrease during the same period (Caspi et al. 2005). It also should be noted that the structure and correlates of alcohol disorders tend to change across the life span. For example, a growing body of research suggests that different AUD symptoms have different meanings at different developmental levels (Chung et al. 2005; O’Neil and Sher 2000). Moreover, there are significant age-related changes in the prevalence and course of AUDs (Grant et al. 1994; Hasin and Grant 2004). It will be important for future research to examine adolescent alcohol and other drug use from a developmental perspective with a focus on how changes in the structures of both personality and AUDs interact across the life span. The relationship between personality and adolescent alcohol use is complex. As is true for adults, studies involving adolescents have repeatedly failed to find specific constellations of personality traits that uniquely predict alcohol use (e.g., an “alcoholic personality”). In addition, a dearth of research has examined the stability and structure of adolescent personality traits. At the same time, however, certain personality traits have been consistently associated with adolescent drinking patterns. Disinhibition. Traits related to disinhibition or poor self-regulation have been shown to predict both heavy alcohol use and alcohol use disorders in adolescent samples. For example, Soloff and colleagues (2000) found higher levels of impulsiveness and aggression among a sample of young adults with AUDs than among age-matched control subjects. Similarly, Gabel and colleagues (1999) found that novelty-seeking predicted alcohol and other drug dependence symptoms both in a sample of treatment-referred male adolescents and in age-matched control subjects. Other studies have found relationships between alcohol problems and behavioral undercontrol (King and Chassin 2004), rebelliousness (Brook et al. 1995), low constraint (Chassin et al. 2004), low harm avoidance (Jones and Heaven 1998), and a host of other disinhibited traits (Colder and O’Connor 2002; Moss and Kirisci 1995; Colder and Chassin 1997). Moreover, impulse control disorders such as conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and borderline personality disorder are highly comorbid with alcohol and other drug pathology in adolescents, suggesting the possibility of shared etiological pathways (Clark et al. 1997; Gabel et al. 1999; Serman et al. 2002; Chassin et al. 2002). Indeed, research by Slutske and colleagues (2002) indicates that much of the high genetic correlation between conduct disorder and adult alcohol use disorders is associated with personality traits related to behavioral disinhibition. Additionally, adolescents at high risk for the development of alcohol use disorders because of a family history of alcoholism have been shown to be characterized by high levels of disinhibition. Specifically, several researchers have found high levels of impulsive and externalizing behaviors among adolescent children of alcoholics (COAs). For example, Clark and colleagues (1999) found elevated rates of antisocial disorders among early adolescent COAs compared with their non-COA peers. Similarly, Sher and colleagues (1991) found high levels of behavioral undercontrol among late adolescent college students with family histories of alcoholism. It is important to note, however, that a number of studies have failed to find associations between COA status and externalizing behavior (Alterman et al. 1986; Pihl et al. 1990). Moreover, COA– disinhibition relationships tend to be small and may be the result of shared third variables such as parental antisociality or chaotic family environments (Sher 1997). Despite these methodological concerns, however, the majority of high-risk studies support COA/non-COA behavioral differences across a variety of methodologies and samples (Pihl et al. 1990). Perhaps the most informative data on disinhibition and adolescent alcohol use come from longitudinal studies. These studies often find that externalizing behaviors assessed in childhood/early adolescence predict AUDs in early adulthood. For example, Cloninger and colleagues (1988) used teacher ratings of 431 schoolchildren assessed at age 11 to predict alcohol abuse at age 27. Results showed that high novelty-seeking and low harm avoidance measured during childhood predicted alcohol abuse during adulthood. Similarly, Caspi and colleagues (1997) found that aggression, alienation, low harm avoidance, low control, and low social closeness assessed at age 18 predicted alcohol dependence at age 21. Using a somewhat different methodology, Iacono and colleagues (2002) found that 17-year-olds with attenuated P3 event-related potential amplitudes (often considered a marker of externalizing disorders) were at risk for alcohol and other drug use disorders at age 20. Moreover, studies using growth mixture models suggest that disinhibited traits measured during adolescence are associated with problematic alcohol and other drug use trajectories (Chassin et al. 2002; Chassin et al. 2004). Neuroticism/Negative Affectivity. Negative affectivity also has been shown to predict alcohol problems in adolescent samples. For example, Colder and Chassin (1993) found that negative affect was associated with both heavy drinking and frequency of alcohol use in a sample of 452 early adolescents. In a later assessment of the same sample, Colder and Chassin (1997) found that negative affect (1) predicted alcohol use and (2) interacted with impulsivity to predict both alcohol use and alcohol-related impairment. Other cross-sectional studies have yielded similar findings (Labouvie et al. 1990; Loukas et al. 2000; Krueger 1999). High-risk and prospective studies also suggest a relationship between negative affectivity and AUDs. For example, Elkins and colleagues (2004) found high rates of negative emotionality among nonalcoholic adolescents with a parental history of alcoholism. Similarly, Zimmerman and colleagues (2003) found that baseline anxiety disorders predicted the subsequent onset and course of AUDs in an adolescent community sample. It should be noted, however, that several researchers have found nonsignificant relationships between negative affectivity and alcohol use problems (Brook et al. 1986; White et al. 1986). In addition, some research using adult samples indicates that negative emotionality is a consequence, rather than a cause, of alcohol pathology (Sutherland 1997). Interestingly, other studies suggest that negative emotionality is only predictive of alcohol problems among adolescents who use drinking as a coping strategy (Cooper 1994; Newcomb et al. 1988). More research is needed to clarify the mediational role of drinking motives in the negative affectivity/alcohol problem relationship. A Snapshot of Research Findings on Adolescent Alcohol Use Disorders and Personality Traits Findings Study Disinhibition/Poor Self-Regulation Higher rates of impulsiveness and aggression were found in young adults with alcohol and other drug use disorders than among an age-matched control group. Soloff et al. 2000 Higher rates of novelty-seeking predicted substance dependence symptoms in both a sample of treatmentreferred male adolescents and age-matched control subjects. Gabel et al. 1999 Alcohol problems found associated with: Behavioral undercontrol Rebelliousness Low constraint Low harm avoidance Other disinhibited traits Impulse control disorders such as conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and borderline personality disorder are highly comorbid with substance use disorders in adolescents. King and Chassin 2004 Brook et al. 1995 Chassin et al. 2004 Jones and Heaven 1998 Colder and O’Connor 2002; Moss and Kirisci 1995; Colder and Chassin 1997 Clark et al. 1997; Gabel et al. 1999; Serman et al. 2003; Chassin et al. 2002 Adolescents at high risk for development of alcohol use disorders because of family history have been found to have high rates of disinhibition: Early adolescent children of adolescents had higher rates of antisocial disorders than non-COA peers. • Clark et al. 1999 College students showed higher levels of behavioral undercontrol. • Sher et al. 1991 Some studies have failed to find associations between COA status and externalizing behavior. • Alterman et al. 1986; Pihl et al. 1990 Relationships between COA status and disinhibition are small and may be caused by third variables such as parental antisociality or chaotic home environments. BUT: High novelty-seeking and low harm avoidance among 11- • Sher 1997 Cloninger et al. 1988 year-olds predicted alcohol abuse during adulthood. Aggression, alienation, low harm avoidance, low control, and low social closeness at age 18 predicted alcohol dependence at age 21. Caspi et al. 1997 Disinhibited traits measured during adolescence are associated with problematic alcohol and drug use trajectories. Chassin et al. 2002, 2004 Negative Affectivity/Neuroticism Negative affect was associated with both heavy drinking and frequency of alcohol use in a sample of early adolescents. Colder and Chassin 1992 A later assessment of the same sample found that negative affect predicted alcohol use and interacted with impulsivity to predict both alcohol use and alcohol-related impairment. Colder and Chassin 1997 High rates of negative emotionality were found among nonalcoholic adolescents with a parental history of alcoholism. Elkins et al. 2004 Baseline anxiety disorders predicted the subsequent onset and course of AUDs in an adolescent sample. Zimmerman et al. 2003 Brook et al. 1986; White et al. 1986 Sutherland 1997 Cooper 1994; Newcomb et al. 1988 BUT: Some researchers have found that relationships between negative affectivity and alcohol use problems were not significant. Some research with adults indicates that negative emotionality is a result rather than a cause of alcohol pathology. Recent studies suggest that negative emotionality is only predictive of alcohol problems among adolescents who use drinking as a coping strategy. SOURCE: www.MonitoringtheFuture.org Functional Relations Between Personality and Drinking Within the adult literature, there has clearly been a move away from simply demonstrating correlations between specific personality traits and alcohol involvement and toward specifying the ways that personality could affect alcohol involvement through plausible mediating mechanisms. To date, several mediating models have been proposed, including those which suggest that certain personality traits are related to specific drinking motivations (e.g., anticipated social facilitation or self-medicating motives), deviant socialization into heavy-drinking peer groups, and pharmacological vulnerability to reinforcement from alcohol (Sher et al. 1999). In turn, these mediators are believed to account, in part, for the association between personality and alcohol involvement. Available data suggest that these same general mechanisms are applicable to adolescent drinking, although there is probably some developmental specificity, with deviant socialization being a more important mechanism during adolescence. ALCOHOL EXPECTANCIES AND RELATED CONCEPTS Although the expectancy concept1 has been used across a wide variety of domains within behavioral science, it has received particular attention within the alcohol field (see Goldman et al. 1987, 1999). (1 Expectancies are stored information [memories] that anticipate future events.) And within the alcohol field, one point of special focus has been expectancy and the development of drinking patterns from childhood through adolescence and into young adulthood (Christiansen et al. 1989; Dunn and Goldman 1996, 2000; Gaines et al. 1988; Lang and Stritzke 1993; Miller et al. 1990; Smith et al. 1995; Zucker et al. 1995). The original application of the term “expectancy” within the alcohol field was to experimental conditions in balanced-placebo studies of the psychopharmacological effects of alcohol. In these conditions, the participants were led to believe that they were drinking alcohol but actually may have received either alcohol or a placebo beverage, or were led to believe that they were drinking a nonalcoholic beverage but actually received a beverage which may have contained alcohol (see Goldman et al. 1987). In general, according to these studies, the behavior displayed after alcohol consumption often was as much a function of the expectation (instructional set) that alcohol had been administered as it was related to what actually was administered (up to particular dosage levels). In the early 1980s, reports appeared in the literature describing studies in which participants responded to questionnaires that inquired directly about their expectancies—that is, the effects they anticipated if they were to consume alcohol (e.g., alcohol makes it easier to talk to people, to pick a fight, to be sexually responsive; see review by Goldman et al. 1999). Since that time, hundreds of studies of this type have been completed, with the consistent finding that particular response patterns on these questionnaires correlate substantially with self-reported alcohol use, and that these same response patterns can be used to predict later drinking over a time-lagged (longitudinal) period. Among those expectancies these studies most often identified as being related to higher levels of drinking are those for enhanced social/sexual functioning and positive emotional outcomes. Summed across studies, the data suggest that participants who hold more expectancies for positive/arousing outcomes from drinking, or more strongly endorse such expectancies on a Likert scale (with which respondents rate the extent to which they agree or disagree with a given statement), tend to drink more. Of special relevance to the present review is that these expectancies are measurable in children before they ever begin to drink (Christiansen et al. 1989; Dunn and Goldman 1996, 1998; Gaines et al. 1988; Lang and Stritzke 1993; Miller et al. 1990; Noll et al. 1990; Smith et al. 1995; Stacy et al. 1991; Zucker et al. 1995). And expectancies, even at this early point in development, are related to how early children will begin to drink and to how much they will drink when they begin drinking. It is important to note that some assessment devices for obtaining expectancies from children have been accommodated to their developmental level (in terms of reading level or mode of presentation; see Dunn and Goldman 1996, 1998; Miller et al. 1990). It is also important to note that the rudimentary development of expectancies has been established even before the start of elementary school (Noll et al. 1990). Perhaps most interesting is the finding that children in general shift from a primary emphasis on the negative or adverse effects of drinking alcohol before about age 9 to a primary emphasis on the positive and arousing effects of alcohol by about age 13 (Dunn and Goldman 1996, 1998). Those at highest risk for excessive drinking show the largest emphasis on positive/arousing effects. Although speculation as to the processes that might cause this developmental shift has been offered, the actual causes remain to be determined. It is, of course, quite possible to simply regard alcohol expectancies as a list or inventory of expected effects of alcohol. The history of the use of the expectancy concept in several different venues in behavioral science suggests, however, that expectancies can be an active process variable, with causal or mediational influence over behavioral output. For example, expectancy explanations have been applied to a wide variety of clinical phenomena including pain reduction, placebo effects, psychotherapy, and hypnosis (see Kirsch 1999), and to explanations of other psychological phenomena including comparative judgment (Ritov 2000), music appreciation (Krumhansl and Toivaine 2000), and even operant (Dragoi and Staddon 1999) and classical conditioning (Rescorla and Wagner 1972). Research also has linked underlying neural phenomena with expectancy. For example, neuronal signaling involving the neurotransmitter dopamine in the nucleus accumbens (part of the brain’s reward/reinforcing circuitry) has been noted to encode expectations about external rewards (Kupfermann et al. 2000). Emotional responses, centering on the operation of the amygdala (another component of the brain’s reward circuitry), also can be linked to expectancy (Kupfermann et al. 2000). Within the alcohol and substance abuse fields, expectancy has been understood to reflect differential dopamine activity in genetically selected animal lines (Katner et al. 1996) and the manner in which well-established individual differences in the metabolism of alcohol influence alcohol consumption (McCarthy et al. 2000). Viewed in this way, alcohol expectancies can have significant implications for how people make choices to use alcohol and about how much to use. Six increasingly stringent levels of evidence support a causal role (see Goldman 2002): (1) expectancies correlate with reports of alcohol use, accounting for up to 50 percent of the variance in drinking outcomes (Earlywine 1994; Leigh and Stacy 1993); (2) expectancies are found in children before drinking begins and predict drinking prospectively into adolescence and young adulthood (Dunn and Goldman 1996, 1998; Zucker et al. 1995); (3) expectancies increase with drinking experience (Smith et al. 1995); (4) at the point in life at which drinking decreases in many people, expectancies typically decrease (Sher et al. 1996); (5) when tested with statistical methods for demonstrating mediation, expectancies can be shown to mediate the influence of other known antecedents of drinking risk (Finn et al. 2000); and (6) expectancies can be manipulated in true experiments, with consequent changes in drinking levels (Roehrich and Goldman 1995). Despite the extensive body of research that has related the expectancy concept to alcohol (and other drug) use and abuse, precise processes have only recently begun to emerge. One approach has been to treat expectancies as the primary content of memory and then to use memory models and methods as a basis for examining expectancy operation. This effort has made use of empirical models of memory networks and associated mathematical techniques (e.g., multidimensional scaling and clustering) to depict these networks (Rather et al. 1992). Based on these models, experiments have been performed using well-established cognitive paradigms (e.g., implicit and explicit stimulus priming, false memory techniques, Stroop task, free association), resulting in findings consistent with the empirically derived memory models (e.g., Kramer and Goldman 2003; Stein et al. 2000). Particularly noteworthy were studies showing that cognitive priming influences actual alcohol consumption (Roehrich and Goldman 1995; Stein et al. 2000). Such approaches are only just beginning to be applied to the development of behavioral approaches to reduce the risk for excessive alcohol use and abuse. INFLUENTIAL THEORIES OF THE DEVELOPMENT OF ADOLESCENT PROBLEMS WITH ALCOHOL AND OTHER SUBSTANCES The theories outlined below are among the historically most influential in research on the origins and progression of problems in adolescents with alcohol and other drugs. These theories have tended to be directed toward antisocial and deviant involvement with alcohol and other drugs; they fail to address the underage drinking behavior of youth thought to be successful and mainstream. It is the goal of NIAAA’s underage drinking initiative to stimulate the synthesis and testing of new and comprehensive models that reflect the complex multicausality of all underage drinking behavior within a developmental framework. The “Gateway,” or Stage Theory This theory comes from epidemiological research that has examined patterns of alcohol and other drug use progression among adolescents. The original findings suggested that adolescents initially experiment either with alcohol and/or cigarettes (as legal and culturally accepted drugs) and then progress to marijuana. Once experience with marijuana as an illicit drug is acquired, adolescents may then try other illicit drugs such as heroin and cocaine. The experimentation with alcohol and tobacco, as legal drugs, is viewed as a necessary intermediate or “gateway” to illicit drug use (Kandel and Faust 1975). Opponents of this theory suggest that there may be a common risk factor (or factors) for illicit drug use that could account for the relationship between marijuana and other drug use independent of initial exposures to alcohol and tobacco. Examples of a theorized “third factor” include a genetic predisposition to problematic involvement with alcohol and other drugs, a predisposition toward adolescent high-risk behavior in general, or shared opportunities to obtain both marijuana and other drugs (Morral et al. 2002). A less controversial offshoot of this theory deals with the age of first experimentation with a broad class of abused drugs (whether it includes alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, or hard drugs), and the timing of stages of regular use and problematic involvement. The literature converges around the association of early alcohol and other drug use, including underage drinking, and subsequent problematic involvement (Choi et al. 2001; Choi et al. 1997; Kandel and Logan 1984; Schuckit and Russell 1983; Yamaguchi and Kandel 1984; Hawkins et al. 1997; Grant and Dawson 1998; Warner and White 2003). For this reason, substantial attention has been focused on prevention interventions that delay the initiation of alcohol and other drug use. Problem Behavior Theory Problem behavior theory is an influential conceptual framework for understanding not only problematic alcohol and other drug use but also a wide variety of high-risk adolescent behaviors (Jessor and Jessor 1977). The theory proposes a syndrome of comorbid adolescent problem behaviors that may manifest themselves within the same person (Jessor 1991). For example, adolescents with alcohol problems may engage in a spectrum of problem behaviors, such as illicit drug use, delinquent behaviors (e.g., truancy, petty theft, vandalism, lying, running away), risky and precocious sexual activity, and other high-risk behaviors (e.g., drag racing, drunk driving). A single, possibly genetic, factor is hypothesized to underlie a general syndrome of deviance that may predate adolescence and persist into adult life. Developmental support for this conceptualization comes from the findings of the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study. In one group of adolescents in this study, significant conduct problems were observed in preadolescence, adolescence, and later adulthood (life-course-persistent antisocial behavior). Another adolescent group was characterized as having behavioral deviancy that began and ended during adolescence (adolescence-limited antisocial behavior). Adolescents in this group ultimately had a positive adult outcome (Moffit 1993). Comorbidity Theory Epidemiological research has demonstrated significantly elevated rates of alcohol and other drug use problems among adolescents with defined psychiatric disorders. Most, but not all, clinical and community studies suggest that psychiatric disorders precede the development of adolescent alcohol and other drug use problems in these people (e.g., Ellickson and Hays 1991; Boyle et al. 1992; Van Kammen and Loeber 1994; Kellam and Anthony 1998). Some studies have found that psychiatric symptoms emerge during or after problematic involvement with alcohol and other drugs (e.g., Brook et al. 1998; Fergusson and Horwood 1996). Developmental research has suggested that the initial subsyndromal symptoms of most psychiatric disorders (except depression) precede the onset of adolescent alcohol and other drug use in people with both. Over time, these people receive full psychiatric diagnoses and subsequently progress to substance use and a substance use disorder diagnosis (Costello et al. 1999). The childhood psychiatric disorders known as the disruptive behavior disorders (i.e., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder) have the strongest associations with problematic involvement with alcohol, tobacco, and other drugs during adolescence. It is noteworthy that among these, adolescent conduct disorder increases the risk for alcohol abuse and dependence (Deykin et al. 1987; Disney et al. 1999; Lewinsohn et al. 1993; Moss and Lynch 2001). Depression also is associated with problematic involvement with a wide variety of alcohol and other drugs during adolescence (Kandel et al. 1997). Some anxiety disorders, such as separation anxiety, may be protective against alcohol and other drug use behavior during adolescence, whereas others, such as generalized anxiety disorder, may enhance alcohol drinking and augment risk (Kaplow et al. 2001). Adolescents with comorbid psychiatric disorders present a significant challenge for both prevention and treatment of adolescent alcohol problems. Maladaptive Coping Theory High-risk behaviors also may be adaptive to the extent that they serve a social or maturational goal such as separating from parents, achieving adult social status, or gaining peer acceptance (Spear 2000). Engagement in high-risk behaviors may help an adolescent cope with failure, boredom, stress, social anxiety or isolation, unhappiness, rejection, and low self-esteem. One example of maladaptive coping is an adolescent’s reported use of alcohol and other drugs as a means of gaining social status and acceptance from peers and, at the same time, enhancing mood and assuaging feelings of low self-worth (DuRant 1995; DuRant et al. 1995). Thus, problematic involvement with alcohol and other drugs for some adolescents may be a maladaptive means of coping with the stresses and social pressures that are characteristic of the adolescent stage of development, particularly in the absence of adult support, guidance, and monitoring. Patterson’s Developmental Theory This theory was originally proposed to explain the development of juvenile delinquency; however, consistent with the observation that problem behaviors frequently co-occur in adolescents, it also has been used to understand and address problematic involvement with alcohol and other drugs of abuse. Patterson and colleagues (Dishion et al. 1991; Patterson et al. 1989) are proponents of a developmental theory of conduct problems which assumes that adolescent problem behavior is a consequence of poor parental family management practices interacting with the child’s own aggressive and oppositional temperament. Here, temperament refers to the early and genetically determined behavioral characteristics that, over time and life experiences, evolve into personality. Deficits in parenting skill, such as harsh and inconsistent punishment, increased parent-child conflict, low parental involvement, and poor parental monitoring, result in school behavior and performance problems. The poorly performing and poorly behaving child may be socially rejected by average children; however, he or she forms close friendships with other problematic children. This process of forming close peer relationships is augmented by the negative interactions with caregivers in the home. As the child affiliates with more deviant children, he or she adopts deviant behavior as a norm and becomes less involved in home life. Other deviant children become powerful social role models; the child learns from them further deviant and socially unacceptable behavior, including experimentation with alcohol and other drugs. Early experimentation consistently has been found to be a risk factor for later problematic involvement with a wide variety of drugs. These children, therefore, may be viewed as being on a developmental pathway of deviancy and alcohol and other drug abuse that begins in infancy and is compounded by unskilled parenting and the formation of social relationships with other problem children (Vuchinich et al. 1992). Prevention interventions that are based on this theoretical approach offer parenting skills training to teach parents more effective ways to discipline and monitor their children and to reduce the negative environment of the home. Tutoring and other forms of educational support may be provided to reduce academic failure. Social skills training also may be offered to the child to help him or her cope with rejection by normal peers and to provide a mechanism for graceful resistance to peer pressure to use alcohol and illicit drugs. REFERENCES Alterman, A.I.; Bridges, K.R.; and Tarter, R.E. Drinking behavior of high risk college men: Contradictory preliminary findings. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 10:305–310, 1986. PMID: 3526955 Boyle, M.H.; Offord, D.R.; Racine, Y.A.; et al. Predicting substance use in late adolescence: Results from the Ontario Child Health Study follow-up. American Journal of Psychiatry 149:761– 767, 1992. PMID: 1590492 Brook, J.S.; Whiteman, M.; Gordon, A.S.; and Cohen, P. Dynamics of childhood and adolescent personality traits and adolescent drug use. Developmental Psychology 22:403–414, 1986. Brook, J.S.; Whiteman, M.; Finch, S.; and Cohen, P. Aggression, intrapsychic distress, and drug use: Antecedent and intervening processes. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 34:1076–1084, 1995. PMID: 7665446 Brook, J.S.; Cohen, P.; and Brook, D.W. Longitudinal study of co-occurring psychiatric disorders and substance use. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 37:322– 330, 1998. PMID: 9519638 Caspi, A.; Begg, D.; Dickson, N.; et al. Personality differences predict health-risk behaviors in young adulthood: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73:1052–1063, 1997. PMID: 9364760 Caspi, A.; Roberts, B.W.; and Shiner, R.L. Personality development: Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology 56:453–484, 2005. PMID: 15709943 Chassin, L.; Pitts, S.C.; and Prost, J. Binge drinking trajectories from adolescence to emerging adulthood in a high-risk sample: Predictors and substance abuse outcomes. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology 70:67–78, 2002. PMID: 11860058 Chassin, L.; Flora, D.B.; and King, K.M. Trajectories of alcohol and drug use and dependence from adolescence to adulthood: The effects of familial alcoholism and personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 113:483–498, 2004. PMID: 15535782 Choi, W.S.; Pierce, J.P.; Gilpin, E.A.; et al. Which adolescent experimenters progress to established smoking in the United States. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 13:385–391, 1997. PMID: 9315272 Choi, W.S.; Gilpin, E.A.; Farkas, A.J.; and Pierce, J.P. Determining the probability of future smoking among adolescents. Addiction 96:313–323, 2001. PMID: 11182877 Christiansen, B.A.; Smith, G.T.; Roehling, P.V.; and Goldman, M.S. Using alcohol expectancies to predict adolescent drinking behavior at one year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 57: 93–99, 1989. PMID: 2925979 Chung, T.; Martin, C.S.; and Winters, K. C. Diagnosis, course, and assessment of alcohol abuse and dependence in adolescents. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 5–27. PMID: 15789857 Clark, D.B.; Pollock, N.; Bukstein, O.G.; et al. Gender and comorbid psychopathology in adolescents with alcohol dependence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 36:1195–1203, 1997. PMID: 9291720 Clark, D.B.; Parker, A.M.; and Lynch, K.G. Psychopathology and substance-related problems during early adolescence: A survival analysis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 28:333–341, 1999.PMID: 10446682 Cloninger, C.R.; Sigvardsson, S.; and Bohman, M. Childhood personality predicts alcohol abuse in young adults. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 12:494–505, 1988. PMID: 3056070 Colder, C.R., and Chassin, L. The stress and negative affect model of adolescent alcohol use and the moderating effects of behavioral undercontrol. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 54:326–333, 1993. PMID: 8487542 Colder, C.R., and Chassin, L. Affectivity and impulsivity: Temperament risk for adolescent alcohol involvement. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 11:83–97, 1997. Colder, C.R., and O’Connor, R. Attention bias and disinhibited behavior as predictors of alcohol use and enhancement reasons for drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 16:325–332, 2002. PMID: 12503905 Cooper, M.L. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment 6:117–128, 1994. Costa, P.T., Jr., and McCrae, R.R. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 1992. Costello, E.J.; Erkanli, A.; Federman, E.; and Angold, A. Development of psychiatric comorbidity with substance abuse in adolescents: Effects of timing and sex. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 28:298–311, 1999. PMID: 10446679 Deykin, E.Y.; Levy, J.C.; and Wells, V. Adolescent depression, alcohol and drug abuse. American Journal of Public Health 77:178–182, 1987. PMID: 3492151 Digman, J.M. Five robust trait dimensions: Development, stability, and utility. Journal of Personality 57:195–214, 1989. Digman, J.M. Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology 41:417–440, 1990. Dishion, T.J.; Patterson, G.R.; Stoolmiller, M.; and Skinner, M.L. Family, school and behavioral antecedents to early adolescent involvement with antisocial peers. Developmental Psychology 27:172–180, 1991. Disney, E.R.; Elkins, I.J.; McGue, M.; and Iacono, W.G. Effects of ADHD, conduct disorder, and gender on substance use and abuse in adolescence. American Journal of Psychiatry 156:1515– 1521, 1999. PMID: 10518160 Dragoi, V., and Staddon, J.E.R. The dynamics of operant conditioning. Psychological Review 106:20–61, 1999. PMID: 10197362 Dunn, M.E., and Goldman, M.S. Empirical modeling of an alcohol expectancy memory network in elementary school children as a function of grade. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 4:209–217, 1996. Dunn, M.E., and Goldman, M.S. Age and drinking-related differences in the memory organization of alcohol expectancies in 3rd, 6th, 9th, and 12th grade children. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology 66:579–585, 1998. PMID: 9642899 Dunn, M.E., and Goldman, M.S. Validation of multidimensional scaling-based modeling of alcohol expectancies in memory: Age and drinking-related differences in expectancies of children assessed as first associates. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:1639–1646, 2000. PMID: 11104111 DuRant, R.H. Adolescent health research as we proceed into the twenty-first century. Journal of Adolescent Health 17:199–203, 1995. PMID: 8519790 DuRant, R.H.; Getts, A.; Cadenhead, C.; et al. Exposure to violence and victimization and depression, hopelessness, and purpose in life among adolescents living in and around public housing. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 16:233–237, 1995. PMID: 7593657 Earlywine, M. Anticipated biphasic effects of alcohol vary with risk for alcoholism: A preliminary report. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 18:711–714, 1994. PMID: 7943680 Elkins, I.J.; McGue, M.; Malone, S.; and Iacono, W.G. The effect of parental alcohol and drug disorders on adolescent personality. American Journal of Psychiatry 161:670–676, 2004. PMID: 15056513 Ellickson, P.L., and Hays, R.D. Antecedents of drinking among young adolescents with different alcohol use histories. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 52:398–408, 1991. PMID: 1943094 Eysenck, H.J. Biological dimensions of personality. In: Pervin, L.A., ed. Handbook of Personality Theory and Research. New York: Guilford, 1990. pp. 244–276. Fergusson, D.M., and Horwood, L.J. The role of adolescent peer affiliations in the continuity between childhood behavioral adjustment and juvenile offending. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 24:205–221, 1996. PMID: 8743245 Finn, P.R.; Sharkansky, E.J.; Brandt, K.M.; and Turcotte, N. The effects of familial risk, personality, and expectancies on alcohol use and abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109:122–133, 2000. PMID: 10740943 Gabel, S.; Stallings, M.C.; Schmitz, S.; et al. Personality dimensions and substance misuse: Relationships in adolescents, mothers and fathers. American Journal on Addictions 8:101–113, 1999. PMID: 10365190 Gaines, L.S.; Brooks, P.H.; Maisto, S.; et al. The development of children’s knowledge of alcohol and the role of drinking. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 9:441–457, 1988. Goldberg, L.R. An alternative description of personality: The Big Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59:1216–1229, 1990. Goldberg, L.R. The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment 4:26–42, 1992. Goldman, M.S. Expectancy and risk for alcoholism: The unfortunate exploitation of a fundamental characteristic of neurobehavioral adaptation. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26:737–746, 2002. PMID: 12045484 Goldman, M.S.; Brown, S.A.; and Christiansen, B.A. Expectancy theory: Thinking about drinking. In: Blane, H.T., and Leonard, K.E., eds. Psychological Theories of Drinking and Alcoholism. New York: Guilford, 1987. pp. 181–226. Goldman, M.S.; Del Boca, F.K.; and Darkes, J. Alcohol expectancy theory: The application of cognitive neuroscience. In: Leonard, K.E., and Blane, H.T., eds. Psychological Theories of Drinking and Alcoholism, Second Edition. New York: Guilford, 1999. pp. 203–246. Grant, B.F., and Dawson, D.A. Age of onset of drug use and its association with DSM–IV drug abuse and dependence: Results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. Journal of Substance Abuse 10:163–173, 1998. PMID: 9854701 Grant, B.F.; Harford, T.C.; Dawson, D.A.; et al. Prevalence of DSM–IV alcohol abuse and dependence: United States, 1992. Alcohol Health & Research World 18(4):243–248, 1994. Halpern-Felsher, B.L., and Biehl, M. Developmental and environmental influences on underage drinking: A general overview. In: National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. pp. 402–416. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Hasin, D.S., and Grant, B.F. The co-occurrence of DSM–IV alcohol abuse in DSM–IV alcohol dependence: Results of the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions on heterogeneity that differ by population subgroup. Archives of General Psychiatry 61:891–896, 2004. PMID: 15351767 Hawkins, J.D.; Graham, J.W.; Maguin, E.; et al. Exploring the effects of age of alcohol use initiation and psychosocial risk factors on subsequent alcohol misuse. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 58:280–290, 1997. PMID: 9130220 Iacono, W.G.; Carlson, S.R.; Malone, S.M.; and McGue, M. P3 event-related potential amplitude and the risk for disinhibitory disorders in adolescent boys. Archives of General Psychiatry 59:750–757, 2002. PMID: 12150652 Jacobs, J.E. Perceptions of risk and social judgments: Biases and motivational factors. In: National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. pp. 417– 436. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Jessor, R. Risk behavior in adolescence: A psychosocial framework for understanding and action. Journal of Adolescent Health 12:597–605, 1991. PMID: 1799569 Jessor, R., and Jessor, S.L. Problem Behavior and Psychosocial Development: A Longitudinal Study of Youth. New York: Academic Press, 1977. Jones, S.P., and Heaven, P.C. Psychosocial correlates of adolescent drug-taking behaviour. Journal of Adolescence 21:127–134, 1998. PMID: 9585491 Kandel, D., and Faust, R. Sequence and stages in patterns of adolescent drug use. Archives of General Psychiatry 32:923–932, 1975. PMID: 1156108 Kandel, D.B., and Logan, J.A. Patterns of drug use from adolescence to young adulthood: I. Periods of risk for initiation, continued use, and discontinuation. American Journal of Public Health 74:660–666, 1984. PMID: 6611092 Kandel, D.B.; Johnson, J.G.; Bird, H.R.; et al. Psychiatric disorders associated with substance use among children and adolescents: Findings from the Methods for the Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Mental Disorders (MECA) Study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 25:121– 132, 1997. PMID: 9109029 Kaplow, J.B.; Curran, P.J.; Angold, A.; and Costello, E.J. The prospective relation between dimensions of anxiety and the initiation of adolescent alcohol use. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 30:316–326, 2001. PMID: 11501249 Katner, S.N.; Kerr, T.M.; and Weiss, F. Ethanol anticipation enhances dopamine efflux in the nucleus accumbens of alcohol-preferring (P) but not Wistar rats. Behavioral Pharmacology 7:669– 674, 1996. PMID: 11224464 Kellam, S.G., and Anthony, J.C. Targeting early antecedents to prevent tobacco smoking: Findings from an epidemiologically based randomized field trial. American Journal of Public Health 88:1490–1495, 1998. PMID: 9772850 King, K.M., and Chassin, L. Mediating and moderated effects of adolescent behavioral undercontrol and parenting in the prediction of drug use disorders in emerging adulthood. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 18:239–249, 2004. PMID: 15482079 Kirsch, I., ed. How Expectancies Shape Experience. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 1999. Kramer, D.A., and Goldman, M.S. Using a modified Stroop task to implicitly discern the cognitive organization of alcohol expectancies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 112:171–175, 2003. PMID: 12653426 Krueger, R.F. Personality traits in late adolescence predict mental disorders in early adulthood: A prospective-epidemiological study. Journal of Personality 67:39–65, 1999. PMID: 10030020 Krumhansl, C.L., and Toivaine, P. Melodic expectations: A link between perception and emotion. Psychological Science Agenda 13:8, 2000. Kupfermann, I.; Kandel, E.R.; and Iverson, S. Motivational and addictive states. In: Kandel, E.R.; Schwartz, J.H.; and Jessell, T.M.; eds. Principles of Neural Science, Fourth Edition. New York: McGraw- Hill, 2000. pp. 998–1018. Labouvie, E.W.; Pandina, R.J.; White, H.R.; and Johnson, V. Risk factors of adolescent drug use: An affect-based interpretation. Journal of Substance Abuse 2:265–285, 1990. PMID: 2136115 Lang, A.R., and Strizke, W.G.K. Children and Alcohol. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 11: Ten Years of Progress. New York: Plenum, 1993. pp. 73–85. PMID: 8234939 Leigh, B.C., and Stacy, A.W. Alcohol outcome expectancies: Scale construction and predictive utility in higher order confirmatory models. Psychological Assessment 5:216–229, 1993. Lewinsohn, P.M.; Hops, H.; Roberts, R.E.; et al. Adolescent psychopathology: I. Prevalence and incidence of depression and other DSM–III–R disorders in high school students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 102:133–144, 1993. Erratum in: Journal of Abnormal Psychology 102:517, 1993. PMID: 8436689 Loukas, A.; Krull, J.L.; Chassin, L.; and Carle, A.C. The relation of personality to alcohol abuse/dependence in a high-risk sample. Journal of Personality 68:1153–1175, 2000. PMID: 11130736 McCarthy, D.M.; Wall, T.L.; Brown, S.A.; and Carr, L.G. Integrating biological and behavioral factors in alcohol use risk: The role of ALDH2 status and alcohol expectancies in a sample of Asian Americans. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 8:168–175, 2000. PMID: 10843299 Miller, P.M.; Smith, G.T.; and Goldman, M.S. Emergence of alcohol expectancies in childhood: A possible critical period. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 51:343–349, 1990. PMID: 2359308 Moffitt, T.E. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review 100:674–701, 1993. PMID: 8255953 Morral, A.R.; McCaffrey, D.F.; and Paddock, S.M. Reassessing the marijuana gateway effect. Addiction 97:1493–1504, 2002. PMID: 12472629 Moss, H.B., and Kirisci, L. Aggressivity in adolescent alcohol abusers: Relationship with conduct disorder. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 19:642–646, 1995. PMID: 7573787 Moss, H.B., and Lynch, K.G. Comorbid disruptive behavior disorder symptoms and their relationship to adolescent alcohol use disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 64:75–83, 2001. PMID: 11470343 Newcomb, M.D.; Chou, C.P.; Bentler, P.M.; and Huba, G.J. Cognitive motivations for drug use among adolescents: Longitudinal tests of gender differences and predictors of change in drug use. Journal of Counseling Psychology 35:426–438, 1988. Noll, R.B.; Zucker, R.A.; and Greenberg, G.S. Identification of alcohol by smell among preschoolers: Evidence for early socialization about drugs occurring in the home. Child Development 61:1520–1527, 1990. PMID: 2245743 O’Neill, S.E., and Sher, K.J. Physiological alcohol dependence symptoms in early adulthood: A longitudinal perspective. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 8:493–508, 2000. PMID: 11127421 Patterson, G.R.; DeBaryshe, B.D.; and Ramsey, E. A developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. American Psychologist 44:329–335, 1989. PMID: 2653143 Pihl, R.O.; Peterson, J.; and Finn, P. Inherited predisposition to alcoholism: Characteristics of sons of male alcoholics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 99:291–301, 1990. PMID: 2212280 Rather, B.C.; Goldman, M.S.; Roehrich, L.; and Brannick, M. Empirical modeling of an alcohol expectancy memory network using multidimensional scaling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 101:174–183, 1992. PMID: 1537963 Rescorla, R.A., and Wagner, A.R. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review 87:532–552, 1972. Ritov, I. The role of expectations in comparisons. Psychological Review 107:345–357, 2000. PMID: 10789199 Roberts, B.W., and DelVecchio, W.F. The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin 126:3– 25, 2000. PMID: 10668348 Roehrich, L., and Goldman, M.S. Implicit priming of alcohol expectancy memory processes and subsequent drinking behavior. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 3:402–410, 1995. Schuckit, M.A., and Russell, J.W. Clinical importance of age at first drink in a group of young men. American Journal of Psychiatry 140:1221–1223, 1983. PMID: 6614235 Serman, N.; Johnson, J.G.; Geller, P.A.; et al. Personality disorders associated with substance use among American and Greek adolescents. Adolescence 37:841–854, 2002. PMID: 12564834 Sher, K.J. Psychological characteristics of children of alcoholics. Alcohol Health & Research World 21(3):247–254, 1997. PMID: 15706777 Sher, K.J.; Walitzer, K.S.; Wood, P.K.; and Brent, E.E. Characteristics of children of alcoholics: Putative risk factors, substance use and abuse, and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 100:427–448, 1991. PMID: 1757657 Sher, K.J.; Wood, M.D.; Wood, P.K.; and Raskin, G. Alcohol outcome expectancies and alcohol use: A latent variable cross-lagged panel study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 105:561–574, 1996. PMID: 8952189 Sher, K.J.; Trull, T.J.; Bartholow, B.D.; and Vieth, A. Personality and alcoholism: Issues, methods, and etiological processes. In Leonard, K.E., and Blane, H.T., eds. Psychological Theories of Drinking and Alcoholism, 2 ed.: The Guilford Substance Abuse Series. New York: Guilford, 1999. p. 54–105. Shiner, R.L. A developmental perspective on personality disorders: Lessons from research on normal personality development in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Personality Disorders 19:202–210, 2005. PMID: 15899716 Slutske, W.S.; Heath, A.C.; Madden, P.A.F.; et al. Personality and the genetic risk for alcohol dependence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 111:124–133, 2002. PMID 11871377 Smith, G.T.; Goldman, M.S.; Greenbaum, P.E.; and Christiansen, B.A. Expectancy for social facilitation from drinking: The divergent paths of high-expectancy and low-expectancy adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 104:32–40, 1995. PMID: 7897051 Soloff, P.H.; Lynch, K.G.; and Moss, H.B. Serotonin, impulsivity, and alcohol use disorders in the older adolescent: A psychobiological study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:1609–1619, 2000. PMID: 11104107 Spear, L.P. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 24:417–463, 2000. PMID: 10817843 Stacy, A.W.; Newcomb, M.D.; and Bentler, P.M. Cognitive motivation and drug use: A 9-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 100:502–515, 1991. PMID: 1757664 Stein, K.D.; Goldman, M.S.; and Del Boca, F.K. The influence of alcohol expectancy priming and mood manipulation on subsequent alcohol consumption. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109:106–115, 2000. PMID: 10740941 Sutherland, I. The development and application of a questionnaire to assess the changing personalities of substance addicts during the first year of recovery. Journal of Clinical Psychology 53:253–262, 1997. PMID: 9075054 Tellegen, A. Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1985. Van Kammen, W., and Loeber, R. Delinquency, Drug Use and the Onset of Adolescent Drug Dealing. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh, 1994. Vuchinich, S.; Bank, L.; and Patterson, G.R. Parenting, peers, and the stability of antisocial behavior in preadolescent boys. Developmental Psychology 28:510–521, 1992. Warner, L.A., and White, H.R. Longitudinal effects of age at onset and first drinking situations on problem drinking. Substance Use & Misuse 38:1983–2016, 2003. PMID: 14677779 White, H.R.; Johnson, V.; and Horwitz, A. An application of three deviance theories to adolescent substance use. International Journal of the Addictions 21:347–366, 1986. PMID: 3721640 Yamaguchi, K., and Kandel, D.B. Patterns of drug use from adolescence to young adulthood: III. Predictors of progression. American Journal of Public Health 74:673–681, 1984. PMID: 6742253 Zimmermann, P.; Wittchen, H.U.; Hofler, M.; et al. Primary anxiety disorders and the development of subsequent alcohol use disorders: A 4-year community study of adolescents and young adults. Psychological Medicine 33:1211–1222, 2003. PMID: 14580076 Zucker, R.A.; Kincaid, S.B.; Fitzgerald, H.E.; and Bingham, C.R. Alcohol schema acquisition in preschoolers: Differences between children of alcoholics and children of nonalcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 19:1011–1017, 1995. PMID: 7485810 Environmental and Contextual Considerations A number of environmental factors can influence an adolescent’s risk for drinking, including parenting styles, an adolescent’s choice of peer groups, and even whether he or she is active in after-school activities. Alcohol advertising, the price of alcohol, and the degree to which underage drinking laws are enforced also play a role. It is difficult to establish the degree to which alcohol use is influenced by environmental factors. This article describes some of the environmental influences that may increase the risk for underage drinking. Key words: adolescent; underage drinking; environmental factors; risk factors; protective factors; drinking and driving; alcoholic beverage; AOD (alcohol and other drug) product advertising; AOD price; sales and excise tax; minimum drinking age laws; zero tolerance laws OVERVIEW The spectrum of environmental factors that can influence an adolescent’s drinking ranges from parents and family to the community at large and includes the availability, price, and advertising of alcohol. For a variety of reasons, measuring the impact of an environmental feature on drinking in a young person can be a challenge. Research has found, for example, that adolescents with supportive parents who monitor their children’s activities are less likely to be involved in risky behaviors than adolescents with less attentive parents. At the same time, genetic influences on personality can influence parenting styles as well as choice of peer groups and involvement in activities. Innate traits may help prompt an adolescent to, for example, choose a peer group inclined to risky behavior; however, that peer group is itself an environmental factor that encourages risky activity. One goal of research is to be able to provide an understanding of the interactions of genetics vs. environmental factors and their relative contributions to risky behavior. On a larger scale, alcohol advertising is pervasive in this culture, and much of it is presented in ways that appeal to youth. Some research suggests an association between adolescents’ reactions to alcohol advertising and their desire or intention to drink. Results have been mixed, however, in studies aimed at establishing whether alcohol advertising actually causes youth to drink. In contrast, most studies looking at the impact of alcohol price or tax changes on youth have found that young people’s alcohol consumption drops significantly in response to tax and price increases. Other research has examined the effects of alcohol prices or taxes on the harmful consequences of drinking; most studies looking at traffic fatalities have found that higher prices and taxes are associated with reductions in traffic crash fatalities among younger drivers. All States now have laws making it illegal to sell alcohol to people younger than age 21. Numerous studies have established the effectiveness of underage drinking laws in reducing both drinking and alcohol-related crashes among people under age 21. The National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) estimates that a legal drinking age of 21 saves 700 to 1,000 lives annually. All 50 States now also have zero-tolerance laws, which make it illegal for people younger than age 21 to drive after any drinking. These laws also have contributed to declines in alcohol-related traffic deaths among those younger than age 21. For a number of reasons, zerotolerance laws have not been vigorously enforced. This lack of vigorous enforcement occurs in spite of evidence from studies done before universal adoption of zero-tolerance laws indicating that States instituting these laws saw substantial declines in the proportion of people younger than age 21 who drove after any drinking. PARENTS, PEERS, AND COMMUNITY INFLUENCES Parenting styles, choice of peer group, and the community context in which adolescents are raised have all been heavily researched as possible risk-promoting or protective influences on drinking-related outcomes (Halpern-Felsher and Biehl 2004). As might be anticipated, numerous studies have found that children with loving, supportive, and involved parents had better developmental outcomes and were less likely to use alcohol than children raised in less supportive homes. Parental support encompassed monitoring their children’s activities while supporting their independence and setting limits (Barnes et al. 2000; Bogenschneider et al. 1998; Reifman et al. 1998; DiClemente et al. 2001; Davies et al. 2001; Steinberg et al. 1994). However, although parents’ awareness of their children’s activities is certainly important, as is controlling their whereabouts, the source of parents’ information about what their children are doing also is critical. Research suggests that facilitating children’s willingness to share information about their lives may be associated with better outcomes (Stattin and Kerr 2000). Parents who drank more and who held favorable views about drinking had offspring who drank more. Similarly, adolescents who spent more time with peers who consumed alcohol were more likely to drink (Colder and Chassin 1999; Curran et al. 1997; Sieving et al. 2000; Stice et al. 1998). Although it is tempting to conclude that the variables noted above truly have a causal influence on drinking, the evidence at this point does not establish a causal or mediational influence. Because we know that genetic risk for alcohol use patterns can manifest itself through personality variables, and that these same personality variables can influence parenting styles, choice of peer groups, and even engagement in after-school programs (and other environmental choices), it is difficult to establish the degree to which alcohol use is influenced independently by parents, peers, and environment. These questions must be pursued using genetically informed research designs and by experiments in which these variables are manipulated independently of genetic factors. THE ROLE OF ADVERTISING Youth are exposed to a significant amount of alcohol advertising. Alcohol ads appear in virtually all types of media. Such ads are common on television and often are presented in ways that appeal to youth and are shown at times when many youth are likely to see them. Half of televised beer ads, for example, air during Saturday or Sunday afternoon sporting events—programs that are popular among youth (Snyder et al. 2000). Beer is the beverage of choice for many youth, and between 1998 and 2002, industry spending on televised beer ads increased 45 percent to $972 million. Over the same period, spending on liquor advertising increased 530 percent to $18 million (Center for Science in the Public Interest [CSPI] 2003). Youth also routinely see ads for alcoholic beverages in magazines, on billboards, and on the Internet. For example, the Center on Alcohol Marketing and Youth (CAMY) found that youth saw 49 percent more beer ads and 20 percent more distilled spirits ads than did adults (CAMY 2004). CAMY researchers also reported that 12- to 17-year-olds hear more alcohol ads on the radio than do adults (CAMY 2003). Radio alcohol ads were frequently placed on stations with youth formats and were aired when youth were most likely to be listening (CAMY 2003). A study of Internet use by youth found that alcoholrelated Web sites contained features appealing to youth, such as video games and cartoons, but had few effective mechanisms to keep underage youth from accessing the Web sites (CAMY 2004). Scientists are trying to determine how advertising affects youth generally and underage alcohol consumption more specifically. A simple model of the effects of alcohol advertising would posit that greater amounts of advertising lead to more exposure to advertising, which leads to more drinking. Thus, much of the research in this area has been focused on explicating part or all of this sequence by: quantifying the number of alcohol portrayals in various media (including advertising); estimating exposure to advertising in various populations; studying whether exposed populations recall and are aware of alcohol advertising; examining how awareness affects alcohol expectancies and intention to drink; studying cross-sectionally the association between advertising and alcohol outcomes; and studying prospectively the causal relationships among advertising variables and drinking outcomes, such as initiation, escalation, and levels and frequency of consumption. Among these, the longitudinal studies are of greatest interest because they have the potential to address the fundamental questions of cause and effect. Assessing the effect of advertisements on the drinking behavior of individuals or populations is a complicated endeavor. It often is difficult to ascertain the specific effects of advertising because they must be measured against a background dense in alcohol messages and images. In addition, advertisements or alcohol-related messages will influence different individuals and different populations differently at different developmental stages and times in their lives. And furthermore, the mechanisms by which advertising may affect actual drinking behavior have not been extensively studied and are not well understood. One line of research in this area has directly studied young people’s reactions to alcohol advertisements and the correlates of those reactions. A study of third, sixth, and ninth graders showed that the third grade children who found alcohol ads desirable also were more likely to see positive benefits from drinking and to desire products with alcohol logos. Older children in the study who found the ads and logo products appealing were more likely to already be engaged in drinking behaviors (Austin and Knaus 2000). A related survey of 9th and 12th grade students examined the effect of media exposure on drinking behavior (Austin et al. 2000). Students reported on their television viewing habits, viewing perceptions, desire for alcohol products, and alcohol use. Findings supported a positive and indirect effect of media on adolescent drinking. The media influence beliefs about the appeal and desirability of alcohol, and the beliefs in turn influence drinking (Austin et al. 2000). Another study examined brain response to viewing alcoholic beverage pictures and nonalcoholic beverage pictures in 15- to 17-year-old heavy drinkers and nondrinkers using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Heavy-drinking teens showed substantially greater brain activation while viewing the alcohol ads relative to the nonalcohol ads, and this pattern differed significantly from that of nondrinkers (Tapert et al. 2003). Brain regions showing differential brain response suggested that heavy-drinking teens attended more closely, recalled pleasure and positive affect, and generated increased appetitive response while viewing an assortment of alcohol advertisements. On the other hand, Zogg and colleagues (2004), in an expectancy study of perceived positive and negative outcomes of alcohol use, found no predictive effects of exposure to televised alcohol advertisements, televised sports (which is dense in alcohol advertising), or firsthand observation of others drinking. SIDEBAR Social Policy and Law In a society in which alcohol is widely available and aggressively promoted and where alcohol use remains normative behavior among youth, what social policies should be adopted toward adolescent drinking? This question is not just about defining the legal drinking age. Social policy and law are not the same thing. Law is one tool of social policy, with some advantages (e.g., deterrence) and some disadvantages (e.g., individual and social costs of enforcement). Institutions of social control offer another means of strongly discouraging alcohol use by youngsters under a certain age, regardless of what age the law defines as the minimum for legal alcohol consumption. An important goal of research is to determine the ways in which the law can most efficiently be deployed while encouraging nonlegal institutions to play a more substantial role than they now do. Age of Lawful Access The law can use different ways of drawing the line between legal and illegal conduct. Right now, the law, by and large, uses an approach to defining the legal age of access to alcohol that is both binary (legal or not) and categorical (based on a simple age classification). An example of another type of approach is graduated licensing of young drivers, in which conditions are placed on driving during a transitional phase before they have unrestricted access to driving a car. Research is needed to explore the role of alcohol use in the lengthening transition from adolescence to adulthood, including what some investigators have called the periods of emerging adulthood and young adulthood. This research should be linked to studies of other developmental domains, including work and relationships with sexual partners and parents. This body of research may prove to have important implications for all transitional legal arrangements. Sanctions for Underage Drinkers An important objective with regard to drinking laws and youth is identifying the appropriate sanctions for violators. The policy challenge is to optimize the usefulness of laws against underage possession and related offenses. This requires attention to the types of sanctions that are needed as well as the enforcement strategies and judicial procedures that are used. In general, the goal should be to increase the declarative