Maureen McClung November 18, 2005 Persistence of the Grinnellian Niche The concept of the ecological niche is one that has been developed and disputed repeatedly by many scientists over the last century (see reviews in Schoener 1989; Griesemer 1992). Even the identity of the first person to use the term “niche” as it relates to ecology is unresolved. However, most authors agree that it was Joseph Grinnell (1917b) who popularized its use in his paper entitled “The niche-relationships of the California thrasher”. Since his application of the term “niche” to describe an organism’s place in nature, the concept has undergone several major shifts towards more mathematical and operational definitions. With these transitions have come theoretical difficulties, which have caused some (James et al 1984) to call for a return to the basic concept put forth by Grinnell. In this paper, I will review the history of the biological niche concept, discuss its present status, and identify current issues to be addressed. In his 1917 paper on the California thrasher, Toxostoma redivivum, Grinnell attributed the severely restricted range of this bird to physiological and psychological adjustments to a narrow range of environmental conditions. He described the range as mostly warm, southerly-facing hill slopes in chaparral habitat. Environmental factors like temperature and humidity seemed to be the most significant determinants of range. However, he also identified predator avoidance, nesting, and feeding behavior as important reasons for the thrasher’s dependence on chaparral habitat, thus this concept was not purely spatial. Grinnell (1917a) believed this association was “distinctive enough to be included among the characters of the species described along with its habitat and the features of its bodily structure.” In subsequent works, Grinnell (1928, cited in Schoener 1989) developed his concept of the niche to be the “ultimate unit in the general association occupied by each single species”. In 1927, Elton devoted a section of his book, Animal Ecology, to his version of the niche concept. He thought that the niche of an animal referred to its place in the biotic environment, particularly its relation to food and enemies. He later defined the niche as meaning the “mode of life”, especially the mode of feeding of an animal (Elton 1933). This concept was different from Grinnell’s because it placed more emphasis on trophic relations than on habitat. For example, birds of prey occupy a niche for eating mice and rabbits across habitats. In a forest this niche is filled by the tawny owl and in a grassland it is filled by the kestrel (Elton 1927). This example also illustrates how Elton thought that different animals could occupy the same niche. For Grinnell (1917a), the niche was a characteristic specific to a species. Elton’s niche concept is thought to have marked a transition from a focus on a species’ place in nature to the role of a species in a community (Griesemer 1992). The next big leap in the evolution of the niche concept was made by Hutchinson, who developed the theory of the niche into a geometric definition of the occupant in terms of environmental dimensions. In 1958, he introduced the n-dimensional hypervolume, which is a theoretical space defined by points that correspond to states of the environment which would permit a species to exist indefinitely. This theory inextricably linked niche theory to the competitive exclusion principle, which states that no two organisms occupying the same niche (i.e. complete competitors) can coexist indefinitely (Krebs 1978). Hutchinson thought that the range of niche dimensions (e.g. temperature, food size, etc.) determined the fundamental niche of a species, but then interactions such as competition limited species so that they occur in a smaller realized niche. The concept of a realized niche explained how two species could have similar fundamental niches and still coexist. Hutchinson’s redefinition shifted thinking of a niche as an attribute of the environment to an attribute of the population or species in relation to its environment (Colwell 1992). The final major development in the concept of the niche was the interpretation of dimensions of niche space as actual resource utilization (Levins 1966, 1968; MacArthur and Levins 1967). This final theory is sometimes called “modern niche theory” (Schoener, 1989). Where Hutchinson’s hypervolume succeeded in theory, it failed in practice. The main difficulty was how to empirically determine all the dimensional states permitting a species to survive (Schoener 1989). To simplify this problem, the modern niche theory took resource dimensions one at a time and located the organisms on the dimension in terms of utilization rather than fitness (Cooper 1995). Levins (1968) thought a good niche theory should ask questions about determinants of niche breadth, the limits to similarity among coexisting species, resource partitioning, and evolution. This modern theory of the niche had more theoretical fecundity than Hutchinson’s in that it produced generalizations beyond the competitive exclusion principle. For example, MacArthur and Levin’s (1967) theory of limiting similarity addressed issues like limits to species number, degree of invasibility, and niche shifts. Their theory suggested that there is a universal limit to the similarity in niche dimensions that two sympatric species can exhibit and still coexist. Although this conclusion has been heavily criticized (Abrams 1983), the general messages from the early models still seem valid. First, there are likely to be some limits to similarity in coexisting competitors. Second, these limits suggest more than just differences in species, but also the variability within them, the nature of the resource and utilization curves, etc. (Begon et al 2006). Currently, the most common