APPLICATION FOR ADMISSION TO PSYCHIATRIC EPIDEMIOLOGY TRAINING PROGRAM 1. NAME___________________________________________________________ 2. MAILING ADDRESS______________________________________________ 3. TELEPHONE NUMBER(S)_________________________________________ 4. EMAIL ADDRESS________________________________________________ 5. WHAT IS YOUR DATE AND STATE (OR COUNTRY IF FOREIGN BORN) OF BIRTH? DATE___________________STATE/COUNTRY_______________________ 6. CITIZENSHIP STATUS: THE NIMH STIPULATES THAT ONLY “US CITIZENS OR INDIVIDUALS LAWFULLY ADMITTED FOR PERMANENT RESIDENCE MAY APPLY.” PERMANENT RESIDENTS MUST SUBMIT A NOTARIZED STATEMENT INDICATING POSSESSION OF THE ALIEN REGISTRATION RECEIPT CARD—1-151 OR 1-551. INDIVIDUALS WITH TEMPORARY OR STUDENT VISAS ARE NOT ELIGIBLE FOR SUPPORT. ARE YOU A US CITIZEN?_____YES_____NO IF “NO” PLEASE ANSWER BELOW OF WHAT COUNTRY ARE YOU A CITIZEN?_______________________ DO YOU HAVE A VALID US “GREEN CARD”______________________ (IF INFORMATION REQUESTED BELOW IS LISTED ON ENCLOSED CV, PLEASE INDICATE) 7. EDUCATION: PLEASE PROVIDE INFORMATION REGARDING ALL SCHOOLS ATTENDED AND DEGREES RECEIVED SINCE HIGH SCHOOL. SCHOOL DATES ATTENDED DEGREE MAJOR ________________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________________ ________________________________________________________________________ 1 8. EMPLOYMENT: PLEASE LIST ALL EMPLOYMENT (LAST 5 YEARS) RELEVANT TO YOUR APPLICATION TO THIS PROGRAM. EMPLOYER POSITION DATES OF EMPLOYMENT ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ 9. OTHER EXPERIENCE: PLEASE DESCRIBE OTHER ACTIVITIES OR EXPERIENCE RELEVANT TO YOUR APPLICATION TO THIS PROGRAM. ____________________________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________________________________ 10. AWARDS: PLEASE LIST ANY AWARDS, HONORS, FELLOWSHIPS OR GRANTS RECEIVED. ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ 11. OTHER FUNDING: DO YOU PRESENTLY HAVE OR HAVE YOU BEEN NOTIFIED THAT YOU WILL RECEIVE ANY FELLOWSHIPS OR GRANTS THAT WILL OVERLAP WITH THE P.E.T. FELLOWSHIP? _____YES_____NO IF “YES”, FROM WHAT SOURCE? ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ 12. PUBLICATIONS (AUTHORS, TITLE, JOURNAL/BOOK, DATE): ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________________ 13. PREDOCS ONLY: WHAT IS YOUR REGISTRATION STATUS AT COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE ARTS AND SCIENCES? _____CURRENTLY REGISTERED DEPARTMENT________________________ _____ADMITTED _____APPLICATION FOR ADMISSION PENDING _____OTHER (EXPLAIN) 2 14. ACADEMIC/CAREER GOALS: STATE IN ONE OR TWO PAGES YOUR ACADEMIC AND CAREER GOALS. PLEASE BE AS SPECIFIC AS POSSIBLE. STATE WHAT TRAINING YOU WISH TO RECEIVE IN THIS PROGRAM THAT WILL FURTHER THOSE GOALS. WHY ARE YOU INTERESTED IN STUDYING PSYCHIATRIC EPIDEMIOLOGY? 16. TRANSCRIPTS: PLEASE SUBMIT TRANSCRIPTS OF YOUR UNDERGRADUATE AND GRADUATE ACADEMIC RECORDS, GRADUATE RECORD EXAMINATIONS/MEDICAL COLLEGE APTITUDE TEST. 17. LETTERS OF RECOMMENDATION: PLEASE ARRANGE TO HAVE THREE (3) LETTERS OF RECOMMENDATION SENT TO US BY INDIVIDUALS WHO ARE FAMILIAR WITH YOUR WORK. (PLEASE ASK THEM TO WRITE TO US. WE WILL NOT CONTACT THEM.) 18. OPTIONAL: YOU MAY SUBMIT REPRINTS OF YOUR PUBLICATIONS, UNPUBLISHED PAPERS FOR COURSES, OR COPIES OF MASTER’S THESES OR DOCTORAL DISSERTATIONS, OR ANY OTHER OF YOUR WRITINGS THAT MAY BE HELPFUL IN EVALUATING YOUR CAPABILITIES AND INTEREST. DEADLINE FOR APPLICATIONS: DECEMBER 1st 3 TO FAMILIARIZE YOU WITH OUR TRAINING PROGRAM, WE HAVE ATTACHED A DESCRIPTION EXTRACTED FROM A GRANT APPLICATION THAT WE WROTE SEEKING FUNDING FOR THE PROGRAM. IT BEGINS WITH A SECTION ABOUT THE RATIONALE FOR A TRAINING PROGRAM IN PSYCHIATRIC EPIDEMIOLOGY AND IS FOLLOWED BY A DESCRIPTION OF THE PROGRAM’S STRUCTURE AND FUNCTIONING. IN ADDITION TO THIS MATERIAL, THERE ARE SEVERAL FACTS YOU SHOULD KNOW ABOUT APPLYING TO THE PROGRAM. WE ROUTINELY SEEK APPLICATIONS EACH SPRING WITH A DEADLING OF DECEMBER 1ST . WE ATTEMPT TO GIVE ANSWERS TO PROSPECTIVE FELLOWS AS SOON AS POSSIBLE, BUT ARE DEPENDENT ON OUR FUNDING SOURCE ABOUT THE SIZE OF OUR BUDGET FOR THE NEXT YEAR AND THUS THE AVAILABILITY OF THE STIPENDS. THE EARLIEST DATE FOR FUNDING IS JULY 1ST, WITH A USUAL START DATE OF SEPTEMBER 1. PRE-DOCTORAL FELLOWS MUST BE ENROLLED IN A Ph.D. PROGRAM. APPLICATIONS FOR Ph.D. PROGRAMS ARE OBTAINED BY WRITING TO: COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY GRADUATE SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES ADMISSIONS OFFICE, 107 LOW MEMORIAL LIBRARY NEW YORK, NY 10027 PHONE: (212) 854-4737 WWW: HTTP://WWW.COLUMBIA .EDU/CU/GSAS EMAIL: REF8@COLUMBIA.EDU CURRENT LEVELS FOR PRE-DOCTORAL FELLOWS. Career Level Years of Experience Stipend for FY 2014 Predoctoral All $22,920 Monthly Stipend $1,910 POST-DOCTORAL STIPENDS DEPEND ON THE NUMBER OF YEARS OF EXPERIENCE SINCE RECEIVING A Ph.D. or M.D. Career Level Years of Experience Stipend for FY 2014 Monthly Stipend Postdoctoral 0 $42,840 $3,570 1 $44,556 $3,713 2 $46,344 $3,862 3 $48,192 $4,016 4 $50,112 $4,176 5 $52,116 $4,343 6 $54,216 $4,518 7 or More $56,376 $4,698 4 COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY PSYCHIATRIC EPIDEMIOLOGY TRAINING PROGRAM COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF PUBLIC HEALTH DEPT. OF EPIDEMIOLOGY/ PET PROGRAM 722 WEST 168TH STREET, 7TH FLOOR NEW YORK, NY 10032 (212) 305-7789/(212) 342-4549 EMAIL: PET@COLUMBIA.EDU _____________________________________________________________ Co-SPONSORED BY: DEPARTMENT OF EPIDEMIOLOGY DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHIATRY SCHOOL OF PUBLIC HEALTH COLLEGE OF PHYSICIANS AND SURGEONS PROGRAM DIRECTOR EZRA SUSSER, M.D.. Co-DIRECTORS: BRUCE DOHRENWEND, Ph.D. KATHERINE M. KEYES WILD, Ph.D. TRAINING Co-ORDINATOR: SHARON SCHWARTZ, Ph.D. STEERING COMMITTEE & FACULTY: ALAN BROWN, M.D. DEBORAH HASIN, Ph.D RICHARD NEUGEBAUER, Ph.D. EZRA SUSSER, M.D. KATHERINE M. KEYES WILD, Ph.D. BRUCE DOHRENWEND, Ph.D. ROBERTO LEWIS-FERNANDEZ, M.D. SHARON SCHWARTZ, Ph.D. MYRNA WEISSMAN, Ph.D. FACULTY: PAUL APPELBAUM, M.D. CHERYL CORCORAN, M.D. MADELYN S. GOULD, Ph.D. CHRISTINA HOVEN, Ph.D. CATHERINE E. MONK, Ph.D MARK OLFSON, M.D. BLAKE TURNER, Ph.D. MELANIE M. WALL, Ph.D. LAWRENCE YANG, Ph.D. LISA BATES, Sc.D. MINDY FULLILOVE, M.D. MARK HATZENBUEHLER, Ph.D. DENISE KANDEL, Ph.D. KIMBERLY NOBEL, M.D. RUTH OTTMAN, Ph.D. NICOLE SCHUPF, Ph.D KATHERINE M. KEYES WILD, Ph.D. 5 2. BACKGROUND As the longest running Psychiatric Epidemiology Training program in the country, we propose to combine the experience that our long history provides with fresh ideas and approaches to produce a strong training program for a new generation of scholars. At Columbia, we are fortunate to have an exceptional faculty whose research programs allow us to propose a comprehensive approach that seeks to advance knowledge at multiple levels (biological, individual, interpersonal and contextual) and integrate that knowledge through an understanding of the dynamic interplay between levels. Our goal is to recruit scholars with expertise in at least one level and to provide training that allows them to work together across levels to develop more complete understandings of the causes and consequences of mental illnesses. We begin the explication of our rationale for training and our plans for achieving it by reviewing the compelling evidence that exists concerning the importance of the problem. Based on both their prevalence and the distress and disability they produce psychiatric disorders represent a substantial health burden in the United States and around the world (Kessler et al 2005, Demyttenaere et al. 2004, Merikangas et al. 2010, Mathers and Loncar 2006). At the individual level, these disorders are associated with a variety of functional impairments, emotional distress (Birnbaum et al 2010), social dislocations (Agerbo et al 2004), serious physical illness (Prince et al. 2007) and early death (Parks et al. 2006, Piatt et al. 2010). The societal costs are also enormous. In addition to the costs of treatment and social services, lost income due to mental illnesses has been estimated to be over 193 billion dollars per year in the United States (Kessler et al. 2008). Moreover, mental illnesses are connected to major social problems such as homelessness (Folsom et al. 2005), HIV infection (Collins et al 2006), interpersonal violence (Swanson et al. 2006) and arrest (Link et al. 2011). At the same time epidemiological true prevalence studies consistently show that only a minority of the people experiencing a mental disorder in the United States receive treatment (Wang et al. 2005). The situation is even direr in developing nations where resources are limited and mental illnesses receive low priority (Wang et al. 2007, Prince et al. 2007). These considerations highlight the importance of continued research into the causes and consequences of psychiatric disorders and underscore the need for continued training in this area. Created in 1972 the Columbia University Psychiatric Epidemiology Training Program (PET) provides rigorous training in a multidisciplinary context focused on understanding and addressing mental illnesses. It is one of only a few programs designed to provide research training in the epidemiology of mental disorders. The rationale for continued training, which we elaborate below, is supported by our 39 year record of training generations of highly productive psychiatric epidemiologists and the ongoing need to train still new generations. The program has supported and trained 167 researchers who have made valuable contributions to the research, clinical, and community settings where they are based. Many have become leaders in the field. As our recent experience demonstrates, demand for training remains high with far more highly qualified applicants seeking positions than we have positions to provide. As a consequence, the fellows we enroll are exceptionally well qualified, extremely talented and therefore highly promising. Demand for researchers trained in psychiatric epidemiology also remains strong as the job procurement record of recent graduates demonstrates. Major changes in the field of psychiatric epidemiology (e.g. Susser et al 2006, Weissman et al. in press), advances within epidemiology more broadly (e.g. Rothman et al 2008, Schwartz et al. 2011), progress in the conceptualization of psychiatric illness (Kessler et al. 2011, Cuthbert and Insel 2010) as well as growing awareness of the complex interconnections between psychiatric epidemiology and the biological, developmental and social sciences (e.g. Brown and Derkits 2010, Koenen 2010, Koenen et al. 2009, Link 2008, Galea 2007) underscore the importance of training programs such as PET. Our training plan incorporates themes we have emphasized since the program’s inception, elaborates them 6 and moves them in new directions and to new substantive areas. Fifteen years ago we proposed to expand our training from a strong base in individuals’ experienced adversity (Dohrenwend 1998, 2000) “upward” to contextual factors and “downward” toward biological and genetic ones. Ten years ago we further expanded this conceptualization to include a life course perspective (Susser et al. 2000) that considers a person (biology, psychology), in context (family, social network, neighborhood) through time (development, context change). Five years ago we emphasized the need to advance this framework by expanding our capability to conceptualize, measure, and test ideas about the dynamic interplay among levels (e.g., biological, individual, social). This framework has been extremely useful in articulating the range of factors that influence the onset and course of mental illnesses and in helping us develop a strong training program to respond to the need for expertise in the areas the framework specifies. In the next five years we plan advances in each component of our framework as described below. In each instance our aim is to take stock of current knowledge and by so doing push understanding further to provide the knowledge base necessary to facilitate approaches that reduce the incidence and shorten the course of mental illnesses. Psychiatric Epidemiology: The Core Focus of PET We begin with a definition of epidemiology because it informs what is – and is not – highlighted in our training program. Epidemiology is the study of the distribution (incidence and prevalence) and determinants of states of health in human populations and the evaluation of population health interventions designed to address those determinants. The ultimate goals of epidemiology are to contribute to the understanding of the etiology and course of disease and thereby to its prevention and control. Psychiatric epidemiology involves the investigation of disorders of thoughts, feelings and behaviors. It includes the variety of problems that are described in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manuals of the American Psychiatric Association and the kinds of basic neurobiological dysfunctions that are envisioned by the Research Domain Criteria (Cuthburt and Insel 2010). It uses epidemiologic methods to understand the onset, course (duration and reoccurrence) and consequences of these disorders with the goal of transforming such understanding into useful actions designed to prevent disorder and mitigate the distress and disability that they entail. Principles of Training Over the years we have developed an approach to training that prepares fellows to identify important research problems and to tackle these problems using theory, strategic study designs, and innovative measurement approaches in a multi-disciplinary context. Through course work, field placements and a faculty-fellows seminar, trainees are exposed to research that follows this model in their own area of interest and in the field more generally. Below we describe our approach using examples of research conducted by PET faculty. All of these studies are available to fellows as potential field placements. We provide examples of fellows’ involvement here and, more extensively, in the Progress Report. Framework for Identifying Important Research Questions The Columbia University Psychiatric Epidemiology Training Program challenges trainees to identify research questions that deepen understanding and open the possibility for effective intervention. Identifying the questions that will matter most in these respects is a non-trivial undertaking. Our entire program is structured to achieve the aim of effectively training new generations of scholars who are adept at identifying such questions and skilled in answering them. To encourage the initial creative spark that leads to a good research question we provide a strong knowledge base in the core issues facing psychiatric epidemiology. We then encourage trainees to view these core issues from multiple perspectives so that new ideas emerge as problems are interrogated from different vantage points. 