and deterrent effects of the law without harming the young person’s future life prospects. These judgments require research on a variety of issues, including the attitudinal and behavioral effects of different types of sanctions, different types of adjudicatory procedures, and different types and levels of enforcement. More generally, a better understanding is needed of the attitudes of young people at different developmental stages toward obedience to law, and of the ways in which decisions regarding use of alcohol (as well as tobacco, marijuana, and other drugs) affect and are affected by attitudes toward the law. These inquiries need to be tied to the developmental perspectives used to understand other aspects of underage alcohol consumption. Ways of Raising the “Price” of Underage Drinking Threatening to punish young people for obtaining alcohol is one way of raising the price of alcohol use. Another is to curtail the supply by deterring retailers and other adults from selling or giving alcohol to underage drinkers. Curtailing the supply makes the young person spend more time looking for alcohol, thereby increasing the “search costs.” Restricting outlets also can do this. A final way to raise the price is by increasing excise taxes. One political concern raised by tax increases and outlet restriction is that (unlike the other mechanisms) these tools also raise the price for adult purchasers. All of these issues require systematic understanding of where and how underage drinkers get their alcohol and, more generally, of the market for youthful drinking. END OF SIDEBAR Image advertising, which focuses on the lifestyle of the product user rather than the product itself, is preferred by underage youth (seventh grade) and has been associated with intentions to drink in the future (Kelly and Edwards 1998). A study involving male and female Anglo and Latino adolescents found that, both for males and females, positive responses to beer advertisements were associated with greater present and planned alcohol use. No differences were found related to ethnicity (Slater et al. 1997). Another study conducted focus group discussions with students ages 9 to 15 to learn what aspects of television alcohol advertisements made them attractive to young people. The students responded positively to ads with humor, talking animals, and youthful lifestyle appeal and negatively to the product focus of the ads (Waiters et al. 2001). Although they are informative and interesting, these studies do not address the question of causality: Do alcohol advertisements cause youth to drink, or do youth who already drink pay more attention to alcohol advertising? Two recent cross-sectional studies found positive associations between advertising and consumption. Collins and colleagues (2003) measured advertisement awareness, drinking beliefs, and drinking behavior among eighth grade students. These researchers found that boys are more likely to be aware of and remember beer marketing and may be more likely to drink as a result of this awareness. Another study examined whether recall of and liking of alcohol advertisements leads to greater intentions to drink in the future and higher consumption of alcohol (Chen and Grube 2001). This study sampled students in grades 5 to 8 and grades 9 to 11 and measured their response to 16 alcohol ads and 4 soft drink ads. The study found that liking specific elements of alcohol ads (characters, humor, story line) predicted liking the advertisements, and that liking the advertising directly predicted current drinking levels and had significant indirect effects on drinking and future intentions to drink. Results of earlier studies that examined the relationship between liking alcohol advertising and current and future intentions to drink, however, were mixed (Kelly and Edwards 1998; Wylie et al. 1998). A few prospective studies also have addressed this issue. A longitudinal study of New Zealand youth found that liking alcohol advertising at age 18 was related to higher levels of beer consumption at age 21 (Casswell and Zhang 1998). Two additional recent prospective studies found a positive relationship between exposure to advertising and consumption. Ellickson and colleagues (2003) found in a sample of seventh grade drinkers and nondrinkers from North Dakota that several forms of advertising predicted future adolescent drinking for both groups. And Stacy and colleagues (2004) found that exposure to advertising increased the risk of subsequent beer consumption. Another group of potentially informative investigations are econometric studies of the relationship between alcohol advertising and consumption. Results of these studies also have been mixed. A study by Saffer (2002) found that advertising increased consumption, whereas other studies found that alcohol advertising affects brand choice but not overall consumption (Nelson and Moran 1995; Gius 1996). Another study by Saffer and Dhaval (2003) suggests that a complete ban on alcohol advertising might reduce the prevalence of monthly drinking by 12- to 18-year-olds from about 25 percent to 21 percent and of binge drinking from 12 percent to 7 percent (Saffer and Dhaval 2003). Despite their potential effectiveness for reducing underage drinking, comprehensive advertising bans are not likely to receive public support, and partial bans are likely to prompt the alcohol industry to increase their ads in other media (Saffer 2002). In general, research on the impact of alcohol advertising on actual drinking behavior has been mixed, and observed effects have been small. In addition, many of the cited studies are subject to recall bias. Furthermore, many studies have been cross-sectional, making it difficult to draw definitive conclusions about the relationship between advertising and alcohol consumption (Grube 2004). TEXT BOX By the Numbers: Alcohol Advertising, Price, and Legislation Industry spending on TV beer ads, 2002: $972 million (up 45 percent from 1998) (CSPI 2003). Industry spending on TV liquor ads, 2002: $18 million (up 530 percent from 1998) (CSPI 2003). Young people see 49 percent more beer ads and 27 percent more ads for distilled spirits than adults see (CAMY 2004). Studies have found that young people’s alcohol consumption drops significantly in response to price or tax changes, in some cases exceeding the reductions estimated for the general population (Grossman et al. 1987; Coate and Grossman 1988; Kenkel 1993; Sutton and Godfrey 1995; Ruhm 1996; Grossman et al. 1998). When States increased the legal drinking age to 21, alcohol-related crashes among people younger than 21 decreased an average of 16 percent (Shults et al. 2001). NHTSA estimates that the legal drinking age of 21 saves 700 to 1,000 American lives annually, and has prevented more than 21,000 traffic deaths since 1976 (NHTSA 2003). The first 30 States to adopt zero-tolerance laws had a 19percent decline in the proportion of people younger than 21 who drove after drinking, when compared with States without these laws, and a 23-percent decline in the proportion who drove after five or more drinks (Wagenaar et al. 2001). END OF TEXT BOX THE EFFECT OF PRICE ON ADOLESCENT ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION A substantial body of research has shown that higher prices or taxes on alcoholic beverages are associated with lower levels of alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems (Leung and Phelps 1993; Kenkel and Manning 1996; Chaloupka et al. 1998; Cook and Moore 2002). Estimates vary, however, in the extent to which consumption or problems change in response to a given price or tax change. Some studies have examined these effects among young people separately from the general population. Most such studies have found that young people’s alcohol consumption drops significantly in response to price or tax changes, in some cases exceeding the reductions estimated for the general population (Grossman et al. 1987; Coate and Grossman 1988; Kenkel 1993; Sutton and Godfrey 1995; Ruhm 1996; Grossman et al. 1998). An exception is the study by Dee (1999), which found only small and statistically insignificant effects of beer taxes on teens’ drinking behavior. In addition, Chaloupka and Wechsler (1996) found that, although higher beer prices tend to decrease drinking and binge drinking among U.S. college students, price is a relatively weak tool for influencing these behaviors, especially among males. In a study of the population age 17 and older, Manning and colleagues (1995) found that alcohol consumption decreased in response to price increases for all but the top 5 percent of drinkers, who exhibited no significant price response. Several studies have examined the effects of alcohol prices or taxes on traffic crash fatalities and other alcohol-related problems. Most such studies have reported that higher taxes or prices were associated with significant reductions in traffic crash fatalities or drunk driving, particularly among younger drivers and during nighttime hours (Saffer and Grossman 1987; Chaloupka et al. 1993; Kenkel 1993; Ruhm 1996). A few later studies have questioned these findings. Dee (1999) found some evidence that beer taxes tend to reduce teen traffic fatalities but concluded that those results were not robust and should be viewed with skepticism. Young and Likens (2000) found no significant effects of beer taxes on traffic crash fatality rates, either for young drivers or the general population. Mast and colleagues (1999) found mixed results, with several analyses indicating significant but relatively small effects of beer taxation on traffic fatalities. Other research has found associations between higher alcoholic beverage taxes and lower rates of some types of violent crime (Cook and Moore 1993a), reduced incidence of physical child abuse committed by women (Markowitz and Grossman 2000), and lower rates of sexually transmitted diseases (Chesson et al. 2000), as well as with increases in college graduation rates (Cook and Moore 1993b). Further research is needed to clarify the effects that alcoholic beverage prices or taxes have on different drinking behaviors, health-related outcomes, and population subgroups, and to reconcile conflicting findings that have appeared in the literature. To date, however, the weight of evidence suggests that higher prices and taxes can help to reduce alcohol consumption and alcoholrelated problems. THE EFFECT OF DRINKING LAWS ON ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION BY ADOLESCENTS Legal Drinking Age of 21 In 1984, when 25 States had a legal drinking age of 21, the U.S. Congress passed legislation that would withhold highway construction funds from States that did not make it illegal to sell alcohol to people younger than age 21. By 1988, all States adopted such a law. A review of more than 49 studies of legal drinking age changes revealed that in the 1970s and 1980s, when many States lowered the drinking age, alcohol-related traffic crashes increased 10 percent. In contrast, when States increased the legal drinking age to 21, alcohol-related crashes among people younger than age 21 decreased an average of 16 percent (Shults et al. 2001). Wagenaar and Toomey (2002) reviewed more than 48 studies of the effects of drinking age changes on drinking and 57 studies of traffic crashes. They concluded that increases in the age of legal alcohol purchase and consumption have been the most successful intervention to date in reducing drinking and alcoholrelated crashes among people under age 21. One national study of laws raising the drinking age to 21 indicated that people who grew up in States with a drinking age of 21 relative to those with lower legal drinking ages drank less not only when they were younger than age 21 but also when they were ages 21 to 25 (O’Malley and Wagenaar 1991). NHTSA (2003) estimates that a legal drinking age of 21 saves 700 to 1,000 lives annually and that more than 21,000 traffic deaths have been prevented by such laws since 1976. Zero-Tolerance Laws All States now have zero-tolerance laws that make it illegal for people under age 21 to drive after any drinking. These laws also have contributed to declines in alcohol-related traffic deaths among people younger than age 21. A comparison of the first eight States to adopt zero-tolerance laws with nearby States without such laws revealed a 21-percent greater decline in zero-tolerance law States in the proportion of fatal crashes among drivers younger than age 21 that were of the type most likely to involve alcohol (i.e., single-vehicle fatal crashes at night) (Hingson et al. 1994). Wagenaar and colleagues (2001) found that in the first 30 States to adopt zero-tolerance laws, relative to the rest of the nation, there was a 19-percent decline in the proportion of people younger than age 21 who drove after any drinking and a 23-percent decline in the proportion who drove after five or more drinks. Unfortunately, despite their demonstrated benefits, legal drinking age and zero-tolerance laws generally have not been vigorously enforced (Jones and Lacey 2001). Young drivers are substantially underrepresented in the driving while intoxicated (DWI) arrest population relative to their contributions to the alcohol-crash problem (Preusser et al. 1992; Voas and Williams 1986). Younger drivers may be more likely to drink in locations where DWI enforcement resources are less likely to be deployed. Young drivers with high blood alcohol concentrations also are more likely to be missed by police at sobriety checkpoints (Wells et al. 1997). Stepped-up enforcement of alcohol purchase laws aimed at sellers and buyers can be effective (Preusser et al.1994; Wagenaar et al. 2000) if resources are made available for this purpose. Enforcement of zero-tolerance laws is hindered in some States because their implied-consent laws require either an arrest for DWI or probable cause for a DWI arrest before the evidentiary test can be done to prove a zero-tolerance violation (Ferguson et al. 2000). Thus, in practice, zero-tolerance laws often are not enforced independently of DWI. In States such as New Mexico, where this situation exists, the majority of teenagers are unaware that there is a zero-tolerance law (Ferguson and Williams 2002). REFERENCES Austin, E.W., and Knaus, C. Predicting the potential for risky behavior among those “too young” to drink as the result of appealing advertising. Journal of Health Communications 5:13–27, 2000. PMID: 10848029 Austin, E.W.; Pinkleton, B.E.; and Fujioka, Y. The role of interpretation processes and parental discussion in the media’s effects on adolescents’ use of alcohol. Pediatrics 105:343–349, 2000. PMID: 10654953 Barnes, G.M.; Reifman, A.S.; Farrell, M.P.; and Dintcheff, B.A. The effects of parenting on the development of adolescent alcohol misuse: A six-wave latent growth model. Journal of Marriage and Family 62:175–186, 2000. Bogenschneider, K.; Wu, M.Y.; Raffaelli, M.; and Tsay, J.C. Parent influences on adolescent peer orientation and substance use: The interface of parenting practices and values. Child Development 69:1672–1688, 1998. PMID: 9914644 Casswell, S., and Zhang, J.F. Impact of liking for advertising and brand allegiance on drinking and alcohol-related aggression: A longitudinal study. Addiction 93: 1209–1217, 1998. PMID: 9813902 Center for Science in the Public Interest (CSPI). Alcohol Advertising Expenditures, 1998–2002. Alcohol Policies Project Fact Sheet. Washington, DC: CSPI, 2003. Available online at: http://www.cspinet.org/booze/FactSheets/AlcAdExp.pdf. Center on Alcohol Marketing and Youth (CAMY). Radio Daze: Alcohol Ads Tune in Underage Youth. Washington, DC: Georgetown University, Center on Alcohol Marketing and Youth, 2003. Available online at: http://camy.org/research/radio0303/. Center on Alcohol Marketing and Youth (CAMY). Clicking With Kids: Alcohol Marketing and Youth on the Internet. Washington, DC: Georgetown University, Center on Alcohol Marketing and Youth, 2004. Available online at: http://camy.org/research/internet0304/report-high.pdf. Chaloupka, F.J., and Wechsler, H. Binge drinking in college: The impact of price, availability, and alcohol control policies. Contemporary Economic Policy 14:112–124, 1996. Chaloupka, F.J.; Saffer, H.; and Grossman, M. Alcohol-control policies and motor-vehicle fatalities. Journal of Legal Studies 22:161–186, 1993. Chaloupka, F.J.; Grossman, M.; and Saffer, H. The effects of price on the consequences of alcohol use and abuse. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 14: The Consequences of Alcoholism. New York: Plenum Press, 1998. pp. 331–346. PMID: 9751952 Chen, M.J., and Grube, J.W. “TV, Beer, and Soft Drink Advertising: What Young People Like and What Effects?” Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Research Society on Alcoholism, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, June 2001. Chesson, H.; Harrison, P.; and Kassler, W.J. Sex under the influence: The effect of alcohol policy on sexually transmitted disease rates in the United States. Journal of Law and Economics 43:215–238, 2000. Coate, D., and Grossman, M. Effects of alcoholic beverage prices and legal drinking ages on youth alcohol use. Journal of Law and Economics 31:145–171, 1988. Colder, C.R., and Chassin, L. The psychosocial characteristics of alcohol users versus problem users: Data from a study of adolescents at risk. Development and Psychopathology 11:321–348, 1999. Collins, R.L.; Schell, T.; Ellickson, P.L.; and McCaffrey, D. Predictors of beer advertising awareness among eighth graders. Addiction 98:1297–1306, 2003. PMID: 12930217 Cook, P.J., and Moore, M.J. Economic perspectives on reducing alcohol-related violence. In: Martin, S.E., ed. Alcohol and Interpersonal Violence: Fostering Multidisciplinary Perspectives. NIAAA Research Monograph No. 24. NIH Pub. No. 93–3496. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 1993a. pp. 193–212. Cook, P.J., and Moore, M.J. Drinking and schooling. Journal of Health Economics 12:411–429, 1993b. Cook, P.J., and Moore, M.J. The economics of alcohol abuse and alcohol-control policies. Health Affairs 21:120–133, 2002. PMID: 11900152 Curran, P.J.; Stice, E.; and Chassin, L. The relation between adolescent alcohol use and peer alcohol use: A longitudinal random coefficients model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 65:130–140, 1997. PMID: 9103742 Davies, P.T., and Windle, M. Interparental discord and adolescent adjustment trajectories: The potentiating and protective role of intrapersonal attributes. Child Development 72:1163–1178, 2001. PMID: 11480940 Dee, T.S. State alcohol policies, teen drinking and traffic fatalities. Journal of Public Economics 72:289–315, 1999. DiClemente, R.J.; Wingood, G.M.; Crosby, R.; et al. Parental monitoring: Association with adolescents’ risk behaviors. Pediatrics 107:1363–1368, 2001. PMID: 11389258 Ellickson, P.L.; Tucker, J.S.; and Klein, D.J. Ten-year prospective study of public health problems associated with early drinking. Pediatrics 111(5 Pt 1):949–955, 2003. PMID: 12728070 Ferguson, S.A., and Williams, A.F. Awareness of zero tolerance laws in three states. Journal of Safety Research 33:293–299, 2002. PMID: 12404994 Ferguson, S.A.; Fields, M.; and Voas, R.B. “Enforcement of Zero Tolerance Laws in the United States.” Paper presented at the 15th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety, Stockholm, Sweden, May 2000. Gius, M.P. Using panel data to determine the effect of advertising on brand-level distilled spirits sales. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 57:73–76, 1996. PMID: 8747504 Grossman, M.; Coate, D.; and Arluck, G.M. Price sensitivity of alcoholic beverages in the United States: Youth alcohol consumption. In: Holder, H.D., ed. Control Issues in Alcohol Abuse Prevention: Strategies for States and Communities. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 1987. pp. 169– 198. Grossman, M.; Chaloupka, F.J.; and Sirtalan, I. An empirical analysis of alcohol addiction: Results from the Monitoring the Future panels. Economic Inquiry 36:39–48, 1998. Grube, J.W. Alcohol in the media: Drinking portrayals, alcohol advertising, and alcohol consumption among youth. In: National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. pp. 597–624. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Halpern-Felsher, B.L., and Biehl, M. Developmental and environmental influences on underage drinking: A general overview. In: National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. pp. 402–416. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Hingson, R.; Heeren, T.; and Winter, M. Lower legal blood alcohol limits for young drivers. Public Health Reports 109:738–744, 1994. PMID: 7800781 Jones, R.K., and Lacey, J.H. Alcohol and Highway Safety 2001: A Review of the State of Knowledge. DOT Pub. No. HS–809–383. Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 2001. Available online at: http://www.nhtsa.dot.gov/people/injury/research/AlcoholHighway . Kelly, K.J., and Edwards, R.W. Image advertisements for alcohol products: Is their appeal associated with adolescents’ intention to consume alcohol? Adolescence 33:47–59, 1998. PMID: 9583659 Kenkel, D.S. Drinking, driving, and deterrence: The effectiveness and social costs of alternative policies. Journal of Law and Economics 36:877–913, 1993. Kenkel, D.S., and Manning, W.G. Perspectives on alcohol taxation. Alcohol Health & Research World 20(4):230–238, 1996. Leung, S.F., and Phelps, C.E. My kingdom for a drink. . . . ? A review of estimates of the price sensitivity of demand for alcoholic beverages. In: Hilton, M.E., and Bloss, G., eds. Economics and the Prevention of Alcohol-Related Problems. NIAAA Research Monograph No. 25. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 1993. pp. 1–31. Manning, W.G.; Blumberg, L.; and Moulton, L.H. The demand for alcohol: The differential response to price. Journal of Health Economics 14:123–148, 1995. PMID: 10154654 Markowitz, S., and Grossman, M. The effects of beer taxes on physical child abuse. Journal of Health Economics 19:271–282, 2000. PMID: 10947580 Martin, S.E.; Snyder, L.B.; Hamilton, M.; et al. Alcohol advertising and youth. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26:900–906, 2002. PMID: 12068260 Mast, B.D.; Benson, B.L.; and Rasmussen, D.W. Beer taxation and alcohol-related traffic fatalities. Southern Economic Journal 66:214–249, 1999. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA). Traffic Safety Facts 2002: Alcohol. DOT Pub. No. HS–809–606. Washington, DC: NHTSA, 2003. Available online at: http://wwwnrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/pdf/nrd-30/NCSA/TSF2002/2002alcfacts.pdf. Nelson, J.P., and Moran, J.R. Advertising and U.S. alcoholic beverage demand: System-wide estimates. Applied Economics 27:1225–1236, 1995. O’Malley, P.M., and Wagenaar, A.C. Effects of minimum drinking age laws on alcohol use, related behaviors and traffic crash involvement among American youth: 1976–1987. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 52:478–491, 1991. PMID: 1943105 Preusser, D.F.; Ulmer, R.G.; and Preusser, C.W. Obstacles to Enforcement of Youthful (Under 21) Impaired Driving. DOT Pub. No. HS–807–878. Washington, DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1992. Preusser, D.F.; Williams, A.F.; and Weinstein, H.B. Policing underage alcohol sales. Journal of Safety Research 25:127–133, 1994. Reifman, A.; Barnes, G.M.; Dintcheff, B.A.; et al. Parental and peer influences on the onset of heavier drinking among adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 59:311–317, 1998. PMID: 9598712 Ruhm, C.J. Alcohol policies and highway vehicle fatalities. Journal of Health Economics 15:435– 454, 1996. PMID: 10164038 Saffer, H. Alcohol advertising and youth. Journal of Studies on Alcohol (Suppl. 14):173–181, 2002. PMID: 12022723 Saffer, H., and Dhaval, D. Alcohol Advertising and Alcohol Consumption by Adolescents. NBER Working Paper No. 9676. New York: National Bureau of Economic Research, 2003. Available online at: http://www.nber.org/papers/w9676. Saffer, H., and Grossman, M. Beer taxes, the legal drinking age, and youth motor vehicle fatalities. Journal of Legal Studies 16:351–374, 1987. Schults, R.A.; Elder, R.W.; Sleet, D.A.; et al. and the Task Force on Community Preventive Services. Reviews of evidence regarding interventions to reduce alcohol-impaired driving. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 21(Suppl.1):66–88. 2001. Erratum in: American Journal of Preventive Medicine 23:72, 2002. PMID: 11691562 Sieving, R.E.; Perry, C.L.; and Williams, C.L. Do friendships change behaviors, or do behaviors change friendships? Examining paths of influence in young adolescents’ alcohol use. Journal of Adolescent Health 26:27–35, 2000. PMID: 10638715 Slater, M.D.; Rouner, D.; Domenech-Rodriquez, M.; et al. Adolescent responses to TV beer ads and sports content/context: Gender and ethnic differences. Journalism and Mass Communications Quarterly 74:108–122, 1997. Snyder, L.B.; Milici, F.F.; Mitchell, E.W.; and Proctor, D.C. Media, product differences and seasonality in alcohol advertising in 1997. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 61:896–906, 2000. PMID: 11188496 Stacy, A.; Zogg, J.; Unger, J; and Dent, C. Exposure to televised alcohol ads and subsequent alcohol use. American Journal of Health Behavior 28: 498–509, 2004. PMID: 15569584 Stattin, H., and Kerr M. Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development 71:1072– 1085, 2000. PMID: 11016567 Steinberg, L.; Fletcher, A.; and Darling, N. Parental monitoring and peer influences on adolescent substance use. Pediatrics 93(6 Pt 2):1060–1064, 1994. PMID: 8197008 Stice, E.; Barrera, M.; and Chassin, L. Prospective differential prediction of adolescent alcohol use and problem use: Examining the mechanisms of effect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 107:616–628, 1998. PMID: 9830249 Sutton, M., and Godfrey, C. A grouped data regression approach to estimating economic and social influences on individual drinking behaviour. Health Economics 4:237–247, 1995. PMID: 7550773 Tapert, S.F.; Cheung, E.H.; Brown, G.G.; et al. Neural response to alcohol stimuli in adolescents with alcohol use disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 60:727–735, 2003. PMID: 12860777 Voas, R.B., and Williams, A.F. Age differences of arrested and crash-involved drinking drivers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 47:244–248, 1986. PMID: 3724162 Wagenaar, A.C., and Toomey, T.L. Effects of minimum drinking age laws: Review and analyses of the literature from 1960 to 2000. Journal of Studies on Alcohol (Suppl. 14):206–225, 2002. PMID: 12022726 Wagenaar, A.C.; Murray, D.M.; Gehan, J.P.; et al. Communities Mobilizing for Change on Alcohol: Outcomes from a randomized community trial. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 61:85–94, 2000. PMID: 10627101 Wagenaar, A.C.; O’Malley, P.M.; and LaFond, C. Lowered legal blood alcohol limits for young drivers: Effects on drinking, driving and driving-after-drinking behaviors in 30 states. American Journal of Public Health 91:801–804, 2001. PMID: 11344892 Waiters, E.D.; Treno, A.J.; and Grube, J.W. Alcohol advertising and youth: A focus group analysis of what young people find appealing in alcohol advertising. Contemporary Drug Problems 28:695–718, 2001. Wells, J.K.; Greene, M.A.; Foss, R.D.; et al. Drinking drivers missed at sobriety checkpoints. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 58:513–517, 1997. PMID: 9273917 Wyllie, A.; Zhang, J.F.; and Casswell, S. Responses to televised alcohol advertisements associated with drinking behaviour of 10-17-year-olds. Addiction 93:361–371, 1998. PMID: 10328044 Young, D.J., and Likens, T.W. Alcohol regulation and auto fatalities. International Review of Law and Economics 20:107–126, 2000. Zogg, J.; Ma, H.; Dent, C.; and Stacy, A. Self-generated alcohol outcomes in 8th and 10th graders: Exposure to vicarious sources of alcohol information. Addictive Behavior 1:3–16, 2004. PMID: 14667417 Interventions for Alcohol Use and Alcohol Use Disorders in Youth Designing effective interventions for adolescents with alcohol use disorders (AUDs) presents several challenges, not the least of which is the accurate diagnosis of these disorders. Diagnostic criteria for AUDs have been derived largely from clinical and research experience with adults. When these criteria were tested among adolescents, numerous developmental differences were found that may affect the applicability of AUD criteria to this age group. Despite the absence of clear diagnostic criteria for use with adolescents, research has identified interventions that show promise for use with youth. This article examines both environmental- and individual-level approaches to underage drinking prevention, including school- and family-based programs, and macroenvironmental and multicomponent comprehensive interventions. Finally, it describes brief and complex treatment interventions. Key words: adolescent; alcohol abuse; alcohol dependence; AOD (alcohol and other drug) use pattern; diagnostic criteria; biological development; psychological development; environmental-level prevention; individual-level prevention; family intervention; school-based intervention; brief intervention; Project Northland OVERVIEW The ultimate goal of research on drinking by youth is to reduce the rates of drinking by adolescents and successfully treat those who develop problems linked to alcohol use. Prevention efforts may be aimed at keeping adolescents from starting to drink or at preventing the escalation of drinking and negative consequences. Research can provide the science on which to base the design of interventions and the means for determining which interventions are effective. A valid diagnostic system is essential for assessing the nature and magnitude of adolescent problem drinking. Existing diagnostic criteria are derived largely from experience with adults, but developmental differences in alcohol use patterns suggest the need to adapt criteria to make them relevant and informative for an adolescent’s stage of maturation. Prevention efforts approach the issue of youth drinking in two ways: Environmental-level interventions seek to reduce the availability of alcohol to youth and opportunities to drink, increase penalties for violation of minimum legal drinking age laws, and reduce community tolerance for alcohol use by youth. Individual-level interventions seek to change knowledge, attitudes, and skills so that youth are better able to resist influences that support drinking. In their efforts to reduce adolescent drinking, schools and families can act at both the environmental and the individual level. School curricula operate at the individual level by trying to provide students with the knowledge, skills, and motivation to resist pressures to drink. At the environmental level, schools can make changes to discourage violation of alcohol rules and engage students’ involvement in their schools, a factor that has been found to predict less alcohol and other drug involvement. The ability of parents to influence whether their children drink is well documented and is consistent across racial/ethnic groups. Family interventions encourage parents to be aware of the risks from underage drinking, communicate with children, clarify expectations, set rules and consequences about alcohol use, and monitor children’s activities. In addition to changing the knowledge and skills of young people, families can create an environment that reduces alcohol availability and increases the costs associated with drinking. Research is providing data on the effectiveness of school- and family-based intervention programs and the elements that successful programs incorporate. One goal of continuing research is to improve investigators’ ability to measure outcomes and to compare studies and the methods they use as a means of changing adolescent behavior. Community-level environmental interventions include strategies such as implementing restaurant/bar server training, checking alcohol vendors for compliance with underage laws, deterring adults from purchasing alcohol for minors, strengthening policies to detect and stop underage drinking parties, and instituting publicity for policies aimed at enforcement of laws against driving under the influence (DUI) and underage drinking. Community prevention trials have demonstrated that such efforts can reduce alcohol-impaired driving and fatal crashes among underage drivers and sales of alcohol to minors. The most comprehensive interventions encompass coordinated school, family, and community programs. One such universal prevention program, Project Northland, was tested in 22 school districts in northern Minnesota in a randomized trial. The intervention included school curricula, peer leadership, parental involvement programs, and communitywide efforts to address community norms and alcohol availability. The intervention was delivered to a single cohort from grades 6 through 12. Comparisons in such measures as “tendency to use alcohol” and drinking five or more drinks in a row revealed differences between intervention and comparison communities. Although the Project Northland intervention was able to reduce rates of drinking among students who were nondrinkers at the start of the project, the effort had no effect on those who already had been drinking. These very early starters are likely to have particular risk factors that make them more likely to drink and less likely to respond to more broadly targeted interventions; the experience with Project Northland suggests that programs may be needed that are aimed specifically at this group. Underscoring the need for effective means of prevention are 2002 prevalence data indicating that, among youth ages 12 to 17, 1.4 million met the criteria set forth in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM–IV) for alcohol abuse and dependence (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration [SAMHSA] 2003). The data, moreover, reveal a major unmet need for treatment for alcohol and related behavioral problems. Only 227,000 of the youth meeting criteria for alcohol problems received any treatment for these disorders in 2002. Data on alcohol problems among youth also may understate the prevalence of these disorders; alcoholism treatment researchers believe that DSM–IV criteria need to be developmentally specific to adequately identify youth with problems. Adolescents in treatment for alcohol use disorders (AUDs) are likely to have more than one substance use disorder and may have other psychiatric comorbidities; the success of treatment is lower with those who have multiple problems than with other subgroups of youth. To date, treatment for adolescent addiction has involved adapting adult treatments to youth. Ongoing research is testing some innovative and developmentally tailored interventions aimed at improving treatment outcomes. Some of the most promising interventions for adolescents with AUDs have been complex, multicomponent therapies. The current health care financing system stresses the need for shorter, more cost-effective treatment, however. An alternative to complex treatments, brief interventions can be directed at drinking or the consequences of drinking. An example of a brief intervention is motivational enhancement, which encourages the person to take responsibility for change and provides a menu of options for change. Early evidence suggests that brief interventions can be helpful in reducing both drinking and its consequences in adolescents. Overall, research points to the importance of applying a more nuanced and detailed understanding of adolescent development to the design of treatments and outcome measures for alcohol use problems in adolescents. DIAGNOSIS OF ALCOHOL ABUSE AND DEPENDENCE IN ADOLESCENTS A valid diagnostic system is essential to advancing treatment and research of adolescent AUDs. Diagnoses should facilitate communication among clinicians and researchers, identify cases for different levels of clinical intervention, provide phenotypes for genetics research, and convey information about prognosis (Robins and Barrett 1989; McGue 1999). DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association [APA] 2000) includes two AUDs, alcohol abuse and alcohol dependence, which are defined by nonoverlapping criterion sets. DSM–IV abuse focuses on negative psychosocial consequences resulting from drinking, as well as hazardous use, and requires the presence of at least one of four criteria. DSM–IV dependence is diagnosed when at least three of seven criteria related to physical dependence, salience of alcohol use, and impaired control over drinking behavior are met within the same 12-month period. Both DSM–IV AUDs require evidence of clinically significant impairment or subjective distress resulting from alcohol use for diagnosis. Diagnostic criteria for AUDs were derived largely from clinical and research experience with adults, and only recently has their validity been assessed among adolescents (Chung et al. 2005). Numerous developmental differences between adolescents and adults may affect the applicability of AUD criteria to youth. For example, adolescents tend to drink less often than adults but typically consume a greater quantity per occasion (Deas et al. 2000). Developmental differences in alcohol use patterns indicate the need to adapt criteria to make them relevant to and properly scaled for an adolescent’s stage of maturation (Brown 1999). Because a construct may manifest itself differently in adolescents and adults (e.g., role impairment at school vs. work), a perspective that takes developmental factors and contextual influences into account is essential for valid assessment of AUD symptoms. DSM–IV AUDs have shown some validity when used with adolescents in that teens classified as having alcohol dependence, abuse, and no diagnosis differ on external measures of alcohol involvement (e.g., Lewinsohn et al. 1996; Winters et al. 1999). Several important limitations have been identified, however, both at the criterion level of how symptoms are defined and measured and at the level of the diagnostic algorithms for alcohol abuse and dependence. At the criterion level, certain symptoms (e.g., withdrawal, use despite medical problems) tend to occur only after years of heavy drinking and have low prevalence and limited utility when applied to teens. Other DSM–IV AUD symptoms appear to be more relevant to specific adolescent subgroups. For example, hazardous use and legal problems have been associated with male gender, increased age, ethnic background, and presence of conduct disorder symptoms in teens (Langenbucher and Martin 1996; Wagner et al. 2002). Ethnicity and gender have been found to influence whether and when certain DSM–IV AUD symptoms tend to occur in teen drinkers (Wagner et al. 2002). Some symptoms, such as tolerance, appear to have a high prevalence among young drinkers in part because they are poorly defined or scaled for the developmental period of adolescence (Martin and Winters 1998). DSM–IV’s definition of tolerance as a “marked increase to obtain the same effect” is only modestly associated with adolescent alcohol dependence. Many adolescent drinkers report marked increases to produce the same effect (e.g., from one drink to three) but are relatively light drinkers, often not having any other symptoms. Some level of tolerance may occur as a normative developmental phenomenon in youth who drink. Other adolescents are heavy drinkers who are not assigned the tolerance symptom; they report high quantities of drinking during early drinking experiences (e.g., six or more drinks) without a subsequent marked increase to produce the same effect (Chung et al. 2001). Better guidelines need to be developed regarding the identification of clinically significant levels of tolerance in teens, or alternatives such as a heavy drinking criterion must be considered (Chung et al. 2001). Some AUD criteria may be interpreted differently or have different meanings when used with adolescents compared with adults, such as “drinking more or longer than intended.” This symptom often is assigned as a result of an adolescent’s poor judgment, inexperience with alcohol’s effects, or social pressures to drink, rather than as a compulsive pattern of alcohol use (Chung and Martin 2005). Research has examined the development of more specific interview probes that query contextual factors, such as adolescents’ motivations for drinking and reasons for limiting alcohol use, as a way to increase the validity of this symptom among youth. Differences in how tolerance and drinking more or longer than intended are assessed affect diagnostic validity and have a large effect on the estimated prevalence of AUDs in adolescent community samples (Chung et al. 2002). There are other limitations of the DSM–IV at the level of diagnostic algorithms, that is, abuse as one out of four criteria and dependence as three out of seven criteria. Some adolescents who engage in relatively low levels of alcohol use meet criteria for an abuse diagnosis only because of arguments with their parents about alcohol use and may be considered to be “diagnostic impostors” (Martin 1999). However, “diagnostic orphans,” who have one to two dependence symptoms and no abuse symptoms, and thus no DSM–IV AUD, are similar to teens with DSM–IV alcohol abuse on drinking levels and clinical outcomes (Pollock and Martin 1999). Diagnostic impostors and orphans limit the ability of the DSM–IV diagnostic system to provide appropriate categories for research studies and to guide the allocation of scarce health care resources. The DSM–IV’s separate criterion sets for abuse and dependence are not well distinguished conceptually or empirically. Data do not support a distinction between the two categories in severity, age of symptom onset, age of onset of the two diagnoses, or symptom profiles identified by latent class analysis and factor analysis (e.g., Martin et al. 1996; Wagner et al. 2002). Some community surveys report higher prevalence of the more severe dependence diagnosis relative to the milder abuse diagnosis (Chung et al. 2002), a situation that does not conform to most disorders in psychiatry or medicine. In contrast to the DSM–IV dichotomy of abuse and dependence, evidence suggests that the latent structure of adolescent alcohol problems represents a continuum of severity distinguished more by the number than the type of symptoms (Chung and Martin 2001). Longitudinal studies indicate that alcohol