Draft paper - subsequently published as: Lewis, A. (2009) Methodological issues in exploring the ideas of children with autism concerning self and spirituality Religion, Disability and Health 13 64-76 ISSN 1522-8967 print/ 1522-9122 online; DOI 10.1080/ 15228960802581446 Methodological issues in exploring the ideas of children with autism concerning self and spirituality Ann Lewis, University of Birmingham * 1. Introduction To consider hearing directly from children with autism about their views concerning spirituality (see footnote concerning use of terms) is bold, possibly foolhardy, on,at least, three counts. 1.1 They are children: A child’s powers of observation are less reliable than an adult’s .. prone to live in a make believe world … they are also very egocentric… very suggestible … often have little notion of the duty to speak the truth.. Finally children sometimes behave in a way evil beyond their years. (Heydon [an English lawyer] 1984: 84) With so many mindbytes to be downloaded, so many mental codons to be replicated, it is no wonder that child brains are gullible, open to almost any suggestion, vulnerable to subversion. (Dawkins, cited in Mills and Keil 2005) 1.2 They have autism: By definition, this brings concomitant communication difficulties, although it is important to remember the heterogeinity of children with autism. DSM-IV-TR (APA 2000) specifies three diagnostic categories, each with four components, that are used to make a diagnosis of autistic disorder. These diagnostic categories include impairments in social interaction, communication, and particular (often very rigid) patterns of behaviour. and 1.3 the focus is on spirituality. 1 The difficulties in researching this area are widely recognised; for example: People are often extremely shy of talking about spirituality in public for fear that they will be thought stupid, or even mentally disturbed .. The disadvantage of encouraging children to talk in religious terms was that it tended to trigger off impersonal “learned” responses rather than reference to the child’s personal experiences (Hay with Nye 2006: 88) 2. Why is it important to hear directly from children with autism about their views concerning spirituality? First, there is increasing recognition of children’s right to express their views (Alderson and Morrow 2004; Lewis and Lindsay 2000). The 1989 United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child (Article 12) emphasises the right of all children to have a voice when decisions are being made about their provision. That provision should (although often does not do so) include meeting spiritual needs/wants. Second, there is considerable evidence of the links between health, and spirituality or faith (Hill and Pargament 2003; Plante and Sherman 2001). Most of this work has concerned adults but in two studies involving children and young people living with serious chronic illnesses, they were also found to derive important coping strategies from a religious or spiritual framework of beliefs (Culliford 2002; Pendleton et al 2002). It would be reasonable to hypothesise that children with autism (and other disabilities) also gain similar support from such beliefs. Third, accounts of faith-based groups in relation to children with special needs or disabilities abound with examples of misunderstandings, prejudice and intolerance towards those children (Turner et al 2004; Vogel et al 2006). For example, Vogel et al (2006) cite reports of children/young people with autism not being given communion on the assumption that such children did not understand the meaning behind this practice. How do adults know that these children do not understand unless they ask the children? However there are also impressive accounts of understanding and inclusion in faith communities (e.g. Gregory 1989) and valuable guidance (Swinton 2001; Swinton and Powrie 2004), some of which does make reference to hearing (usually informally) disabled children’s views. Speraw’s review (2006) reflects these positions and concludes: “Future research must extend to developing insights into the inner life of disabled persons, especially children, as their lived experience has not been explored” (2006: 229). 2 Fourth, children with autism may, by definition, exhibit characteristics which have distinctive and autism-specific features which impact on their religious or spiritual experiences and understanding. Baron-Cohen (2008) discusses “the new idea that the autistic cognitive style searches for truth [author’s italics] (defined as precise, reliable, consistent, or lawful patterns or structure in data) that can lead people with autism to perceive patterns with remarkable accuracy but at the same time renders them challenged ..by information that is ambiguous” (2008: 65). In the present context this predisposition could be seen as likely to support strong religious faith [or fervent atheism] in people with autism. One interpretation of this last point is that people with ASD will be drawn differentially to varous religiosn or sects with, arguably, the more obvioulsy rulle-bound (e.g. Catholicism? Judaism?) attarcting people with ASD more strongly than would, for example, Liberal Quakers. What people with ASD would presumably find difficult or untenable is the agnostic position or the wavering fuzziness of many (perhaps most) people’s religious quests. We lack reliable research evidence about such nuanced realtionships between children in intra-disablity groups and their religious beliefs. 