Autonomic and Situational Determinants of the Subjective Experience of Emotion: An individual difference approach Alexander Genov Research Division, State Farm Insurance Companies, Bloomington, IL, USA (formerly at Clark University, Worcester, MA) Short Title: Autonomic and Situational Determinants of Emotions Please address correspondence to: Alex Genov Usability Laboratory, Strategic Resources State Farm Insurance Companies Three State Farm Plaza Bloomington, IL 61791-0001 Tel: (309) 766-7746 Fax: (309) 753-1903 E-mail: alex.genov.js1v@statefarm.com Some hold that autonomic activity is essential to emotions. Others insist that feelings can be produced if individuals merely believe that their autonomic nervous system has been activated. Both views are supported by data possibly because people differ in the origins of their emotional experience. Some identify their emotions from behavioral cues (“personal”) like autonomic activity, while others from situational cues (“situational”). Individuals were classified into personal and situational. It was demonstrated that these two types were affected differently by the combination of actual and believed autonomic activation. When highly aroused and believing they were highly aroused, personal responders discounted fear, while situational responders augmented it. Results relate facial expressions, physiological activity, and feelings, and support Laird’s individual difference caveat. What kind of an emotion of fear would be left, if the feelings neither of quickened heart-beats nor of shallow breathing, neither of trembling lips nor of weakened limbs, neither of goose-flesh nor of visceral stirrings, were present, it is quite impossible to think. -- William James (1884, pp. 193-4) Most authorities agree that the autonomic nervous system (ANS) plays some role in determining the subjective experience of emotion, or the feeling. There is disagreement, however, on the exact nature of this role. Some hold that the interoception and interpretation of actual ANS activity is essential to emotional experience (James, 1884; Schachter, 1964). Others insist that emotional feelings can be produced without ANS activation if individuals merely believe that their autonomic system has been activated (Valins, 1966). Both views are supported by extensive empirical literature. The present paper integrates these two positions within a larger framework, namely an individual difference theory (Laird, 1974). The premise is that there is evidence for both views because people differ in the origins of their emotional experience: some behave according to the first view, others according to the second. Sympathetic activation is a necessary component of emotional experience and behavior The view that sympathetic activation is necessary for emotional experience and behavior was a central component of James’ theory of emotions. Evidence supporting James’ position on the role of bodily factors in emotions first came from the work of Marañon (1924) who injected naive subjects with epinephrine and then asked them to describe their subjective experiences (cited in Cornelius, 1996). Most subjects reported 3 experiencing a state like an emotion but devoid of genuine subjective quality. Some, however, reported experiencing strong feelings such as intense anxiety and sadness. Those same subjects also reported co-occurring emotional cognitions, e.g. the memory of a deceased relative. Marañon’s results thus highlighted the role of cognitive processes in emotional experience. Later on, Schachter (1964) developed this hypothesis incorporating sympathetic activation (SA) and emotion-related cognitions into the “two-factor” theory of emotion. The basic assumption of Schachter’s theory is that emotional states are determined by the interaction of cognitive factors with a state of undifferentiated SA. To test this proposition, Schachter & Singer (1962) manipulated subjects’ state of SA, their awareness of the source of SA, and the context from which explanations about emotional states can be derived. Results indicated that within the epinephrine-injected (high SA) condition, the situational context influenced subjects’ emotional states only if they had no adequate explanation for their state of high SA. This provided support for Schachter’s claim on the role of cognitions regarding the sources of arousal. Results on the role of arousal itself were more ambiguous. It was expected that placebo (low SA) subjects would report less intense emotional experience and would behave less emotionally than all epinephrine-injected subjects. Contrary to expectations, placebo subjects were consistently more “emotional” than the epinephrine-injected informed subjects. Despite, or maybe due to, the slight mismatch between expectations and results, Schachter’s experiment gave rise to a voluminous body of research evaluating the role of SA in emotions (for reviews, see Reizensien, 1983; Ross & Olson, 1981). The placebo 4 group results stimulated the development of a major paradigm in social-psychological research on emotion, namely misattribution of sympathetic activation. In the case of the placebo group, the SA naturally produced by invasive medical procedures, such as injections, was interpreted to have affected subjects’ emotional state. On that interpretation, placebo subjects were actually in the same position as the epinephrineinjected ignorant subjects. That is, they were aroused without having an adequate explanation for the SA. In the epinephrine-injected informed group, on the other hand, both natural SA and that produced by the epinephrine were presumably “explained away” as a result of the information about the symptoms, leading to a reduced subjective experience. This interpretation has been empirically well supported (for review, see Ross & Olson, 1981). Another line of research has followed the interpretation of results for Schachter’s epinephrine-injected condition. It is known as the excitation-transfer paradigm (Zillmann, 1971). According to Zillmann (1971), “if some stimuli produce ... slowly decaying excitation, this excitation will be carried over into subsequent, and possibly entirely unrelated experiences. ” Here it is assumed that people do not, or cannot, distinguish the excitation accompanying different emotions and that the transferred autonomic excitation may trigger another emotion if the organism moves to a different situation. One way to produce SA available for transfer is through physical exercise. Such SA has been shown to affect feelings of anger and aggressive behavior, sexual excitation, helping giving, feelings of elation, reactions to advertisements, and interpersonal attraction (for review, see Zillmann, 1996). 