definition of “niche” coincides with the occupantbased niches described by Hutchinson (1957) and MacArthur and Levins (1967). These theories have generated many questions that tend to be geared towards understanding resource distributions, utilization curves, and mechanisms underlying exploitative competition. A review of the many recent studies aimed at exploring the complexity of niche relations is beyond the scope of this paper, however a few examples will provide a sample of current work both in the lab and the field. In order to explore how niche breadth responds to competition, Bolnick (2001) presented populations of cadmium-intolerant Drosophila melanogaster with both cadmium-free and cadmium-laced media. He found that populations experiencing high competition (high density) were more likely to develop cadmium-tolerant flies after several generations since there was less competition for cadmium-laced media. This experiment demonstrates that as competition increases, organisms may experience pressure to expand their niche breadth. This study is an example of how the modern niche theory, which emphasizes looking at resource dimensions one at a time, can provide evidence for competitive effects on niche determination. Harmon et al’s (2005) study on convergence of morphological traits of lizards in the genus Anolis is another example of how niche dynamics are studied today. The anole radiation is a classic case of convergent evolution in which different species have evolved similar ecomorphs in allopatry. Through morphological measurements taken from different species on different islands, they found that Caribbean anole lizards show multidimensional convergence in phenotypic characteristics. Single dimension convergence has been widely reported, but this study is unique in contributing support to a multidimensional response in phenotype, as would be expected by Hutchinson. These types of studies are valuable in furthering understanding of the modern niche theory, however they operate under the assumptions made by Hutchinson’s model which are subject to several problems. Hutchinson (1958) himself identified the pitfalls in his theory when he first introduced the n-dimensional hypervolume. His theory assumes the niche to be equally optimal in all areas, however it is inevitably suboptimal near borders. It also assumes that all environmental variables can be linearly ordered, which is not possible. The model also refers to a single instant in time, which is challenged, for example, when considering nocturnal and diurnal competitors. Only a few species are considered at a time, which means the abstraction makes little difference to the whole community. Finally, Hutchinson’s model attributes differences in species to present or past interspecific interactions a priori. James et al (1984) suggested reverting to the original Grinnellian niche, with some adjustments, in order to avoid unrealistic assumptions. For example, they studied the niche of the wood thrush, Hylocichla mustelina, by employing multiple comparisons that focused on the geographical ecology of a single species. James et al suggested beginning with a single species approach before broadening to studies of assemblages. Perhaps by combining both the “habitat-based” niche model of Grinnell and the “occupant-based” models of Hutchinson and Levins, the details that define niche dynamics can be better understood and, of course, generate more questions for research. References Abrams, P. A. 1983. The theory of limiting similarity. Annual review of ecology and systematics 14: 359-377. Begon, M., C. R. Townsend, and J. L. Harper. 2006. Ecology: from individuals to ecosystems. Blackwell Publishing, Malden, Massachusetts, USA. Bolnick, D. I. 2001. Intraspecific competition favours niche width expansion in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 410: 463-466. Colwell, R. K. 1992. Niche: a bifurcation in the conceptual lineage of the term. Pages 241-248 in E. Fox-Keller and E. A. Lloyd, editors. Keywords in evolutionary biology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA. Cooper, G. 1998. Generalizations in ecology: a philosophical taxonomy. Biology and Philosophy 13: 555-586. Elton, C. 1927. Animal ecology. Sidgewick and Jackson, London, UK. Elton, C. 1933. The ecology of animals. Methuen, London, UK. Griesemer, J. R. 1992. Niche: historical perspectives. Pages 231-240 in E. Fox-Keller and E. A. Lloyd, editors. Keywords in evolutionary biology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA. Grinnell, J. 1917a. Field tests and theories concerning distributional control. American Naturalist 51: 115-128. Grinnell, J. 1917b. The niche-relationships of the California thrasher. Auk 34: 427-433. Grinnell, J. 1928. Presence and absence of animals. University of California Chronicle 30: 429-450. Harmon, L. J., J. J. Kilbe, J. M. Cheverud, and J. B. Losos. 2005. Convergence and the multidimensional niche. Evolution 59: 409-421. Hutchinson, G.E. 1957. Concluding remarks. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology 22: 415-427. Krebs, C. J. 1978. Ecology: the experimental analysis of distribution and abundance. Harper and Row, New York, USA. Levins, R. 1966. The strategy of model building in population biology. American Scientist 54: 421-431. Levins, R. 1968. Evolution in changing environments. Princeton University press, Princeton, Connecticut, USA. MacArthur, R.H. and R. Levins. 1967. The limiting similarity, convergence, and divergence of coexisting species. American Naturalist 101: 377-385. James, F. C., R. F. Johnston, N. O. Wamer, G. I. Niemi, and W. J. Boecklen. 1984. The Grinnellian niche of the wood thrush. American Naturalist 124: 17-47. Schoener, T.W. 1989. The ecological niche. Pages 79-113 in J. M. Cherret, editor. Ecological concepts. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, UK.