W3005 1/29/02 Axonal navigation Prof. Kelley Motility is an important part of the developmental program for neurons. The cells move themselves, both inside and outside the nervous system. Cell migration makes important contributions to morphogenesis of nervous systems as we saw in the previous lecture. We can see the importance of axonal navigation very clearly in the disordered state of the cerebellum from mice with the weaver mutation, named after the disordered locomotion they display. Normally, cells within the cerebellum migrate on the processes of a special kind of glial cell, the radial glial cell (in the cerebellum these are called Bergman glia). In weaver mice, the migration fails to take place and the orderly structure of the cerebellum is disordered. Studies in tissue culture show that the problem with the mutant is not in the glial cells but instead with the migrating granule cells. Study question: In which direction are the granule cells migrating?: Pia to ventricle or ventricle to pia? In addition, parts of developing neurons move. A specialized structure, the axonal growth cone, is responsible for the navigation of developing axons to their final targets. Cajal described the growth cone from Golgi impregnations of developing neurons. Somehow he was able to infer that growth cones were mobile; he described them as “sniffing” their way through the nervous system, a description that emphasizes the receptive capabilities of these structures. Ross Harrison, the inventor of tissue culture, was the first to describe growth cones in living tissues: neurons of developing frogs. When we examine a young neuron in tissue culture we can see the emergence of various processes, usually called neurites. Eventually one of these exhiibits the morphological features of the growth cone and we call the process that bears the growth cone an axon. If, however, we amputate the young axon, another neurite can differentiate into an axon. Why is axonal navigation important? This process is essential for the formation of specific connections between nerve cells, the basic “wiring diagram” of the brain. Navigation results in an initial mapping of neurons to their targets; synaptic connections are further refined by experience (activity). Mutants that are deficient axon migration are neurologically abnormal. This is the wiring diagram of axons from identified neurons in embryonic grasshoppers. Corey Goodman and his laboratory used the grasshopper for these studies because each neuron is unique and can be identified by location and characteristics of the cell body from animal to animal (this is true of many invertebrates but is rare in vertebrates). They have not only described the choices made by growth cones but have discovered many of the molecular cues that guide fasiculation. Drosophila melanogaster (the fruit fly) has a very similar wiring diagram. In this genetic organism, mutants can be used to identify molecules involved in axon guidance because the mutants are miswired (we will consider the case of the robo mutant later in this lecture). In Drosophila, the entire genome is sequenced and powerful methods are available to determine which gene has been mutated. Many of our insights into the rules that govern axonal navigation come from studies in Drosophila and other invertebrates. Cajal in Golgi stained sections and Ross Harrison in one of the earliest uses of tissue culture observed axons emerging from developing neurons. When the axon emerges it often has an enlarged, club-shaped end. This protruberance is called the growth cone. David Bentley at Berkeley observed axons emerging from sensory neurons in the developing limbs of embryonic grasshoppers. These axons traveled in predictable ways making brief contacts with certain other cells along their way into the brain (recall that the cell bodies of primary sensory neurons are inside the CNS). Often only the earliest arriving growth cones actually have to make the hard choices, later arriving growth cones just fasiculate on older actions (no brainer). The early emerging axons are called pioneer axons and the specific cells that they encounter along the way are called guidepost cells. When the growth cone is traveling between guidepost cells is has a simple shape. When it encounters these cells, however, many spikes called philipodiae emerge from the growth cone itself. When growth cones reach decision points, their morphology becomes more complex. We can see this change in behavior in the pioneer axons on the previous slide. One of the clearest examples of choice behavior is at the optic chiasm, the point at which nerve fibers (axons) decide whether to remain on the same side of the brain (ipsilateral) or cross (contralateral). The growth cone is capable of behaving like an autonomous organelle. This attribute is clearly shown when it is cut off from the rest of neuron, as in this slide. The stump can continue to move and extend philipodia. Eventually the growth cone will run out of steam (ATP) and be unable to move. However, it is clear that