5 A major assumption of Zillmann’s work is that conscious awareness of the sources of sympathetic excitation or arousal would prevent arousal transfer. Foster, Witcher, Campbell, and Green (1998) have questioned this assumption based on results of a metaanalysis on the role of arousal in attraction. Foster et al. claim that arousal has an “automatic effect on attraction that is independent of factors associated with the attribution of arousal, such as arousal source ambiguity.” According to them, following the automatic emotional judgment stage, “individuals may become aware that their attraction to the target was influenced by their arousal level. Such individuals may adjust their attraction judgment …” (p. 95). Thus, it may be the case that awareness of the sources of SA do not completely remove the excitations transfer effects but only reduce them for some people. In summary, various lines of emotion research provide evidence for the claim that SA is causally involved in emotional experience and behavior. Research has shown that a state of elevated diffuse sympathetic activation determines the intensity of emotional experience. In line with James’ theory, this evidence shows that SA, if not necessary, is at least sufficient in determining subjective experiences of emotion. Next, we turn to a view, which downplays the role of actual SA in emotions, stating that it is neither necessary nor sufficient in determining emotional experience. SA is not a necessary component of emotional experience and behavior Virtually dismissing the role of the physiological component of Schachter’s theory, Valins (1966) suggests that a person need not perceive his or her actual SA in order to 6 experience emotion. The mere belief that such SA has occurred, regardless of its veridicality, is claimed to be sufficient. To test this proposition, Valins (1966) conducted an experiment in which he manipulated participants’ belief that their heart rate changed in response to viewing slides of seminude females and measured their “liking” for the slides. Results showed that subjects who heard a marked change in what they believed to be their heart rate, found the photographs they were viewing during the change as more attractive than did subjects in the control group. This effect was stable over a two-month period. Parkinson (1985) considers two lines of criticism directed toward Valins’ method. One deals with possible demand characteristics (Parkinson, 1985; Beck, Gibson, Elliot, Simmons, Matteson, & McDaniel, 1988). Specifically, by making participants aware of important aspects of the research question, i.e. the relationship between physiological reactions and feelings, Valins may have inadvertently led participants to give answers in accordance with the hypothesis. The second criticism concerns Valins’ theoretical dismissal of the role of actual SA. This has led Valins (1966; Valins & Ray, 1967) to omit any measurement of actual physiological indexes such as heart rate (Goldstein, Fink, & Mettee, 1972). Despite these criticisms, data from false physiological feedback studies suggest that emotional experience and behavior can be influenced by the mere belief that SA has occurred, regardless of the actual physiological state of individuals. In fact, the false autonomic feedback paradigm has been widely used and the basic results have been replicated repeatedly (Traux, 1984). These results seemingly contradict claims regarding 7 the essential role of actual SA in emotions. Resolving this apparent contradiction using an individual difference approach, is the goal of this investigation. Individual differences in cue response The role of individual differences in emotional experience and behavior has been explored in the past (e.g., Valins, 1967; Fenigstein & Carver, 1978). A research program that has addressed this issue very systematically is that pursued by Laird and his collaborators (Laird, 1974; Laird & Crosby, 1974; Laird & Berglas, 1975; Duncan & Laird, 1980; Wixon & Laird, 1980; Bresler & Laird, 1981). The basic premise of Laird’s research is a self-perception one (Bem, 1972), namely that we have no privileged access to our internal states but instead are socialized into the labeling them early in life. According to Bem, “individuals come to ‘know’ their own attitudes, emotions, and other internal states partially by inferring them from observations of their own overt behavior and/or the circumstances in which this behavior occurs” (Bem, 1972, p.2). This self-perception assertion has received substantial empirical support (Bem, 1964, 1965, 1966; Bandler, Madaras, & Bem, 1968; for a review, see Bem, 1972). Taking the position that our internal states are generated from the perception of our behavior and the situation, Laird has identified individual differences in the way people use these two kinds of information (1974; see Laird & Bresler, 1992 for review). These types of cues have been called personal and situational, respectively (Laird & Berglas, 1975). According to Laird’s theory, these two kinds of cues are not rivals. Rather, “it appears that everyone uses situational cues and that the differences lie entirely in whether people also use personal cues (Kellerman, Lewis, & Laird, 1989).” 8 To examine this hypothesis empirically, Laird has applied the following basic strategy: in a well-disguised manner, he manipulates a behavior (e.g. a facial muscle configuration) presumed to express a feeling and then asks subjects to report on their subjective experience. Previous research has shown repeatedly that, when asked to rate their emotional feelings after the disguised facial expression manipulation, some people are influenced by their facial expression (personal cue responders) while others are influenced by cues from the situation such as the titles of abstract pictures (situational cue responders). That is, if personal cue responders assume the facial configuration of a frown while looking at a picture called “Spring,” they will feel more angry than happy. In contrast, if situational cue responders make a frowning face while looking at the same “happy” picture, they will feel more happy than angry. These individual differences have been established most frequently in research on the effects of facial expressions and postures. In addition, subjects whose feelings are more affected by their emotional facial expressions, have been shown to exhibit coherent behavioral patterns that would be predicted by self-perception theory. For example, individuals more responsive to the facial expression manipulation, are more responsive to the manipulation of their posture (Duclos et al., 1989), are more empathetically correct in identifying the emotions of others when they imitate their expressions (Wixon & Laird, 1981), change their attitudes more in the induced compliance procedure (Duncan & Laird, 1977; Rodewalt & Comer, 1979), conform less (Comer, 1975), are more sensitive to pain and tolerate less of it in the Cold Pressor Test (Genov et al., unpublished manuscript), and so on. 9 A study by Duncan and Laird (1980) examined at the relationship between the effects of expressive behavior and of SA in a misattribution context. The facial expression manipulation was used to divide a group of mildly snake-phobic subjects into those more responsive to personal cues and those more responsive to situational cues. Then, everybody was given a placebo pill. Half of participants were told that the pill would calm them and half were told that it would arouse them. Finally, all subjects were asked to approach and touch a live snake. Results were concordant with the predictions of selfperception theory and with data on individual differences in the use of the two kinds of cues. Subjects who were responsive to situational cues showed a positive placebo effect: when they were led to believe the pill would calm them, they reported feeling less fear and showed less avoidance behavior. When they believed that the pill would arouse them, they felt more fear and showed more avoidance behavior. For Situational cue responders, taking a “calming” or an “arousing” pill had defined the situation as calming or arousing and this perception of the situation translated into emotional feelings and behavior. In contrast, subjects who were more responsive to personal cues exhibited a reverse placebo effect: if they were led to believe that the pill would relax them, they felt more afraid. If they believed that the pill would arouse them, they felt less afraid. It can be argued that situational cue responders in Duncan and Laird’s (1980) study were, in essence, showing Valins-type effects because they were acting on the belief that they were either calm or