7 We facilitate this activity by exposing trainees to the diverse interests of a large and very active faculty, who embrace multidisciplinary approaches and work with multidisciplinary teams. In keeping with this orientation, we set forth a framework that begins with a traditional strength of our program – its focus on an individual’s exposure to adversity and his/her use of coping resources to adapt to that exposure. We expand from that focus to consider both broader contextual factors and biological mechanisms by conceptualizing persons in context through time. This multifaceted, multilevel approach provides the fellows a rich source of potential research questions. The Focus on Adversity in Explaining Mental Health Disparities The Columbia Program has traditionally emphasized the role of adversity in the etiology and course of psychiatric disorders. This emphasis derived from the consistent association between socioeconomic status and mental disorder, studies of extreme situations, and conceptualization of the stress process. Under the direction of Bruce Dohrenwend, the program made great strides toward understanding the role of adversity. These include findings from his innovative quasi-experimental study reported in Science that tested the relative importance of social causation versus social selection in explaining the association between socioeconomic status and several mental illnesses (Dohrenwend et al. 1992). The emphasis on adversity remains prominent in the work of PET faculty. For example, Dohrenwend (2006) has tackled the problem of within category variability to develop a more powerful measure of stressful circumstances. Additionally in a more recent Science article Dohrenwend and colleagues (2006) supplemented data from the National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study (NVVRS) with information from military records to provide strong objective measures of exposure to combat. These painstakingly collected records avoid biases that could cloud the interpretation of self report measures thereby allowing a declarative statement concerning the role of combat exposure in the development of PTSD. Current fellow Ben Adams, a co-author of the 2006 Science paper, is using data from this study to put forward and test a theory about the role of post-traumatic appraisal in the stress process. This work integrates the objective measures of the combat exposure with the soldiers’ subjective appraisal of the event to provide a more complete framework for understanding the consequences of combat exposure. Working within the domain of adversity, Meyer, Schwartz and Frost (2008) constructed comprehensive measures of adversity including measures derived from the Dohrenwend approach mentioned above. By constructing a novel sample of people who varied in terms of sexual preference, ethnic minority status and gender this team was able to investigate whether and to what extent disadvantaged statuses were associated with stress exposure of many kinds and whether stress exposure was related to mental disorder and suicide ideation. The novel sample and the comprehensive set of measures addresses core issues pertinent to the role of adversity in mental health that could not be addressed before. Current fellows Alicia Lukachko and David Barnes are using these data to examine contextual factors that change the role of religious affiliation for Gay, Lesbian and Bisexual populations. Tracy McFarlane is using these data to test a theory of positive marginality – the benefits that can accrue from stigmatized group membership. The Expansion to Contextual Factors and Biological Mechanisms. Expanding from our program’s roots in adversity and stress we have developed our program’s focus in two directions. One of these involves the revitalization of epidemiology's concern with the health of populations through a greater emphasis on the social, cultural and historical contexts in which individual risk factors occur. The second involves the incorporation and development of research that uses biological concepts and measures to study the origins of mental disorders. Our rationale for this 8 two-pronged expansion derived from critiques of epidemiology’s overly narrow and excessively descriptive risk-factor approach (Susser and Susser 1996). Needed, we argued, was an analytical epidemiology that could investigate mechanistic explanations from a socio-cultural and/or a biological vantage point to create what some of us have called an “eco epidemiology” (Susser and Susser 1996) that “adopts, develops and teaches methods to understand both biological and social complexities” (March and Susser 2006). Progress and Plans Regarding Expansion “Up” to Contexts We propose a continuation of our emphasis on contexts with a move to a deeper consideration of global mental health. Before explicating our plans in this regard we briefly recount the rationale for an emphasis on contexts more generally and provide a description of some of the U.S.-based work we have done and are doing in this area. Rationale for a Focus on Contexts. Our rationale for a focus on contexts stems from many sources. From “eco epidemiology” comes the idea that biologic events are context dependent; "no biological entity can conform entirely to universal laws because of the overarching contexts and interactions between levels within a biological structure" (Susser and Susser 1996). Contexts are also critical in Link and Phelan’s concept of "fundamental social causes" of mental and physical health problems (1995, 2008, 2010). This is the idea that social factors such as socioeconomic status remain associated with disease through time even though specific risk factors for disease and the prevalence of the diseases themselves change in dramatic ways. Thus, it is critical to study contextual factors that characterize groups of people linked through such factors as neighborhood, occupation, friendship, or family. Finally Galea (2007) focuses attention on what he terms “macrosocial determinants” that “are beyond the individual and are explicitly a function of population systems such as culture, economics, political systems and population dynamics such as migration and urbanization.” These frameworks all point to important contextual issues that must be addressed in any comprehensive account of mental health problems. First, stress exposure can involve entire contexts as recent studies directed by PET faculty underscore. For example Christina Hoven and colleagues (2005) documented the substantial consequences of the 9/11 attacks on children in grades 4-12 six months after the attack. Faculty member Sandro Galea has directed studies of the mental health effects of civil war in Liberia (Galea et al. 2010), hurricane Katrina (Schumacher et al. 2010) and of the long term consequence of 9/11 on adults (Digrande et al. 2011). When entire populations are impacted in this way the salience of contextual factors becomes even more apparent. Whether and to what extent infrastructure at the contextual level exists to address stressors (e.g. provide housing) or deliver needed mental health services is critical to the health of populations in these very stressful circumstances. Second, social, cultural and historical contexts can also put people “at risk of risk” (Link and Phelan 1995, 2010). That is, contextual factors determine exposure to individually based risk factors such as stressful life events, infections or environmental toxins. A striking example is the association between famine and the subsequent development of schizophrenia. Both Susser et al.’s (1996) study of the Dutch Famine and the two recent replications of that finding in China (St. Clair et al. 2005; Xu et al. 2009) indicate that large scale social and political factors shape exposure to poor nutrition which subsequently puts people at risk for schizophrenia. Another example studied by PET faculty and fellows concerns the growing evidence that migration sets the stage for subsequent racial discrimination and exclusion that is associated with the onset of psychosis in Western Europe and elsewhere (March et al. 2008, March et al. 2010, Veling et al. 2010). 9 Third, contextual factors such as stress exposure can modify genetic or other biological risk factors (e.g., Caspi et al. 2003). Although the replication of the specific Caspi et al. finding has been debated, the more general idea that genes and environments interact or are mutually related through epigenetic and other processes is strong. The interplay between genes and environments is of great interest to PET faculty and a strong program of research is building in this area. One example is newly recruited faculty member (and former PET fellow) Karestan Koenen, whose research career has included an emphasis on gene-environment interplay. A recent study conducted by Koenen, Galea and colleagues (2009) examined the influence of the interplay between the serotonin transporter polymorphism 5HTTLPR and the county-level social environment (e.g., crime rate and unemployment) on PTSD in the Hurricane Ike study. Susser is a participant in ongoing follow-up studies of the Dutch famine, which reported that early prenatal famine leaves epigenetic marks (reduced methylation) on the imprinted gene IGF2 that remain detectable sixty years after the exposure (Heijmans et al. 2008). While he and colleagues are investigating the role of epigenetic effects in linking early prenatal famine to schizophrenia, they have also suggested an alternative hypothesis that periconceptional famine may predispose to genetic mutations (e.g. individually rare copy number variations or CNVs) that have been related to schizophrenia in recent studies (e.g., McClellan et al. 2006; 2007). Similarly, the relation of older paternal age at conception to both schizophrenia and autism, first established by PET faculty (Brown et al. 2002; Reichenberg et al. 2006, Malaspina et al. 2009), have been hypothesized to be related to either epigenetic effects or de novo mutations in the male germ line (Malaspina et al., 2002). As research on the interplay between genes and environments grows it is clearly critically important for fellows to be familiar with both domains and with the many ways in which genes and environment can influence one another. One sign of our growing recognition in this area is that early career social epidemiologists from Europe often come to spend six month periods at Columbia with us, in order to learn more about the integration of epigenetics and genetics with psychiatric epidemiology. For example, in the past year we have had such visiting faculty from Cambridge University, UK; Leiden, Netherlands; and University of Oslo, Norway. Fourth contexts are important because as Link (2008) points out the diffusion of any new health enhancing knowledge or technology is strongly influenced by contextual factors. This has been shown for example in studies of the diffusion of statins and beta blockers as they influence cholesterol levels and recovery from acute myocardial infarctions respectively (Chang and Lauderdale 2009; Skinner and Stager 2005). As advances are made in the pharmacological and psychosocial treatment of mental illnesses these same processes are likely to apply and, as a consequence, their diffusion needs to be understood and addressed so that benefits are distributed as rapidly (to improve population health) and as evenly as possible (to minimize disparities). Growing Emphasis on Global Mental Health. Each element of the rationale for training in contextual factors mentioned above is strongly implicated in an area in which we plan to expand our emphasis in the five years ahead – global mental health. In no way minimizing our attention to such factors in the United States we turn to global mental health because of the critical nature of the problems associated with the global burden of disease, our own interest and expertise in the area and the emphasis that has been placed on this domain within NIMH. Estimates of the life time prevalence for mental disorders from the World Health Organization are between 12 and 49% (WHO 2000) and 12 month prevalence is between 4 and 26% (Demyttenaere et al 2004). In keeping with these facts estimates of the burden of disease due to neuropsychiatric disorders is large with nearly three quarters of this substantial burden residing in what have been called “low” and “middle-income” countries (Lopez et al 2006). In the absence of intervention this burden is projected to grow dramatically in part due to the demographic and epidemiological transitions in low and middle-income countries (Mathers and Loncare 2006). Estimates of the low proportion of people with identified disorders who receive treatment for those disorders indicate an enormous global 10 problem (Wang et al 2007). This burden is almost certainly underestimated in low and middle income countries because it is based on community surveys which generally omit childhood disorders and late life disorders and could not reliably measure uncommon severe disorders such as schizophrenia. Infrastructure for delivering mental health services is often meager leaving many people with severe problems either untreated or poorly treated (Saxena et al. 2007). Moreover the capacity within high income countries to partner with countries with fewer resources in joint efforts to address these problems is underdeveloped. Drawing on a strong tradition within epidemiology at the Mailman School of Public Health begun by Susser (Mervyn) and Stein we have expanded our interest and expertise in global mental health. Chair of the Department of Epidemiology and PET Faculty Member, Sandro Galea is engaged in projects around the world that examine the mental health consequences of traumatic events induced by war, terrorism or disaster. He has on-going studies in post-war Liberia, in Ethiopia and among Israelis and Palestinians exposed to multiple terrorist attacks (see e.g. Galea et al 2010). Richard Neugebauer, a member of the PET steering committee, studies the mental health and developmental consequences of the Rwandan Genocide (Neugebauer 2009). He is also actively engaged in an analysis of two randomized controlled trials of the efficacy of group interpersonal psychotherapy for the treatment of depression in Sub-Saharan Africa (Bolton et al. 2003). Ezra Susser, former Department Chair and Co-Director of PET has been involved in the global mental health field for over 20 years and continues to build on the longstanding connections developed over that period of time, especially though not only, in Latin America and sub-Saharan Africa. He is actively involved as a consultant, advisor and mentor on work in Argentina, Brazil, India, South Africa and elsewhere, and is Chair of the Scientific Advisory Board of Geocode, a European Union funded collaboration that seeks to build capacity across the globe in birth cohort research, genomics, and epigenetics, with a key focus being mental disorders. In 2010, Susser and Galea established and colead a Global Mental Health Program at Columbia. Our interest in global mental health has been matched by a growing interest in this area within NIMH. The institution of a new Office on Research on Disparities and Global Mental Health is one strong sign of this interest (and is led by Pamela Collins who was on the PET Steering Committee before being recruited to that position). In concert with the development of this office a Request for Applications (RFA-MH-11-070) was released entitled Collaborative Hubs for International Research on Mental Health (U19). In keeping with our growing interest in this area we responded to this RFA for the region of Latin American, and under the leadership of Susser (contact PI), Galea (PI), and Chilean colleagues Rojas and Alvarado (PIs), we received an excellent score that positions us well to become one of three funded HUBS. In addition, Susser was one of the PIs on an application for sub-Saharan Africa (contact PI: Crick Lund, UCT), that received a similar score and is well positioned to become another one of the three funded Hubs. This and the Global Mental Health Program we already have in place, as well as the broad range of global mental health research of our faculty, provide an exceptionally strong context for training. Current PET postdoctoral fellow Joy Noel Baumgartner is strongly integrated into research on Global mental health through her mentor Ezra Susser. She helped to write and is closely involved in an R21 (together with a second PI from South Africa) designed to build a platform for the first incidence study of psychoses in sub-Saharan Africa. Her main interest is in testing, refining and elaborating measures of social integration that can be used effectively in different cultures and for people with different types of mental health problems. If they are funded, this work could also be pursued in the Latin American and Sub-Saharan Hubs. As a starting point, she is refining the widely used World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule (WHODAS 2.0) for African contexts, building on her dissertation work in Tanzania and her new pilot studies in South Africa. 