because the growth cone doesn’t need its parent cell body, it has all the machinery it needs locally to sense the environment and to grow towards or away from local cues. How do growth cones move? One molecular motor of the growth cone is actin. We can see the distribution of molecules such as actin very clearly in growth cones grown on a flat surface, such as a tissue culture dish. The growth cone has a central club-shaped region from which emerges a veil of ruffling, membrane-bound cytoplasm. The filopodia emerge from this veil. The filopodial domain of the growth cone is filled with microfilaments made up of polymerized actin,, here revealed by staining to its binding partner fluorescent phalloidin. The central club shaped region contains numerous microtubules (revealed by staining with an antibody to tubulin, a major component of microtubules. The current model for how the growth cone moves is that the filopodia treadmill. The filopodium elongates by addition of actin monomers at the leading edge. The filopodium moves via breaking and attaching connections of actin filaments to myosin strands, in an energy dependent process that resembles that used in muscle contraction and relaxation. Growth of the axon behind the growth cone requires addition of microtubules. Growth cones move towards some targets and away from others. The recognition of repulsive cues by a growth cone is often followed by growth cone collapse, illustrated in this slide. The growth cones grows actively up to the boundary. When the philopodia sense the repulsive molecule they retract and the entire growth cone becomes smaller or collapses. The growth cone then retracts from the boundary. Growth cone collapse is often used to distinguish repulsive cues from the absence of attractive cues. The importance of local cues in establishing axon trajectories is shown very clearly in this experiment of Bill Harris (then at UCSD now at Cambridge). Harris labeled axons emerging from retinal ganglion cells with the fluoescent dye, diI, and the followed the routes they took to their major target in this case, the optic tectum. The experiments were carried out in embryos of the South African clawed frog, Xenopus laevis, as they are transparent and the axons all very superficial. The upper right hand panel shows the normal pathway of the axons as they head across the forebrain to the tectum. What happens when a small square through which the axons would pass is rotated? How did Harris interpret his results? What is the point of the tissue rotation shown in panel e? Where did the ides of growth cone guidance come from? One very important contribution to understanding the molecular basis of directed growth was made by Paul LeTourneau in his PhD thesis at Stamford. LeTourneau, now at Minnesota, plated individual neurons onto tissue culture plates that contained a grid-like pattern of the positively charged amino acid, polyornithine (porn) over a background of palladium (Pd). When the axons extended they grew along the porn highways and avoided the Pd squares. This result could have been due either to avoidance of Pd or preference for porn. How would you distinguish these possibilities experimentally? What accounts for the preference for porn? LeTourneau went on to show that the effects of the various substrates that he used could be accounted for by the ability of the growth cone to stick or adhere to the substrate. This observation lead to a search for adhesive molecules some of which are shown in the next slide. In the periphery, adhesion to the substrate is often mediated by the extracellular matrix (ECM) molecule laminin. The laminin interacts with integrin receptors on the surface of emerging axons. Other molecules are associated with adherence of one axon to another (fasiculation). These include the fasiclins discovered by Goodman and colleagues and the molecule NCatenin (panel D). Adhesion can de calcium dependent mediated, for example, by the neural cell adhesion molecule NCAM (panel A). Adhesion can be calcium dependent, mediated, for example, by the molecule Ncadherin. Fibronectin can mediate substrate adhesivity in the central nervous system. Shown here are the structures of laminin, fibronectin and the integrin receptor. Axons like to grow on other axons. This phenomenon is known as fasiculation (or bundling). Fasiculation can be promoted by mutual attraction of axons for each other or for their laminin or fibronectin highways. Cell Adhesion Molecules (CAMs, especially fasiclin) pull axons together. Alternatively the surrounding areas of axons can be repulsive and axons are then pushed together. Semaphorins, for example, push axons together. To actually make connections with a target, axons need to leave the bundle (defasiculate).: Polysialic acid residues added to CAMs help axons defasiculate Semaphorins come in several forms, some attached to the membrane and others which can diffuse. Semaphorins can be either attractive or repulsive, as can other guidance molecules depending on the biochemical state of the cell.The ability of developing axons to grow towards some brain regions and away from others was recognized very early in experiments on the retinotectal system of frogs. As we saw in Bill Harris’ experiment, the optic tectum is a major target for axons of retinal ganglion cells. Cells from the nasal or anterior part of the eye (the part nearest the nose) grow into posterior tectum while axons from retinal ganglion cells in the posterior part of the eye (temporal near the temple) grow into the anterior part of the tectum. Friedrich Bonhoeffer asked whether these two tectal targets had different properties. He prepared membranes from each tectal region, layered the posterior membranes with fluorescein (a yellow fluorescent dye) and laid them down as strips on a tissue culture plate. Axons from either the nasal or temporal retina were labeled red with rhodamine and he observed their behavior on the strips. Describe the results of this experiment. What conclusions could Bonhoeffer draw? Could he distinguish repulsive from attractive cues? What other methods might he have used? The current thinking about the retinotectal system is that, at a gross level, the behavior of axons can be understood in terms of different receptors expressed by cells in different parts of the retina and different ligands (molecules to which receptors bind) expressed in different locations in the tectum. One of the major decisions that a growth cone has to make is whether to cross to the other side of the nervous system. We have already seen that growth cones display a complex morphology as they make this choice at the optic chiasm. One of the interesting questions for these kinds of axons, the commisural or crossing axons, is why axons don’t cross back again to the other side. If for example the midline puts out an attractive cue, what makes it unattactive after an axon crosses? Corey Goodman tackled this question using mutations in Drosophila. He reasoned that one way things might go wrong is to have axons cross and recross the midline endlessly leading to very fat commisures. This is actually the phenotype observed in the robo or roundabout mutation. One idea is that the midline contains both potentially attractive and repellant signals. Axons would be kept from recrossing the midline by the repellant activity. The state of the growth cone, however, must change in this scenario so that it no longer finds the midline attractive Some support for this last idea comes from experiments in the developing spinal cord. Some of the neurons in this part of the brain will extend axons to cross at the ventral midline and then travel forward. A series of experiments by Tom Jessell, Mark Tessier-Levigne and colleagues established that these axons are attracted by a molecule called netrin. Netrins are short range chemoattractants that are chemically very closely related to the ECM molecules, laminins. Netrins act as long range signals that guide axons to the midbrain in both vertebrates and invertebrates. Netrins act as repellants for neurons that grow away from the midline That axons change their state after crossing is suggested by an experiment in which axons that have not yet crossed (green) are attracted to the ventral midline (floorplate or FP) while those that have already crossed (red) do not turn towards the FP. Not the obvious club-shaped growth cones at the tips of the red axons. The turning behavior of a growth cone towards an attractant is often calcium dependent as in the case of this frog growth cone being led on by nerve growth factor, a diffusible sustance which may act as a chemoattractant for growth cones at a distance in one of Mu-Ming Poo’s experiments while at Columbia (he is now at Berkeley). Poo showed that the same molecule can be either an attractant or a repulsant depending on the biochemical state of the cell. In the first three panels, a neuron is attracted to BDNF, another growth factor. The turning response is calcium sensitive (middle panels) and becomes an avoidance response when cAMP is manipulated. • Summary The growth cone responds to cues in the immediate environment and cues that are further away; some of the cues are attractive and some are repulsive. Growth cones also like to migrate on other axons (fasciculation); when they reach their destinations (targets) they peel off (defasiculate); fasciculation and defasiculation are due to the balance of attractive and repulsive cues. As it travels through the developing brain, an individual growth cone samples territories that differ successively. Axons are pushed from behind by more distant repellents, pulled from ahead by more distant attractants and hemmed in (kept “in line” or “on the road”) by local attractant/repellent signals. Remember the important principle of location, location and location. Many areas of the developing brain contain topographic representations. Antagonistic (attractive vs repellant) gradients along two axes may establish topographic connections. How synaptic connections actually form will be the subject of our next lecture.