1 Cross-cultural Argument Interactions Between Israeli-Jews and Palestinians By Don Ellis and Ifat Maoz In : Journal of Applied Communication Research, 30, 181-194 (2002). There are numerous cultural and political gaps that create distance and tension between groups. Such gaps are particularly wide when groups from different cultures argue. The study of argument has a long history in philosophy (Toulmin, 1988), rhetoric (Perelman, 1982), communication (Meyers, 1989), and discourse studies (Van Eemeren, Grootendorst, & Henkemans, 1996). And more contemporary communication perspectives have examined argument in contexts such as interpersonal relations (Canary & Sillars, 1992) and organizational contexts (Garrett & Meyers, 1991), among others. All of these studies point to different approaches to the production and interpretation of arguments. In this paper we test predictions about arguments between groups in political conflict (Israeli-Jews and Palestinians) from two theoretical perspectives, namely, a cultural communication perspective and theories of majority/minority relations. The intercultural context is one arena of inquiry that has received relatively little attention with respect to 2 argument. There is certainly tremendous interest in intercultural communication in general, and in the various discourse strategies and "speechways" of different groups (cf. Hymes, 1974). Moreover, the ways in which the communication patterns that characterize different cultural groups promote miscommunication and stereotypes has been of particular interest (e.g. Orbe, 1998). But studies that examine actual argument exchanges between cultural groups, Arabs and Israeli-Jews in particular, are rare. There are studies that describe the argument styles and strategies of single cultures. Hatim (1991), for example, studied argumentation as a text type and found that Arabic as a language lacked a particular form of counter-argumentation, and this produced a communicative deficiency that had pragmatic implications. The Jewish-Israeli preference for direct argument has been noted by Shiffrin (1984) and Katriel (1986). Different cultural patterns of argument lead to miscommunication, stereotypes, confusions, breaches of decorum, and face threats. Johnstone (1986) suggested that strategies of logical argumentation and persuasive style attributable to Arab and western differences were responsible for an abusive and failed interview with Iran's Ayatollah Khomeini. Other scholars (e.g. Lippi-Green, 1997) 3 explain how numerous linguistic variations between cultures lead to discrimination and miscommunication in intercultural encounters. Such situations typically exacerbate tensions between different cultures especially when one is powerful and the other is not. There is very little research that examines crosscultural argument interactions between cultures engaged in extreme macro-political conflict. This study addresses this deficiency in the research by analyzing the argument differences between Israeli-Jews and Palestinians. These are two cultures engaged in extreme conflict over history, politics, culture, and territorial resources. The context of investigation was reconciliation-aimed grass-roots dialogue groups of Israeli-Jews and Palestinians that attempted to create an environment for the two groups to communicate and learn more about each other. We begin with a closer look and review of the cultural communication styles that characterize the two groups. Jewish-Israeli and Arab cultural communication patterns Modern Arab and Jewish-Israeli cultures have each emerged from the special circumstances of its history. There is some documentation as to the different 4 communicative norms that have resulted from these histories (cf. Katriel, 1986; Feghali, 1997). These contrasting norms of communication make for difficult and uncoordinated communication. This is particularly true for the role of argument as a pragmatic feature of each culture. Several researchers describe the Arab interactional ethos of musayra.1 Katriel (1986) and Griefat and Katriel (1989) explain how musayra shapes Arab communication patterns. Musayra means "to accommodate" or "to go along with" and is a communication pattern that orients the speakers toward harmonious social relations, indirection, and a concern for face saving for the addressee. Musayra emerges from the core values of Arab culture that have to with honor, hospitality, and collectivism. There are four essential communication features of musayra (see Feghali, 1997). The first is repetition which is discourse marked by reiterations of conventional oaths and formulas (Gilsenan, 1983). Repetitiveness is a remnant of an oral tradition which relied on repetition as a memory aid and facilitated understanding. Repetition in musayra is used primarily for complimenting and praising others, especially in asymmetrical status relations. Interestingly, repetition is also related to Arab argumentative style where repeated phrases and words move belief rather that 5 Western style logic (Johnstone Koch, 1983). The second is indirectness, which is quite consistent with typical collectivist and high context cultures. Indirectness is an interactional strategy that reflects the cultural tendency to be interpersonally cautious and responsive to the context. It facilitates politeness and face saving. Elaboration is a third feature of musayra and refers to an expressive and encompassing style. Elaboration leads to a deeper connection with the