11 In keeping with the idea that understanding global mental health has direct relevance for understanding the mental health problems of new immigrants, faculty member Roberto LewisFernandez has a program of research focused on understanding the way culture affects individuals’ experience of mental disorder and their help-seeking expectations (Lewis-Fernandez et al, 2009, 2010). Several other senior faculty members (Brown, Dohrenwend, Hasin, Link, Weissman), though less centrally involved in global mental health have studies located in other countries and strong interests in processes pertaining global health such as trauma (Dohrenwend), the provision of interpersonal psychotherapy (Weissman), gene-environment interaction (Hasin) and stigma (Link). The strong senior leadership in global mental health is augmented by the interest and expertise of three junior PET faculty. Lisa Bates is Co-PI of a study that examines women’s health and well-being in Bangladesh in the context of economic development; she is designated as the PI of a grant in preparation for a related study in Pakistan, where Dr. Susser and Dr. Atif Rahman will be co-leaders. Magdalena Cerda is using a quasi-experimental strategy made possible by the introduction of a cable car to poor isolated communities in Columbia to study the implications of neighborhood change on violence and mental health; both Galea and Susser are mentors on her K Award and she is part of the Hub for Latin America. Larry Yang conducts research on schizophrenia in China and currently has a 7 percentile score on an R03 to study the deficit syndrome using a large epidemiological study (Phillips et al. 2009) of untreated people with schizophrenia. In addition Yang developed a pilot proposal to study stigma in cross cultural contexts as part of the U19 HUB grant. Of the six main faculty with interest and expertise in global mental health three (Bates, Cerda and Galea) are new to our faculty since the last submission indicating a strong growth for this new emphasis. Our emphasis on contexts in general and global mental health in particular offers fellows at different levels strong training and development opportunities. Progress Regarding Expansion “Down” to Biological Factors Biologic and genetic studies are at the forefront of research in psychiatric disorders, and here too epidemiology can play a unique and central role (Weissman et al., in press). During the past five year period we have worked to consolidate and expand training and research opportunities for the fellows in these areas. We began the last 5 year period of funding with substantial strength in this area as represented in the research programs of Co-Director Ezra Susser and faculty members Alan Brown, Susan Hodge, Ruth Ottman and Myrna Weissman. We have added to that strength through the recruitment of four new faculty -- Cheryl Corcoran a biologically oriented psychiatrist studying the prodrome for psychosis, Karestan Koenan who has a strong program of research focused on genetic factors and gene-environment interplay, Kimberly Noble a pediatrician epidemiologist who studies the biological pathways that link adversity to cognitive functioning and mental health outcomes, and Nicole Schupf who studies biological and genetic factors in Alzheimer’s Disease. Sandro Galea, the new chair of epidemiology strengthens several aspects of our program including this one through his studies of gene-environment interactions in PTSD. In light of the strong emphasis on neuroscience and its importance for understanding the biology and genetics of mental illnesses, Weissman et al. (in press) point to the critical role that epidemiology can play in advancing such understandings. Based on Weissman and colleagues analysis, we identify four ways in which epidemiology is critical to the advancement of biological and genetic understandings of mental illnesses. 1) Epidemiology Provides Population-Based Data and Designs. Weissman et al. (in press) make a compelling case that epidemiological designs and findings can facilitate a partnership with the basic sciences. Specifically they apply the concept of translational epidemiology to psychiatry and propose 12 that “the representative samples, novel designs, and hypotheses offered by epidemiology can be translated into experimental approaches in the clinical and basic sciences.” As examples they point out that community studies provide large generalizable samples for collecting genetic information and biomarkers and can also be used to construct case-control studies targeting biological factors of interest to basic science investigations. Furthermore epidemiological studies have spawned hypotheses like the gateway hypothesis for drug addiction and paternal age as a risk for schizophrenia and autism. These hypotheses have become the foundation for subsequent animal and genetic studies. In short, the case is made that epidemiology is critically needed to assist in the full realization of the tremendous advances that have taken place in the biological sciences over the past decade. 2) Epidemiology is Critical to the Identification of Biological Risk Factors. Among the many examples Weissman et al. provide, several critical findings have emerged from the work of PET faculty. For example, informed by the nuerodevelopmental approach and the idea that there are critical periods in which environmental exposures might be particularly important, Brown and Susser have studied cohorts who were ascertained in utero and have now been followed through adult life. Facilitated by the collection and storage of maternal sera, these studies provide some of the most convincing evidence concerning the role of infections in the etiology of schizophrenia (Brown et al 2004; 2005). The approach also allows the study of environmental toxins and nutrition. For example, evidence is building that in-utero iron deficiency may play a role in psychosis as a paper from PET fellow Beverly Insel has indicated (Insel et al. 2008). In keeping with the success of this approach Brown has three ongoing R01’s focused on schizophrenia, autism (in Finnish cohorts) and bi-polar disorder (in a California cohort) and Susser is involved with studies in China and Israel forming an excellent context for discovery and training of fellows. Brown has also demonstrated that in utero infectious exposures are associated with core phenotypes that have been consistently observed in schizophrenia, including neurocognitive anomalies (Brown et al, 2009). 3) Epidemiology is Critical for Studying the Interplay Between Genes and Environments. In describing the rationale for the importance of contextual factors (see above) we argued that a thorough understanding of contextual factors was critical to understanding the interplay between genes and environments. Of course the same is true for genetic factors. To the extent that genes modify environmental exposures such as personally experienced stressful life events or disadvantaged contextual circumstances (e.g. Koenen el al. 2009) we need to understand genes to understand environmental exposures. To do so a thorough understanding of informative epidemiological designs such as high risk and twin studies is required. Additionally, new ideas relating to epi-genetics will likely deepen the need to understand genetics in relation to environments in the time ahead. The programs of research of Brown, Galea, Koenen, Susser and Weissman provide superb contexts for training in these areas. 4) Epidemiology is Important for Understanding Biological Pathways from Adversity to Disorder. Understanding the role of adversity in the onset and course of mental disorders will be greatly enhanced and more deeply understood to the extent that the biological pathways linking adversity to disorder are identified. Recent research strongly suggests the possibility of new understanding in this area. For example the work of Boyce and colleagues (2005) interrogates the interactive influences of socioeconomic adversities and neurobiological responses showing that they operate in concert to induce both physical and mental disorders in childhood. In keeping with this general paradigm, new faculty member Kim Noble uses imaging (MRI) to understand the neuro-cognitive mechanisms linking socioeconomic adversity and brain functioning (Nobel et al. 2006; 2007). All of the PET fellows have benefited from the development of this area of research, but some have been particularly drawn to it. Fellows recently graduated from PET who focused on biological factors included Anna Keski-Rahkonen twin studies of eating disorders, James Gangswich, insulin 13 dependence and depression, and Beverly Insel iron deficiency and maternal-fetal blood incompatibity and schizophrenia. Current fellow David Freedman is studying early developmental risks for neurocognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia and newly admitted postdoctoral fellow Keely CheslackPostava will be working on the development of autism in offspring of mothers exposed to environmental pollutants both under the mentorship of Alan Brown. In sum we continue to have the capacity to expose students to and mentor them in cutting edge research that has a biologic focus. A Life-Course Developmental Perspective. In addition to an emphasis that focuses “up” to contexts and “down” to biological determinants, our program strongly emphasizes a developmental psychopathology perspective – what might be considered a horizontal dimension. Both epidemiologists at large and our faculty have increasingly recognized the critical importance of understanding individual trajectories through time - ranging from prenatal and infant periods, through childhood and adolescence, into early, middle, and late adulthood - and how these trajectories are influenced by biological and social factors. Indeed, this developmental perspective applies not only to one generation but can be extended to multiple generations and to the idea that the life trajectory of one generation has impact of the trajectory of another. Many of the critical questions facing the field can only be answered with data that contain information collected across the life course (Kuh et al 2003). An appreciation of the power and necessity of these designs has led many of our program faculty to prospective longitudinal investigations of mental illnesses spanning multiple life-stages from in utero exposures to adult aging (e.g., Brown et al 2009) and across more than one generation (e.g.,Weissman 2006). In Brown’s studies in Finland, data from the early prenatal period through childhood development, adolescent neurocognition, and adult onset of schizophrenia will be investigated using systematically collected national registries and records. His team and collaborators in Finland are also leading studies on developmental trajectories that are associated with development of autism. A compelling rationale for focusing on a life course approach is evident in considering some obvious benefits of the approach. First, it allows the study of linkages between circumstances at one time and circumstances at another thereby deepening the understanding of the outcomes of interest. Second, it can capture the accumulation of adversity or advantage necessary to examine concepts like allostatic load (McEwen 1998) and weathering (Geronimus 2001). Third it facilitates the assessment of critical or sensitive times when specific exposures could have particularly strong impacts on mental health years or even decades later (Brown 2010). Fourth, when the design involves multiple generations or sibling pairs it becomes possible to address substantive questions such as the effect of one generation on the next (Cohen et al. 2006) or to refine causal inference through the use of sibling designs (Susser et al. 2006). Fifth the data structure that life course studies provide allows the use of methodological approaches such as fixed effects analyses (Allison 2005), latent growth curves, and growth mixture models that can improve causal inference and facilitate targeted tests of hypotheses. Finally, a life course approach is particularly congenial with our programs strong emphasis on integration across multiple levels of influence. Instead of forcing questions about biological or social influence into an either/or framework it allows us to assess, for example, how a genetic or biological circumstance early in life might lead to social consequences that then become new risks for the exacerbation of the condition in question or risks for entirely different disorders. Similarly social adversity can lead to biological consequences which then become risks for the development of disorder. For all of these reasons the life course/developmental psychopathology component remains central to our program and the training it provides. Over the past five years we have made substantial progress in incorporating an emphasis on the life course. Co-Director Pat Cohen’s study has continued to be the centerpiece in this regard. The 14 “children” in her study were born between 1964 and 1974 and now have children of their own. These and other data on this cohort are being used to study reciprocal effects of family members on one another over time. For example, former fellow Deidre Anglin used these data to study the impact of maternal separation and its duration on schizotypal symptoms much later in life (Anglin, Cohen and Chen 2008). A particularly novel set of findings from this study addressed Axis II disorders, the long term course of these disorders and their relationship with Axis I disorders (Crawford et al. 2008; Johnson et al. 2008). Cohen’s research has also introduced PET fellows to concepts and methods for studying how individual trajectories are influenced by individual and contextual factors (see Cohen et al. 2003). Her most recent work incorporates genetic data and will allow tests of the influence of genetic factors on trajectories of psychiatric disorders. A growing strength in the Epidemiology Department resides in the Imprints Center Directed by Ezra Susser and the newly established Life Course Cluster within Epidemiology that is also led by Susser. These overlapping groups carry out studies in birth cohorts across the globe, with the aim of deepening our understanding of the origins and unfolding of health and disease. The center brings together investigators who are conducting studies in 18 birth/pregnancy cohorts from 12 countries. Psychiatric outcomes over the life course are a primary focus of most of the studies. Past and current fellows have used these remarkable data to investigate the role of maternal fetal blood incompatibility, iron deficiency, living conditions and social statuses in the onset of mental disorders (as in the work of PET fellows Insel, March and Breshnahan). Susser, Brown and Link are each PI’s on separate grants focused on birth/pregnancy cohorts with psychiatric outcome measures thereby insuring excellent opportunities for new cohorts of PET fellows in this domain. One cohort of particular interest, and in which Susser and Bresnahan have played a central role, is