3. Challenges Generic research on children and spirituality has not explicitly included children with special needs/disabilities so it is not clear whether they have been omitted, given the likely methodological difficulties in accessing their views, or subsumed within larger groups (Hay with Nye 2006; Francis 2001; Kimes-Myers 2002). If we accept the importance of hearing directly from children and young people with autism about their views concerning spirituality then a number of specific methodological challenges arise including the following: 3.1 Gatekeepers Children with autism, like other children with special needs or disabilities, may through well-meaning protectiveness, not be included when children’s views are sought concerning spirituality. For example, when seeking the views of children, in mainstream schools, with epilepsy about their condition (Lewis, Parsons and Smith, 2007) one headteacher anticipated that the child, a 7 year old with epilepsy, would not be able to say anything about her epilepsy because the child did not understand the condition. This was understandable in that it is probable that nobody at the school had talked with the child about epilepsy, given the widely reported culture of secrecy around the condition (Baker et al. 2005; Lewis, Parsons and Smith 2007). However in the event, this child was thoughtful and articulate; for example, trying to explain her experience of a seizure: “(Like) it is raining in my head. Like when it is thundering outside”. The dearth of research evidence about spirituality in relation to the views of children with autism may point to similar cautiousness. 3 This caution may be compounded in some faiths and cultures by views of these children as particularly ‘spiritual’ or on a higher spiritual plane than ‘neuro-normals’; the presumed heightened spirituality of children with autism leading to a perception that it would be inappropriate or irreligious to probe their spirituality (Shaked and Bilu 2006; see symposium papers). 3.2 Ethical protocols There is increasing emphasis on the importance of following ethical protocols (e.g. British Psychological Society 1996; British Educational Research Association 2004) in research, particularly when that research involves children or other ‘vulnerable’ participants. An element of such protocols is usually ensuring, or, at a minimum, checking, that participants understand matters such as confidentiality, anonymity, rights to withdraw, and the importance of recurrent fully informed consent to participate. It is unclear whether children really understand these protocols and when, rarely, they are tested explicitly younger children (chronological age <10) have been found not to understand/believe the ethical assurances given by the researchers (Hurley and Underwood 2002). Children with autism may find ethical protocols particularly problematic as they relate closely to personal relationships and associated expectations or conventions. 3.3 Ground rules for the interaction It is well recognised that, by definition, establishing a joint focus or arriving at a shared agenda is problematic when engaging with children with autism. Shaked and Bilu, citing Scarry 1985, describe this succinctly as a “breakdown in the taken-for-granted experience of reality as shared” (2006: 2). As a result, particular methods have been developed to try to circumvent such difficulties (see below). These methods also try to address associated difficulties such as a communication/ language mismatch for these children, i.e. they know more than they can say or, conversely, say more than they understand. Rocha et al (2007) report a successful parent-implemented intervention programme which targeted getting and sustaining joint attention with children with autism. Researchers working with such children might similarly need to explicitly add joint attention-getting skills to their research repertoire. These children’s emotional support needs may also be reflected in a need for structure and predictability. This makes visual stimuli, often used elsewhere in research in hearing children’s views, possibly problematic as particular stimuli may prompt rigid responses, as found in the use of cue cards with some young children with autism (Lewis et al 2005; Lewis et al in press). Cue cards, six cards based on extensive piloting and reflecting visual prompts for key narrative elements, were otherwise highly effective in a diverse group of disabled children and young people from primary school age children with learning disabilities to young offenders. 