problems which occur in adolescence and young adulthood are only modestly associated (e.g., Baer et al. 1995; Rohde et al. 2001). The alcohol abuse diagnosis appears to be particularly transient, with a high rate of transitions into and out of this category (Nelson and Wittchen 1998). Many adolescents with AUDs mature out of problem drinking (Labouvie 1996; Maisto et al. 2002), whereas others show a more chronic course through adulthood (Abrantes et al. 2002). Multiple developmental trajectories of adolescent-onset AUDs exist (e.g., Schulenberg et al. 2001) and have been characterized as developmentally limited or persistent, with diagnoses that may be relatively continuous or intermittent (Zucker et al. 1995). Ongoing longitudinal research will help investigators understand more about the clinical course and prognosis of adolescent-onset abuse and dependence and will help them test the predictive validity of diagnostic criteria, course specifiers, and algorithms in the DSM–IV and beyond. TEXTBOX DSM–IV Diagnostic Criteria for Alcohol Abuse and Dependence Alcohol Abuse (A) A maladaptive pattern of drinking, leading to clinically significant impairment or distress, as manifested by at least one of the following occurring within a 12-month period: Recurrent use of alcohol resulting in a failure to fulfill major role obligations at work, school, or home (e.g., repeated absences or poor work performance related to alcohol use; alcohol-related absences, suspensions, or expulsions from school; neglect of children or household) Recurrent alcohol use in situations in which it is physically hazardous (e.g., driving an automobile or operating a machine when impaired by alcohol use) Recurrent alcohol-related legal problems (e.g., arrests for alcohol- related disorderly conduct) Continued alcohol use despite having persistent or recurrent social or interpersonal problems caused or exacerbated by the effects of alcohol (e.g., arguments with spouse about consequences of intoxication). (B) Never met criteria for alcohol dependence. Alcohol Dependence (A) A maladaptive pattern of drinking, leading to clinically significant impairment or distress, as manifested by three or more of the following occurring at any time in the same 12-month period: Need for markedly increased amounts of alcohol to achieve intoxication or desired effect; or markedly diminished effect with continued use of the same amount of alcohol The characteristic withdrawal syndrome for alcohol (or a closely related substance) or drinking to relieve or avoid withdrawal symptoms Persistent desire or one or more unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control drinking; or drinking in larger amounts or over a longer period than intended Important social, occupational, or recreational activities given up or reduced because of drinking A great deal of time spent in activities necessary to obtain, to use, or to recover from the effects of drinking Continued drinking despite knowledge of having a persistent or recurrent physical or psychological problem that is likely to be caused or exacerbated by drinking. (B) No duration criterion separately specified, but several dependence criteria must occur repeatedly as specified by duration qualifiers associated with criteria (e.g., “persistent,” “continued”). SOURCE: American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC: APA, 1994. END OF TEXTBOX PREVENTION OF UNDERAGE DRINKING Intervention Approaches Environmental-level interventions seek to reduce opportunities (availability) for underage drinking, increase penalties for violating minimum legal drinking age (MLDA) and other alcohol use laws, and reduce community tolerance for alcohol use and misuse by youth. Individual-level interventions seek to change knowledge, expectancies, attitudes, intentions, motivation, and skills so that youth are better able to resist the pro-drinking influences and opportunities that surround them. This section discusses four types of individual- and environmental-level programs: schoolbased programs, family-based programs, macroenvironmental programs, and multicomponent programs. School-Based Prevention Programs School-based curricula to prevent use of alcohol and other drugs by youth have a long history. However, the use of research-based findings to guide the content and evaluation of such curricula is a fairly recent development (Bangert-Drowns 1988; Dielman 1995). The first schoolbased programs were primarily informational and often used scare tactics—it was assumed that if youth understood the dangers inherent in alcohol misuse, they would choose to abstain. These programs were ineffective. Better programs are now available, but researchers have found that sometimes they are not used (Silvia and Thorne 1997) or implemented as designed (Dusenbury et al. 2003). Efforts to clarify theoretical and methodological issues relevant to improving school-based prevention curricula have made steady progress. However, methodological issues remain a critical barrier to interpreting the large number of published studies, as many were conducted with less than optimal degrees of scientific rigor. Additionally, variations in design and methodology make comparisons across studies difficult. For example, there is wide variability in alcohol use outcome measures, and it is common for some measures within a single study to show significant intervention effects whereas others do not (Foxcroft et al. 2003). Researchers are increasingly interested in collecting information on alcohol-related problems and high-risk drinking practices in addition to more straightforward measures of quantity and frequency of drinking. Outcomes based solely on knowledge and attitudes are no longer acceptable. Variation in measures makes comparisons across studies difficult. Also, the frequent use of study-specific composite scales (based on combinations of individual measures) often makes practical interpretations of findings difficult. This latter problem, coupled with the failure to report effect sizes, makes it difficult to judge the likely benefit from implementing programs on a large scale (Gorman 1995). Analysis based on intention-to-treat is the most relevant from a public health standpoint, but application of this analytic standard often eliminates statistical significance (Foxcroft et al. 2003). Differences in program intensity (number of sessions), followup periods, age/grade of students, program goals, population characteristics, and attrition also impede metaanalysis and cross-study comparisons. However, the following general statements are supported by the literature: Programs that rely primarily on increasing knowledge about the consequences of drinking are not effective. Effective programs often: – Are based on social influence models – Include norm setting – Address social pressures to drink and teach resistance skills – Include developmentally appropriate information – Include peer-led components – Provide teacher training – Are interactive. Unfortunately, effect sizes generally are small. Even state-of-the-art programs are not sufficient to prevent adolescent use and misuse of alcohol in the absence of social and environmental change. Much of the literature suggests universal prevention curricula are less effective with higher risk students—those who have initiated drinking prior to grades five or six; additional research is needed in this area because of inconsistencies in the literature (Gottfredson and Wilson 2003; Hansen 1992; Komro and Toomey 2002; Tobler 1986; Tobler and Stratton 1997; National Research Council [NRC] and Institute of Medicine [IOM] 2004; Wagenaar and Perry 1994). School curricula operate at the individual level by trying to provide students with the knowledge, skills, and motivation needed to resist pressures to drink. However, schools also may be considered from an environmental perspective. Policies and practices within the school, such as consistent enforcement of sanctions for violating alcohol rules, are another arena for intervention. Students’ bonding or attachment to their schools is found to predict less alcohol and other drug involvement, so overall school climate and cohesiveness also seem to be important. However, there are few studies linking specific school policies with alcohol use and even fewer studies of policy changes (Flay 2000). Family-Based Prevention Programs The ability of parents to influence whether their children drink is well documented and is consistent across racial/ethnic groups (e.g., Barnes et al. 2000; Steinberg et al. 1994). Setting clear rules about children not drinking, consistently enforcing those rules, and monitoring child behavior reduce the likelihood of underage drinking. Family conflict and lack of cohesion are associated with increased risk (Bogenschneider et al. 1998). Family interventions encourage parents to be aware of the risks from underage drinking, to communicate with children, to clarify expectations regarding alcohol use, to set rules and consequences for violations, to monitor children’s activities, and to reduce the availability of alcohol in the home. Additionally, content on family management practices and communication skills often are included. Parent-directed programs have been included with school-based interventions, some of which have evidence of success; but these components have not been evaluated separately (Flay 2000). Stand-alone family interventions have been successful in reducing alcohol use and other risk behaviors (Komro and Toomey 2002). The Iowa Strengthening Families Program (ISFP), delivered when students were in grade six, has shown long-lasting preventive effects on alcohol use, even when evaluated on the basis of intent-to-treat (Spoth et al. 2001, 2004). This finding is striking on two counts: First, it suggests that the intervention succeeded in changing the normative environment of schools in which the program was offered, because even students whose families did not participate benefited. Second, the increase in effect size over time and the duration of effects into high school compares favorably with school-based interventions. A recent Cochrane review identified the ISFP as one of two potentially effective interventions for the primary prevention of alcohol misuse by youth (Foxcroft et al. 2003). Family interventions operate at both the individual and environmental level. Interventions seek to change behavior of both parents and children by increasing knowledge and skills. However, by changing parent practices, they affect a primary social environment for the child. This microenvironment-level change effectively reduces availability and increases “costs” associated with drinking, which probably accounts for the lasting intervention effects that have been observed. Families in distress or youth who are exhibiting behavior problems may need more intensive interventions (selective and indicated prevention). Tiered or stepped-intervention strategies have been described to restrict more costly services to the subset of families in most need (Dishion and Kavanagh 2000; Sanders 2000). Macroenvironmental Interventions Environmental approaches may have both direct and indirect influences on drinking by youth. Enforcement of MLDA laws directly reduces alcohol availability, a critical element in comprehensive risk models (Wagenaar and Toomey 2002). Penalties for alcohol use and misuse that apply directly to youth increase the social “cost” of drinking, which is expected to affect decisions about drinking. Changes in monetary price have been associated with decreases in use and related problems (Leung and Phelps 1993; Kenkel and Manning 1996; Chaloupka et al. 1998; and Cook and Moore 2002). Public awareness campaigns in support of environmental change serve to change community norms regarding the acceptability of underage drinking, which should further reduce opportunities to drink and increase social costs to young drinkers (NRC and IOM 2004; Toomey and Wagenaar 2002; Wagenaar and Perry 1994). Environmental interventions are among the recommendations included in the recent NRC and IOM report, Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility (NRC and IOM 2004), and by the Panel on Prevention and Treatment of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) Task Force of the National Advisory Council on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Such programs seek to reduce commercial and social availability of alcohol and/or reduce driving while intoxicated. They may use a variety of strategies, including implementing server training, instituting compliance checks in outlets, deterring adults from purchasing for minors (shoulder tap) or providing alcohol to minors (public education and policies), restricting drinking in public places, enforcing penalties for use of false IDs, strengthening policies to detect and terminate underage drinking parties, establishing penalties for providing alcohol to a minor, enforcing DUI and zero-tolerance laws, and creating publicity regarding policies and sanctions. Three community trials in the United States are noteworthy and are described below. Collectively, they show the utility of community environmental strategies to reduce underage drinking and related problems. The Massachusetts Saving Lives Program. This 5-year comprehensive intervention implemented in six communities was designed to reduce alcohol-impaired driving and related traffic deaths. This program decreased fatal crashes, particularly alcohol-related fatal crashes involving drivers ages 15–25, and reduced the proportion of 16- to 19-year-olds who reported driving after drinking relative to the rest of Massachusetts. It also increased teen awareness of penalties for drunk driving and for speeding. Other significant outcomes related to traffic safety were not age-specific (Hingson et al. 1996; Hingson and Howland 2002). The Community Prevention Trial Program. This program was implemented in three intervention communities matched to three comparison sites. The formal goal of the project was to assist each experimental community to make effective, long-term changes to reduce alcoholinvolved injuries and death but not necessarily to change individual drinking patterns. The intervention strategies included efforts to reduce alcohol availability to minors. Sales to apparent minors (people of legal drinking age who appear younger than age 21) were significantly reduced in the intervention communities compared with the control sites (Grube 1997; Holder 2000). Communities Mobilizing for Change on Alcohol. This program was a randomized 15community trial to reduce the accessibility of alcoholic beverages to youths under the legal drinking age. It emphasized environmental factors that affect the supply of alcohol to youth, using a community organizing approach to achieve policy change among local institutions. Among the significant findings were that merchants in participating communities were less likely to sell alcohol to minors and that 18- to 20-year-olds were less likely to try to purchase alcohol or provide alcohol to younger teens. There also was a decline in DUI arrests among 18- to 20-yearolds. There were no program effects, however, on self-reported drinking by 12th graders, the youngest age group surveyed. This may be a result of the short duration of the intervention—2.5 years—or it may be that younger adolescents obtain alcohol from adults and are not directly affected by changes in commercial availability (Wagenaar et al. 2000a,b). Community-level interventions clearly can reduce commercial sales of alcohol to minors, and this can affect overall drinking by older adolescents. It remains to be seen whether sustained interventions can reduce social availability of alcohol to younger adolescents. Additionally, the fact that community interventions can simultaneously reduce alcohol-related problems among adults (e.g., injury) and youth (e.g., availability) increases their cost-effectiveness and should make them attractive to policymakers (Foxcroft et al. 2003). Multicomponent Comprehensive Interventions Comprehensive interventions provide coordinated programs at the school, family, and community levels and target multiple pathways for risk. Ideally, they also should integrate universal, selective, and indicated prevention programs and treatment for youth who are alcohol dependent. To date, one such program, Project Northland, has been evaluated. Project Northland is a comprehensive universal prevention program that was tested in 22 school districts in northeastern Minnesota in a randomized trial. The intervention included (1) innovative social behavioral school curricula, (2) peer leadership, (3) parental involvement programs, and (4) communitywide task force activities to address larger community norms and alcohol availability. The intervention was delivered to a single cohort in grades 6 through 12. Intervention intensity and focus varied over the study period. The first phase (grades 6 through 8) had strong school and family components. By the end of grade 8, fewer students had initiated alcohol use, and the prevalence of alcohol use (past month and past week) was significantly lower in the intervention communities compared with control communities (Perry et al. 1996). During the next phase of the study, grades 9 and 10, there was minimal intervention. In grades 11 and 12, intervention activities resumed, and the community component to reduce availability was featured more prominently. Significant differences were observed between intervention and comparison communities during each project period for “tendency to use alcohol” (a composite measure that combined items about intentions to use alcohol and actual use) and “five or more in a row.” The rates of increase in underage drinking prevalence were lower in the intervention communities during phase 1; higher during the interim period (suggesting a “catch-up” effect while intervention activities were minimal); and again lower during phase 2 when intervention activities resumed (Perry et al. 2002). Based on its success, Project Northland has been designated a model program by SAMHSA, and its materials have been adapted for a general audience and marketed by Hazelden. It now is being replicated in ethnically diverse urban neighborhoods. Very Early Interventions The Project Northland findings at the same time point to a dilemma that may be a significant hurdle when working to prevent underage drinking in the highest risk groups. The program began in sixth grade, when children were approximately 12 years of age, and although it was able to reduce rates of drinking among those who were nondrinkers at the initiation of the project, the intervention had no effect on those who had already begun drinking. The study was not able to parse out the reasons for these differential effects on initial nonusers vs. users, but youth who are already drinking at sixth grade are very much an early onset group, given that the median age of onset of first use is age 14. Given also what is known about the impulsivity, heavier drinking by parents, and conflicted family backgrounds of early onset users (Ellickson et al. 2003; Mayzer et al. 2003), it is likely that the social micronetworks within which the early onset drinkers moved would have insulated them to a greater degree from the program’s effects. For this subgroup, earlier precursive risk intervention programs may be necessary (Nye et al. 1999; Spoth et al. 2001; Zucker and Noll 1987). TREATMENT FOR ADOLESCENT ALCOHOL USE DISORDERS Prevalence data on binge and heavy drinking, collected in the 2002 U.S. National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH) (SAMHSA 2003), indicate a public health problem of considerable dimensions in youth ages 12 to 17. Binge drinking is well established by midadolescence, as reported by 12 percent of 15-year-olds, 18 percent of 16-year-olds, and 25 percent of 17-yearolds. Not only are these youth at high risk for serious accidents and adverse social, health, and academic consequences related to their alcohol use, but some also may be at risk for developing multiple behavioral disorders including alcohol abuse and alcoholism. At the same time, as already discussed, alcoholism treatment researchers who specialize in youth diagnosis and treatment believe that DSM–IV criteria are inadequate to identify youth who have AUDs. They conclude that diagnosis of youth substance use disorders needs to be developmentally specific, to meet fewer criteria than required by DSM–IV, and to add criteria salient to youth drinking practices (Chung et al. 2003; Clark 2004). An Unmet Need Nonetheless, the NSDUH data indicate a major unmet need for effective health services to prevent and treat alcohol and other associated behavioral problems. Among youth these ages, 1.4 million met DSM–IV criteria for alcohol abuse or dependence, but only 227,000 actually received any treatment for alcohol use disorders in 2002 (SAMHSA 2003). Further, current services are not optimally designed for youth access or engagement (Brown 2001). Youth prefer easy access, low threshold approaches that accentuate strategies adolescents normally use to stop drinking (Metrik et al. 2003), and treatments that do not remove them from their primary home or academic settings (Brown 2001). Youth