the Norwegian MoBa study, a pregnancy cohort of 109,000 that is arguably the richest extant pregnancy cohort data set. They are collaborating closely with Norwegian investigators to study diagnostic outcomes such as autism spectrum disorders and ADHD as well as outcome domains such as cognition and language. A study of schizophrenia in this cohort is already being planned. Conceptual Integration – Dynamic Interplay Over the past ten years we have worked to incrementally expand our scope of vision “up” to contexts, “down” to biology and genetics, and “across” the life course. Our emphasis in the next five years is to further develop and integrate across these multiple levels. Specifically, our aim is to understand how different levels mutually influence one another. Over the past ten years we have been recruiting faculty and developing critical connections among them to allow such integration, selecting a multidisciplinary cohort of faculty and fellows interested in the spectrum of issues and bringing the people and ideas together in our faculty fellow seminar. Over time, faculty and fellows have come to recognize the explanatory power of each level of analysis, know enough about each level to identify instances in which it might be incorporated, and develop relationships with others who have expertise in these different levels. Over the next five years, we will solidify and integrate what we have learned. Spurring this integration are biological theories and research that are consistent with our multilevel framework. For example the work of Michael Meaney and Frances Champagne has shown that characteristics of the structure of DNA that do not alter the sequence of DNA can nevertheless have profound effect on the expression of genes through “epigenetic processes” (Meaney 1988; Champagne 2010). In this way environments can be involved in gene expression. Further work in this area is exploring the possibility of the “inheritance” of environmental effects through a process in which transmission occurs through a genetic mechanism that involves epigenetic changes to gene expression (Champagne, 2011). These insights open the possibility for deeper understanding of mutual influence between genes and stressful events, and between genes and contexts large and small thereby directly engaging the framework that serves as the basis of our rationale for training. 15 Another set of concepts and measures directs attention to the physiological effects of stress. One example in this domain is the work of McEwen (1998) and his ideas about allostatic load. The theory as specified by McEwen provides an explanation for how stress and adversity can have untoward physiological consequences that translate into pathophysiology, disease, and death. As currently operationalized through multiple biomarkers, measures of this concept have been reliably associated with low SES, social isolation, and neighborhood disadvantage (Bird et al. 2010, Dowd et al 2009 Seeman et al 2004). In another domain substantial effort has been directed toward investigating biological aging, weathering and similar concepts. The biomarker of telomere length has begun to produce a set of associations that is, like the patterns for allostatic load, suggestive of dynamic interplay between biological and social factors. Studies have found that telomere length is associated with child abuse, ambient stress in adult life and measures of socioeconomic status (Tyrka et al 2010, Cherkas 2006). Although these associations are not fully understood, they signal challenging connections between biology, adversity and context in a life course framework. These biologic theories, concepts and measures open an enormous opportunity for integration with social and psychological theories about the stress process (Dohrenwend 1998), social conditions (Link and Phelan 1995, 2010), child rearing (Johnson et al. 2006) and the timing of all of these factors across the life course (Susser et al 2006). We have begun to seek this kind of integration in our work. Sometimes we capitalize on the unusual opportunity afforded when, for example maternal sera have been stored and can be studied for evidence of fetal exposure to infection, nutritional deficiencies, or environmental toxins (Brown 2010). In other instances we have collected samples that will allow investigation of gene-environment interactions, as is the case with respect to the previously mentioned cohort that Patricia Cohen has been following since the mid 1970’s. In a study funded by NICHD, Link and Susser are currently collecting blood samples from individuals who are part of a pregnancy cohort initiated in the late 1950’s and early 1960’s. Influenced by the integrative multidisciplinary approach valued by PET, this work explicitly seeks to understand the dynamic interplay between social conditions (like SES), development (social and cognitive) and emerging health over the life course. Because measurements are available during pregnancy, early childhood, adolescence and now middle age the mutual influence of multiple levels can be observed over the life course. Finally, in an effort to advance an eco-epidemiologic approach that considers multiple levels of organization and how they relate through time, we propose an exploration of the utility of “complex systems approaches.” While traditional approaches to causal inference have great utility for addressing many types of causal problems, the dynamic interplay across levels and between individuals can be hidden by the requirements of standard methods (Galea et al. 2010; Schwartz et al. 2006). New approaches, grounded in systems thinking and most developed in infectious disease contexts, are being examined for their potential to overcome this limitation. This perspective relies on a metaphor of contagion, where the behavior of one individual can influence the behavior and outcomes of others (Galea 2010). Members of PET faculty are actively engaged in trying to understand the relationship between these newer methods and current epidemiologic approaches (e.g., Schwartz et al. 2011), and testing and validating these new methods for understanding population health (Galea et al. 2010). For example, Galea and colleagues (2009) have applied agent-based modeling, one type of a systems approach, to understand how environment and social networks shape drug dependence. This work provides trainees with exposure to and opportunity for research on and with these newly emerging methods. Implications of the Framework for Interventions The framework described above is particularly useful for guiding interventions designed to reduce the burden of pain and suffering associated with mental illnesses. Focus on multiple levels enhances 16 the possibility of developing interventions targeting different levels (contexts, individual behaviors, biological mechanisms) and, when appropriate, coordinating efforts across levels to achieve maximum impact. Our focus on life course will facilitate the development of knowledge relevant for determining the optimal timing of interventions in the life course. Finally our plan for integrating knowledge across levels will provide information critical to tailoring interventions and maximizing their effectiveness. Thus our scheme is designed to facilitate the translation of what we learn to effective action. Methodologies For Addressing Research Questions: Strategic Designs and Innovative Measures Our training approach stresses both the importance of asking theoretically grounded and important research questions and the use of rigorous and strategic methods to pursue answers. In what follows we draw on research conducted by faculty, some of which has already been mentioned above in regard to their substantive import, to exemplify the broad range of methodological approaches we seek to encourage. One approach involves testing theories using novel research designs that enhance causal inference. Prominent examples here are Dohrenwend and colleagues' use of a quasiexperimental strategy to test theoretically derived predictions about the classic social causation-social selection issue (Dohrenwend et al. 1992); Susser and colleagues (1996) use of the Dutch Famine Study to investigate the association between in-utero nutritional deprivation among pregnant women and the development of schizophrenia in their offspring and Schwartz et al.'s (1994) use of data on parental exposure to the Holocaust to test theories about the non-genetic familial transmission of mental disorder. This tradition of identifying circumstances that are, or come close to being natural experiments remains strong within our group. One example is Magdelena Cerda’s study of the introduction of a cable car to some poor isolated communities in Columbia South America and not to others. As an exogenous source of neighborhood change, the design allows strong inference with regard to the implications of such changes for violence and mental health. Similarly Galea harnesses large scale natural and man made circumstances (war, 9/11, hurricanes and earthquakes) to understand the role of stressful circumstances on psychopathology and Susser uses sibling designs to buttress causal inference in studies of the life course influences on mental health sites. An increasingly important study design entails testing ideas through long term follow-up studies of informative samples. A prominent example here is Pat Cohen and colleagues' follow-up of a community sample of over 800 children first assessed 30 years ago at age 6 -10. The maintenance of this cohort and multiple assessments of a broad range of risks, contexts, and functional measures provide strong data for illuminating developmental processes. Over 200 publications have resulted from this study thus far. The unique assessment of personality disorders since early adolescence has shown the strong risk of early personality disorders for future disorders and dysfunction in a wide range of areas (Crawford et al. 2008; Johnson et al. 2008). New work includes DNA assessment of genetic risks identified in other studies, an investigation of contexts that promote adaptive function in symptomatically vulnerable cohort members, illumination of the origins and meaning of diagnostic comorbidity, and continuity of problems over three generations. Each of these issues is investigated in a population-based longitudinal epidemiologic sample, and the availability of such data requires and enhances the development of novel and sophisticated analytic methods (e.g. Chen and Cohen 2006). Another example is Ezra Susser and Alan Brown’s follow-up of the offspring of mothers ascertained between 1959 and 1967 from the Kaiser Foundation Health plan in Alameda County CA. Using a nested case control design they conducted assays on the stored sera of mothers to show associations between indicators of influenza infection, lead exposure, and iron deficiency during gestation and schizophrenia in the offspring 30 to 38 years later (Brown et al. 2004a, 2004b, 2005). Building on this work, Susser (with colleague Stephen Buka as co-PI) subsequently led a multisite collaborative same sex sibling study named the Early Determinants of Health (EDAH). EDAH combined the Alameda county cohort with another large and comparable US cohort (The New England Family Study, 17 representing the Boston and Providence sites of the Collaborative Perinatal Project, also ascertained 1959 to 1966). EDAH encompassed not only neuropsychiatric outcomes, but also breast cancer and cardiovascular risk and fertility and other outcomes, and sought to identify common early causes (e.g. fetal inflammation for depression and cardiovascular disease) as well as differences in effects of early exposures (e.g. rapid fetal growth may increase risk of breast cancer but decrease risk of cardiovascular disease and depression) across these health domains. The first publications will appear later this year in a special issue of the Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease. Myrna Weissman and colleagues (Weissman et al 2004; 2006, Weissman and Olfson 2009, Warner et al 2008, Ritsher et al. 2001) combine a family study-high-risk design with a prospective longitudinal follow-up of three generations of families that were initially identified in 1982. This novel design provides opportunities to test ideas about the intergenerational transmission of disorders (Weissman et al. 2004), possible precursors to disorders such as the startle response (Warner et al 2008), the impact of involvement in treatment in one generation on the depression levels of the next generation assessed 20 years later (Weissman et al 2005) and social causation versus social selection processes with respect to the relationship between socioeconomic status and depression (Ritsher et al 2001). Notably none of these factors could have been studied as well outside of this rich design. Still other design approaches involve the translation of basic social science and epidemiologic understandings of problems into effective interventions. One example in this regard involves the work of Dan Herman and Ezra Susser on the Critical Time Intervention (CTI), which is designed to prevent recurrent homelessness among persons with severe mental illness by enhancing continuity of care during the transition from institutional to community living. Originally tested in an RCT with adult men discharged from a shelter-based psychiatry program, the CTI program was found not only to help people stay housed for longer but also to reduce negative symptoms for at least nine months after the end of the time limited nine month intervention (Susser et al. 1997; Herman et al. 2000). More recently Herman and colleagues (in press) evaluated the effectiveness of CTI in an RCT funded by NIMH and the New York State Office of Mental Health and found that it significantly reduced homelessness after discharge from hospital, again the effect persisted for at least nine months after the end of the intervention.) CTI is now being adapted and tested across the US and in many other countries for persons with mental illness (e.g. Netherlands, Australia, Brazil, UK), and for other kinds of transitions (e.g., from prison to community, from foster care to unsupported living, for the point of first contact with mental health services). Other faculty involved in intervention studies include work by Neugebauer and colleagues evaluating the effect of psychotherapy on women experiencing miscarriage (Neugebauer et al. 2007) and on youth in war torn Uganda (Bolton et al 2007) and Link and colleagues’ evaluation of outpatient commitment under Kendra’s law in New York State (Phelan et al. 2010; Link et al. 2011). We also emphasize the development of new measurement approaches that permit the investigation of previously intractable research problems. For example, Hasin developed the Psychiatric Research Interview for Substance and Mental Disorders (PRISM) to overcome difficulties in differentiating expected intoxication or withdrawal effects from symptoms of psychiatric illnesses. Results have shown considerably better test-retest reliability for the PRISM than for other widely-used diagnostic instruments (Hasin et al. 2006). Concerned about the adequacy of measures of environmental stress and the feasibility of gold standard approaches to capturing it, Dohrenwend and colleagues have developed narrative based approaches to rating life events and proposed a strategy for testing the validity of protocols that are less labor intensive (Dohrenwend 2006). The strategy for testing is currently under way. Finally, Link and Phelan have continued to develop measures related to mental illness stigma and discrimination in the context of two grants. In the first (an