other speaker and helps to affirm the social positions of the speakers. The final characteristic is affectiveness or the intuitive-affective style of emotional appeal. Again, affectiveness allows for identification with the other and the maintenance of positive face. In sharp contrast are the cultural communication patterns of Israeli-Jews. Their speech is direct, pragmatic, and places an emphasis on assertiveness. This sort of speech is termed dugri ("straight talk") and is well documented and described by Katriel (1986). JewishIsraeli dugri speech is the opposite of musayra. It is direct, explicit, and clear. It is simple and to the point with ideas and logic foregrouned and affective and emotional appeals backgrounded. Dugri represents a dominant Jewish-Israeli identity that emerged in reaction to 6 historical oppression and the Diaspora experience of Jews (Bar-On, 1999). Dugri is the interactional code that projects this identity into interaction. Face and interpersonal considerateness in dugri are not determined by the communication in a given situation, but realized in other culture-specific ideas of strength, integrity, and the ability to perform dugri. A general pattern of dugri-like communication is apparent in other research that focuses on Jewish oral and conversational styles (Tannen, 1984; Schiffrin, 1984; Zupnik, 2000). This pattern--which is direct, assertive, polyvocal, and peppered with numerous interruptions and turn exchanges--is consistent with the dugri concern for directness and the speaker's identity and face saving rather than the addressee's. Cultural communication patterns and context. Dugri and musayra have been studied in a variety of contexts and been shown to be descriptive of Jewish and Arab cultures respectively. Nevertheless, context shapes interactions and can be expected to influence communication styles (Coover & Murphy, 2000). Speakers may choose to insert and express these speech styles, which are characteristic of their culture, in one context but not another. Minorities, for example, intensely claim their identity in some situations 7 and weakly claim it in others (Ethier & Deaux, 1994). Thus, given cultural changes and particular situations it is unreasonable to assume that dugri and musayra will always be the preferred communication style. Zupnik (2000) reports from ethnographic informants that Arab speakers will employ musayra in situations of self interest, and when less powerful speakers are communicating with the socially more powerful. Also, there are occasions when the more powerful use musayra toward the less powerful; for instance, a man toward a women, a healthy person toward a sick person, and when a powerful person is behaving magnanimously. Musayra is increasingly generational and subject to contextual constraints. The same is true for Jewish-Israeli dugri speech. As the construction of the dominant “Sabre” (native JewishIsraeli) identity gives way to other more complex representations and constructions of identity (Bar-On, 1999), so does its interactional projection. Dugri is expected to give way to other more complex and diverse forms of communicating and relating to others. Israeli-Jews make jokes about "doing dugri" or "being dugri" in a conversation or with an individual, thus belying the notion that it is a determinate style of communication. The dugri cultural code softens and hardens as situations dictate. 8 One context of communication for Israeli-Jews and Palestinians that has received very little research attention has been the context of politically focused dialogues between the two sides. Several studies have examined effects of interaction in dialogic encounters between Israeli-Jews and Palestinians (Bar and Bargal, 1995; Bargal, 1990; Katz and Kahanov 1990, Maoz, in press). However, scarce research attention has been given to systematic analysis of communication patterns that are manifested within these dialogues (for exceptions see BarOn 1999; Zupnik, 2000), and no research concentrates specifically on styles of arguments employed by each side in political discussions. It is difficult to predict the extent to which dugri and musayra communication styles will be operational in the context of political dialogues between Israeli-Jews and Palestinians. This question is particularly interesting given the macro political context of Jewish-Palestinian relations these dialogues are embedded in. This is a context in which the sides clearly see themselves as adversaries and as members of national groups in conflict. The nature of this political context may be thus more predictive of argument than cultural communication styles. 9 Argument is a highly characteristic communicative exchange in dialogues concerned with political conflict. Both sides use argument in an effort to present themselves as just and right to the other side. Thus, musayra and dugri become potentially important predictors of how each national group argues. Below we briefly describe arguments in groups and then posit two theories that lead to contrasting predictions about group argument. One approach is based on cultural communication differences (dugri and musayra), the other is political in nature and derived from majority/minority power relations theory. Argument in small groups The research on conversational argument and group decision-making (cf Brashers, Adkins, & Meyers, 1994; Meyers, 1997) describes the character and structure of argument in small groups. Meyers et al. (1991) found in their study