4 4. General guidance in accessing children’s views (drawing on Lewis 2004, Lewis and Porter 2007; Lewis, Parsons and Robertson 2007) The importance of modifying interview techniques when communicating with children with disabilities or special needs has been well-documented (Lewis and Lindsay 2000; Lewis and Porter 2007). To ilustarte: Cederborg et al (2007) working within an applied cognitive psychology paradigm in relation to suspected child abuse, concluded that when children with severe learning difficulties (‘intellectual disabilities’) were given a second chance to provide information then they could give new and supplementary information. These researchers make the proviso that information from such repeated interviews may not be reliable unless the interviewers have received specific training in talking with these kinds of children and young people in this context. 4.1 Permitting/encouraging ‘don’t know’ responses and requests for clarification Understanding of complex abstract concepts, such as those associated with religion, may prompt a high proportion of non responses (see McKenna’s work (1998) on children’s views about understanding of Christian symbols involving primary age children with learning difficulties.) Ceci and Bruck (1995, 1993) provide powerful illustrations from courtroom contexts of distortion arising from the interviewer’s apparent refusal to believe that a child had forgotten a detail, or had a legitimate reason for not wanting to repeat an earlier remark. While there is less at stake in a research context, their warning about distortion arising from, in effect, disallowing ‘don’t know’ responses or silence, is applicable. There is evidence that encouraging children to explicitly say that they do not know the ‘answer’ or do not understand a question is valuable. Saywitz and her co-workers (Saywitz 1995) interviewed 6-8 year olds using a range of questions with linguistic formats which varied from very easy to very complex. In summary, this study demonstrated that these young children could be taught to identify non comprehension and to ask for re-phrasing. Both strategies led to improvements in the accuracy of the answers then given. The need for such training is suggested in a more recent study, involving nearly 400 2-5 year olds, which found that children rarely answered ‘I don't know’ even when they had been encouraged to admit to not knowing (Fritzley and Lee 2003). Colleagues working in the autism field suggested three possible repercussions in relation to children with autism. First, these children and young people may be happy to respond that they don’t know. Indeed that response may feel safer for them than trying to answer a question to which they do not have a response. So for those children it will be very important that they develop skills through which they are able to request the person asking the question to clarify what they mean. Second, children with autism, 5 may give over-full responses. So the interviewer may be answered with a lengthy monologue addressing every aspect of the question. Third, linking with a lack of joint focus, above, the child may neither say ‘don’t know’ nor answer the question but instead talk at length to their own agenda. 4.2 Stressing not knowing events/views of child to counter the child’s assumption that the adult knows the answer Children, more readily than adults, will assume that the adult questioner knows more than they do. This is especially so if the adult has credibility with the child so, in this context, building rapport may be counter-productive in that it may lead to a greater proneness to suggestibility, possibly less true of children with autism than it is of other children. A possible way to deal with this is to use a ‘ventriloquist’ interviewer such as a soft toy which ‘asks the questions’ as children may more readily perceive the soft toy to be asking a genuine question whereas the sincerity of the adult questioner may be perceived as false. Current work concerning attempts to teach people with autism a ‘theory of mind’ and facial recognition, through using virtual worlds as well as objects and toys imbued with personal characteristics, is relevant here (Parsons et al 2004). Using a toy can motivate the child to want to answer, particularly if it is a ‘character’ in which they are interested, such as Thomas the Tank Engine, Teletubbies or a dinosaur. It may also be easier for the child than trying to relate directly to a person because potentially confusing contextual factors are reduced. 4.3 Use of statements compared with questions A range of work with children has shown the value of making statements that prompt a response, rather than a direct question, to elicit views. The tendency for adults, particularly teachers, to use question-answer-feedback routines has been described by some writers as reflecting power relationships (Edwards and Mercer 1987). Through the use of questions, the adult keeps the ‘upper hand’. Thus in the research context, the use of statements rather than questions also reflects an implied power relationship. Preece (2002) notes, in relation to the possible content of statements/questions directed to children with autism, those children’s likely difficulties in anticipating the future, showing personal insight and accepting change. These characteristics will influence both explicit and intuitive decisions about the phrasing of statements. 4.4 Use of repeat questions Repeating a question has been found to lead to fewer correct but not more incorrect answers in mainstream 6, 8 and 10 year olds; that is, when the question was repeated they either did not answer or they said ‘don’t know’ (Moston 1987). There is a possible analogy here with classroom talk in which children are tuned into the idea that a repeat question means that the first 6 response was incorrect. Note that these dangers of repeating a question applied to closed, but not to open, questions (Bull 1995). As Beresford (1997) notes, there can be value in repeating the focus of an open question. She reports a study about children’s experience of pain in which children were asked about migraine headaches. The children were asked (individually) how they would describe the headache to a parent, a friend and a doctor. The answers varied depending on the role of the listener with a concise and unemotional account being given to a parent, an emotive account being given to a friend and a full account being given to a doctor. This illustrates the impact of the perceived context on the nature of the information the child chooses to voice but it would be useful to know how the reported variability in imagined accounts would have matched actual accounts in the real contexts. There is some evidence that closed questions may be more effective than open questions in engaging with children with autism. Guldberg (personal communication) found that children with autism showed more responses and initiated communication more often in structured, compared with unstructured, situations in the classroom. Unstructured situations led to less communication. This may explain why the PECS (Picture Exchange Communication System) is very useful for such children; that is, it not only provides a structure to the communication but also, through that structure, explicitly teaches children about reciprocity. 4.5 Use of successive prompts In general, young children tend to be accurate but incomplete in their accounts (Saywitz and Snyder 1993). There is a well-documented tendency to confabulation, i.e. filling in gaps in memory with invention, which is more likely for areas about which memory is unsure. Work on the forensic interviewing of children (Ceci and Bruck 1995) illustrates the ways in which successive prompting tends to lead to inaccurate responses. With repeated questioning children fill in imaginary details which they then come to believe with great conviction. Interviewers of children with autism may inadvertently reinforce this because such children tend to have a difficulty with personal memory of events (Jordan and Powell 1995). In line with this, confabulation seems to be more likely in relation to descriptions of people or things rather than events (Spencer and Flin 1990). A useful caution is given by Beresford (1997) that children’s apparent weaknesses may more fairly be seen as reflecting their different priorities. For example, she found that when children were interviewed about the birth of a sibling, they did not recall details to the same degree as did the mother. So, Beresford suggests, this was not a reflection of memory loss but a difference in the significance of the event for the children compared with the mother. 4.6 Right to silence 7 Finally, we need to acknowledge that children may not wish to express their views: We need to recognise the choice of a child to be silent but also recognise that silence gives a message of its own that we should hear. Lewis and Porter 2007: 230 5. Conclusion Recognising the methodological points reviewed briefly here, some feasible approaches when exploring with children with autism their views concerning spirituality are: self generated approaches, such as digital cameras (Germain, 2004; Lewis et al 2005; Beresford et al 2004) and ‘ideal self’ drawings (Moran 2006). The search, as Richard Rorty noted in discussing Descartes, is for wisdom not certainty. I concur with Hay and Nye (2006:85) that “The most important preliminary statement one needs to make is about integrity”. “There are no universal solutions, nor a perfect methodology .. . there is a need for transparency around the difficulties and a fuller sharing of the methods used, including those which were not successful. This calls for a willingness to be flexible in using more innovative approaches and developing new skills, and to be thorough and systematic in validating our analysis” Lewis and Porter 2007: 230 extend/ beef up concl Footnote Use of terms 1. The term ‘spirituality’ is contentious and often ambiguous (sometimes used interchangeably with ‘religion’/‘religiosity’/faith) but is used here to mean: “(an) awareness of the transcendent ('the beyond in our midst’), the awareness of something beyond intellectual knowledge or normal sensory experience .. thus concerned with: meaning and purpose in life; interconnectedness and harmony with other people, planet Earth and the Universe; and a right relationship with God/a power or force in the Universe which transcends the present context of reality”. (http://www.oxford.anglican.org/page/2316/ accessed 31/10/07). 8 2. This contrasts with the term ‘religion’: “a shared framework of theistic beliefs and rituals that gives a social context within which spirituality is expressed and nurtured, and the meaning of life explored” (http://www.oxford.anglican.org/page/2316/ accessed 31/10/07). 3. The word ‘autism’ is used to encompass the whole spectrum of autistic spectrum disorders /conditions (i.e. including classic autism and Asperger’s) (see Baron-Cohen 2008). References Alderson, P. and Morrow, G. (2004) Ethics, social research and consulting with children and young people. London: Barnardo's. American Psychiatric Association (APA) (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Baker, G.A., Spector, S., McGrath, Y. and Soteriou, H. (2005) Impact of epilepsy in adolescence: a UK controlled study. Epilepsy and Behavior, 6 556562. Baron-Cohen, S. (2008) Autism, hypersystemizing, and truth Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 61 1 64-75 Beresford, B., (1997) Personal Accounts: Involving Disabled Children in Research. Stationary Office: London Beresford, B et al (2004) Developing an approach to involving children with autistic spectrum disorders in a social care research project. British Journal of Learning Disabilities 32 180-85 Bull. R. (1995) ‘Innovative techniques for the questioning of child witnesses, especially those who are young and those with learning disability’ pp 179-194 in M.S. Zaragoza, (Ed) Memory and Testimony in the Child Witness Thousand Oaks: Sage Ceci, S.J. and Bruck, M. (1993) The suggestibility of the child witness: An historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin 113 349-403 Ceci, S. and Bruck, M. (1995) Jeopardy in the Courtroom Washington DC: American Psychological Association 9 Cederborg, A-C., La Rooy, D. and Lamb, M.E. (2007) Repeated interviews with children who have intellectual disablities Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disablities Culliford, L. (2002) Spirituality and clinical care. British Medical Journal 325:1434-1435 (21 December). Dawkins, cited in Mills, C. and Keil, F. (2005) The development of cynicism. Psychological Science, 16, 385-390. Francis, L.J. (2001) The Values Debate: A voice from the pupils. London: Taylor and Francis Fritzley, V.H. and Lee, K. (2003) Do young children always say yes to yes-no questions? A metadevelopmental study of the affirmation bias, Child Development 74 5 1297–1313. Germain, R. (2004) An exploratory study using cameras and Talking Mats to access the views of young people with learning disabilities on their out of school activities. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32 4 170-174. Gregory, S (1989) People with Disabilities and the Religious Community. University of Connecticut, USA. Guldberg, K. (2007) personal communication Hay, D with Nye, R (2006) The Spirit of the Child London: Jessica Kingsley Heydon 1984 – cited in J. Cashmore and K. Bussey (1996) ‘Judicial Perceptions of Child Witness Competence’ Journal of Law and Human Behavior, 20 3 313-334 Hill, P.C. and Pargament, K.I. (2003) Advances in the conceptualization and measurement of religion and spirituality: Implications for physical and mental health research. American Psychologist 58 1 64-74 Hurley, J.C. and Underwood, M.K. (2002) Children's understanding of their research rights before and after debriefing: Informed assent, confidentiality, and stopping participation Child Development 73 1 132–143. Jordan, R and Powell, S. (1995) Understanding and Teaching Children With Autism London: Wiley Kimes-Myers, B. (2002) Young Children and Spirituality. London: Taylor and Francis La Rooy, D. 10 Lewis A. (2004) ‘And When Did You Last See Your Father?’ Exploring the views of children with learning difficulties/disabilities. British Journal of Special Education, 31(1) 4-10 Lewis, A., Newton, H. and Vials, S. (in press) Realising child voice: The development of Cue Cards. Support for Learning Lewis, A. and Lindsay, G. (eds) (2000) Researching Children’s Perspectives. Buckingham: Open University Press Lewis, A., Parsons, S. and Robertson, C. (2007) My school, my family, my life: Telling it like it is. A study drawing on the experiences of disabled children, young people and their families in Great Britain in 2006. London: Disability Rights Commission/ Birmingham: University of Birmingham, School of Education. http://83.137.212.42/sitearchive/DRC/library/research/education/my_school_ my_family_my_life.html Summary at http://83.137.212.42/sitearchive/DRC/library/research/education/my_school_ my_family_my_life.html Lewis, A., Parsons, S and Smith, P (2007) Understanding of epilepsy by children with, or without, epilepsy. Final report to Epilepsy Action. Birmingham: School of Education / Leeds: Epilepsy Action. Website: http://www.epilepsy.org.uk/research/funded.html Lewis, A. and Porter, J (2007) Research and pupil voice. pp 222-232 in L Florian (ed) Handbook of Special Education. London: Sage Lewis, A. and Porter, J. (2004) Interviewing children and young people with learning disabilities: guidelines for researchers and multi-professional practice. British Journal of Learning Disabilities 32 4 191-197 Lewis, A., Robertson, C. and Parsons, S. (2005). Experiences of Disabled Students and their Families. Phase 1. Research report to Disability Rights Commission, June 2005. Birmingham: University of Birmingham, School of Education. http://83.137.212.42/sitearchive/DRC/library/research/education/experiences_ of_disabled.html McKenna, U. (1998) Religious understanding in primary age children with learning difficulties. MA thesis, University of Warwick. Moran, H. (2006) A very personal assessment: Using a Personal Construct Psychology assessment technique (Drawing the Ideal Self) with young people with ASD to explore the child’s view of the self. Good Autism Practice 7 Moston, S. (1987) The suggestibility of children in interview studies First Language 7 19 67-78 11 ADD TO TEXT Parsons, S., Lewis, A. and Ellins, J. (in press). The views and experiences of parents of children with autistic spectrum disorder about educational provision: comparisons with parents of children with other disabilities from an online survey. European Journal of Special Needs Education. Parsons, S., Mitchell, P. and Leonard, A. (2004) The use and understanding of virtual environments by adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34 4 449-466 Pendleton, S., Cavilli, K. S., Pargament, K. I., and Nasr, S. (2002). Spirituality in children with cystic fibrosis: A qualitative study. Pediatrics, 109, 1–11. Plante, T. G., and Sherman, A. C. (Eds.). (2001) Faith and health: Psychological perspectives. New York: Guilford Press. Preece, D. (2002) Consultation with children with ASD about their experience of short term residential care British Journal of Learning Disabilities 30 97-104 Rocha, M. L.; Schreibman, L. and Stahmer, A. C. (2007) Effectiveness of training parents to teach joint attention in children with autism. Journal of Early Intervention, 29 2 154-173 Saywitz, K.J. (1995) ‘Improving Children’s Testimony: The question, the answer and the environment’ pp 113-140 in M.S. Zaragoza (Ed) Memory and Testimony in the Child Witness Thousand Oaks: Sage Saywitz, K.J., and Snyder, L. (1993). Improving children’s testimony with preparation. pp. 117-146 in G.S. Goodman & B.L. Bottoms (Eds.) Child victims, child witnesses. Understanding and improving testimony New York: Guilford Press Shaked, M. and Bilu, Y. (2006) Grappling with affliction: Autism in the Jewish Ultraorthodox Community in Israel. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 30 1 -27 Spencer, J.R. and Flin, R. (1990) The Evidence of Children London: Blackstone Press Speraw, S. (2006) Spiritual experiences of parents and caregivers who have children with disabilities or special needs. Issues in Mental Health Nursing 27 213-230 Swinton, J. (2001) A Space to Listen: Meeting the spiritual needs of people with learning disabilities. London: Foundation for People with Learning Disabilities Swinton, J., and Powrie, E. (2004) Why are we here? Meeting the spiritual needs of people with learning disabilities. London: Foundation for People with Learning Disabilities. 12 Turner, S. et al (2004) Religious expression amongst adults with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities. 17 161171. Vogel, J., Polloway, E.A. and Smith, J.D. (2006) Inclusion of people with mental retardation and other developmental disabilities in communities of faith, Mental Retardation 44 2 100-111 Acknowledgment With thanks to Karen Guldberg (School of Education, University of Birmingham) for insightful discussions about the issues raised here. 13