NIMH R21) they translate insights developed by social psychologists to capture race and gender prejudice (stigma consciousness, rejection sensitivity) for use among people with mental illnesses. In the second (NSF 18 funded), they develop implicit measures of mental illness prejudice in collaboration with “project implicit” at Harvard University. As foreshadowed above measures of biologic factors can also provide a rich source of innovative measurement when such measures are imported from their bench science origins into epidemiologic studies. When such measures are incorporated into epidemiological studies the capacity to test previously untestable ideas is greatly enhanced as the work of several faculty demonstrate. Implications for Training Clearly, to advance the field of psychiatric epidemiology, future researchers must be well versed in a broad range of methodologic and substantive issues and equipped with diverse research skills. These include first and foremost an emphasis on testing ideas through the novel application of theories, study designs, and measurement innovations as specified above. Doing this requires knowledge of epidemiologic methods, the construction and psychometric evaluation of measures, statistical analysis techniques, clinical diagnoses, the fundamentals of genetic, biological, psychological and sociological theories, concepts, and measures, and the ability to conceptualize these in a multilevel framework. As detailed below, the training vehicles consist of course work, field placements, and participation in the faulty-fellow seminar. These are designed to train pre- and post-doctoral fellows from a variety of disciplines to become conversant in all, and expert in some, of the areas mentioned above. Conclusion PET at Columbia is fortunate to have an exceptionally strong faculty who are pressing knowledge forward in multiple programs of research that provide superb contexts for training a new generation of scholars. The PET program organizes this resource via the conceptualization presented above that seeks to advance knowledge at multiple levels and integrate that knowledge through an understanding of the dynamic interplay between the levels. As an established program we seek to benefit from the wisdom that our long history provides but at the same time engage new ideas and flexibly change to address new pressing questions. As documented above our approach is strongly influenced by new developments and new emphases and our fellows have been productively situated within that context. Additionally, we have recruited new faculty both from within Columbia and from outside it to bring new ideas and new strengths to our program. Below, we describe the structure of our program, indicate how we adapt and change it over time and provide evidence regarding the strength of the research context and the success of fellows who have been trained in the program. 3. PROGRAM PLAN a. Program administration Program Director Bruce G. Link has been Director of the program since September 1995. His strong scientific background, intense involvement in ongoing research projects and commitment to teaching and mentoring are central to the success of the training program. His guidance and intellectual perspective permeates all aspects of the program. In addition to mentoring many of the PET students, Dr. Link provides the fellows’ introduction to the field in his course on Psychiatric Epidemiology. Dr. Link received his Ph.D. in Sociology from Columbia University in 1980, was trained in the Psychiatric Epidemiology Training Program at Columbia from 1976-1979, and received a B.S. in biostatistics in 1982 as an NIH funded post-doctoral fellow. He became the Training Coordinator of the Psychiatric 19 Epidemiology Training Program when he was appointed an Assistant Professor of Epidemiology in the School of Public Health at Columbia. He was promoted to Associate Professor of Public Health (with tenure) in 1988 when he assumed the role of Co-Director of the Training program; he became a full Professor in 1998. Dr. Link’s excellence in research and mentoring was recognized in 2002 when he was the recipient of three prestigious awards – the Leonard Pearlin Award for distinguished contributions to the Sociology of Mental Health, the Presidential Teaching Award at Columbia, and election to the Institute of Medicine. The multi-disciplinary character of the PET program is enhanced by Dr. Link’s joint appointments in the Departments of Epidemiology, Psychiatry, Socio-medical Sciences, and Sociology, and his directorships of the Center for Social Inequalities and Health and the RWJ Scholars Program. He expends 15% effort in his role as program director. Administrative structure: The program is administered by the program director in consultation with the co-directors, training coordinator and the steering committee. Co-Directors: Patricia Cohen, Bruce P. Dohrenwend, and Ezra Susser: The Co-Directors play a critical role in the program in three regards. First, they attend most of the weekly Faculty Fellow seminars whereas other faculty tend to select the ones in which they are most interested. They know all of the fellows in the program and are therefore a consistent resource for the fellows. Second, they each bring particular expertise in areas critical to the program-- Dr. Pat Cohen in design and analysis issues with a focus on the life course, Dr. Bruce Dohrenwend in theory, study design and the stress process, and Dr. Ezra Susser in public health interventions and the incorporation of biologic and genetic variables in a global context. Finally, they inform the program policies through regular meetings with the Director and Training Coordinator. All of the Co-directors serve as field placement preceptors and Steering Committee members. The extensive involvement of the co-directors provides breadth and depth to the available expertise, strengthening the program both intellectually and administratively. Training Coordinator: Sharon Schwartz: Dr. Schwartz is responsible for the day to day administration of the program including scheduling and chairing the weekly Faculty-Fellow Seminar, coordinating core program courses, advising fellows about courses, field placements and university resources, preparing administrative reports, conducting program and field placement evaluations, soliciting and monitoring applications to the program, and interacting with support staff concerning financial and other administrative issues. She also serves as a member of the Steering Committee and faculty. Beyond these concrete tasks the Training Coordinator is responsible for integrating the program components and facilitating connections among faculty and fellows to create and maintain a sense of identity and a vibrant research community. Dr. Schwartz expends considerable time and effort to ensure that fellows receive an optimal training experience. She continually monitors for necessary adjustments to meet the changing needs of each cohort. Her ability to do so is enhanced by departmental support for her teaching activities. For example, this support allows her to attend seminars on advances in pedagogy and mentoring to ensure that the program training techniques are as current and evidence based as the research methods being taught. She is a member of the Glenda Garvey Teaching Academy and the ASPH/Pfizer Health Academy of Distinguished Teachers. In 2000 she was awarded the Presidential Teaching Award at Columbia. The Program Director, Co-Directors and Training Coordinator meet on a regular basis to ensure that the program is running smoothly. They all have long-standing involvement with the program, providing it with a sense of leadership and continuity. The Steering Committee: Alan Brown, Deborah Hasin, Roberto Lewis-Fernandez, Richard Neugebauer, Myrna Weissman and the director, co-directors and training coordinator will continue to 20 serve on the Steering Committee. These Steering Committee members represent a broad range of disciplines including epidemiology, genetics, psychiatry, neurobiology and sociology. Karestan Koenen, a former PET fellow who has just returned to Columbia, will join the Steering Committee. She will strengthen the representation of genetics and epigenetics in programmatic decision making. The main functions of the Steering Committee are to help set policy, make admission decisions and provide program oversight. For example, the training program plan in this current application was developed in consultation with and with considerable input from the Steering Committee. Members of the Committee interview prospective candidates and make admissions decisions. They are consulted concerning such issues as the approval of new courses, the addition of new faculty, and any unusual difficulties regarding the fellows’ progress. Additionally, they serve as an advisory group suggesting, and helping to implement, changes in program content and functioning. The Steering committee almost always makes decisions via consensus. In the few instances when decisions cannot be made in this way, and routinely for the selection of incoming fellows, the committee members vote with each Committee member, including the Director and Co-Directors, having one vote. Support Staff: The program currently employs Brenda Scariff as a part-time administrative assistant. Her duties include administration of fellow stipend awards, tuition and travel expenses, maintenance of financial records, arrangement of meetings, interfacing with the university bureaucracy on behalf of the fellows and general office management. She works closely with the Training Coordinator and administrative personnel in the Department of Epidemiology to ensure that the program runs smoothly. The work of Ms. Scariff enhances the fellows experience by buffering them from potentially time consuming administrative tasks and through their general support. b. Program Faculty Table 1 documents the large number of faculty in the participating departments and the number of these faculty members associated with our training program. All PET program faculty (shown in Table 2) are available to supervise fellows in field placements on an on-going basis and present their work at seminars throughout the year. Some faculty members also teach courses for the program. Faculty members continuing in that role since the last grant period (in addition to those serving on the Steering Committee) are. Mindy Fullilove, Madelyn Gould, Dan Herman, Susan Hodge, Christina Hoven, Denise Kandel, Mark Olfson, Ruth Ottman, Jo Phelan and J. Blake Turner. We have also added new members to the faculty to ensure that we have the expertise necessary to in the areas that we wish to consolidate and develop during the current grant period, as described in the Background. The new faculty, with their areas of expertise in parentheses, are: Paul Appelbaum (law and ethics), Lisa Bates (social epidemiology), Magdalena Cerda (global, innovative methods), Cheryl Corcoran (biological psychiatry), Sandro Galea (global, cross-level, PTSD), Kimberly Nobel (development, neuro-cognition), Nicole Schupf (genetics, neurobiology), Larry Yang (schizophrenia prodrome and global). The faculty members therefore represent the full range of disciplines and expertise necessary for the integrated approached outlined in the Background section. In selecting PET faculty we have attempted to consolidate our current strengths while keeping abreast of new developments that need to be integrated for an “eco-epidemiology” approach. While each fellow cannot gain expertise in all these areas, they will be exposed to the different frameworks, concepts and terminology to prepare them to engage in inter-disciplinary work. In the current grant, we have augmented our faculty with an eye to enhancing representation of more “down-stream” disciplines, especially neurobiology and genetics, and the upstream global context. 21 As indicated in Tables 3 and 4, the faculty have a wide range of grant and contract support which provides a rich array of research opportunities for the fellows. As indicated in Tables 5a and 5b the faculty members have mentored a large number of students from these grants, including trainees on this T32, indicating their ability to appropriately mentor our trainees. As discussed in the Progress Report, all of the current trainees are actively engaged in research with faculty. One measure of the productivity of these research activities is indicated in Tables 6a and 6b which list the publications for trainees that derived directly from their fellowship research projects. The 15 predoctoral fellows supported by the program over the past 10 years produced almost 70 papers directly tied to research conducted during their tenure in the program (usually 5 years). Our 25 postdoctoral fellows have also produced almost 70 papers derived directly from their research in the program, although their tenure in the program is generally only two or three years. The modal number of papers is 2 per fellow (including fellows who have not yet completed their tenure in the program). Many of these papers appear in prestigious journals across a wide range of disciplines, for example the Journal of Health, Social Behavior, Social Science and Medicine, American Journal of Public Health, American Journal of Epidemiology, Archives of General Psychiatry and American Journal of Psychiatry. This level of productivity mirrors the career trajectories of the majority of our fellows most of who go on to have exceptionally productive research careers (see Progress Report). Because of the high level of collaboration among the faculty, trainees are afforded varied but integrated mentoring on their research. For example nearly 20% of the fellows’ publications deriving from their field placements include more than one PET faculty member as a co-author (these publications are noted with two asterisks (**). c. Proposed Training Program Components Our program consists of four distinct but interrelated components integrated by a fifth component, the Faculty-Fellow Seminar. Outlined below are the core elements of the program required of all fellows. Predoctoral fellows take additional coursework to fulfill the requirements of the Ph.D. program in epidemiology. The doctoral curriculum has been recently overhauled with the goal of creating more epidemiologic methods courses offered only to the doctoral students, providing more training in professional skills (e.g., grant writing, publications) and more emphasis on and time for actual research projects. Component I: Coursework in Epidemiology: The goal of this component is to introduce fellows to key issues and concepts in psychiatric epidemiology. Knowledge in these areas is imparted by coursework in epidemiologic methods and substantive issues in psychiatric epidemiology. Epidemiologic methods are covered in a three course sequence - Principles of Epidemiology I, Design and Conduct of Chronic Disease Epidemiological Studies (Epidemiology II), and Principles of Epidemiology III. PET faculty members (Galea, Susser) participate in the teaching of Epidemiology I. The second course is taught by a PET faculty member (Schwartz). Substantive issues in psychiatric epidemiology are addressed in two courses designed specifically for PET fellows (although open to all students) and taught by program faculty. The first course, Psychiatric Epidemiology, is taught by the Program Director, Bruce Link; the second course, Biologic and Genetic Issues in Psychiatric Epidemiology, is a reading seminar taught by Alan Brown. This sequence familiarizes the fellows with perspectives on psychiatric disorders from social, through 22 individual, neurologic and genetic factors, to reflect the