of the distribution of argument acts in groupdiscussions that argument was mainly characterized by Assertions, Elaborations, and Agreement (p. 60). In another study of the distribution of argument acts among male and female group participants, Meyers et al. (1997), found that 10 women tended to be more cooperative and demonstrate more connection in their argument than man. The present study uses a multi-stage argument coding procedure offered by Meyers and Brahsers (1995; 1998). This scheme includes first order categories (Arguables [Generative Mechanisms and Reasoning Activities], Convergence-Seeking Activities, Disagreement-Relevant Intrusions, and Delimitors). Each of the above first order categories can be further classified into its respective second order category (e.g. assertions, propositions, elaborations, etc. see Table 1). The hypotheses presented in the following sections relate to argument acts defined within the above described classification scheme. Cultural communication style and argument Hubbard (1997) in her observations of grassroots Israeli-Palestinian dialogue groups, noted that argument was the most persistent characteristic of these groups. We might expect from the previous discussion that dugri speech would be characteristic of Israeli-Jews and the mode of speech preferred by them during argument since Israeli-Jews have a longer history of an argumentative style (Schiffrin, 1984). And musayra is not argument oriented at all. 11 Research has clearly identified that Jewish argument is dugri-like and quite consistent with the entire dugri code. Musayra is more bound up with politeness and addressee face than with argument. Spolsky and Walters (1985) explain how certain Jewish styles of worship and communication are reminiscent of the learning style in the yeshivas (schools of religious learning) that value equality and argument and refutation. The tendency for Jewish argument to reflect a linear process of western style logic is well understood (cf. Schiffrin, 1984). Interestingly, the little research that exists on Arab argument patterns is consistent with musayra. Hatim (1991) states clearly that argumentation in modern Arabic "is closely bound up with politeness, 'saving face' and other related pragmatic phenomena" (p. 189). Arab argument shows a preference for what is called "through argument" or the tendency to advocate a given stance and make no direct concessions to an adversary. Johnstone (1986) reported on an example of this in her article analyzing a journalist's clumsy interview with Iran's Ayatollah Khomeini. The interview collapsed because of the incompatibilities of the two cultures and how they expected each to argue. Given this line of literature, we pose the following hypotheses with respect to argument and 12 differences in communication style between Israeli-Jews and Palestinians. H1: For first order categories the pattern of argument for Israeli-Jews will be consistent with the dominant/ assertive tradition of dugri. That is, Israeli-Jews will produce more generative mechanisms, reasoning activities, and disagreements. They will also produce more of the second order categories that are consistent with these such as assertions, and elaborations. H2: For first order categories the pattern of argument for Palestinians will be consistent with the musayra accommodating communication style. That is, Palestinians will produce more convergence seeking and delimitors. They will also produce more of the second order categories that are consistent with this style such as frames and forestalling. The majority/minority power relations approach to argument The theory of majority/minority power relations and the dual processes of influence (Moscovici, 1985) is a 13 second strand of research that informs this investigation. The majority/minority approach becomes relevant when we take into consideration, apart from cultural differences, the macro political context of the relations between the sides, which is characterized by considerable power asymetry. Though the peace process has begun, the IsraeliPalestinian situation remains one of occupation and domination of the Palestinians, and of the territory they live in, by the Israeli state and army forces. The Israeli government has strategic and military control over major parts of the territories inhibited by Palestinians, has control over resources such as water supply and work permits in Israel for Palestinians, and restricts movement of Palestinians both inside the territories and from the territories to Israel. Majority/minority research shows that there is an expected influence of majorities in groups (in our case the Israeli-Jews) but also a subtle process of minority influence (in our case the Palestinians). Minorities can exert considerable influence on group conditions (Crano, 1994). They can alter the internal states of majorities when they argue and maintain a consistent position. Argument from minorities in groups performs the 14 communicative functions of educating, confronting, and gaining advantage. Meyers, Brashers, and Jerzaks' (in press) research on conversational argument and group decision making reports on data that pertain quite explicitly to the present investigation, and provide a theoretical foundation for making predictions about how the relationship between majorities and minorities should influence the argumentative processes between Israeli-Jews and Palestinians. Meyers et al. (in press) found that majorities agree more and disagree less. This is explained as producing repetitive agreement amongst themselves and creating an environment of unity that is very persuasive. Minorities, on the other hand, disagree more and tend to be defensive. Secondly, Meyers, Brashers, and Jerzak found that argumentative consistency was strongly related to success. And finally, they found that majorities and minorities simply argue differently with majorities converging and agreeing more, and minorities disagreeing, objecting, and reframing more. Based on this power relations' majority/minority approach to interaction, we posit the following hypotheses with regard to communication style and argument between Israeli-Jews and Palestinians. 