aroused, irrespective of the veridicality of this belief. Personal cue responders, on the other hand, exhibited effects similar to those produced by Schachter’s injection manipulation. That is, they paid attention to their actual level of SA and 10 interpreted any changes in it in light of the available cognitive explanation, i.e. expectations about the effect of the pill. It should be stressed that in Duncan and Laird’s (1980) study SA was neither manipulated directly nor measured. Instead, it was assumed that there was naturally occurring fear-related SA, which could be attributed to a neutral source (the pill). Thus, the question of whether actual SA affects personal cue responders more remains unanswered. This was the focus of the present study. The present experiment As noted above, Valins’ work on the false autonomic feedback effect has been criticized on the basis of possible demand characteristics and the lack of measures of actual SA. The last criticism is especially potent since Valins claims to be evaluating the role of the actual SA component of Schachter’s two-factor theory. Schachter and Singer’s (1962) cornerstone study on the role of autonomic and cognitive activity has also received its dose of scrutiny and criticism. Raising some serious objections to the original methodology, a study by Mezzacappa, Katkin, and Palmer (1999) conceptually replicated Schachter and Singer’s epinephrine-ignorant3 condition with several important modifications. First, Mezzacappa et al. measured SA continuously using more reliable and valid indexes, i.e. pre-ejection period, heart rate, and skin conductance. Second, following the recommendations of Plutchik and Ax (1967) and Marshall and Zimbardo (1979), Mezzacappa et al. used a much stronger SA manipulation – injections of 11 epinephrine titrated to subjects’ body weight. Third, the replication included three emotion-producing conditions: “amusement”, “anger” and “fear”. Fourth, the stimulus material consisted of film clips whose effectiveness in producing amusement, anger, and fear has been well documented (Gross & Levenson, 1995). The results of interest indicated that participants in the epinephrine condition were indeed more autonomically aroused than control participants and reported stronger feelings of fear. From the three target emotions (i.e. fear, anger, and amusement), fear may be the most closely related to epinephrineinduced SA. One of the conclusions was that “individual differences in sensitivity to these effects have yet to be closely examined” (pp.197-8). The present study incorporated many of the methodological modifications of Mezzacappa et al. and used an individual difference approach to evaluate the relative role of SA and the belief of SA in emotions. Many studies have shown that manipulations of real SA increase emotional feelings. On the other hand, there is evidence that the mere belief that autonomic changes have taken place can influence subjects’ emotional experience and behavior. The resolution of this apparent dilemma may come from Laird’s work on individual differences. It may be the case that while everybody uses situational cues extensively, some people rely more heavily on personal cues (including SA) in their emotional behavior. Valins’ feedback manipulation can be seen as substituting in some ways essentially personal cues (i.e., the interoception of one’s heartbeat) with situational ones (i.e., the auditory perception of what one believes to be his or her heartbeat). This is so because in everyday life, auditory perception usually provides information about the 3 Mezzacappa et al. (1999) state that “all subjects were deceived about what symptoms the drug would elicit” 12 situation. Thus, in terms of individual differences, Valins’ false feedback manipulation can be seen as measuring people’s sensitivity to situational cues while Schachter’s epinephrine injection manipulation measures individuals’ sensitivity to personal cues. In this sense, both Schachter and Valins may be correct. Since in most of the research groups of subjects have been compared, the effects of actual SA may have occurred only in part of the group. Thus, it may simply be the case that the different effects are observed with different people. The present study was designed to test this hypothesis directly in a factorial experiment. More specifically, we expected that an increase in actual SA, as indexed by an increase in heart rate and skin conductance, will lead to an increase in self-reported feelings of fear for personal cue responders but not for situational cue responders. Personal cue responders have been shown to be more affected by bodily changes in their emotional experience (see Laird & Bresler, 1992 for review). We expected that the belief of increased SA would lead to an increase in self-reported feelings of fear for both situational and personal cue responders. This is because situational cues, unlike personal or bodily cues, have been shown to affect both personal and situational cue responders (see Laird & Bresler, 1992 for review). Method Participants 31 male and 39 female (N = 70) Clark University undergraduates and graduate students 18 to 37 years of age (M = 20.3) volunteered to participate in the study. (p. 184), not specifying whether they kept subjects ignorant or whether they misinformed them. 13 Apparatus and Materials Disguised Facial Expression Manipulation (DFEM): Materials for the DFEM, used to classify participants into personal or situational, consisted of four black-and-white abstract pictures consisting of random patterns of lines. Each picture had a title in the bottom left hand corner (Betrayal, Rip-off, Spring, and Dancing, respectively). An emotionrating visual analogue scale adapted from Plutchik (1980) was used to quantify the strength of the two target emotions: happiness and anger. The rest of the emotions featured on the scale as distracters included: sadness, fear, disgust, anxiety, surprise, and interest. The assignment of participant to each of the two cue response categories (i.e., “Personal” and “Situational”) followed the method employed in a number of previous studies (e.g., Duclos et al, 1989; Flack et al, 1999; Wilcox & Laird, 2000; Duclos & Laird, in press). The procedure involved a smile trial with two smiles (smile 1 and 2) and a frown trial (with frown 1 and 2). The “happy” score for frown 1 was subtracted from the “happy” score for smile 1, and the “angry” score for smile 1 was subtracted from the angry score for frown 1, and these two differences were added. A positive number was obtained if the person was happier when smiling and angrier when frowning. This procedure was repeated for the second pair of smile and frown trial. Participants who had positive scores on both pairs of trials (i.e., had reported feelings consistent with their facial expressions consistently across the two pairs of trials) were assigned to the Personal Cue group, while all others were assigned to Situational Cue group. Emotional stimuli: A preliminary study (see Appendix 1) was conducted to select and pre-test the film clips to be used as stimulus material. We could not use previously tested 14 material (e.g., Gross & Levenson, 1995) because we needed four film clips that are as similar as possible to use in a within-subject experiment. Results indicated that the film clips did not differ from one another in their effect on HR and did not produce a significant increase in HR. On the other hand, the clips did result in a significant increase in SCL above baseline but again did not differ from one another in SCL. If SCL is taken as an index of emotional response (e.g., Dawson, Schell, & Filion, 1990), it can be claimed that the film clips effectively elicited emotional reactions without significantly changing HR, which was used as an index of manipulated SA in the main study. Physical Exercise: A Fitron stationary bicycle was used to induce an increase in HR and SCL. Belief of SA: A Matrix MR-700 quartz metronome was used to manipulate participants’ belief of either high or low SA, as indexed by HR. Measure of subjective emotional experience: An emotion rating scale similar to the one used in the DFEM was used to quantify the intensity of 12 emotions felt by participants while watching the film clips. In addition to the target “scared”, “anxious”, and “tense”, the emotion labels included: “relaxed”, “angry”,” happy”, “sad”, “depressed”, “upset”, “confused”, “irritable”, and “sleepy”. In this measure, participants are asked to indicate how strongly they feel a certain emotion at the moment, by placing a mark on a six-inch line. Only the end points of the line are labeled as “do not feel at all” and “feel very strongly” respectively. Scoring consisted of measuring the distance, in quarter inch increments, from the "Do not feel at all" end of the line to the mark placed by the participant. The possible response range was 0 to 16. Since the scale was used four times 15 throughout the study (i.e., after each film clip), in order to encourage independent judgments, four different versions of the scale were prepared in which the order of the emotion labels varied. Supplementary Materials: Four scales from the Myers-Briggs (Briggs & Myers, 1976) personality inventory were given to participants at four times during the procedure, one before each film clip. The scale was introduced in the procedure in order to occupy participants’ attention during the time needed to reach the best excitation-transfer state. This has been found to be the period when a person is actually aroused but thinks that he is not; or does not attribute SA to the exercise (Cantor et al., 1975; Cacioppo et al., 1987). Participants were also asked an open-ended “body awareness” question twice during the procedure – before the film clip in each low-SA rest condition. It asked participants how they felt “right now”. It served the same purpose as the personality scales. Funnel Questionnaire: At the end, participants were asked to fill out a funnel questionnaire consisting of increasingly specific questions about their understanding of the purposes of the experiment. The purpose of the funnel questionnaire was to identify participants who had become aware of the actual hypothesis of the study. Physiological measures: Participants were seated in a comfortable, slightly reclined armchair. Physiological activity was measured using J & J I-330 modules and Unicomp software from the American Biotech Company. A 486 IBM- compatible computer was programmed to process physiological data from each participant by averaging them into 10second intervals. Heart rate (HR) was measured using the most common, non-invasive, and inexpensive method, namely with a photoplethysmograph (Papillo & Shapiro, 1990). The 16 photoplethismograph attached to the thumb of the non-dominant hand. The computer program averaged heartbeats over 10-second intervals. Skin conductance level (SCL) was recorded using constant voltage (0.166 VDC) passed between two reusable silver-silver chloride sensors (calibration = +/- 3%; range 0-50 micromohs). The sensors were placed on the volar surfaces of the medial phalanges of the first and third fingers of the non-dominant hand. The electrolite used was Signa electrode gel (conductivity not less than 40,000 micromohs; impedance 2.5 ohms or less). Procedure 1. Overall arrangement and cover story The basic elements of the procedure were as follows: (1) baseline physiological readings; (2) the Disguised Facial Expression Manipulation intended to classify participants into Personal and Situation cue responders; (3) four trials representing all combinations of high and low actual SA and belief about SA. In each trial participants watched a film clip and then described the co-occurring feelings; (4) the measurement of feelings of fear in response to four film clips. Because we needed the participants to be unaware of the true nature of the experiment, these elements had to be presented as part of a bogus study “on the effects of various physical and cognitive tasks on brain activity.” The four elements of the experiment were presented to participants as: (1) baseline physiological readings; (2) a task measuring the effects of facial muscle activity on memory for abstract pictures; (3) a task measuring the effect of physical activity and film clips on various physiological indexes; (4) controlling for the extraneous effects of random changes in mood. 17 2. Baseline physiological readings Decoy EEG electrodes were attached to participants’ foreheads with tape, and actual photoplethysmograph and skin conductance sensors were placed on the non-dominant hand. Following the recommendations of Jennings, Berg, Hutcheson, Obrist, Porges, and Turpin (1981), one-minute baseline HR and SCL measures were taken. It is suitable for the photoplethysmograph method of HR recording (Jennings et al., 1981) and has been used in related procedures (Mezzacappa, Katkin, & Palmer, 1999). 3. Disguised Facial Expression Manipulation Next, participants underwent the DFEM intended to classify them into those more responsive to personal and those more responsive to situational cues. The task was introduced to participants as one on the effect of cranial muscle activity on memory for abstract pictures and EEG activity. First, on a picture from Gray’s anatomy, participants were shown the facial muscles presumably implicated in the task, namely the ones at the corner of the mouth and the ones above the eyebrows. Then participants were given the following instructions: “One thing to keep in mind is that we all have moment-to-moment random fluctuations in mood that we don’t pay much attention to. To control for any extraneous effects of such fluctuations, it is important that you notice your mood and report it after every trial. This will be done using brief emotion rating scales.” The procedure that followed, included four randomized trials, two of which include “frown” manipulations and two, “smile” manipulations. During the two frown manipulations, participants are instructed to pull their eyebrows down and together, and while doing this to clench their 18 teeth, holding this configuration for 20 seconds. At the same time, they viewed one or the other of two abstract pictures entitled “Dancing” and “Spring” (one in each frown trial). During the two smile manipulations, participants are instructed to open their mouth very slightly, and to contract the muscles at its corners by drawing them up and back, holding this configuration for 20 seconds. During these trials, the two abstract pictures were entitled “Betrayal” and “Rip-off” (one in each smile trial). The titles of the pictures (which portray highly abstract/nonsense geometrical forms) were deliberately chosen to clash with the facial expression in each trial. That is, the meaning of the title (e.g., Spring) is intended to provide a situational cue for a positive emotion (e.g., happiness). The concurrent facial expression (e.g., a frown), on the other hand, is intended to provide a personal behavioral cue for a negative emotion (e.g., anger). After each trial, participants are asked to rate their feelings using the Emotion Rating Scale. Finally, participants were shown a fifth abstract picture, this time without a title, and were asked to indicate if they had seen it among the four pictures of the task. This was part of the cover story. 4. Belief of SA manipulation During the initial instructions, we told participants the following: “One technical problem we’ve been having with the equipment is that the video input to the computer produces a sound artifact in the computer program, so your pulse rate will result in click from the hard drive. These clicks won’t sound very much like heart rate because the computer averages the beats, so they’ll sound a bit mechanical. Don’t pay attention to the 19 clicks. We have tried to get rid of them but couldn’t figure out how.” These instructions were part of the cover story designed to convince participants that the metronome clicks they were going to hear corresponded to their own heart rate. 5. Manipulation of actual SA and the belief of SA For the purpose of counterbalancing, half of the participants were exposed to conditions of high and low actual SA in the following order: high-low-high-low. The other half of the participants were exposed to actual SA in the following order: low-high-lowhigh. These two orders were selected to enable us to use the excitation transfer procedure in the context of a within-subjects experimental design. That is, each participant was exposed to two high SA and two low SA conditions in the same experiment. a. High-low-high-low actual SA order After the DFEM, participants in the high-low-high-low condition were asked to go to the exercise bike and to start peddling vigorously. After about a minute, they were asked to pause briefly and to hold the hand, to which the sensors were attached, as still as possible. This was done to minimize the movement artifact inherent in the photoplethysmograph method of heart rate measurement. The experimenter noted participants’ current pulse rate and if it was at or above 165% of their individual baseline, asked them to step down, take a seat in the chair and relax. If participants’ heart rate was less than 165 % of baseline, they were asked to continue peddling for a bit longer. This increase in HR is based on Cantor et al. (1975) and Cacioppo et al. (1987) who used similar levels. After participants sat back in the chair, they were asked to fill out the first 20 personality scale, which took them on the average of 5 minutes. Next, when participants’ HR went down to 140% of their individual baseline level, or 5 minutes after they stopped peddling, they were given the following instructions: “The next part of the study involves viewing a short film clip. The clips in the study have been chosen with respect to some formal properties, about which I will tell you at the end of the session. Again, to control for any extraneous effects of random fluctuations in mood, I will ask you to fill out a brief emotion rating scale at the end of each clip. For that purpose, please pay attention to any slight changes in your mood.” Next, the experimenter surreptitiously placed a metronome next to the computer and started it in an unobtrusive way at the exact same time as the VCR, using a remote control. Half of participants in the high-low-high-low actual SA condition heard clicks they believed to be their own HR at a “high” 112 beats-per-minute. The other half heard clicks at a “low” 72 beats-per-minute. At the end of the clip participants filled out Emotion Rating Scale #1. Next participants in this condition were asked to fill out the second personality scale and to answer the open-ended “body awareness” question. When participants’ HR reached baseline level, or after 9 minutes after ending the exercise, they were shown the second film clip. During the clip, participants heard either “fast” (112 bpm) or “slow” (72 bpm) metronome clicks. At the end of the clip, participants filled out Emotion Rating Scale #2. Next, the above two steps were repeated with a counterbalanced presentation of the metronome clicks with participants filling out Emotion Rating Scales 3 and 4 at the end of film clips three and four, respectively. b. Low-high-low-high actual SA order 21 The only difference in the procedure between conditions a. and b. was that participants in the low-high-low-high SA condition started the sequence of high and low SA by watching film clip #1 while their HR was at baseline. After the DFEM, participants in this condition were asked to fill out the first personality scale and the body awareness question. Next, they were shown the first film clip and at the same time heard either “fast” or “slow” metronome clicks. At the end of the clip, participants filled out Emotion Rating Scale #1. All the instructions and the steps in the procedure were the same as those in condition a. except for order of presentation. Finally, participants were debriefed. Experimental Design The study employed a mixed 2 x 2 x 2 factorial design: (cue response) x actual SA x believed SA, respectively. The between-subjects (blocking) factors were Cue response (personal vs. situational) and Order of condition (1, 2, 3, 4). The latter was included to control for a possible extraneous effect of condition order. The within-subject factors were actual SA (high vs. low) and the belief of SA (high vs. low). Every participant was exposed to all four conditions: 1) high actual SA / high believed SA; 2) high actual / low believed; 3) low actual / high believed; and 4) low actual / low believed, presented in counterbalanced order. Counterbalancing was done with respect to false feedback (4 sequences: low-low-high-high; low-high-high-low; high-high-low-low; high-low-low-high) and actual SA (2 sequences: high-low-high-low; low-high-low-high). The resulting combinations made a total of 4 between-group cells. Each participant saw the four film clips in the same order: 1, 2, 3, and 4. However, as a result of the counterbalancing 22 described above, each film clip appeared equally frequently in all four possible combinations of actual SA and the belief of SA. Results 1. Funnel Questionnaire. Based on their responses to the funnel questionnaire, 20 participants were excluded from the self-report analyses because they had indicated awareness of the experimental hypothesis, namely the effect of SA on emotional experience. A roughly equal number of personal (N = 9) and situational (N = 11) cue responders were excluded4. 2. Facial expression manipulation The DFEM resulted in classifying 31 participants in the “Personal cue response” category, and 39 participants in the “Situational cue response” category. The distribution of type of cue response is consistent with that found in previous research (e.g., Duclos et al., 1989; Duncan & Laird, 1980). All the analyses were done with the 22 unaware Personal cue responders and the 28 unaware Situational cue responders. 3. Manipulation check for actual physiological activation The experimental design included two high SA conditions in which all participants were asked to exercise, and two low actual SA conditions in which all participants filled out 4 When the self-report analyses were done with all 70 participants, the pattern of results remained the same. However, in the latter analyses the level of significance decreased from p <.01 to p < .05 and the effect size decreased from a large ² of .14 to a medium ² of .06 (Cohen, 1988). The drop in the strength of results after including the “aware” participants justifies our decision to present results only with the “unaware” 23 questionnaires and rested, presented in counterbalanced order. To check for the effectiveness of exercise in producing an increase in HR and SCL, these two measures were averaged over the two exercise conditions, 240 seconds in length each. HR and SCL were also averaged over the two no-exercise conditions, again 240 seconds in length each. Next, two paired-samples t tests were performed. Mean HR in the exercise conditions (M = 95.49, SD = 15.69) was significantly higher than mean HR in the no exercise conditions (M = 87.23, SD = 13.89), t (48) = 8.83, p < .001 (two-tailed). Mean SCL in the exercise conditions (M = 16.07, SD = 9.36) was significantly higher that mean SCL in the no exercise conditions (M = 14.33, SD = 8.52), t (48) = 2.73, p < .01 (two-tailed). 4. Subjective experience of fear The fear score was a composite of the scores for “afraid”, “anxious”, and “tense”. This technique was used in order to capture the subjective experience of fear/suspense more fully. Previous research has found that films rated as scary elicited self-reports of “tenseness” as well as “fear” (Gross & Levenson, 1995; Philippot, 1993). Before combining these scores into a composite index, a correlational analysis revealed that the three sets of scores are highly significantly correlated across all four film clips. Correlations ranged between .43 and .73, all being significant at the .01 level (two-tailed). An overall 4-way ANOVA was performed to test for main effects of and the interaction among two between subjects factors, i.e. cue response and film clip order. No main effects were found for cue response, F (1, 42) = .03, or for film clip order, F (3, 42)= .31. There was no significant interaction as well. participants. 24 Film clip order was excluded from the analysis and the effects of the within subjects factors, i.e. actual SA and believed SA, crossed with type of cue response, were tested using a 3-way ANOVA. Results show that there were no significant main effects for any of the three variables. Overall, personal cue responders did not differ from situational cue responders in their report of fear. In addition, believed SA and actual SA taken separately did not result in differential fear ratings. However, there was a highly significant (actual SA) x (believed SA) x (type of cue response) interaction, F (1, 48) = 7.63, p < .01, ² = .14, observed power = .77. Graphically, the results are represented in Figure 1. These results indicate that when participants were highly aroused, high believed SA had a discounting effect for personal cue responders and a augmenting effect for situational cue responders. In other words, when personal cue responders were actually highly aroused and heard fast clicks indicating high SA, they reported less fear than when they heard slower clicks indicating less SA (i. e., slower HR). On the other hand, when situational cue responders were actually highly aroused and heard faster clicks, they reported more fear. This effect was not observed in the low actual SA condition. To test for the significance of the effect of believed SA, two mixed 2-way ANOVAs were performed, one for the high and one for the low actual SA conditions. Type of cue response was the between-subjects factor and actual SA was the within-subjects factor. Within the high actual SA condition there was a significant (type of cue response) x (believed SA) interaction, F (1, 48) = 8.14, p < .01, ² = .15. No significant effects were found within the low actual SA condition, F (1, 48) = 1.58, p = .21. 25 Discussion The observed results, characterized by a three-way interaction between actual SA, believed SA, and cue response, paint a picture more complex than expected. For personal cue responders, the combination of high actual and low believed SA increased the experience of fear, but when the high actual SA was combined with the belief of high SA, there was a marked discounting effect. That is, when personal cue responders’ HR and SCL were elevated and, at the same time, they heard fast clicks presumably reflecting their quickened HR, these individuals reported less fear than when they were aroused and heard slower clicks. In contrast to personal cue responders, the same combination between high actual and high believed SA led situational cue responders to augment their experience of fear compared to the low believed SA condition. An explanation for this pattern of results is provided by Foster et al’s (1998) twostage theory of arousal and attraction, combined with Laird’s emotional cue response theory. The basic idea of Foster et al.’s model is that initially SA automatically influences a person’s emotional experience. This influence is independent of the source to which the person attributes the SA. Subsequently, emotional experience can change based on whether the person attributes his or her SA to the target stimulus or to a neutral source. We suggest that the personal cue responders in our study, being more attuned to their bodily states, used the fast metronome clicks to correctly attribute their high actual SA to the exercise, i.e. to a neutral source. As a result, they discounted their subjective 26 experience in a fashion predicted by Foster et al. (1998) and Zillmann (1996). According to Zillmann, “the awareness of residual excitation, ... , prevents transfer effects” (p. 258). Such an interpretation is consistent with previous research findings. For example, Nisbett and Schachter (1966) manipulated subjects’ expectation of the severity of electric shocks and their attribution of the source of the SA naturally produced by electric shocks. Subjects in the low severity expectation condition, who were led to attribute their SA to a placebo pill, reported less pain and tolerated more intense electric shocks than subject who attributed their SA to the shock itself. In effect, these subjects behaved like the personal cue responders in this experiment who might have attributed their SA to the exercise and discounted their subjective experience of fear. Personal cue responders in the present experiment also behaved like the personal cue responders in Duncan and Laird’s (1988) study. When placed in a fear-producing situation and given a placebo pill, Duncan and Laird’s personal cue responders exhibited the so-called reverse placebo effect. That is, when they believed that the pill would calm them they felt more fear. When they believed that the pill would arouse them, they felt less fear. This latter discounting effect is what we observed with Personal cue responders in the present study. In contrast, for situational cue responders in our study the fast clicks provided a strong situational cue of SA, which may have prevented the discounting effect and even created an augmenting one. As a result, situational cue responders felt more fear and exhibited the so-called Valins effect. This is what we had expected based on the results for Duncan and Laird’s (1980) situational cue responders. When the letter were placed in a fear-producing situation and given a placebo pill, they exhibited the classic placebo effect. 27 That is, when they believed that the pill would calm them they felt less fear. When they believed that the pill would arouse them, they felt more fear. The above results and their interpretation fit our general theory that both personal and situational cue responders would be influenced by situational cues, and that actual SA would play an additional role in the personal cue group. What we had not expected was the discounting effect in the latter group. One way to look at this is to suggest that personal cue responders integrated information from their bodily reactions and situational information in generating feelings, an effect demonstrated by numerous arousal misattribution studies. We showed that in contrast to personal cue responders, situational cue responders rely primarily on cues from the environment. Our experiment provided such cues in the form of fast or slow metronome clicks indicating fast of slow HR. Analysis of variance showed that when situational cue responders believed they were highly aroused (i.e., heard faster clicks), they felt more fear, irrespective of their actual level of SA. In summary, our model of emotional experience presents a promising way of integrating elements of James’ theory, Schachter’s theory, Valins’ position, and Laird’s individual difference approach. The model was generally supported with the exception of the discounting effect observed for personal cue responders. Although unexpected, this effect is consistent with the theoretical framework. Implications for Emotion Theory and Everyday Life Investigators have identified individual variation in the extent to which people can 28 correctly perceive their autonomic reactions (e.g., Katkin, 1985) or the extent to which there is within-individual correlation between autonomic reactions and self-reported subjective experience (Lang, Greenwald, Bradley, & Hamm, 1993). The particular contribution of the present study was to test empirically an important factor which has been somewhat underinvestigated in the field of emotion research, namely individual differences in how people’s subjective experiences arise, and what they are about. This study tested the hypothesis that some individuals’ feelings are more affected by their bodily reactions (including their actual level of SA) while those of other individuals may be more affected by situational factors, such as their belief of SA. In our experiment, individuals were classified into those more affected by bodily cues and those more affected by situational cues, based on the extent to which their feelings were affected by their facial expressions of emotion or by cues from the situation. Then it was shown that these two types of individuals were affected differently by the combination of actual SA and the belief of SA. There was no difference in the effect of believed SA on participants’ feelings when participants were not highly aroused. However, when they were highly aroused and believed they were highly aroused, individuals more responsive to personal cues discounted their experience of fear. In the same condition, individuals more responsive to situational cues augmented their experience of fear. Two important questions related to the individual difference construct tested in the present study remain to be answered. One concerns the mechanism through which the emotional experience of personal cue responders is affected. Do personal cue responders simply produce more bodily cues (e.g., physiological reactions, expressive behavior, etc.)? 29 Are they more sensitive to such cues? Or is it both? Follow-up studies will explore further any physiological differences between personal and situational cue responders and will investigate directly the possibility that personal cue responders are better at detecting changes in their physiology and are more attentive to bodily cues. The second question concerns situational cue responders’ sensitivity to situational cues. Future studies will look more closely at the nature of situational cues and the factors affecting their strength and the mechanisms through which they affect emotional experience and behavior. The results of the present study relate to everyday life in important ways. Many medical and psychiatric treatments are directed at changing patients’ SA levels. For example, to combat anxiety SA levels are controlled by administering beta blockers. One important implication of our results is that this method would work most effectively only for individuals responsive to personal or bodily cues. Individuals responsive to situational cues, on the other hand, would benefit more from placebo treatments. Another use of beta blockers has been in heart disease treatments. Any possible emotional side effects may be underestimated because, as our results suggest, they would occur only for personal cue responders. Finally, our theory has been applied to social problems such as eating disorders, post-traumatic stress disorders and other mental health issues, such as PMS. In conclusion, the current project was directed at understanding the nature and genesis of emotional feelings. Such understanding can be later translated into the practical arena of emotional and behavioral self-control, behavioral medicine, and clinical practice. 30 References Bandler, R.J., Madaras, G.R., & Bem, D.J. (1968). Self-observation as a source of pain perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 205-209. Beck, R. C., Gibson, C., Elliot, W., Simmons, C., Matteson, N., & McDaniel, L. (1988). False physiological feedback and emotion: Experimenter demands and salience effects. Motivation and Emotion, 12 (3), 217-236. Bem, D. (1972). Self-perception theory. In Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6). New York: Academic Press. Bem, D. J. (1964). An experimental analysis of beliefs and attitudes. Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan. Bem, D. J. (1965). An experimental analysis of self-persuasion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1, 199-218. Bem, D. J. (1966). Inducing belief in false confession. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 707-710. Bresler, C., & Laird, J. D. (1983). Short-term stability and discriminant validity of the "self-situational" cue dimension. Paper presented at Eastern Psychological Association Meeting, Philadelphia, April 1983. Briggs, K. C., & Myers, I. B. (1976). Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., & Crites, S. L. (1996). Social neuroscience: Principles of psychophysiological SA and response. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic mechanisms and processes. New York: Guilford. Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., Stonebraker, T. B., & Petty, R. E. (1987). Self-report and cardiovascular measures of SA: Fractionation during residual SA. Biological Psychology, 25, 135-151. Cannon, W. B. (1929). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, rear, and rage. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Cantor, J. R., Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1975). Enhancement of experienced sexual SA in response to erotic stimuli through misattribution of unrelated residual excitation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol.32, No.1, 69-75. Cornelius, R. R. (1996). The science of emotions: Research and tradition in the psychology of emotions. Prentice Hall. Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Filion, D. L. (1990). The electrodermal system. In J. Cacioppo and L. Tassinary (Eds.), Principles of psychophysiology: Physical, social, and inferential elements. New York: Cambridge University Press. Duclos, S., & Laird, J. D. (in press). The deliberate control of emotional experience through the control of expressions. Duclos, S., Laird, J. D., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., Stern, L, & Van Lighten, O. (1989). Categorical vs. dimensional effects of facial expressions on emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 100-108. 31 Duncan, J. W., & Laird, J. D. (1980). Positive and reverse placebo effects as a function of differences in cues used in self-perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1024-1036. Fenigstein, A., & Carver, C. S. (1978). Self-focusing effects of heart rate feedback. . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1241-1250. Flack, W. F., J. D. Laird, et al. (1999). Additive effects of facial expressions and postures on emotional feelings. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 203-217. Foster, C. A., Witcher, B. S., Campbell, K., & Green, J. D. (1998). SA and attraction: Evidence for automatic and controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 (1), 86-101. Goldstein, D., Fink, D., & Mettee, D. R. (1972). Cognition of SA and actual SA as determinants of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 41-51. Greenwald, M. K., Cook III, E. W., & Lang, P. J. (1989). Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 51-64. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion, 9 (1), 87-108. James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188-205. Jennings, J. R., Berg, W. K., Hutcheson, J. S., Obrist, P., Porges, S., & Turpin, G. (1981). Publication guidelines for heart rate studies in man. Psychophysiology, 18 (3), 226-231. Katkin, E. S. (1985). Blood, sweat, and tears: Individual differences in autonomic selfperception. Psychophysiology, 22 (2), 125-137. Kellerman, J., Lewis, J., & Laird, J. D. (1989). Looking and loving: The effect of mutual gaze on feelings of romantic love. Journal of Research in Personality, 23, 145161. Laird, J. D. (1974). Self-attribution of emotions: The effects of expressive behavior on the quality of emotional experience. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 33, 475-486. Laird, J. D., & Berglas, S. (1975). Individual differences in the self-attribution of emotion. Journal of Personality, 43, 286-304. Laird, J. D. & Bresler, C. (1990). William James and the mechanisms of emotional experience. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 636-651. Laird, J. D., & Crosby, M. (1974). Individual differences in the self-attribution of emotion. In H. London & R. Nisbett (Eds.), Thinking and feeling: The cognitive alteration of feeling states. Chicago: Aldine. Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261-273. Marañon, G. (1924). Contribution à l’étude de l’action émotive de l’adrénaline. Revue Française d’Endocrinologie, 2, 301-325. Mezzacappa, E. S., Katkin, E.S., & Palmer, S. N. (1999). Epinephrine, SA, and emotion: A new look at the two-factor theory. Cognition and Emotion, 13 (2), 181-199. 32 Nisbett, R. E., & Schachter, S. (1966). Cognitive manipulation of pain. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 227-236. Papillo, J. F., & Shapiro, D. (1990). The cardiovascular system. In J. Cacioppo and L. Tassinary (Eds.), Principles of psychophysiology: Physical, social, and inferential elements. New York: Cambridge University Press. Parkinson, B. (1985). Emotional effects of false autonomic feedback. Psychological Bulletin, 98 (3), 471-494. Philippot, P. (1993). Inducing and assessing differentiated emotion-feeling states in the laboratory. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 171-193. Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience: Vol.1, Theories of emotion (pp.3-33). New York: Academic Press. Ross, M., & Olson, J. M. (1981). An expectancy-attribution model of the effects of placebos. Psychological Review, 88, 408-437. Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. New York: Norton Schachter, S. (1964). The interaction of cognitive and physiological determinants of emotional state. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.1). New York: Academic Press. Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379-399. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan. Traux, S. R. (1984). Determinants of emotion attributions: A unifying view. Motivation and Emotion, 8 (1), 33-54. Valins, S. (1966). Cognitive effects of false heart-rate feedback. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4 (4), 400-408. Valins, S. (1967). Emotionality and autonomic reactivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6 (4), 458-463. Valins, S., & Ray, A. A. (1967). Effects of cognitive desensitization on avoidance behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7 (4), 345-350 Wilcox, K., & Laird, J. D. (2000). The impact of media images of super-slender women on women’s self-esteem. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 278-286 Wixon, D. R., & Laird, J. D. (1981). Effects of mimicry on the judgment of facial expressions in others. Paper presented at the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston. Zillmann, D. (1971). Excitation transfer in communication-mediated aggressive behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 419-434. Zillmann, D. (1996). Sequential dependencies in emotional experience and behavior. In R. D. Kavanaugh, B. Zimmerbegh, & S. Fein (Eds.), Emotion: Interdisciplinary perspectives. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 33 Figure 1 Results for the Subjective Experience of Fear Presented by Level of Believed SA High Actual Arousal 20 18 16 14 12 Fear Ratings 10 8 6 4 2 0 15.39 11.5 High 14.36 12.57 Personal Situational Low Believed Arousal Low Actual Arousal 20 18 16 14 12 Fear Ratings 10 8 6 4 2 0 14.68 13.04 13.89 12.64 Personal Situational High Low Believed Arousal 34 Appendix 1 Six 3-minute clips were edited frame-by-frame from the films Pet Sematary I (Mary Lambert, Director, 1989) and Pet Sematary II (Mary Lambert, Director, 1992). The editing involved the selection of meaningful 3-minute-long scenes and the removal of any startling or especially gory moments. Six male and four female Clark University undergraduate and graduate student volunteers watched the film clips and rated several emotional feelings associated with them on a visual analogue scale adapted from Plutchik (1980). The emotion labels included “scared”, “anxious”, and “tense”, “angry”, “sad”, “happy”, “confused”, “depressed”, “irritable”, “relaxed”, “sleepy”, and “upset”. Participants’ physiological reactions to the film clips were recorded using the apparatus described in the procedure of the main study. Each participant watched all six film clips in randomized order. The procedure involved the following steps. First, participants read and signed the informed consent form. Next, one-minute SCL and HR baseline readings were recorded. Following that, participants watched Clip 1. Skin conductance level (SCL) and heart rate (HR) were recorded for the duration of the clip. Next, participants filled out an Emotion Rating Scale indicating the intensity with which they experienced several feelings as a result of watching the film clip. This procedure was repeated for each clip. Results indicate that none of the six film clips produced a change in HR above baseline level, F (6, 42) = 1.23, p = .31. In contrast participants’ SCL during the films clips increased significantly as compared to baseline, F (6, 42) = 6.25, p < .001. Helmert contrasts revealed that the six clips did not differ from one another in HR or SCL. We computed a composite score by adding up the scores for “afraid”, “anxious”, and “tense”. To check for the statistical appropriateness of such a decision, participants’ selfreport of fear, anxiety, and tension were correlated for each film clip. Sixteen5 out of the total of 18 correlations were substantial and statistically significant, with Pearson r ranging from .70 to .92. These results indicate that it was appropriate to combine the scores for “scared”, “anxious”, and “tense” into a composite index, which we called “fear”. An initial repeated measures ANOVA revealed that participants reported statistically comparable levels of fear across all six film clips, F (5, 45) = 1.75, p = .143. A visual inspection of the means suggested that clips two and three yielded somewhat higher scores than the other four clips. They were removed from the analysis. A second ANOVA on the remaining four clips confirmed and strengthened the finding that clips one, four, five, and six produced the same subjective experience of “fear” in participants, F (3, 27) = .61, p = .61. The correlations between self-reports of “scared” and “anxious” for clip 2 was .42, n. s.. The correlations between self-reports of “scared” and “tense” for clip 5 was .33, n. s.. 5 35