perceive traditional services (e.g., alcoholism treatment programs, Alcoholics Anonymous) as less helpful than brief interventions tailored to salient adolescent concerns (D’Amico et al. 2004). Consequently, alternative formats, attention to developmental transitions, and social marketing are needed to more adequately address alcohol problems emerging in adolescence (Brown 2001; Kypri et al. 2004; O’Leary et al. 2002). Alcohol abuse and dependence: The unmet need for treatment in youth ages 12–17, in the past year. In 2002, only 16 percent of the 1.4 million youth ages 12 to 17 estimated to have alcohol use disorders (AUDs) reported receiving any type of service for these problems. SOURCE: SAMHSA, National Survey on Drug Use and Health, calculated from 2002 raw data tables available on SAMHSA Web site, http://oas.samhsa.gov/nsduh.htm. Heterogeneity in Adolescents With Alcohol Use Disorders According to 2002 NSDUH data, nicotine use and illicit drug use are much higher among drinking youth ages 12 to 17 who reported they were binge drinkers or heavy drinkers in the past 30 days than they were among those who reported drinking less. Research on adolescents in treatment for alcohol use disorders reflects a similar pattern; these youth typically use cigarettes, are likely to have more than one substance use disorder, and may manifest psychiatric comorbidities as well (e.g., Abrantes et al. 2004; Myers and Brown 1994; Rowe et al. 2004). Alcohol-dependent adolescents with psychiatric comorbidities fare more poorly after treatment: These youth have lower abstention rates, relapse more rapidly, and show deterioration in their mental health symptoms following relapse to alcohol or other drugs (Tomlinson et al. 2004; McCarthy et al. 2005). Also, adolescents in substance abuse treatment who have combined heavy alcohol use and drug disorders manifest a more severe problem profile and less successful treatment outcomes (Grella 2003). Thus, by the time many youth reach treatment, they are already on a developmental pathway that ultimately, unless deflected, could lead to even more harmful behavioral lifestyles, medical disorders, and social consequences. It is important to keep in mind, however, that adolescents in addiction treatment are a heterogeneous group and follow multiple pathways of change post-treatment, including successful ones (Abrantes et al. 2004; Brown 2001). Results from several studies of alcoholdependent youth consistently demonstrate that although a portion of adolescents abstain and others quickly return to problematic use after treatment, the majority of adolescents change their use patterns over time, both improving and deteriorating as they face new developmental challenges (e.g., Brown 2004). Among treated youth, alcohol use following treatment also plays a significant role in relapse to other drugs (e.g., Brown et al. 2000), as well as in functioning in school, with peers and family, and in physical and mental health (see Brown and D’Amico 2003). See Chung and colleagues (2003), “Course of Alcohol Problems in Treated Adolescents,” for analyses of (1) four longitudinal, alcohol-focused treatment outcome studies that cover 1 to 8 years post-treatment and (2) how these treatment outcomes vary by subtypes of patients, settings, and trajectories. NIAAA Research on Adolescent Treatment Out of concern over the emerging evidence on the nature and magnitude of alcohol use and associated problems in underage youth, in 1997 NIAAA formally initiated a program of research to develop effective treatment interventions for adolescents with alcohol disorders. Prior to this, adult addiction treatments were extended to adolescents and rarely had been rigorously evaluated in youth (e.g., Brown et al. 2005). A total of 20 clinical projects have been funded under this NIAAA program, 14 of which were cofunded by SAMHSA’s Center for Substance Abuse Treatment. The majority of these clinical studies are randomized controlled clinical trials. The objective of this initial wave of studies is to design and test innovative and developmentally tailored interventions and, in so doing, provide evidence-based knowledge to improve treatment outcomes for adolescents who have primary alcohol use disorders or manifest at least one or two symptoms of alcohol dependence (i.e., are “diagnostic orphans” [Pollock and Martin 1999]). The results of these projects will be forthcoming over the next few years and will provide new information on the potential efficacy of family-based, cognitive-behavioral, brief motivational, and guided self-change interventions in a range of settings. They also will provide information on the efficacy of these treatments in subgroups of adolescents, including homeless and runaway youth, high school students, juvenile justice–involved youth, and minority youth. This treatment research also will shed light on distinctive features of adolescent treatment including processes of change and factors contributing to post-treatment success. Adolescent Treatment Interventions Complex interventions have been developed and tested in adolescents referred for treatment of alcohol and other drug disorders. Many of these patients are likely to have more than one substance use disorder (e.g., alcohol and marijuana) and to have other psychiatric disorders as well (e.g., depression, anxiety, or conduct disorder). Brief interventions are, as a rule, delivered to adolescents in general medical settings (e.g., primary care clinics, emergency rooms) or in school-based settings. The range in severity of substance use problems encountered in the nonaddiction specialty settings is greater than in treatment centers, thereby providing the opportunity to intervene before serious social consequences and alcohol use disorders develop (Wagner et al. 1999). Complex Interventions. Some of the most promising interventions for adolescents with alcohol use disorders have incorporated multiple components and systems. These include (1) family therapies with both familial and community components (i.e., multidimensional family therapy [MDFT]) (Faw et al. 2005; Liddle 2004) and multisystemic therapy (MST) (Swensen et al. 2005) and (2) cognitive-behavioral therapies (CBT) (Waldron and Kaminer 2004). Several studies have demonstrated significant improvement among teens with alcohol use disorders who were receiving family-based intervention, group or individual cognitive-behavioral therapy, and therapeutic community interventions (e.g., Waldron and Kaminer 2004; Swensen et al. 2005). All forms of these treatments have substantive differences in intervention design and delivery as well as efficacy evaluation compared with adult alcoholism treatment research (e.g., Brown et al. 2005; Kaminer and Slesnick 2005; Deas et al. 2000). In particular, consideration of youth motivation appears critical in engagement and retention of youth in single- component and complex interventions (e.g., Faw et al. 2005) as well as their continued success following treatment (e.g., Brown and Ramo in press; Kelly et al. 2002). Although limited at this time, evidence is emerging that pharmacologic treatment of co-occurring psychiatric disorders benefits adolescents with alcohol use disorders (e.g., Cornelius et al. 2005). Research on adolescents funded by NIAAA and the National Institute on Drug Abuse has shown that longer adolescent treatments generally show better outcomes. Yet longer (usually complex) treatments can be expensive, and the current health financing system stresses the need for shorter, more costeffective treatment. This poses a major challenge to alcohol and other drug treatment research today—to identify active ingredients and mechanisms of action of specific components in complex treatments and to determine if such treatments can maintain their effectiveness in reduced forms. Several models have been proposed to explain adolescent relapse following treatment (e.g., cognitive-behavioral, self-medication) and to predict clinical course after treatment (Brown 2004; Tomlinson et al. 2004). Environmental factors of exposure to substances and use patterns of peers in the immediate social network most consistently emerge as proximal risk factors for adolescent alcohol relapse (e.g., Brown et al. 2005). Personal characteristics including coping skills, self-esteem, and outcome expectancies have been associated with clinical course, as have personality/temperament features linked to disinhibition and negative reactivity (see Brown et al. 2005 for a review). Because a substantial portion of youth relapses are planned rather than unexpected, motivation for sustained abstinence appears to play a critical role in the initial decisions of youth to return to alcohol or other drug involvement after treatment. Although current evidence suggests that developmental factors such as stage of neurocognitive development, psychiatric disorders, and emotional self-regulation play a role in the decisionmaking process regarding relapse, research is needed to explicate the role of each on variability in clinical course. Brief Interventions. A primary function of brief interventions is to motivate people to initiate specific health-related behavior changes. The target of the intervention may be the harmful health behavior itself or consequences of that behavior (e.g., alcohol-related problems). One of the best known of these time-limited strategies (one to five sessions) is motivational enhancement (Miller and Sanchez 1994). This intervention is based on a nonauthoritarian empathic approach that encourages people to take personal responsibility for change, provides objective personalized assessment results on the relative magnitude of the problem behavior, provides explicit advice on the direction to change, and delineates a menu of change options. Brief interventions are flexible in that they can be used to motivate a person to engage in treatment or they can be used as a stand-alone early intervention. Early evidence on the effectiveness of brief interventions in reducing or eliminating alcoholrelated problems in adolescents indicates that they may be effective in reducing both drinking and its consequences (e.g., drunk driving) (Tevyaw and Monti 2004). Recent school-based brief intervention studies suggest that reductions in alcohol use and consequences are mediated by purposeful self-change efforts on the part of teens (e.g., Brown et al. in press) and that expectations of reduction/cessation outcomes may be critical to this change process (e.g., Metrik et al. 2004). One 4-year followup of college freshmen found, however, that reduction in consequences had a lasting effect, whereas reductions in quantity and frequency of alcohol use had washed out by then (e.g., Baer et al. 2001). FUTURE INTERVENTION RESEARCH In most adolescent alcoholism treatment studies, developmental criteria have been limited to age and grade as indicators of position along the developmental continuum. However, there is growing recognition of the important contribution that developmentally specific theories, models, and methods can make to the design of innovative and more effective adolescent treatment strategies, outcome measures, and evaluation (Brown 2004). REFERENCES Abrantes, A.; McCarthy, D.M.; Aarons, G.A.; and Brown, S.A. “Trajectories of Alcohol Involvement Following Addiction Treatment Through 8-Year Follow-Up in Adolescents.” Paper presented at the Annual Scientific Meeting of the Research Society on Alcoholism, San Francisco, CA, July 2002. Abrantes, A.M.; Brown, S.A.; and Tomlinson, K.L. Psychiatric comorbidity among inpatient substance abusing adolescents. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse 13:83–101, 2004. American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 2000. Baer, J.S.; Kivlahan, D.R.; and Marlatt, G.A. High-risk drinking across the transition from high school to college. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 19:54–61, 1995. PMID: 7771663 Baer, J.S.; Kivlahan, D.R.; Blume, A.W.; et al. Brief intervention for heavy-drinking college students: 4-year follow-up and natural history. American Journal of Public Health 91:1310–1316, 2001. PMID: 11499124 Bangert-Drowns, R.L. The effects of school-based substance abuse education: A meta-analysis. Journal of Drug Education 18:243–264, 1988. PMID: 3058921 Barnes, G.M.; Reifman, A.S.; Farrell, M.P.; and Dintcheff, B.A. The effects of parenting on the development of adolescent alcohol misuse: A six-wave latent growth model. Journal of Marriage and Family 62:175–186, 2000. Bogenschneider, K.; Wu, M.Y.; Raffaelli, M.; and Tsay, J.C. Parent influences on adolescent peer orientation and substance use: The interface of parenting practices and values. Child Development 69:1672–1688, 1998. PMID: 9914646 Brown, S.A. “A Double-Developmental Model of Adolescent Substance Abuse.” Paper presented at the Annual Scientific Meeting of the Research Society on Alcoholism, Santa Barbara, CA, 1999. Brown, S.A. Facilitating change for adolescent alcohol problems: A multiple options approach. In: Wagner, E.F., and Waldron, H.B., eds. Innovations in Adolescent Substance Abuse Intervention. Oxford, England: Elsevier Science, 2001. pp. 169–187. Brown, S.A. Measuring youth outcomes from alcohol and drug treatment. Addiction 99 (Suppl.2):38–46, 2004. PMID: 15488104 Brown, S.A., and D’Amico, E.J. Outcomes for alcohol treatment for adolescents. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 16: Research on Alcoholism Treatment. New York: Plenum, 2003. pp. 289–312. PMID: 12638643 Brown, S.A., and Ramo, D.E. Clinical course of youth following treatment for alcohol and drug problems. In: Liddle, H., and Rowe, C., eds. Treating Adolescent Substance Abuse: State of the Science. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, in press. Brown, S.A., and Tapert, S.F. Health consequences of adolescent alcohol involvement. In: National Research Council and Institute of Medicine, Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. pp. 383–401. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Brown, S.A.; Tapert, S.F.; Tate, S.R.; and Abrantes, A.M. The role of alcohol in adolescent relapse and outcome. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 32:107–115, 2000. PMID: 10801072 Brown, S.A.; Anderson, K.G.; Ramo, D.E.; and Tomlinson, K.L. Treatment of adolescent alcoholrelated problems: A translational perspective. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 327–348. PMID: 15789874 Brown, S.A.; Anderson, KG.; Schulte, M.T.; et al. Facilitating youth self-change through school based intervention. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors: Special Issue, in press. Chaloupka, F.J.; Grossman, M.; and Saffer, H. The effects of price on the consequences of alcohol use and abuse. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 14: The Consequences of Alcoholism. New York: Plenum, 1998. pp. 331–346. PMID: 9751952 Chung, T., and Martin, C.S. Classification and course of alcohol problems among adolescents in addictions treatment programs. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25:1734–1742, 2001. PMID: 11781506 Chung, T., and Martin, C.S. What were they thinking? Adolescents’ interpretations of DSM–IV alcohol dependence symptom queries and implications for diagnostic validity. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 80:191–200, 2005. PMID: 15894432 Chung, T.; Martin, C.S.; Winters, K.C.; and Langenbucher, J.W. Assessment of alcohol tolerance in adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 62:687–695, 2001. PMID: 11702808 Chung, T.; Martin, C.S.; Armstrong, T.D.; and Labouvie, E.W. Prevalence of DSM–IV alcohol diagnoses and symptoms in adolescent community and clinical samples. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 41:546–554, 2002. PMID: 12014787 Chung, T.; Martin, C.S.; Grella, C.E.; et al. Course of alcohol problems in treated adolescents. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27:253–261, 2003. PMID: 12605074 Chung, T.; Martin, C.S.; and Winters, K.C. Diagnosis, course, and assessment of alcohol abuse and dependence in adolescents. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Springer, 2005. pp. 5–27. PMID: 15789857 Clark, D. The natural history of adolescent alcohol use disorders. Addiction 99(Suppl. 2):5–22, 2004. PMID: 15488102 Cook, P.J., and Moore, M.J. The economics of alcohol abuse and alcohol-control policies. Health Affairs 21:120–133, 2002. PMID: 11900152 Cornelius, J.R.; Clark, D.B.; Bukstein, O.G.; et al. Fluoxetine in adolescents with comorbid major depression and an alcohol use disorder: A 3-year follow-up study. Addictive Behaviors 30:807– 814, 2005. PMID: 15833583 D’Amico, E.J.; McCarthy, D.M.; Metrik, J.; and Brown, S.A. Alcohol-related services: Prevention, secondary intervention and treatment preferences of adolescents. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse 14:61–80, 2004. Deas, D.; Riggs, P.; Langenbucher, J.; et al. Adolescents are not adults: Developmental considerations in alcohol users. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 24:232–237, 2000. PMID: 10698377 Dielman, T.E. School-based research on the prevention of adolescent alcohol use and misuse: Methodological issues and advances. In: Boyd, G.M.; Howard, J.; and Zucker, R.A.; eds. Alcohol Problems among Adolescents: Current Directions in Prevention Research. Hillsdale, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1995. pp.125–146. Dishion, T.J., and Kavanagh, K. A multilevel approach to family-centered prevention in schools: Process and outcome. Addictive Behaviors 25:899–911, 2000. PMID: 11125778 Dusenbury, L.; Brannigan, R.; Falco, M.; and Hansen, W.B. A review of research on fidelity of implementation: Implications for drug abuse prevention in school settings. Health Education Research 18:237–256, 2003. PMID: 12729182 Ellickson, P.L.; Tucker, J.S.; and Klein, D.J. Ten-year prospective study of public health problems associated with early drinking. Pediatrics 111:949–955, 2003. PMID: 12728070 Faw, L.; Hogue, A.; and Liddle, H.A. Multi-dimensional implementation evaluation of a residential treatment program for adolescent substance abuse. American Journal of Evaluation 26:77–94, 2005. Flay, B.R. Approaches to substance use prevention utilizing school curriculum plus social environment change. Addictive Behaviors 25:861–885, 2000. PMID: 11125776 Foxcroft, D.R.; Ireland, D.; Lister-Sharp, D.J.; et al. Longer-term primary prevention for alcohol misuse in young people: A systematic review. Addiction 98:397–411, 2003. PMID: 12653810 Gorman, D.M. On the difference between statistical and practical significance in school-based drug abuse prevention. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy 2:275–283, 1995. Gottfredson, D.C., and Wilson, D.B. Characteristics of effective school-based substance abuse prevention. Prevention Science 4:27–38, 2003. PMID: 12611417 Grella, C.E. Alcohol use outcomes at 1 year among adolescents in the drug abuse treatment outcomes studies (DATOS-A). In: Chung, T.; Martin, C.S.; Grella, C.E.; et al., eds.; Course of alcohol problems in treated adolescents. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27:254–255, 2003. PMID: 12605074 Grube, J.W. Preventing sales of alcohol to minors: Results from a community trial. Addiction 92(Suppl. 2):S251–S260, 1997. PMID: 9231448 Hansen, W.B. School-based substance abuse prevention: A review of the state of the art in curriculum, 1980–1990. Health Education Research 7:403–430, 1992. PMID: 10171672 Hingson, R.W., and Howland, J. Comprehensive community interventions to promote health: Implications for college-age drinking problems. Journal of Studies on Alcohol (Suppl. 14):226– 240, 2002. PMID: 12022727 Hingson, R.; McGovern, T.; Howland, J.; et al. Reducing alcohol-impaired driving in Massachusetts: The Saving Lives Program. American Journal of Public Health 86:791–797, 1996. PMID: 8659651 Holder, H.D. Community prevention of alcohol problems. Addictive Behaviors 25:843–859, 2000. PMID: 11125775 Kaminer, Y., and Slesnick, N. Evidence-based cognitive-behavioral and family therapies for adolescent alcohol and other substance use disorders. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 17: Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Prevention, Treatment. New York: Plenum, 2005. pp. 383–405. PMID: 15789877 Kelly, J.F.; Myers, M.G.; and Brown, S.A. Do adolescents affiliate with 12-step groups? A multivariate process model of effects. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 63:293–304, 2002. PMID: 12086130 Kenkel, D.S., and Manning, W.G. Perspectives on alcohol taxation. Alcohol Health & Research World 20(4):230–238, 1996. Komro, K.A., and Toomey, T.L. Strategies to prevent underage drinking. Alcohol Research & Health 26(1):5–14, 2002. PMID: 12154652 Kypri, K.; McCarthy, D.M.; Coe, M.T.; and Brown, S.A. Transition to independent living and substance involvement of treated and high-risk youth. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse 13:85–100, 2004. Labouvie, E. Maturing out of substance use: Selection and self-correction. Journal of Drug Issues 26:457–476, 1996. Langenbucher, J.W., and Martin, C.S. Alcohol abuse: Adding content to category. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 20 (Suppl. 