15 H3: Israeli-Jews will engage in more convergence and less disagreement than will Palestinians for the first order categories. Palestinians will engage in more disagreement. H4: For the second order categories Israeli-Jews will engage in more agreement and Palestinians in more frames and objections METHOD Data and Participants The data were collected in the spring of 1998 in a series of workshops conducted in the framework of an Israeli-Palestinian joint project designed to promote coexistence and peace building between the sides.2 These workshops were held in the post-Oslo era during a period of the rule of the more hawkish Likud party in Israel. This period was marked by difficulties from both sides in implementing agreements and arrangements, and by stagnation in proceeding with the political process toward a final peace agreement. The workshops investigated here are similar to such other ongoing intergroup events that 16 foster communication between the two sides (Sulieman, 1997; Maoz 1997; in press). The workshops were organized within the framework of a peace education project aimed at reconciliation between the sides. In these workshops 15 and 16-year old youths from pairs of Jewish-Israeli and Palestinian schools met for two days to discuss social, cultural, and political issues. Each workshop included approximately 20-23 youth from each side. These were divided into smaller mixed groups of 5 to 8 Jewish-Israeli and 5 to 8 Palestinian youths. Each such mixed small group was led jointly by a Jewish-Israeli and a Palestinian facilitator. The data discussed here are based on transcriptions of four such meetings of mixed small recorded in three separate workshops. For each group the entire meeting was transcribed and excerpts are used in the current analysis. The data were recorded and transcribed by the second author. Moreover, workshop participants were interviewed after each meeting to clarify and deepen understandings of group members' impressions and experiences in the discussions. The group meetings were conducted in English that constitutes a sort of lingua franca for Israeli-Jews and for Palestinians. On occasion individuals chose to 17 speak in their native language and these were translated by the facilitators. Coding Procedures The coding scheme used to identify and track the argument process appears in Table 1. It is the Conversational Argument Coding Scheme developed by Canary, Meyers, Seibold, and colleagues (Canary, Brossmann, & Seibold, 1987; Meyers, Seibold, & Brahsers, 1991; Meyers & Brahsers, 1998). As Meyers and Brashers (1998) continue to point out, this coding scheme is reasonably complex and requires training and practice. They suggest a multi-stage process and describe this process in detail in Meyers and Brashers (1995). We followed this process and trained coders to use the system. We began by reading transcripts carefully and identifying different aspects of group arguments. Next, the data were parceled to separate arguments from nonarguments. Nonarguments were not used in this study. Each argument and its supporting categories were highlighted. Finally, all coders made sure they could identify the argument content of each statement. 18 Following this procedure, the coders applied the conversational Argument Coding Scheme. In accordance with Meyers and Brahsers (1995; 1998) the coders began by coding first order categories (Arguables [Generative Mechanisms and Reasoning Activities], Convergence-Seeking Activities, Disagreement-Relevant Intrusions, and Delimitors). Coders then returned to the transcripts and coded each of the above first order category into its respective second order category (e.g. assertions, propositions, elaborations, etc. see Table 1). Coder training. Four people were trained on the coding system. All of them completed the parceling task. Each coder was taught the system and studied the coding literature carefully (e.g. Meyers & Brashers, 1995). The coders spent about three months learning the system and perfecting their coding ability. They practiced on transcripts different from those used in this study, and discussed the process until all differences and confusions were clarified. When the coders reached sufficient agreement and skill with the coding system, they coded the transcripts used in this study. Intercoder reliability. Reliability was calculated using Cohen's kappa. The reliabilities for this study are considered very high. The index of reliability for 19 argument/nonargument was .83; it was .81 for first order categories, and .80 for second order categories. RESULTS There were 232 coded units of which 131 were produced by Palestinians and 101 by Israeli-Jews. The data were subject to discriminate analysis and analysis of variance (ANOVAs) using the argument codes (Table 1) as predictor variables to predict membership in a particular group: Jewish-Israeli or Palestinian. The data were converted to ratios in order to minimize the influence of unequal frequencies of different variables. This data strategy boosted the conservatism of the test and increased the confidence of significant results. Discriminate analyses were performed on both the first order and second order codes. The results of these analyses were used to confirm or disconfirm the hypotheses and thereby test whether cultural differences in communication style or majority/minority power relations best explained the resultant patterns of argument. The first discriminant analysis was performed on the first order variables and produced one significant function, 2 (5) = 15.05, p < .01. This function 20 differentiated Israeli-Jews from Palestinians. There were qualities, then, of argument that predicted membership in each of the ethnic groups. The correlations between the predictor variables and the discriminate function (Table 2) indicate that these discriminating qualities consist of a combination of delimitors, reasoning, and convergence. As indicated by the correlations in Table 2 delimitors, followed by reasoning and convergence, were the strongest discriminators. The ANOVA on the argument variables yielded significant main effects for the three highest correlations in Table 2 only. They were delimitors F(1,230) = 9.23, p < .01; reasoning F(1,230) = 4.01, p < .05; convergence F(1,230) = 3.97, p < .05. The means ratios in Table 3 indicated that Israeli-Jews engaged in significantly more convergence and delimitors, and Palestinians reasoned more. ________________________ Insert Table 2 ________________________ ________________________ Insert Table 3 ________________________ 21 The discriminant analysis performed on the second order variables also produced a single significant discriminate function, 2(12) = 20.18, p < .05. This function also differentiated between Palestinians and Israeli-Jews. The correlations between predictor variables and discriminate function (Table 4) indicate that frames, acknowledgments, and elaborations were the most significant predictors of group membership. The only significant univariate ANOVA tests were for these three variables. They were frames F(1,230) = 7.03, p < .01; acknowledgments F(1,230) = 3.97, p < .05; and elaborations F(1,230) = 3.83, p < .05. The mean ratios in Table 5 indicate that IsraeliJews engaged in more acknowledgments and frames, while the Palestinians elaborated more. ______________________ Insert Table 4 ______________________ ______________________ Insert Table 5 ______________________ 22 Taken together these results suggest an interesting pattern of argument that are more consistent with expectations derived from the majority/minority power relations approach (hypotheses 3 and 4) than with those derived from the intercultural difference approach to communication style (hypotheses 1 and 2). For the first order variables the canonical discriminant function for the group centroids was -.23 for the Palestinians, and .30 for the Israeli-Jews. Thus, the negative correlations in Table 2 (reasoning and disagreement) are most characteristic of the Palestinians, and the variables with positive correlations (delimitors and convergence) are most predictive of Israeli-Jews, even though some of these variables are statistically insignificant (disagreement and generative mechanisms). The centroids for the second order variables were -.27 for the Palestinians and .35 for the Israeli-Jews. The same pattern of directionality holds for Table 4. Therefore, Palestinians engage primarily in reasoning activities defined mainly as elaborations or statements that provide evidence, support, or reasons for their arguments. They also respond, challenge, and assert more but not in a statistically significant manner. Israeli-Jews, on the other hand, delimit as defined by framing statements that contextualize their arguments. They 23 also converge by acknowledging or indicating their comprehension of the others' point of view. DISCUSSION In this study we found that group argument patterns during political dialogues between Israeli-Jews and Palestinians were not consistent with predictions from cultural communication theory. A theory of cultural communication predicts that Israeli-Jews will employ more assertive dominant patterns of argument, which are consistent with the dugri ethos (Katriel, 1986). And Palestinians, consistent with the musayra ethos, would communicate in a manner more accomodating to the other, and less overtly aggressive or dominant (Griefat & Katriel, 1989). We also posed an alternative theory rooted in the minority/majority influence approach. This theory derives its assumptions more from the political environment than the cultural communication one, and poses the possibility of minority influence through a more assertive communication approach that facilitates minority interests. This study tested predictions from these two theoretical approaches in the context of political grassroots dialogue between the two sides. The results clearly indicate that argument patterns employed by both sides in these dialogues do not follow cultural 24 communication styles predictions. In fact, the results are the opposite and clearly supportive of expectations from the assumptions of majority/minority power relations. Palestinians manifested a more dominant and assertive argument style. They spoke more and engaged in more reasoning and elaboration. The Israeli-Jews were more submissive, accommodating, and generally hesitant. They used more delimitors, convergence, and frames. The findings in this study are consistent with a variety of other research programs and begins to formulate a clearer picture of how group communication works as an interplay between the political and the cultural. On the one hand, the results of this study are supportive of previous studies that illustrate how minority group members become especially dominant and influential when the issue discussed between groups represents the main points of dispute (Mugni & Perez, 1991). Maoz (in press) also found that Palestinians are more dominant—as defined by talking turns and introducing new topics (Blum-Kulka, 1998)—when the dialogues were centered on the main conflict between the two sides. Interestingly, the same study found that when the interaction was about generally non conflictual issues such as school life or joint educational work the Israeli-Jews were significantly more dominant than the Palestinians. The indigenous cultural style of each group is modified by the political context. There is a very 25 interesting and important power issue at work here. The attrition of musayra and dugri in the political context of grassroots peace initiatives suggests that these cultural communication styles are quite pliant and even reversable in some contexts. Even though there are considerable power differences between Israelis and Palestinians, aspects of this power are reversed at the micro communication level. The Palestinians are empowered by this context while the Israeli-Jews are left as defensive and hard pressed to defend themselves. This implies a new more symmetrical relationship between the two groups that has implications for egalitarianism and future interactions. This may be a context where the two groups can meet and work together on more equal grounds. Zupnik (2000) found that dugri and musayra changed from intergroup interactions to intragroup interactions. When Israeli-Jews were talking to Israeli-Jews dugri was the featured cultural style. The same was true for Palestinians and musayra. In mixed situaitons each group used its preferred cultural style more with one another than with members of the other group. But Zupnik (2000) and the data reported in this study suggests that cultural commmunication styles are modified during intergroup contact. It appears that reconciliation-based grassroots dialogues are a powerful context for ameliorating differences between these two groups. The Israeli-Jews avoid their aggressive style, and the Palestinians take 26 advantage of the opportunity to make assertions regarding their complaints, elaborate on these assertions, and argue when necessary. Maoz (in press), for example, has reported on how these dialogues are often uncomfortable for IsraeliJews because they are strongly challenged by Palestinians who use the opportunity to present a version of Palestinian suffering previously unfamiliar to the Israeli-Jews. They explain Palestinian suffering and the injustices causes by the conflict with the Israeli-Jews. The Israeli-Jews are overwhelmed and unprepared for this kind of exchange. The data in this study document more precisely the mechanics of this process. This study reports some of the first empirical evidence of the communication patterns between Israeli-Jews and Palestinians. In particular, it demonstrates the interplay between culture and argument and how each is a resource during political dialogue. Future research should investigate contexts other than political ones and how they may contribute to an environment of egalitarianism. Maoz (2000) suggests that other non political and non conflictual contexts of dialogue can elicit different patterns of communication between both sides in which the Jewish-Israeli majority group reassumes its dominant role. It would be interesting, then, to further investigate how such non politically-oriented dialogues influence argument behavior of both majority and minority group members. 27 Convergence seeking is a powerful influence on what happens in groups. Agreement and acknowledgement are the distinguishing characteristics of convergence seeking. Through convergence seeking the Israeli-Jews try to create a less conflictual atmosphere. This pattern of an IsraeliJewish tendency to lessen the conflict combined with a Palestinian tendency to use argumentative patterns to promote conflict, has important implications for planning future communication between these two national groups. Moreover, the present study underscores how both power and context must be a consideration. Understanding that cultural communication patterns may be affected by these two factors can help prepare groups for dialogue in the future. Such preparation will lessen the chances of miscommunication, misunderstanding, and frustration in dialogues designed to promote reconciliation between the sides. 28 TABLE 1 Conversational Argument Coding Scheme ___________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________ I. ARGUABLES A. Generative Mechanisms 1. Assertions: Statements of fact or opinion 2. Propositions: Statements that call for support, action, or conference on Argument-related statement. B. Reasoning Activities 3. Elaborations: Statements that support other statements by providing evidence, reasons, or other support. 4. Responses: Statements that defend arguables met with disagreement. 5. Amplifications: Statements that explain or expound upon other statements in order to establish the relevance of the argument through inference. 6. Justifications: Statements that offer validity of previous or upcoming statements by citing rule of logic (provide a standard whereby arguments are weighed). II. CONVERGENCE-SEEKING ACTIVITIES 7. Agreement: Statements that express agreement with another statement 8. Acknowledgment: Statements that indicate recognition and/or comprehension of another statement, but not necessarily agreement, to another's point. III. DISAGREEMENT-RELEVANT INTRUSIONS 9. Objections: Statements that deny the truth or accuracy of any arguable. 10. Challenges: Statements that offer problems or questions that must be solved if agreement is to be secured on an arguable. IV. DELIMITORS 11. Frames: Statements that provide a context for and/or qualify arguables. 12. Forestall/Secure: Statements that attempt to forestall refutation by securing common ground 29 13. V. Forestall/Remove: Statements that attempt to forestall refutation by removing possible objections NONARGUABLES--Statements not related to group argument 30 Table 2 Correlations Between First Order Predictor Variables and Discriminant Function Function 1 Delimitors .76 Reasoning -.50 Convergence .50 Disagreement -.13 Generative Mechanisms Codes .04 Table 3 Mean Ratios of First Order Variables Palestinians(n=) Israeli-Jews(n=) Generative .26 .27 Reasoning .43a .30b Convergence .01a .30b Disagreement .23 .20 Delimitors .06a .20b Ratios with different subscripts in the same row differ significantly from each other. P < .05 31 Table 4 Correlations Between Second Order Predictor Variables and Discriminant Function Function 1 Frames .56 Acknowledgement .43 Elaborations -.42 Forestall/Remove .35 Propositions .24 Forestall/Secure .23 Response -.20 Challenges -.17 Assertions -.06 Table 5 Mean Ratios of Second Order Variables Codes Palestinians(n= Israeli-Jews(n=) Frames .02a .10b Acknowledgement .01a .03b Elaborations .24a .12b Forestall/Remove .01 .03 Propositions .02 .05 Forestall/Secure .04 .08 Response .07 .03 Challenges .12 .09 Assertions .24 .22 Ratios with different subscripts in the same row differ significantly from each other. P < .05 32 REFERENCES Almaney, A.J., & Alwan, A.J. (1982). Communicating with the Arabs: A handbook for the business executive. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press. Bar, H., & Bargal, D., with the assistance of Asaqla,J.(1995). Living with conflict: Encounters between Jewish and Palestinian Israeli Youth. Jerusalem: The Jerusalem Institute for Israel studies. [In Hebrew]. Bargal, D.(1990). Contact is not enough - The contribution of Lewinian theory to inter-group workshops involving Palestinian and Jewish youth in Israel. International Journal of Group Tensions, 20: 179-192. Bar-on, D.(1999). The 'Others' Within Us: A Sociopsychological Perspective on Changes in Israeli Identity. Beer-Sheva: Ben-Gurion University [In Hebrew]. Brashers, D.E., Adkins, M., & Meyers, R.A. (1994). Argumentation in compter-mediated decision groups. In L. Frey (Ed.), Communication in context: Studies of naturalistic groups (pp. 262-283). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Blum-Kulka, S. (1997). Dinner talk: Culture Patterns of Sociability and Socialization in Family Discourse. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 33 Canary, D.J., Brossmann, B.G., & Seibold, D.R. (1987). Argument structures in decision-making groups. Southern Speech Communication Journal, 53, 18-37. Canary, D.J., & Sillars, A.L. (1992). Argument in satisfied and dissatisfied married couples. In W.L. Benoit, D. Hample, & P.J. Benoit (Eds.), Readings in argumentation (pp. 737-764). New York: Foris. Coover, G.E., & Murphy, S.T. (2000). The communicated self: Exploring the interaction between self and social context. Human Communication Research, 26, 125-147. Crano, W.D. (1994). Context, comparison, and change: Methodological and theoretical contributions to a theory of minority (and majority) influence. In S. Moscovici, A. Mucchi-Faina, & A. Maass (Eds.), Minority influence (pp. 17-46). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Ethier, K., & Deaux, K. (1994). Negotiating social identity when contexts change: Maintaining identification and responding to threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 243-251. Feghali, E. (1997). Arab cultural communication patterns. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 21, 345-378. Garrett, D.E., & Meyers, R.A. (1991). Interactive complaint communication: A reconceptualization of consumer 34 complaint behavior. In D.W. Parson (Ed.), Argument in controversy: Proceedings of the seventh SCA/AFA conference on argumentation (pp. 159-166). Annandale, VA: Speech Communication Association. Gilsenan, M. (1983). Recognizing Islam: Religion and society in the modern Arab world. New York, NY Pantheon Books. Griefat, Y., & Katriel, T. (1989). Life demands musayra: Communication and culture among Arabs in Israel. In S. Ting-Toomey & F. Korzenny (Eds.), Language, communication and culture (pp. 121-138). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hatim, B. (1991). The pragmatics of argumentation in Arabic: The rise and fall of a text type. Text, 11, 189199. Hymes, D. (1974). Foundations in sociolinguistics. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Hubbard, A.S. (1997). Face to face at arm's length: Conflict norms and extra-group relations in grassroots dialogue groups. Human Organizations, 56, 265-274. Johnstone, B. (1986). Arguments with Khomeini: Rhetorical situation and persuasive style in corss-cultural perspective. Text, 6, 171-187. 35 Johnstone Koch, B. (1983). Presentation as proof. The language of Arabic rhetoric. Anthropological Linguistics, 25, 47-60. Katriel, T. (1986). Talking straight: Dugri speech in Israeli sabra culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Katz, I. & Kahanov,M.(1990). A survey of dilemmas in moderating Jewish-Arab encounter groups in Israel. Megamot, 33: 29-47 [in Hebrew]. Lippi-Green, R. (1997). English with an accent. New York: Routledge. Maoz, I.(1997). A decade of structured educational encounters between Jews and Arabs in Israel', In D. S. Halperin (Ed.), To live together: Shaping new attitudes to peace through education (pp. 47-56). Geneva: Geneva University and Paris: UNESCO International Bureau of Education. Maoz, I.(in press). Power relations in inter-group encounters: A case study of Jewish-Arab encounters in Israel, International Journal of Intercultural Relations. Maoz, I.(2000). Multiple conflicts and competing agendas: A framework for conceptualizing structured encounters between groups in conflict – The case of a coexistence project between Jews and Palestinians in Israel. Journal of Peace Psychology, 6, 135-156. 36 Meyers, R.A. (1989). Persuasive arguments theory: A test of assumptions. Human Communication Research, 15, 357-381. Meyers, R.A., Seibold, D.R., & Brahsers, D. (1991). Argument in initial group decision-making discussions: Refinement of a coding scheme and a descriptive quantitative analysis. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 55, 47-68. Meyers, R.A. (1997). Social influence and argumentation. In L.R. Frey & J.K. Barge (Eds.), Managing group life: Communication in decision making groups (pp. 183-201). Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin. Meyers, R.A., & Brahsers, D.E. (1995). Multi-stage versus single-stage coding of small group argument: A preliminary comparative assessment. In S. Jackson (Ed.), Argumentation and values: Procedings of the ninth SCA/AFA conference on argumentation (pp. 93-100). Annandale, VA: Speech Communication Association. Meyers, R.A., & Brashers, D.E. (1998). Argument in group decision making: Explicating a process model and investigating the argument-outcome link. Communication Monographs, 65, 261-281. Meyers, R., Brashers, D.E., and Jerzak, J. (in press). Majority/minority influence: Identifying argumentative 37 patterns and predicting argument-outcome links. Journal of Communication. Moscovici, S. (1985). Social influence and conformity. In G. Lindzey, E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology, 3rd ed., (Vol. 2, pp. 347-412). New York: Academic Press. Mugny, G., & Perez, J.(1991). The Social Psychology of Minority Influence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Orbe, M.P. (1998). Constructing co-cultural theory: An explication of culture, power, and communication. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Perelman, Ch. (1982). The realm of rhetoric. Notre Dame/London: University of Notre Dame Press Schiffrin, D. (1984). Jewish argument as sociability. Language in Society, 13, 311-335. Spolsky, B., & Walters, J. (1985). Jewish styles of worship: A conversational analysis. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 56, 51-65. Suleiman, R. (1997). The planned encounter between Israeli Jews and Palestinians as a microcosm: A socialpsychological perspective. Iyunim Bechinuch, 1(2): 71-85 [In Hebrew]. Tannen, D. (1984). Conversational style: Analyzing talk among friends. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 38 Toulmin, S.E. (1988). The uses of argument (9th ed.). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Van Eemeren, F.H., & Grootendorst, R. Henkemans, F.S. (1996). Fundamentals of argumentation theory. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zupnik, Y,J. (2000). Conversational interruptions in Israeli-Palestinian 'dialogue' events. Discourse Studies, 2, 85-110. 39 Notes 1It is important to warn the reader about the perils of overgeneralizing and confusing the term "Arab." As Fegahli (1997) explains, it is a mistake to confuse "Middle East" and "Muslim" with Arab because not all Arabs are Muslim, and the geography of the Middle East is complex. There are non-Arab Muslim countries (e.g. Turkey, Iran, Pakistan) and non-Arab Muslim Middle Eastern countries such as Sudanic countries. There are even considerable differences among the countries that share membership in the League of Arab States, formed in 1945 to promote cooperation among countries of "Arab" language and culture. Almaney and Alwan (1982) specify that Arab is not a race, religion, or nationality. We follow Feghali's (1997) lead by accepting the best definition of "Arab" as anyone who speaks Arabic and feels as an Arab. This captures anyone outside the region of the Middle East, and includes those with pride, knowledge, and competence in Arabic communication patterns and customs. Although there are many differences among Arabic communities there are still a collection of people who share beliefs, values, and practices, and sustain a communal identity. 2We thank IPCRI--The Israel/Palestine Center for Research and Information and Dr. Gershon Baskin the Israeli director of IPCRI, Dr. Zakaria Al-Qaq the Palestinian Director and Dr. Marwan Darweish, director of IPCRI's peace education project, for providing access to these encounters. 40