knowledge base required for our ecoepidemiologic approach. For fellows lacking social science training, courses are recommended in Sociology and Social Psychology in GSAS downtown and in the Department of Socio-medical Sciences at the School of Public Health. For fellows with minimal biologic training, courses are available in the various departments of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences as well as the School of Nursing. In addition, a consortium agreement allows fellows to take courses at other universities in the New York Metropolitan area. This has allowed fellows to find courses that meet their specific interests, and to have contact with experts outside of Columbia. Component II: Coursework in Clinical Psychiatry: Knowledge in this domain is imparted by a course designed for fellows without clinical experience. It includes an introduction to the major types of psychopathology, DSM-IV (soon to be DSM-V) criteria, the clinical interview and a survey of the major research instruments used to detect disorders in community and patient populations. The course is taught by Dr. Larry Amsel, a psychiatrist and diagnostician who is the Assistant Director for Education Training for Columbia Presbyterian Hospital Inpatient Unit. Dr. Amsel has been successfully teaching this course for the past 10 years; it is exceptionally well received and critically important for epidemiologists who need to know about the clinical phenomena they study. Component III: Coursework in Statistics: The goal of this component is to ensure that fellows have sufficient background in measurement and statistics to conduct quantitative research and to work productively with biostatisticians. Among the core courses taken are: Introduction to Biostatistics, Problems in Measurement. Categorical Data Analysis, and Applied Regression Analysis. Courses are also offered in Statistical Genetics taught by a PET faculty member (Hodge). Fellows are directed to go beyond these basic courses to master more advanced topics required for their research. Such coursework can be found in biostatistics and in the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences. Component IV: Field Placements: This component provides fellows with hands-on supervised research experience with an established researcher. Fellows participate in field placements for 12 to 30+ hours per week depending on stage in the program and whether the fellow is pre or postdoctoral. For example, pre-doctoral fellows in the first two years of coursework, average at the lower end of involvement whereas postdoctoral fellows in their final year typically engage full time in their research. As mentioned above, time allotted for research will be expanded for predoctoral fellows under the new curriculum. The program has developed specific procedures to ensure that fellows are acquainted with the full range of available field placements and to help them select a research placement that meets their needs. First, each fellow meets with the training coordinator at the beginning of each semester to review their short and long term training goals (see Progress Report). The trainees are given guidance about the types of research experiences that would help them achieve their goals. Second, the fellows receive a written description of the research conducted by each faculty member that they can use to familiarize themselves with placement possibilities and use as a basis for asking questions to faculty about those possibilities. Third, at the beginning of each academic year, a faculty-fellow seminar is set aside for each faculty member to give a short presentation of the research placement possibilities he/she can provide. The fellows are encouraged to speak with multiple faculty members before deciding on a field placement. In order to foster communication with faculty about field placement expectations, we have developed a detailed set of guidelines outlining areas that fellows and faculty should discuss to ensure that fellows’ needs are met (see Progress Report and Appendix Item 1). The protocol includes issues such as the frequency of meetings, method of feedback, project scope, and authorship. We have developed and revised this set of guidelines over the years to minimize 23 miscommunication and maximize the chances for a productive field placement experience. By all accounts it has been successful but should this proactive approach fail, input from the Training Coordinator, Director and Co-Directors can be accessed with respect to any problems that arise. Typically, fellows formulate and develop their initial research projects using data collected by the preceptor and his/her team. Fellows are expected to take primary responsibility for the project they develop in consultation and collaboration with their preceptors and research colleagues. Fellows are expected to be first author on papers they develop and co-authors when they collaborate on papers they did not initiate. In addition, fellows participate in staff meetings to develop collegial associations and to become aware of the full range of substantive and technical issues involved in carrying out studies in psychiatric epidemiology. As fellows near completion of their training they are encouraged to develop their ideas into independent grant proposals. Fellows also often augment their central projects with short-term field placements designed for training in specific skills such as field work and questionnaire development. Component V: Faculty-Fellow Seminar: The Faculty-Fellow Seminar serves many functions. It provides a forum for collegial interactions and the development of professional networks; an arena for fellows to present results of their research and receive feedback and an opportunity to keep abreast of recent developments in the field. Perhaps the most important function of the seminar is to integrate the program by providing a time and place for program participants to meet on a regular basis. The common experience of hearing and discussing the same material facilitates the development of intellectual ties between faculty and fellows and among the fellows themselves. The seminars are either presentations of ongoing research by fellows, faculty or guest speakers or workshops on special topics. While presentations by faculty and fellows provide the backbone of the Faculty-Fellow Seminar, an effort is made to recruit speakers and develop workshops on topics and approaches that complement those fellows are exposed to through program courses and faculty (see Appendix Item 2 for a list of the seminars for the current grant period). Recent changes in the structuring of the epidemiology department have enhanced the interdisciplinarity of the seminars and our ability to expose fellows to a broader range of research topics. The department is now organized into “clusters”, an identified group of researchers with overlapping research interests. The PET training program is in the Neuro-Psych cluster. This is an attempt to integrate the neurological sciences most closely relevant to psychiatric disorders into psychiatric research. One faculty-fellow seminar a month has been designated as a “cluster seminar” where attendees and topics expand psychiatric disorders to include consideration of disorders that are considered “neurologic” but that clearly overlap those delineated in the DSM. This cluster seminar expands the trainees’ exposure to biologic issues. Additional Training Activities: In the past, the program tried to meet the fellows’ requests for training in career development skills. The epidemiology department now recognizes this need as well and has integrated this aspect of training into the departmental doctoral curriculum. For example, there is now a course devoted to training in giving presentations, writing grants and papers. This changed curriculum will enhance the experience of the PET pre-doctoral trainees. Columbia also provides a vast number of seminars and conferences each week on a wide array of topics of interest to the fellows. Fellows frequently attend Psychiatric Grand Rounds, Child Psychiatry Rounds, Imprint Center Seminars (lifecourse epidemiology), the Seminar in Socio-medical Sciences and talks sponsored by the HIV Center, the Robert Wood Johnson Society Scholar’s Program (headed by Bruce Link), the Center for Social Inequalities in Health (headed by Bruce Link and Jo Phelan) as well as monthly Epidemiology Grand Rounds and Public Health Grand Rounds. The School of Public Health recently launched a Research Resources Initiative to provide training, guidance and practical 24 help for faculty and post-doctoral fellows in grant activities. In addition to one-on-one help, they provide a wealth of seminars, workshops and training programs on issues related to research and grant writing. Our fellows have been eager participants in all these activities. Examples of Fellows' Programs To indicate how the training components are integrated into an individual fellow's program, we provide three examples (see Table A). The first is typical of a pre-doctoral fellow seeking a Ph.D. in epidemiology with a specialization in psychiatric epidemiology. The second is typical of a postdoctoral fellow from a social science discipline who has had considerable training in methodology but no clinical training. Finally, we offer an example of the program of a post-doctoral fellow who is a psychiatrist with considerable clinical expertise but little if any methodological training. The main difference between the program of the social scientist and the psychiatrist is that the social scientist takes the full clinical sequence while the psychiatrist takes the full methods sequence. The proposed training outlined in Table A is a reflection of the main training activities of the past 5 years and as well as proposed changes for the new funding period. These changes are planned in response to trainee suggestions, faculty interests, developments in psychiatry epidemiology, and the new directions that have evolved from serious reappraisal of the doctoral program in the department of epidemiology. Table A Example of Fellows’ Course Program Predoctoral Fellow – Ph.D. Candidate in Epidemiology Spring Year 1 History of Epidemiology Publications, Presentations and Grants Critical Thinking in Epidemiology (Epi IV) Applications of Epi Research Methods II Biology and Physiology for Epidemiologists Applied Regression II Psychiatric Epidemiology Clinical Seminar in Psychiatry Faculty-Fellow Seminar Faculty-Fellow Seminar Year 2 Concepts in Causal Inference (Epi V) Advanced Topics in Epidemiologic Methods (Epi VI) Biological Aspects of Psychiatric Disorders Responsible Conduct of Research Faculty-Fellow Seminar Faculty-Fellow Seminar Electives Electives Year 3 Qualifying Exams Proposal Faculty-Fellow Seminar Faculty-Fellow Seminar Years 4&5 Dissertation Work Fall Postdoctoral Fellow – Social Scientist Year 1 Psychiatric Epidemiology Analysis of Categorical Data Clinical Seminar in Psychiatric Disorders Design and Conduct of Epidemiologic Studies Principles of Epidemiology Faculty –Fellow Seminar Faculty-Fellow Seminar 25 Year 2 Principles of Epidemiology (3) Biological Aspects of Psychiatric Disorders Faculty-Fellow Seminar Selected Problems in Measurement Responsible Conduct in Research Faculty-Fellow Seminar Year 3 Faculty-Fellow Seminar Electives as needed Faculty-Fellow Seminar Electives as needed Postdoctoral Fellow - Psychiatrist Fall Spring Year 1 Psychiatric Epidemiology Introduction to Biostatistics Principles of Epidemiology Faculty-Fellow Seminar Analysis of Categorical Data Design and Conduct of Epidemiologic Studies Faculty –Fellow Seminar Year 2 Principles of Epidemiology (3) Applied Linear Regression Faculty-Fellow Seminar Social Science Methods as needed Selected Problems in Measurement Responsible Conduct in Research Faculty-Fellow Seminar Year 3 Faculty-Fellow Seminar Faculty-Fellow Seminar Duration of Training As the sample programs described in Table A indicate, we believe that postdoctoral fellows benefit most from three years of training whereas pre-doctoral fellows need a minimum of five years to complete their Ph.D. degrees. While postdoctoral fellows can complete the majority of their coursework in two years, a third year devoted full-time to their field placement affords them the opportunity to integrate and consolidate what they have learned. It also provides an opportunity to launch their research careers by giving presentations at professional meetings, writing papers, developing a publication record and if appropriate and desired writing a K-Award or other grant. Pre-doctoral fellows need two to two and one half years to complete course work, six to nine months to complete qualifying exams and two years to complete a doctoral dissertation. Guiding Fellows through the Program and Monitoring their Progress Typically, the process of guiding fellows begins before acceptance into the program. When fellows first apply they are sent material about course offerings, research opportunities, program requirements and program rules. During the interviewing process gaps in their academic background (e.g., methodology) are discussed with suggestions for appropriate courses. Potential research opportunities are discussed with faculty members who may serve as future field placement preceptors. Upon acceptance into the program, fellows meet with the Training Coordinator and the Director to discuss their goals during their tenure in the program and the combination of courses, field placement and other training activities that would meet these goals. Field placements are discussed at this initial meeting and fellows are advised to contact a range of potential preceptors during their first semester in the program. The Training Coordinator meets formally with each fellow at the beginning of each 26 semester to discuss their progress in the program guided by an assessment tool developed to help the fellows evaluate their training needs and the program’s ability to meet those needs. Informally, the location of the Training Coordinator’s office, next to that of the fellows’, facilitates ongoing daily contact with the fellows. Once a fellow has chosen his/her program and field placement, progress is monitored in a number of ways. First, each fellow has a field placement preceptor who takes responsibility for this component of the fellows program. Any difficulties are brought to the attention of the Training Coordinator; discussions with fellows are initiated immediately upon detection of any problems. A systematic and formal review of the field placement is done each spring when fellows and their preceptors complete an evaluation form designed to assess the fellows' progress in and satisfaction with their field placement (see Appendix Item 3). Progress is also monitored through fellows' presentations at the Faculty-Fellow Seminars. These presentations are perhaps the most direct opportunity that faculty have for monitoring fellows' progress. Faculty and fellows alike tend to be forthright in identifying problems in the research presented at the seminar during the discussion section of the seminar. After each presentation, the Training Coordinator meets with the fellow to debrief. These discussions include substantive issues raised at the seminar as well as presentation style. The fellow is also advised to meet with the faculty members who had the most serious difficulties with the research presented. Academic progress is monitored in part from an assessment of fellows' course grades. For predoctoral fellows, academic progress is also monitored through the advisor system of the fellow's degree program. Any severe academic difficulties are brought to the attention of the Steering Committee where a plan can be formulated to attempt to improve the situation. Typically, however, fellows are exceptionally fine students, so that we rarely have to confront such issues. Post-doctoral fellows are formally evaluated in the fall semester of their second year in the program. All fellows write a proposal outlining the research they plan to do in their third year of training. In January, the Steering Committee meets to evaluate the projects for evidence of progress in the program and a productive third year plan. Fellows who are not making progress toward developing research skills are not offered a third year of funding. d. Training Program Evaluation In addition to monitoring fellows' progress, we also have procedures for monitoring participants' satisfaction with the program. Written forms have been developed for evaluating courses, field placements, and the overall program on a yearly basis (see Appendix Items 3 and 4). The evaluations are reviewed and summarized by the Training Coordinator, who distributes the results to the faculty and fellows. The Program Director, Co-Directors and Training Coordinator, in consultation with the fellows, develop plans to address any difficulties raised in the evaluations. These potential solutions are distributed to the faculty and fellows for their comments. To augment these formal, anonymous, evaluations, fellows and faculty are encouraged to offer their opinions and suggestions about all facets of the program. Where possible, alterations are made in the program to rectify any problems indicated. 