8):270A–275A, 1996. PMID: 8947279 Leung, S.F., and Phelps, C.E. My kingdom for a drink. . . ? A review of estimates of the price sensitivity of demand for alcoholic beverages. In: Hilton, M.E., and Bloss, G., eds. Economics and the Prevention of Alcohol-Related Problems. NIAAA Research Monograph No. 25. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 1993. pp. 1–31. Lewinsohn, P.M.; Rohde, P.; and Seeley, J.R. Alcohol consumption in high school adolescents: Frequency of use and dimensional structure of associated problems. Addiction 91:375–390, 1996. PMID: 8867200 Liddle, H.A. Family-based therapies for adolescent alcohol and drug use: Research contributions and future needs. Addiction 99(Suppl.2):76–92, 2004. PMID: 15488107 Maisto, S.A.; Martin, C.S.; Pollock, N.K.; et al. Nonproblem drinking outcomes in adolescents treated for alcohol use disorders. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 10:324–331, 2002. PMID: 12233994 Martin, C.S. “Contrasting Alternative Diagnostic Criteria for Adolescent Alcohol Use Disorders.” Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Research Society on Alcoholism, Santa Barbara, CA, June 1999. Martin, C.S., and Winters, K.C. Diagnosis and assessment of alcohol use disorders among adolescents. Alcohol Health & Research World 22(2):95–105, 1998. PMID: 15706783 Martin, C.S.; Langenbucher, J.W.; Kaczynski, N.A.; and Chung, T. Staging in the onset of DSM– IV alcohol symptoms in adolescents: Survival/hazard analyses. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 57:549–558, 1996. PMID: 8858553 Mayzer, R.; Puttler, L.I.; Wong, M.M.; et al. Development constancy of social misbehavior from early childhood to adolescence as a predictor of early onset of alcohol use. (Abstract). Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27:65A, 2003. McCarthy, D.M.; Tomlinson, K.L.; Anderson, K.G.; et al. Relapse in alcohol and drug disordered adolescents with comorbid psychopathology: Changes in psychiatric symptoms. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 19:28–34, 2005. PMID: 15783275 McGue, M. The behavioral genetics of alcoholism. Current Directions in Psychological Science 8:109–115, 1999. Metrik, J.; Frissell, K.C.; McCarthy, D.M.; et al. Strategies for reduction and cessation of alcohol use: What do adolescents prefer? Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27:74–80, 2003. PMID: 12544009 Metrik, J.; McCarthy, D.M.; Frissell, K.C.; et al. Adolescent alcohol reduction and cessation expectancies. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 65: 217– 226, 2004. PMID: 15151353 Miller, W.R., and Sanchez, V.C. Motivating young adults for treatment and lifestyle change. In: Howard, G.S., and Nathan, P.E., eds. Alcohol Use and Misuse by Young Adults. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press, 1994. pp. 55–81. Myers, M.G., and Brown, S.A. Smoking and health in substance abusing adolescents: A two year follow-up. Pediatrics 93:561–566, 1994. PMID: 8134209 National Research Council (NRC) and Institute of Medicine (IOM), Committee on Developing a Strategy to Reduce and Prevent Underage Drinking. Bonnie, R.J., and O’Connell, M.E., eds. Reducing Underage Drinking: A Collective Responsibility. Washington, DC: National Academies Press, 2004. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/books/0309089352/html. Nelson, C.B., and Wittchen, H.U. DSM–IV alcohol disorders in a general population sample of adolescents and young adults. Addiction 93:1065–1077, 1998. PMID: 9744137 Nye, C.L.; Zucker, R.A.; and Fitzgerald, H.E. Early family-based intervention in the path to alcohol problems: Rationale and relationship between treatment process characteristics and child and parenting outcomes. Journal of Studies on Alcohol (Suppl. 13):10–21, 1999. PMID: 10225484 O’Leary, T.A.; Brown, S.A.; Colby, S.M.; et al. Treating adolescents together or individually? Issues in adolescent substance abuse interventions. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 26:890–899, 2002. PMID: 12068259 Perry, C.L.; Williams, C.L.; Veblen-Mortenson, S.; et al. Project Northland: Outcomes of a communitywide alcohol use prevention program during early adolescence. American Journal of Public Health 86:956–965, 1996. PMID: 8669519 Perry, C.L.; Williams, C.L.; Komro, K.A.; et al. Project Northland: Long-term outcomes of community action to reduce adolescent alcohol use. Health Education Research 17:117–132, 2002. PMID: 11888042 Pollock, N.K., and Martin, C.S. Diagnostic orphans: Adolescents with alcohol symptoms who do not qualify for DSM–IV abuse or dependence diagnoses. American Journal of Psychiatry 156:897– 901, 1999. PMID: 10360129 Robins, L.N., and Barrett, J.E., eds. The Validity of Psychiatric Diagnosis. New York: Raven Press, 1989. Rohde, P.; Lewinsohn, P.M.; Kahler, C.W.; et al. Natural course of alcohol use disorders from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 40:83–90, 2001. PMID: 11195569 Rowe, C.L.; Liddle, H.A.; Greenbaum, P.E.; et al. Impact of psychiatric comorbidity on treatment of adolescent drug abusers. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 26:129–140. 2004. PMID: 15050090 Sanders, M.R. Community-based parenting and family support interventions and the prevention of drug abuse. Addictive Behaviors 25:929–942, 2000. PMID: 11125780 Schulenberg, J.; Maggs, J.L.; Steinman, K.J.; and Zucker, R.A. Development matters: Taking the long view on substance abuse etiology and intervention during adolescence. In: Monti, P.M.; Colby, S.M.; and O’Leary, T.A.; eds. Adolescents, Alcohol, and Substance Abuse: Reaching Teens through Brief Interventions. New York: Guilford, 2001. pp. 19–57. Silvia, E.S., and Thorne, J. School-Based Drug Prevention Programs: A Longitudinal Study in Selected School Districts. Research Triangle Park, NC: Research Triangle Institute, 1997. Available online at: http://www.rti.org/pubs/0397drugfree_schools.pdf. Spoth, R.L.; Redmond, C.; and Shin, C. Randomized trial of brief family interventions for general populations: Adolescent substance use outcomes 4 years following baseline. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 69:627–642, 2001. PMID: 11550729 Spoth, R.; Redmond, C.; Shin, C.; and Azevedo, K. Brief family intervention effects on adolescent substance initiation: School-level growth curve analyses 6 years following baseline. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 72:535–542, 2004. PMID: 15279537 Steinberg, L.; Fletcher, A.; and Darling, N. Parental monitoring and peer influences on adolescent substance use. Pediatrics 93(6 Pt 2):1060–1064, 1994. PMID:8197008 Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), Office of Applied Studies. Results from the 2002 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings. NSDA Series H–22, DHHS Pub. No. SMA 03–3836. Rockville, MD: SAMHSA, 2003. Available online at: http://www.oas.samhsa.gov/nhsda/2k2nsduh/Results/2k2Results.htm . Swensen, C.C.; Henggeler, S.W.; Taylor, I.S.; and Addison, O.W. Multisystemic Therapy and Neighborhood Partnerships: Reducing Adolescent Violence and Substance Abuse. New York: Guilford, 2005. Tevyaw, T.O., and Monti, P.M. Motivational enhancement and other brief interventions for adolescent substance abuse: Foundations, applications, and evaluations. Addiction 99(Suppl. 2):63–75, 2004. PMID: 15488106 Tobler, N.S. Meta-analysis of 143 adolescent drug prevention programs: Quantitative outcome results of program participants compared to a control or comparison group. Journal of Drug Issues 16:537–567, 1986. Tobler, N.S., and Stratton, H.H. Effectiveness of school-based drug prevention programs: A meta-analysis of the research. Journal of Primary Prevention 18:71–128, 1997. Tomlinson, K.L.; Brown, S.A.; and Abrantes, A. Psychiatric comorbidity and substance use treatment outcomes of adolescents. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 18:160–169, 2004. PMID: 15238058 Toomey, T.L., and Wagenaar, A.C. Environmental policies to reduce college drinking: Options and research findings. Journal of Studies on Alcohol (Suppl. 14):193–205, 2002. PMID: 12022725 Wagenaar, A.C., and Perry, C.L. Community strategies for the reduction of youth drinking: Theory and application. Journal of Research on Adolescence 4:319–345, 1994. Wagenaar, A.C., and Toomey, T.L. Effects of minimum drinking age laws: Review and analyses of the literature from 1960 to 2000. Journal of Studies on Alcohol (Suppl. 14):206–225, 2002. PMID: 12022726 Wagenaar, A.C.; Murray, D.M.; Gehan, J.P.; et al. Communities Mobilizing for Change on Alcohol: Outcomes from a randomized community trial. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 61:85–94, 2000a. PMID: 10627101 Wagenaar, A.C.; Murray, D.M.; and Toomey, T.L. Communities Mobilizing for Change on Alcohol (CMCA): Effects of a randomized trial on arrests and traffic crashes. Addiction 95:209–217, 2000b. PMID: 10723849 Wagner, E.F.; Brown, S.A.; Monti, P.; et al. Innovations in adolescent substance abuse intervention. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 23:236–249, 1999. PMID: 10069552 Wagner, E.F.; Lloyd, D.A.; and Gil, A.G. Racial/ethnic and gender differences in the incidence and onset age of DSM–IV alcohol use disorder symptoms among adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 63:609–619, 2002. PMID: 12380858 Waldron, H.B., and Kaminer, Y. On the learning curve: The emerging evidence supporting cognitive-behavioral therapies for adolescent substance abuse. Addiction 99(Suppl. 2):93–105, 2004. PMID: 15488108 Winters, K.C. Treating adolescents with substance use disorders: An overview of practice issues and treatment outcome. Substance Abuse 20:203–225, 1999. PMID: 12511829 Zucker, R.A., and Noll, R.B. The interaction of child and environment in the early development of drug involvement: A far-ranging review and a planned very early intervention. Drugs and Society 2:57–97, 1987. Zucker, R.A.; Fitzgerald, H.E.; and Moses, H.D. Emergence of alcohol problems and the several alcoholisms: A developmental perspective on etiologic theory and life course trajectory. In: Cicchetti, D., and Cohen, D.J., eds. Developmental Psychopathology, Volume 2: Risk, Disorder and Adaptation. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1995. pp. 677–711. Glossary Acetaldehyde: A toxic product that results from the breakdown of alcohol by the enzyme alcohol dehydrogenase. Adenosine triphosphate (ATP): A molecule that provides the energy needed for many key metabolic reactions. ATP is generated largely in the mitochondria. Alcohol dehydrogenase: An enzyme that breaks down alcohol by oxidation, converting it to acetaldehyde. Alcohol poisoning: The result of the acute toxic effects of alcohol consumption, which can range from gastritis and severe gastrointestinal bleeding to respiratory arrest and death. Allele: One of two or more variants of a gene or other DNA sequence. Different alleles of a gene generally serve the same function (e.g., code for a protein that affects eye color) but may produce different phenotypes (e.g., blue eyes or brown eyes). Some alleles may be defective and produce a protein that has no function or an abnormal function. Amino acids: A class of biological molecules, 20 of which serve as the building blocks of proteins. Amygdala: A complex grouping of brain cells that, among other things, is thought to be involved in a person’s emotional reactions and in coordinating the body’s response to stress. Bases: In genetics, the portion of a nucleotide molecule that contributes to the genetic code. DNA bases include adenine, thymine, guanine, and cytosine; in RNA, uracil replaces thymine. Behavioral and cognitive deficits: Difficulties or disorders in social performance or learning, which include attention deficit disorder, behavioral undercontrol/conduct disorder, delinquency, lower IQ, poor school performance, and low self-esteem. Binge drinking: The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism defines binge drinking as the amount of alcohol leading to a blood alcohol content (BAC) of 0.08, which, for most adults, would be reached by consuming five drinks for men or four for women over a 2-hour period. Cancellous bone: The type of tissue found in the marrow cavity of bone. Candidate gene: A gene that has been implicated in causing or contributing to a particular phenotype (e.g., disease). Catecholamine: One of a group of physiologically active substances with various roles in the functioning of the nervous system; also helps regulate heart functioning. Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): An enzyme that catalyzes the transfer of a methyl group to catcholamines, including the neurotransmitters dopamine, norepinephrine, and epinephrine. Central nervous system (CNS): The part of the nervous system consisting of the brain and spinal cord. Children of alcoholics: Biological children, foster children, adopted children, or stepchildren living in households with one or more adults classified as having an alcohol abuse or dependence diagnosis during the past year. Chromosomes: Microscopic rod-shaped structures composed of double-stranded DNA and proteins; can be visualized during a certain phase of the cell cycle. Chromosomes are often regarded as representing the entire genome of an organism. Cortical bone: Tissue that forms the shaft of the bone. DNA (deoxyribonucleic acid): The molecule that carries the genetic code in all organisms except some viruses. DNA is composed of a linear sequence of nucleotides. Dopamine: An excitatory neurotransmitter that plays a role in the reward system in the brain and possibly also in the reinforcing properties of alcohol use. Endophenotype: A heritable trait or characteristic that is not a direct symptom of the condition under investigation but has been shown to be associated with the condition; for example, certain neurobiological characteristics have been noted in people with alcoholism and may be used as endophenotypes to identify people at risk for alcoholism. Environmental-level interventions: Efforts to prevent alcohol use problems by focusing on changing the environment to reduce the availability of alcohol to youth and opportunities to drink, increase penalties for violation of minimum legal drinking age laws, and lower a community’s tolerance for alcohol use by youth. Enzyme: A substance (usually a protein) that speeds up, or catalyzes, a specific biochemical reaction without being itself permanently altered or consumed. Event-related potentials (ERPs): Brain waves elicited by a stimulus. One component of the ERP, measured approximately 300 milliseconds after exposure to the stimulus, is called the P300 signal. It is thought to represent cognitive processing of new information and is commonly reduced in people at risk for alcoholism. Externalizing disorders: A constellation of behaviors including acting negatively on the external environment; and disruptive, hyperactive, and aggressive behavior. In the aggregate, these behaviors may also be referred to as conduct problems and antisocial or undercontrolled behavior. G protein: Intracellular regulatory molecules whose actions are instigated by neurotransmitters. Several types of G proteins exist, including stimulatory G proteins (G s), which enhance the activities of enzymes, and inhibitory G proteins (Gi), which inhibit the activities of other enzymes. Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA): An inhibitory neurotransmitter whose actions are influenced by alcohol; may play a role in alcohol withdrawal. Gene: A combination of DNA segments that together constitute a unit capable of expressing one or more functional gene products. Gene expression: The processes through which the genetic information contained within a gene on the DNA is converted into a gene product (e.g., a protein). Genetic code: The way in which information is carried by the DNA molecules determines the arrangement of amino acids in the proteins synthesized by the cells. Each of the 20 amino acids found in proteins is represented by 1 or more units of 3 consecutive nucleotide bases in the mRNA and in the DNA from which the mRNA is derived. All living organisms and viruses use the same genetic code. Genetic marker: A segment of DNA with an identifiable physical location on a chromosome. The inheritance of a genetic marker can be followed. Genome: The total genetic material of an organism or species. Genomics: The comprehensive study of the interactions and functional dynamics of whole sets of genes and their products. Genotype: The genetic makeup of an individual organism, which is determined by the specific alleles of each gene carried by the individual. Differences in alleles among individuals interact with environmental influences to account for the differences in phenotype observed among those individuals. Glucocorticoid: Any of a group of steroid hormones, such as cortisol, that are produced by the adrenal gland and are involved in the metabolism of carbohydrates, proteins, and fats; plays a key role in the stress response. Hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis: A system consisting of the hypothalamus, the pituitary, and either the ovaries or testes, which is involved in female or male reproduction, respectively, and awakens dramatically at puberty. Indicated prevention: Prevention efforts that identify individuals who are exhibiting early signs of alcohol abuse and other associated problem behaviors and target them with special programs. Individual-level interventions: Efforts to prevent alcohol use problems by focusing on the individual—that is, changing the person’s knowledge, attitudes, and skills, so that he or she is better able to resist influences that support drinking. Insulinlike growth factor-1 (IGF-1): A protein produced by the liver in response to growth hormone (GH). IGF-1 carries out some of the effects of GH at the tissue level. Internalizing disorders: A tendency in some children to express emotional distress within the child’s internal environment rather than his or her external world; behavior includes problems such as being withdrawn, anxious, inhibited, or depressed; also referred to as overcontrolled. Knockout: The deletion or deactivation of a gene in a mouse or other laboratory animal to create a line of animals that are incapable of producing the gene product. Mitochondria: Structures within cells that generate most of the cell’s energy through the production of adenosine triphosphate (ATP). Neuron: A nerve cell. Neuropeptide: A small molecule that can regulate nerve cell function and is made up of amino acids. Neurotransmitter: A chemical messenger (i.e., dopamine, GABA) that conveys a signal from one neuron to another. Nucleotides: Biological molecules with a variety of physiologic and metabolic functions; nucleotides also serve as the building blocks of DNA and RNA. Phenotype: The observable structural or functional characteristics of an individual organism that result from the interaction of its genotype with environmental factors. Phosphorylation: A chemical reaction resulting in the addition of phosphate groups to other molecules (e.g., proteins). Phosphorylation reactions often are critical to regulation of receptor activity and the functions of other proteins. Protein: The product of the genetic information encoded in a gene. Proteins are made up of amino acids; enzymes are one type of protein. Protein kinases: Enzymes that add phosphate groups to other proteins in a chemical reaction called phosphorylation; this activates or inactivates the modified proteins. Receptor: A protein, usually found on the surface of a neuron or other cell, that recognizes and binds to neurotransmitters or other chemical messengers. Reinforcement: A process by which a response or behavior (e.g., alcohol consumption) is strengthened by the anticipation of a reward (e.g., a feeling of euphoria). Ribonucleic acid (RNA): A class of molecules composed of nucleotides, similar to those that form DNA. The major types of RNA are mRNA, tRNA, and rRNA, which play important roles in gene expression. Selected prevention: Prevention strategies that target subsets of the total population which are deemed to be at risk for alcohol problems by virtue of their membership in a particular population segment—for example, children of adult alcoholics, dropouts, or students who are failing academically. Self-regulation: The ability to monitor and modulate internal states, including both the ability to modulate affect and level of arousal and the neurocognitive executive capacities to engage in goal-directed behavior. These executive cognitive capacities include the regulation of attention, planning, organization, concept formation, abstract reasoning, cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring, motor programming, and motor control. Serotonin: A neurotransmitter that subtly modifies neuron function, exerting its effects by interacting with receptors on the neuron’s surface. Synapse: A microscopic gap separating adjacent neurons, where neurotransmitters and receptors cluster. Transgenic animals: An animal into whose genome foreign DNA (i.e., a candidate gene) has been introduced to study the function of DNA. Prevention strategies that address the entire population (national, local community, school, neighborhood) with messages and programs aimed at preventing or delaying the abuse of alcohol. Universal prevention: