Development of Gender 1 Running head: DEVELOPMENT OF GENDER ROLE IDENTITY Developmental Psychology Critiques B: Assignment 2 Nadeira Mia University of Johannesburg 200501093 Development of Gender 2 Development of Gender Role Identity In understanding gender role identity and its development there are many concepts which need to be defined and understood. Fogel (2001) explains that at about 18 months of age an infant can use gender labelling where the child identifies themselves or others as male or female but it is only at about age 3 that they realise that these male and female characteristics are enduring ones. But one wonders how this awareness develops, this essay serves to answer this question, amongst others. First we will look at the concepts around gender role identity, then an in depth explanation of the different theories of gender role identity development will follow, and we will conclude by looking at the recent research around gender role identity and later life. Shaffer (2001) refers to many concepts that refer to gender and gender role development which should be understood before we look at the different theories. Firstly gender typing refers to the process in which the child becomes aware of his/her gender (Shaffer, 2001, p. 456) and is similar to Fogel’s “gender labelling” discussed earlier. A gender role standard refers to “is a value, motive, or a class of behaviour that is considered more appropriate for members of one sex than the other.” (Shaffer, 2001, p. 456). For example playing with dolls would be more appropriate and acceptable for girls than boys. We will now look at gender stereotypes and the different roles that are taken on or are expected from men and women. Gender Role Stereotypes In terms of gender role identity and its development there have been different theories (which will be discussed later), however it is important to remember that to some extent gender role identity is shaped by people and society. This is both beneficial and detrimental. Sex role or gender role stereotypes “are culturally shared assumptions and expectations about sex differences in abilities, personality traits, activities, and roles” (Weinrub, Clemens, Sockloff, Ethridge, Graceley & Myers, 1984, p. 1493). This then refers to society and their prescriptions in terms of what is expected of the respective genders and these take place early in the child’s development. Shaffer (2001, p 465) explains that 2-3 year olds who have already internalised their own gender and that of others, but they also have an understanding of the gender stereotypes. For example female toddlers would prefer to play with their dolls whereas male toddlers would play with cars or guns. Further gender segregation will occur where Shaffer (2001) explains that children would choose same sex children as playmates (p. 467). Development of Gender 3 One wonders then that of all this development of gender role identity occurs in early childhood what happens later on in life? According to Shaffer (2001, p 464) explains that although form about age 8 children can pursue cross sex roles they do not always choose to, thus a process called gender intensification occurs where there is a magnification of sex differences occurring in early adolescence due to the pressure from society to conform to traditional gender roles. Keeping these concepts in mind, we will now look at the different theories around gender role identity development. Theories on Gender Role Identity Development Different theories have been developed over the years to explain gender role identity development and sex differences. We will be discussing six (6) of these theories. Freud’s Psychoanalytical Theory Freud’s classic theory on development can be seen as a starting point in looking at Freud’s Psychoanalytic theory of gender role identity. Freud’s classic psychoanalytic theory explains that children develop an attraction to the opposite sex parent and thus resent and are jealous of the same sex parent. In order to resolve this conflict the child begins to identify with the same sex parent and try to ‘imitate’ the same sex parent. Shaffer (2001) explains “one’s gender identity and preference for a gender role emerge during the phallic stage as children begin to emulate and to identify with the same sex parent” (p 476). For the above mentioned reasons i.e. the conflict with the same sex parent due to the attraction to the opposite sex parent, children begin developing their gender role identity. As such children adopt the characteristics and qualities of the same sex parent (Bandura and Bussey, 1999, p. 3). But Bandura and Bussey (1999) contend that females in particular begin to identify with the same sex parent because they have penis envy or resentment because they do not have visible genitalia but gender role development is more difficult for a girl because like them, their mother lacks a penis. Boys in addition to being attracted to their mother, develop gender role identity with the father due to fear of punishment according to Freud, Ding and Littleton (2005). Ding and Littleton (2005) explain that Freud’s theory on gender role development was widely accepted at first but later was attacked by many who claimed that the theory had little empirical support and had no testable hypothesis, thus the theory was often disregarded in looking at gender identity development. We will now go on to look at the cognitive developmental theory in terms of gender role identity development. Development of Gender 4 Kohlberg’s Cognitive Development Theory According to Shaffer (2001) “Kohlberg suggests that children first establish a stable gender identity and then actively seek out same-sex models and other information to learn how to act like a boy or a girl” (p. 478). This suggests then the fact that one is a specific gender has to develop in the mind of the child before they can learn behaviour/s associated with their own gender and this relies then on the child’s cognitive development in understanding their gender. Ding & Littleton (2005) explain in this case then children’s concept of themselves as male or female are imperative in encouraging children to identify and accept gender roles (p. 158). To some extent it seems that the cognitive development theory expects the child to internalise and process their gender before they can accept the roles and expectations that come with them. There are three (3) stages identified by Kohlberg in terms of cognitive development in terms of gender role identity formation. They are Gender Identity or gender labeling, Gender Stability and Gender consistency (Shaffer, 2005, p. 479). Gender Identity or gender labelling, the first stage refers to when they children identify themselves as “boys or girls” (Ding & Littleton, 2005, p. 159). Shaffer (2005) explains that gender identity develops at around age three (3). Bandura and Bussey (1999) explains that the second stage: gender stability occurs when children realise that gender is stable over time, meaning that a boy will always be a boy and will grow into a man and vice versa for women. The third stage or gender consistency occurs when “children have full appreciation of the permanence of gender over time and across situations” (Ding & Littleton, 2005, p. 159). Explained directly by Kohlberg (1966) gender constancy is the realization that one’s gender is permanent dependent on underlying biological characteristics which does not change with physical appearance, this only develops at about six (6) years of age. Ding and Littleton (2005) thus explains that gender role development is self socialized according to this theory because although society does play a role in socialisation, it is the child who decides what they seek from the environment and behaves according to this. This gives one the idea that the child has a choice as to how they would like to behave and how they should not and one wonders how a child of about four (4) to six (6) years of age can make sense of this and further gender constancy is a prerequisite to gender role development. So one could question the theory’s credibility. The theory is accepted until one considers the ages and stages and the prerequisites. This is because Kohlberg is suggesting that until the child is mature enough to understand gender consistency they cannot develop gender identity and this is in fact false. Shaffer (2001) suggests that two to three year olds can already decide Development of Gender 5 what toys to tend to based on their gender even though they have not as yet developed gender typing/labelling and/or gender constancy. There was very little evidence according to Bandura and Bussey (1999) for the Kohlberg’s cognitive developmental theory. We will now go on to discuss gender schema theory in understanding gender role identity development. Gender Schema Theory Gender Schema theory was developed by Carol Martin and Charles Halverson. Gender Schema theory begins it explanation hat of Kohlberg’s developmental theory. Ding and Littleton (2005) explains “where Kohlberg highlighted the relatively late-developing full understanding of object constancy, the gender schema theorists argue that it is the early cognitive processes underlying children’s ability to label themselves as girls or boys that play the key role in gender development” (p .162). Gender Schema theory thus deviates from Kohlbergs Conitive Development theory in many ways. The central point of gender schema theory is the development of schemas, gender schemas. A gender schema is an “organised set of beliefs and expectations about males and females that guide information processing” (Shaffer, 2005, p. 480). The theory then suggest that once these schemas are developed within the chid, it is elaborated on in terms of activities, interests, personality and social attributes ( Bandura and Bussey ,1999, p. 5). As such the gender schema helps one behave according to traditional gender roles, the child acts in accordance with these roles because they want to be like and accepted my members of their own sex, which is similar to Kohlberg’s gender labelling stage. These labels and schemas guide the child’s thinking and thus their behaviour. In addition to the gender schema, Shaffer (2005) explains that children also develop an own-sex schema and an in-group/out-group schema. The in-group/out-group schema develops first where child “classifies behaviour, roles, and objects for boys and for girls” (Shaffer, 2005, p. 480). For example girls wear the colour pink and play with dolls and boys wear the colour blue and play with guns. Own-sex schemas refer to knowledge of one’s own gender that allows one to act in accordance with gender specific activities and roles. For example it is expected that because girls are supposed to play with dolls they will ignore any information given to them regarding cars or guns because it is not in accordance with their own-sex schema. Interestingly enough the formation of a gender stereotype was seen as a way in which information about genders was stored and processed in the mind of a child and as such was seen as useful and the more well developed a child’s gender schema is the stronger their gender linked preferences (Ding and Littleton, 2005, p. 162). Further the schemas are Development of Gender 6 relatively stable across situations. Thus the more existing knowledge of a specific gender the more informed decision the child can make regarding their preferences in terms of play and roles etc. and this knowledge is carried through from situation to situation. Bandura and Bussey (1999) explain that gender schema theory was seen as useful in its approach because it explained the internal working model of the child but also notes that it is flexible in terms of how much knowledge is stored and the associated preferences. Further it is useful in understanding the cognitive processes underlying gender role development. We will now look at social influences and social learning theory in terms of gender role identity development. Social influences on gender role identity development and Social Learning Theory There are long standing theories which suggest that gender specific behaviour is learnt via other people and the media, basically our society and the larger social context. Thus the knowledge acquired is learnt from those around us. “This is consistent with the behaviourist approach which had been dominant in Western psychology since the early twentieth century: learning takes place through conditioning, rewards and punishment, and observation of behaviours of others” (Ding & Littleton, 2005, p. 152). Thus it is suggested that for infants and children their reality and their world is shaped by their parents/caregivers. Ding and Littleton (2005) contend that the caregivers can shape the child’s perception of themselves using colours and clothes, toys and activities. The interaction with the parents/caregivers is central. In terms of social learning theory we look at observational learning as well as behaviour that is reinforced (or punished). The theory of observational learning was suggested by Bandura (1989) as it was believed that children learn much of their gender specific behaviour by observing and imitating others. “The assumption is that boys will see which toys, activities and behaviour are for boys and girls will learn which activities and behaviours are for girls by selectively attending to and imitating a variety of same-sex models, including peers, teachers, older siblings, media personalities, and their mothers or fathers” (Shaffer, 2001, p. 477). Depending then on people children decide to shadow, their behaviours are learnt. Reinforcement or punishment for specific behaviour too plays an important role in learning gender specific behaviours. Shaffer (2001) and Ding and Littleton (2005) agree that parents are the main agents in reinforcing or punishing and thus fostering gender role specific behaviour in children. Shaffer (2001) explains that parents who show the “clearest patterns of differential reinforcement” have children who quickly know their gender identity and have Development of Gender 7 toy and activity preferences, and understand gender stereotypes. So here even though the main point is whether behaviour is reinforced or punished it still depends on the social, i.e. people around the child. The theory has been very well received over the years and a lot of what the theory states in terms of positive and negative reinforcements as well as observational learning. However the theory has been questioned because it seems that the child is merely accepting all this and is completely passive in the process of gender role identity development (Shaffer, 2001, p. 479). We will now go on to look at the biological influences on gender role identity development. Media also has been seen to play a role in social development of gender role identity. Biological Influences In understanding the genetic influences on gender role identity we will look at genetic influences, and evolutionary psychology. Some evolutionary theories suggest that men are merely contributors in the formation of offspring and women are the caretakers of the infant/child, thus men evolved into more aggressive and females evolved into more nurturing being (Bandura and Bussey, 1999, p. 7). Further Bandura and Bussey (1999) explain that because men are naturally bigger in anatomy than women they are put in a superior position and therefore they evolved to be more aggressive. “According to evolutionary psychology, many current gender differences, such as number of sexual partners preferred and criteria for selecting sexual partners, aggression, jealousy and roles they fulfilled originated from ancestral sex differentiated reproductive strategies” (Bandura and Bussey ,1999, p. 7). This could mean then that the gender differences occurring today may have come about by natural selection theories and were just spilled over into other aspects of ones life. Genetics has been seen to influence ones gender role development and gender identity in terms of cognitive ability, personality and social behaviour. According to Shaffer (2001) the “timing of puberty” effect can explain gender differences. The “timing of puberty effect” suggests that people who reach puberty late perform better on spatial and visual tasks than those who mature early (Shaffer, 2001, p. 472). The evolutionary theory in psychology brings important points to the fore but it has been criticised for ignoring the social aspect and its impact on gender role development (Bandura and Bussey ,1999, p. 7). We will now take a look at Money and Ehrhardts biosocial theory to gender role identity development. Development of Gender 8 Money and Ehrhardt’s Biosocial Theory According to Shaffer (2001) Money and Ehrdhardt (1972) looked at gender role development and although they did agree that there is a biological basis for development of gender role identity and gender differentiation, but biological theorists seem to ignore the social aspect and its impact on the development of gender role identity. They explain that the child’s gender is already decided on conception with the mixing of the x and y chromosomes, following this, the male hormones, testosterone and mullerian inhibiting substance, will be produced and male genitalia will form. If testosterone is absent then female genitalia will form (Shaffer, 2001). Money and Ehrdhardt believe then that after conception the child is exposed to social factors which then shape their gender role identity based on the gender that was biologically determined, because parents and the like interact with the child based on their gender (Shaffer, 2001). Shaffer (2001) also explains that culture to play a role but all of this is related to the social aspect of gender role development, thus the interaction of biological and social factors form the biosocial theory of gender role identity development. Although not much was written about the validity of this theory there seems to now be an integration of theories in understanding gender role identity development, where not only the social and biological factors are considered but also the gender schemas and cognitive developmental models. Regarding this it was said “the processes that different theories emphasize seem to be especially important at different periods” (Shaffer, 2001, p. 481). We will now go on to look at some interesting information on gender role identity development and its effect on different areas in later life. Gender Role Identity Development and Later Life Gender role identity development and the formation of eating disorders What was found with eating disorders is the common idea that eating disorders, such as anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa, are “female” disorders i.e. Disorders that occur mostly in females and not so predominantly in males. The journal article that will be referred to serves to question whether these disorders occur more in females than in males due to gender role identity development factors or to other reasons. It is common knowledge that females are more at risk to developing these disorders and the blame has especially been put on the media who display unrealistic ideals of thinness for women. These ideals may cause women to want to reach them and they may become preoccupied with their bodies and weight loss, this preoccupation can become an obsession and ultimately an eating disorder. “However as noted, clinical evidence suggests that syndromes of anorexia and bulimia do occur with men with a similar symptom picture and course as in women” (Cantrell & Ellis, 1991, p. 54). Development of Gender 9 The researchers explain that men are underrepresented in the literature regarding eating disorders. The results of the study showed that the relationship between gender role identity and eating disorder patterns did differ in men and women., and they attributed the high number of females with eating disorders “not as a function of the inherent femaleness of the disorders but of the complex transitions of the female role in our culture” (Cantrell & Ellis, 1991, p. 54). This suggests then that the female role and the way it is upheld in society is mainly to blame for the amount of eating disorder cases in women and this specifically affects women because they are targeting in adverts for diets etc. SO it is not ones gender that puts one at risk for a make women more at risk. We will now\look at gender role identity and relationship choices. Gender role identity development and its effect on relationship choices “A growing body of literature suggests that gender role identity plays an important role both in friendships and romantic relationships.” (Waack, Gerrity, Taub & Baldo; 2001, p. 33). This was the starting point when the researchers decided to look at gender, gender role identity, relationship types and the effect on relationship behaviours in college students. The article suggests that gender role identity can be defined as “how an individual perceives himself or herself as possessing masculine or feminine qualities” and it is found to be related to how male friends one has, the relationship between intimacy and identity and ones conceptions about friendship (Baldo et al., 2001, p. 35).Results of the study suggested that gender role identity affected relationship behaviour in the following ways- feminine women would display more feminine behaviour than would masculine women, and masculine men would display more masculine behaviour than feminine men (Baldo et al., 2001, p. 40). Results also showed that gender role identity affected the type of relationship and associated relationship behaviours in the following ways: men displayed more masculine behaviours than women irrespective of the type relationship and vice versa. The researchers then questioned gender role identity and its effects on irrational beliefs in the relationship and no significant differences were found. After looking at the related journal articles and the different theories around gender role identity development and think it is safe to say that the way the gender identity development is dependent on social, biological, interpersonal and intrapersonal factors. Some children may be more stimulated in term of activities by their parents and therefore may take all their gender role information from them, others may use other people as models. However as we have seen, gender role identity development cannot be based on just when theory but rather a combination of them would make sense. Development of Gender 10 References Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676-713. Cantrell, P. J., & Ellis, J. B. (1991). Gender role and risk patterns for eating disorders in men and women. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 53-57. Ding, S., & Littleton, K. (2005). Children’s personal and social development. UK: Blackwell. Fogel, A. (2001). Infancy: Infant, Family and Society (4th ed.). USA: Wadsworth. Shaffer, R. D. (2001). Developmental psychology (6th ed.). USA: Wadsworth. Waack, J. L., Gerrity, D. A., Taub, D. B., & Baldo, T. D. (2001). Gender, gender role identity, and type of relationship as predictors of relationship behaviour and beliefs in college students. Journal of College Counselling, 4, 32-48. Weinraub, M., Clemens, L. P., Sockloff, A., Ethridge, T., Graceley, E., & Myers, B. (1984). The development of sex role stereotypes in the third year: Relationship to gender lebelling, gender identity, sex-typed toy preference, and family characteristics. Child Development, 55, 1493-1503.