27 Table 2. Participating Faculty Members (Alphabetically by Faculty Member) Name/Degree(s) Rank Primary (& Secondary) Appointment(s) Role in Program Research Interest Appelbaum, Paul, M.D. Professor Elizabeth K. Dollard Prof. of Field Psychiatry, Medicine, and Placement Law; Director, Division of Preceptor Psychiatry, Law & Ethics, Psychiatry, P&S, NYSPI; Director of the Center for Research on the Ethical, Legal & Social Implications of Genetic Research Legal and ethical issues that impact psychiatric and general medical practice and research. In particular he has examined involuntary commitment and treatment; informed consent and decisional capacity; prediction and management of violence by people with mental illness; and the ethics of research. Bates, Lisa, Assistant Professor Epidemiology and Population Field and Family Health, MSPH Placement Preceptor The effects of social context on health, especially the impacts of immigration and gender both domestically and internationally Brown, Alan S., M.D., Professor M.P.H. Psychiatry, P&S, NYSPI and Field Department of Epidemiology, Placement MSPH Preceptor; teaches the Biological Psychiatry Course Biomarkers and risk of major psychiatric disorders. His work focuses on infectious, inflammatory, nutritional, hormonal, and toxic exposures. Cerda, Magdalena, Ph.D. Assistant Professor Columbia University, MSPH The social epidemiology of risk behaviors and psychiatric disorders, particularly violence, substance abuse and depression. Her current work looks at individual, family, peer and neighborhood influences on common psychiatric disorders and substance abuse using innovative methods. Corcoran, Cheryl, M.D. Assistant Professor Dept. of Psychiatry, Columbia Field University Placement Psychiatrist, NYSPI Preceptor Field Placement Preceptor Understanding prodromal conditions of psychiatric disorders. Focuses on ascertainment, characterization and longitudinal evaluation of prodromal patients; biomarkers of risk; relationship between cannabis use and schizophrenia. 28 Name/Degree(s) Rank Primary (& Secondary) Appointment(s) Role in Program Research Interest Dohrenwend, Bruce, Ph.D. Professor Epidemiology, MSPH; Department of Psychiatry; Columbia University & College of Physicians & Surgeons; Chief, Div. of Social Psychiatry, NYSPI Co-Director Steering Committee Field Placement Preceptor Understanding how adversity and stress are related to psychiatric disorders that vary in rate with social positions defined by gender, racial/ethnic status, and socioeconomic status. Particular focus on warrelated post-traumatic stress disorder. Fullilove, Mindy Thompson, M.D. Professor Psychiatry, Columbia University & College of Physicians & Surgeons; Socio-Medical Sciences, MSPH Field Placement Preceptor Mental health problems of inner city communities. She has followed a series of epidemics, including AIDS, crack cocaine addiction, and violence, and has examined a series of urban processes, such as displacement because of urban renewal. Galea, Sandro, M.D., Professor MPH, Dr.PH Anna Cheskis Gelman & Field Murray Charles Gelman Placement Professor and Chair, Dept. of Preceptor Epidemiology, CU, MSPH Psychiatric consequences of community traumatic events; epidemiology of drug and alcohol abuse. Development of methods that use systems approaches. Global mental health. Gould, Madelyn, Ph.D. Professor Psychiatry, Columbia University, P&S Epidemiology: MSPH Field Placement Preceptor Risk factors for suicidal behaviors and suicide in children and adolescents Hasin, Deborah, Ph.D. Professor Psychiatry, College of Physicians & Surgeons; Epidemiology, MSPH Steering Committee; Field Placement Preceptor Epidemiology of alcohol use disorders. Measurement and definitional issues and gene/environment interactions. Hatzenbuehler, Ph.D. Assistant Professor Sociomedical Sciences, Columbia University Field Placement Preceptor Examines the consequences of social policies that differentially target gays and lesbians for social exclusion (e.g., same-sex marriage laws, employment policies) affect the mental health of lesbian, gay and bisexual populations. Hoven, Christina, Dr.PH, MPH Psychiatry, College of Physicians & Surgeons; Epidemiology, MSPH Field Placement Preceptor Consequences of WTC attacks on NYC school children; psychiatric status of children of first responders in NYC and Israel. Associate Professor 29 Name/Degree(s) Kandel, Denise, Ph.D. Rank Professor Primary (& Secondary) Appointment(s) Role in Program Research Interest Socio-medical Sciences, Field MSPH; Psychiatry, College of Placement Physicians & Surgeons; Preceptor Epidemiology, MSPH Risk factors for nicotine dependence and other substance use disorders in adolescents; prenatal exposures and trajectories. Keyes, Katherine M., Assistant Ph.D. Professor Epidemiology, Columbia University Field Placement Preceptor Life course processes that influence the development and chronicity of psychiatric disorders, with a particular focus on novel methods to achieve more robust inference from observational epidemiological studies. Karestan,Koenen Ph.D. Associate Professor Epidemiology, MSPH Steering Committee, Field Placement Preceptor Epigenetic changes, specifically methylation, as a potential mechanism underlying the association between the built environment and racial discrimination and black-white disparities in substance dependence. Lifecourse epidemiologic approach to examine the interplay of genetic and environmental factors in the production of adverse health behavior, mental disorders, PTSD and chronic physical disease. Lewis-Fernandez, Roberto, M.D. Professor Psychiatry, Columbia College Steering of Physicians & Surgeons; Committee; NYSPI Field Placement Preceptor Developing clinical interventions to help overcome disparities in the care of underserved US cultural groups. Understanding the way culture affects individuals’ experience of mental disorder and their help-seeking expectations Epidemiology and Sociomedical Sciences, MSPH; NYSPI Causes and consequences of psychiatric disorders. The linkages between SES and physical and mental health from a “fundamental cause perspective”; stigma among people with psychiatric disorders and the bases for violent behavior among people with psychiatric disorders. Link, Bruce G., Ph.D. Professor Program Director; Steering Committee; Field Placement Preceptor 30 Name/Degree(s) Rank Primary (& Secondary) Appointment(s) Role in Program Research Interest Monk, Catherine E., Ph.D. Associate Professor Psychiatry, Obstetrics & Gynecology, NYS Psychiatric Institute, Columbia University Field Placement Preceptor Research that straddles developmental neuroscience and perinatal psychiatry. The organizing focus of her research is development and the transmission of risk for psychopathology from one generation to the next Neugebauer, Richard, Ph.D. Research Scientist NYSPI Field Placement Preceptor Posttraumatic stress reactions among Rwandan children and adolescents in the early aftermath of genocide. Noble, Kimberly, M.D., Ph.D. Assistant Professor Pediatrics, Sergievsky Center, Columbia University Field Placement Preceptor Cognitive neuroscience approaches to socioeconomic status influences on child development; the neurobiology of reading impairment in SES diverse children using functional magnetic resonance imaging Olfson, Mark, M.D., M.P.H. Professor Psychiatry, Columbia College Field of Physicians & Surgeons Placement Preceptor Developmentally specific and potentially modifiable predictors of the onset and course of psychiatric and substance use disorders Ottman, Ruth, Ph.D. Professor Epidemiology, Sergievsky Center, MSPH Field Placement Preceptor Genetic epidemiology with a focus on the role of inherited factors in susceptibility to complex disorders; etiology of epilepsy Phelan, Jo, Ph.D. Professor Socio-medical Sciences, MSPH Field Placement Preceptor Stigma associated with mental illness, particularly for genetic etiologic attributions; inequalities in psychiatric disorders Schupf, Nicole, Professor Ph.D., M.P.H., Dr.PH Taub Institute for Research Field on Alzheimer’s Disease & the Placement Aging Brain, Epidemiology, Preceptor MSPH Etiology of Alzheimer’s disease; genetic factors, relationship with Down’s syndrome Schwartz, Sharon, Ph.D. Epidemiology, MSPH Impact of methodology on study results, differences across disciplines. Inequalities in health Professor Training Coordinator; Steering Committee; Field Placement Preceptor 31 Name/Degree(s) Rank Primary (& Secondary) Appointment(s) Role in Program Research Interest Susser, Ezra, M.D., Dr.P.H. Professor Epidemiology, MSPH; Psychiatry, College of Physicians and Surgeons; NYSPI Co-Director; Steering Committee; Field Placement Preceptor Lifecourse influences on psychiatric disorders; geneenvironment interplay in psychiatric disorders, global mental health Turner, J. Blake, Ph.D. Assistant Professor Socio-medical Sciences, MSPH; Research Scientist, NYSPI Field Placement Preceptor Etiology of childhood psychiatric disorders Wall, Melanie M., Ph.D. Professor Biostatistics & Psychiatry, NYS Psychiatric Institute, Columbia University Field Placement Preceptor Modeling complex multilevel and multivariate data on a wide array of psychosocial public health and psychiatric research projects. She is an expert in longitudinal data analysis and latent variable modeling, including structural equation modeling focused on mediating and moderating (Interaction) effects. Weissman, Myrna, Ph.D. Professor Chief, Div. of Epidemiology, NYSPI; Epidemiology, MSPH; Psychiatry , College of Physicians and Surgeons Steering Committee; Field Placement Preceptor Etiology and treatment of depression. Interpersonal psychotherapy, genetic epidemiology Columbia University / Mailman School of Public Health Field Placement Preceptor Effects of culture and stigma on the etiology and course of psychiatric disorders; global mental health; immigrant status and psychiatric disorders Yang, Lawrence H., Ph.D. 32 REFERENCES 1. Agerbo, E., Byrne, M.. Eaton, W.W., and Mortensen, P.B. (2004) Marital and labor market status in the long run in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 61:28-33. PMID: 14706941 2. Allison, P.D. (2005) Fixed Effects Regression Methods for Longitudinal Data Using SAS. Cary, NC, SAS Institute. 3. Anglin, D. M., Cohen, P, & Chen, H. (2008). Duration of early maternal separation and prediction of schizotypal symptoms from early adolescence to midlife. Schizophrenia Research, 103, 143-150 4. Bird, C.E., Seeman, T., Escarce, J.J., Basurto-Davila, R., Finch, B.K., Heron, M., Merkin, S.S., Weden, M, Lurie, N. (2010) Neighbourhood socioeconomic status and biological ‘wear and tear’ in a nationally representative sample of US adults. J Epidemiol Community Health. 64(10):860-5. PMID: 19759056 5. Birnbaum, H.G., Kessler, R.C., Kelley, D., Ben-Hamadi, R., Joish, V.N., and Greenberg, Chen P.E. (2010) Title Employer burden of mild, moderate, and severe major depressive disorder: mental health services utilization and costs, and work performance. Depression and Anxiety Journal. 27(1):78-89. PMID: 19569060 6. Bolton, P., Bass, J., Betancourt, T., Speelman, L., Onyango, G., Clougherty, K.F., Neugebauer, R., Murray, L., and Verdeli, H. (2007) Interventions for depression symptoms among adolescent survivors of war and displacement in northern Uganda: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 298(5):519-27. PMID: 17666672 7. Boyce, W.T. and Ellis, B.J. (2005) Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology. 17 (2): 271-301. PMID: 16761546 8. Brown, A., Hooton, J., Schaefer, C.A., Zhang, H., Petkova, E., Babulas, V., Perrin, M., Gorman, J.M., and Susser, E.S. (2004a) Elevated maternal interleukin-8 levels and risk of schizophrenia in adult offspring. American Journal of Psychiatry. 161:889-895. PMID: 15121655 9. Brown, A.S., Begg, M.D., Gravenstein, S., Schaefer, C.A., Wyatt, R.J., Bresnahan, M., Babulas, V.P., and Susser, E. (2004b) Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the etiology of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 61(8):774-780. PMID: 15289276 10. Brown, A.S., Schaefer, C.A., Quesenberry, C.P. Jr., Liu, L., Babulas, V.P., and Susser, E.S. (2005) Maternal exposure to toxoplasmosis and risk of schizophrenia in adult offspring. American Journal of Psychiatry. 162(4):767-73. PMID: 15800151 11. Brown, A.S.,Vinogradov, S., Kremen, W.S., Poole, J.H., Deicken, R.F., Penner, J.D., McKeague, I.W., Kochetkova, A., Kern, D., and Schaefer, C.A. (2009) Prenatal infection and executive dysfunction in adult schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 166(6):683-690. PMID: 19369317 PMCID: PMC2885160 12. Brown, A.S., Vinogradov, S., Kremen, W.S., Poole, J.H., Deicken, R.F., Penner, J.D., McKeague, I.W., Kochetkova, A., Kern, D., and Schaefer, C.A. (2009) Prenatal exposure to maternal infection and executive dysfunction in adult schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 166(6):683-90. PMID: 19369317 PMCID: PMC2885160 13. Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Milne, B., Amell, J.W., Theodore, R.F., and Moffitt, T.E. (2003) Children's behavioral styles at age 3 are wed to their adult personality traits at age 26. J Pers. 71(4):495-513. PMID: 12901429 14. Champagne, F.A. (2010) Early Adversity and Developmental Outcomes: Interaction Between Genetics, Epigenetics, and Social Experiences Across the Life Span. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 5:564-574. 15. Champagne, F.A. (2011) Maternal imprints and the origins of variation. Horm Behav. Abstract. PMID: 21376726 16. Chang, V.W. and Lauderdale D.S. (2009) Fundamental Cause Theory, Technological Innovation, and Health Disparities: The Case of Cholesterol in the Era of Statins. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 50(3):245–60. PMID: 19711804 PMCID: PMC2885132 33 17. Chen, H. and Cohen, P. (2006). Using individual growth model to analyze the change in quality of life from adolescence to adulthood. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 4:10. PMID: 16504035 PMCID: PMC1397823 18. Cherkas, L.F., Aviv, A., Valdes, A.M., Hunkin, J.L., Gardner, J.P., Surdulescu, G.L., Kimura, M., and Spector, T.D. (2006) The effects of social status on biological aging as measured by white-blood-cell telomere length. Aging Cell. 5(5):361-5. PMID: 16856882 19. Collins, P.Y., Holman, A.R., Freeman, M.C., and Patel V. (2006) What is the relevance of mental health to HIV/AIDS care and treatment programs in developing countries? A systematic review. AIDS. 20(12): 1571-82. PMID: 16868437 PMCID: PMC2801555 20. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S.G., and Aiken, L.S. (2003) Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences Lawrence Earlbaum Associates: Mahwah NJ. 21. Crawford, T.N., Cohen, P., First, M.B., Skodol, A.E., Johnson, J.G., and Kasen, S. (2008) Comorbid Axis I and Axis II disorders in early adolescence: outcomes 20 years later. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 65(6):641-8. PMID: 18519822 22. Cuthbert, B.N. and Insel, T.R. (2010) Toward New Approaches to Psychotic Disorders: The NIMH Research Domain Criteria Project. Schizophr Bull. 36(6):1061-2. PMID: 20929969 PMCID: PMC2963043 23. Demyttenaere, K., et al. WHO World Mental Health Survey Consortium. (2004) Prevalence, Severity, and Unmet Need for Treatment of Mental Disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. JAMA. 291(21):2581-2590. PMID: 15173149 24. DiGrande, L., Neria, Y., Brackbill, R.M., Pulliam, P., and Galea S. (2011) Long-term posttraumatic stress symptoms among 3,271 civilian survivors of the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks on the World Trade Center. American Journal of Epidemiology. 173:271-81. PMID: 21190987 25. Dohrenwend, B.P., Levav, I., Shrout, P.E., Schwartz, S., Naveh, G., Link, B.G., Skodol, A.E., and Stueve, A. (1992). Socioeconomic status and psychiatric disorders: the causation selection issue. Science. 255(5047):94652. PMID: 1546291 26. Dohrenwend, B.P. Editor (1998) Adversity, Stress and Psychopathology. Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Press. 27. Dohrenwend, B.P. (2000) The role of adversity and stress in psychopathyology: some evidence and its implication for theory and research. J Health Soc Behav. 41(1):1-19. PMID: 10750319 28. Dohrenwend, B.P. (2006) Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: Toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychological Bulletin. 132(3):477-95. PMID: 16719570 PMCID: PMC1584216 29. Dohrenwend, B.P., Turner, J.B., Turse, N.A., Adams, B.G., Koenen, K.C. and Marshall, R. (2006) The psychological risks of Vietnam for U.S. veterans: a revisit with new data and methods. Science. 313(5789):97982. PMID: 16917066 PMCID: PMC1584215 30. Dowd, J.B., Simanek, A.M., Aiello, A.E. (2009) Socio-economic status, cortisol and allostatic load: a review of the literature. International Journal of Epidemiology. 38(5):1297-309. PMID: 19720725 PMCID: PMC2755130 31. Folsom, D.P., Hawthorne, W., Lindamer, L., Gilmer, T., Bailey, A., Golshan, S., Garcia, P., Unutzer, J., Hough, R., and Jeste, D.V. (2005) Prevalence and risk factors for homelessness and utilization of mental health services among 10 340 patients with serious mental illness in a large public mental health system. Am J Psychiatry. 162(2):370-6. PMID: 15677603 32. Galea, S. (2007) Macrosocial Determinants of Population Health. New York:Springer. 33. Galea, S., Rockers, P.C., Saydee, G., Macauley, R., Varphilah, S.T., and Kruk, M.E. (2010) Persistent psychopathology in the wake of civil war: long-term posttraumatic stress disorder in Nimba County, Liberia. Am J Public Health. 100(9):1745-51. PMID: 20634461 34. Galea, S., Hall, C., and Kaplan, G.A. (2009) Social epidemiology and complex system dynamic modelling as 34 applied to health behaviour and drug use research. International Journal of Drug Policy. 20(3):209–216. PMID: 18930649 PMCID: PMC2782722 35. Galea, S., Riddle, M. and Kaplan, G.A. (2010) Causal thinking and complex system approaches in epidemiology. Int J Epidemiol. 39(1):97-106. PMID: 19820105 PMCID: PMC2912489 36. Geronimus AT. (2001) Understanding and eliminating racial inequalities in women's health in the United States: the role of the weathering conceptual framework. J Am Med Women’s Assoc, 56(4):133-6 PMID: 11759779 37. Hasin, D., Samet, S., Nunes, E., Meydan J., Matseoane, K., and Waxman, R. (2006) Diagnosis of Comorbid Psychiatric Disorders in Substance Users Assessed With the Psychiatric Research Interview for Substance and Mental Disorders for DSM-IV. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 163(4):689-96. PMID: 16585445 38. Heijmans, B.T., Tobi, E.W., Stein, A.D., Putter, H., Blauw, G.J., Susser, E., Slagboom, P.E., and Lumey, L.H. (2008) Persistent epigenetic differences associated with prenatal exposure to famine in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sc. 105(44):17046-9. PMID: 18955703 39. Herman, D., Opler, L., Felix, A., Valencia, E., Wyatt, R.J., and Susser, E. (2000) A “Critical Time” intervention with mentally ill homeless men: Impact on psychiatric symptoms. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 188(3):135-140. PMID: 10749277 40. Herman, D., Conover, S., Groroochum, P., Hinterland, L. Susser, E. (In Press) A randomized trial of critical time intervention to prevent homelessness in persons with severe mental illness following institutional discharge. Psychiatric Services. 41. Hoven, C.W., Duarte, C.S., Lucas, C.P., Wu, P., Mandell, D.J., Goodwin, R.D., Cohen, M., Balaban, V., Woodruff, B.A., Fan, B., Mei, L., Musa, G.J., Cantor, P.A., Aber, J.L., Cohen, P., and Susser, E. (2005) Psychopathology among New York City school children six months after September 11th. Archives of General Psychiatry. 62(5):545-552. PMID: 15867108 42. Insel, B.J., Schaefer, C.A., McKeague, I.W., Susser, E.S., and Brown, A.S. (2008) Maternal Iron Deficiency and the Risk of Schizophrenia in Offspring. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 65(10)1136-1144. PMID: 18838630 43. Johnson, J.G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S., Skodol, A.E., and Oldham, J.M. (2008) Cumulative prevalence of personality disorders between adolescence and adulthood. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 118(5):410-3. PMID: 18644003 44. Kessler, R.C., Chiu, W.T., Demler, O., Merikangas, K.R., and Walters, E.E. (2005) Prevalence, Severity, and Comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV Disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 62(6):617-27. PMID: 15939839 PMCID: PMC2847357 45. Kessler, R.C., Heeringa, S., Lakoma, M.D., Petukhova, M. Rupp, A.E., Schoenbaum, M., Wang, P.S., and Zaslavsky, A.M. (2008). Individual and societal effects of mental disorders on earnings in the united states: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 165(6):703-11. PMID: 18463104 PMCID: PMC2410028 46. Kessler, R.C., Ormel, J., Petukhova, M., McLaughlin, K.A., Green, J.G., Russo, L.J., Stein, D.J., Zaslavsky, A.M., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Andrade, L., Benjet, C., de Girolamo, G., de Graaf, R., Demyttenaere, K., Fayyad, J., Haro, J.M., Hu, C., Karam, A., Lee, S., Lepine, J.P., Matchsinger, H., Mihaescu-Pintia, C., PosadaVilla, J., Sagar, R., and Ustün, T.B. (2011) Development of Lifetime Comorbidity in World Health Ogranization World Mental Health Surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry. 68(1):90-100. PMID: 21199968 47. Koenen, K.C., Aiello, A.E., Bakshis, E., Amstadter, A.B., Ruggiero, K.J., Acierno, R., Kilpatrick, D.G., Gelernter, J., and Galea, S. (2009) Modification of the Association Between Serotonin Transporter Genotype and Risk of Postraumatic Stress Disorder in Adults by County–Level Social Environment. American Journal of Epidemiology. 169(6):704-711. PMID: 19228812 PMCID: PMC2727213 48. Koenen, K. (2010) Developmental Origins of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Depression and Anxiety. 27(5):413-16. PMID: 20455245 49. Kuh, D., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Lynch, J., Hallgvist, J., and Power, C. (2003) Life Course Epidemiology. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 57(10):778-83. PMID: 14573579 PMCID: PMC1732305 35 50. Leckman-Westin, Emily; Cohen, Patricia R; Stueve, Ann. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Vol.50(9), Sep 2009, pp. 1176-1184. 51. Lewis-Fernández, R., Horvitz-Lennon, M., Blanco, C., Guarnaccia, P.J., Cao, Z., and Alegría, M. (2009) Significance of endorsement of psychotic symptoms by US Latinos. J Nerv Ment Dis. 197(5):337-47. PMID: 19440107 PMCID: PMC2782526 52. Lewis-Fernández, R., Hinton, D.E., Laria, A.J., Patterson, E.H., Hofmann, S.G., Craske, M.G., Stein, D.J., Asnaani, A., and Liao, B. (2010) Culture and the anxiety disorders: recommendations for DSM-V. Depress Anxiety. 27(2):212-29. PMID: 20037918 53. Link, B.G. and Phelan, J. (1995) Social Conditions as Fundamental Causes of Disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. Spec No: 80-94. PMID: 7560851 54. Link, B.G. (2008) Epidemiological Sociology and the Social Shaping of Population Health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 49(4):367-384. PMID: 19181044 55. Link, B.G., Epperson, M.W., Perron, B.E., Castille, D.M., and Yang, L.H. (2011) Arrest Outcomes associated with outpatient commitment in new york state. Psychiatr Serv. 62(5):504-8. PMID: 21532076 56. Lopez, A.D., Mathers, C.D., Ezzati, M., Jamison, D.T., and Murray, C.J.L. (2006) Global Burden of Disease and Risk Factors. New York: Oxford University Press and The World Bank. 57. Malaspina, D., Brown, A., Goetz, D., Alia-Klein, N., Harkavy-Friedman, J., Harlap, S., and Fennig, S. (2002) Schizophrenia risk and paternal age: a potential role for de novo mutations in schizophrenia vulnerability genes. CNS Spectr. 7:26-29. PMID: 15254446 58. Mathers, C.D. and Loncar, D. (2006) Projections of Global Mortality and Burden of Disease from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Med. 3(11):e442. PMID: 17132052 59. March, D. and Susser, E. (2006) The eco- in eco-epidemiology. International Journal of Epidemiology. 35(6):1379-83. PMID: 17127690 60. March, D., Hatch, S.L., and Susser, E. (2010) Psychosis in migrant and minority populations: prescriptions for scientific and social policy. Psychological Medicine. 40(5):737-9. PMID: 19619386 61. McClellan, J., Susser, E., and King, M.C. (2006) Maternal famine, de novo mutations and schizophrenia. Journal of the American Medical Association. 296(5):582-584. PMID: 16882967. 62. McClellan, J.M., Susser, E., and King, M.C. (2007) Schizophrenia: A common disease caused by multiple rare alleles. Br. J Psychiatry 190:194-199. PMID: 17329737 63. McEwen, B.S. (1998) Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. The New England Journal of Medicine. 338(3):171-179. PMID: 9428819 64. Meaney, M.J., Aitken, D.H., Van Berkel, C.H., Bhatnagar, S., and Sapolsky, R.M. (1988) Effect of neonatal handling on age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science. 239(4841 Pt1): 766-8. PMID: 3340858 65. Merikangas, K.R., He, J.P., Burstein, M., Swanson, S.A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., Benjet, C., Georgiades, K., and Swendsen, J. (2010) Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication--Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 49(10):980-9. PMID: 20855043 PMCID: PMC2946114 66. Meyer, I.H., Schwartz, S., and Frost, D. (2008) Social Pattering of Stress Exposures. Social Science and Medicine. 67:368-379. 67. Neugebauer, R., Kline, J., Bleiberg, K., Baxi, L., Markowitz, J.C., Rosing, M., Levin, B., and Keith, J. (2007) Preliminary open trial of interpersonal counseling for subsyndromal depression following miscarriage. Depression and Anxiety. 24(3):219-22. PMID: 16988939 68. Noble, K.G., Wolmetz, M.E., Ochs, L.G., Farah, M.J., and McCandliss, B.D. (2006) Brain-behavior relationships in reading acquisition are modulated by socioeconomic status factors. Developmental Science. 9(6):642-654. PMID: 17059461 36 69. Noble, K.G., McCandliss, B.D., and Farah, M.J. (2007) Socioeconomic background predicts individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science. 10(4):464-480. 70. Parks, J., Svendsen, D., Singer, P., et al. (eds). 2006. Morbidity and Mortality in People with Serious Mental Illness. Alexandra, VA: National Association of State Mental Health Program Directors. 71. Piatt, E.E., Munetz, M.R., & Ritter, C. (2010). An examination of premature mortality among decedents with serious mental illness and those in the general population. Psychiatric Services. 61(7):663-8. PMID: 20592000 72. Pilowsky, D.J., Wickramaratne, P., Talati, A., Tang, M., Hughes, C.W., Garber, J., Malloy, E., King, C., Cerda, G., Sood, A.B., Alpert, J.E., Trivedi, M.H., Fava, M., Rush, A.J., Wisniewski, S., and Weissman, M.M. (2008) Children of depressed mothers 1 year after the initiation of maternal treatment: findings from the STAR*D-Child Study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 165(9):1136-47. PMID: 18558646 73. Phelan, J.C., Link, B.G., and Tehranifar, P. (2010) Social Conditions as Fundamental Causes of Health Inequalities: Theory, Evidence, and Policy Implications. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 51:S28-S40. PMID: 20943581 74. Phillips, M.R., Zhang, J., Shi, Q., Song, Z., Ding, Z., Pang, S., Li, X., Zhang, Y., and Wang, Z. (2009) Prevalence, treatment, and associated disability of mental disorders in four provinces in China during 2001–05: an epidemiological survey. Lancet. 373(9680):2041–53. PMID: 19524780 75. Prince, M., Shekar, S., Maj, M., Maselko, J., Phillips, M., and Rahman, A. (2007) Global Menatal Health 1: No Health Without Mental Health. Lancet. 370:859-77. 76. Ritsher, J.E., Warner, V., Johnson, J.G., and Dohrenwend, B.P. (2001) Inter-generational longitudinal study of social class and depression: A test of social causation and social selection models. British Journal of Psychiatry. 40:s84-s90. PMID: 11315232 77. Rothman, K.J., Greenland, S., Lash, T.L. (2008) Modern Epidemiology. Philadelphia, PA, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins 78. Saxena, S., Thornicroft, G., Knapp, M., and Whiteford, H. (2007) Resources for mental health: scarcity, inquity, and inefficiency. Lancet. 370(9590):878-89. PMID: 17804062 79. Schumacher, J.A., Coffey, S.F., Norris, F.H., Tracy, M., Clements, K., Galea, S. (2010) Intimate partner violence and Hurricane Katrina: predictors and associated mental health outcomes. Violence and Victims. 25(5):588-603. PMID: 21061866 80. Schwartz, S., Gatto, N.M., and Campbell, U.B. (2011) ”What would have been is not what would be: counterfactuals of the past and potential outcomes of the future" Causality and Psychopathology: Finding the Determinants of Disorders and Their Cures Ed. Shrout PE, Keyes K, Ornstein K (Eds.) Oxford University Press New York. 81. Schwartz, S., Diez Roux, A.V., and Susser, E. (2006) “Dependent and dynamic processes” in Susser, E, Schwartz, S, Morabia A and Bromet E. Psychiatric Epidemiology Searching for the Causes of mental Disorders. NY: Oxford University Press. 461-477. 82. Schwartz, S, Levav I and Dohrenwend B. (1994). Non-Genetic familial Transmission of Psychiatric Disorders? Journal of Health and Social Behavior 35:385-402. PMID: 7844332 83. Seeman, T., Glei, D., Goldman, N., Weinstein, M., Singer, B., and Lin, Y.H. (2004) Social relationship and allostatic load in Taiwanese elderly and near elderly. Soc Sci Med. 59(11):2245-57. PMID: 15450701 84. Skinner, J.S. and Staiger, D. (2005) “Technology Adoption from Hybrid Corn to Beta Blockers.” Working Paper No. 1125, National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge, MA. 85. St Clair, D., Xu, M., Wang, P., Yu, Y., Fang, Y., Zhang, F., Zheng, X., Gu, N., Feng, G., Sham, P., and He, L.. (2005) Rates of Adult Schizophrenia Following Prenatal Exposure to the Chinese Famine of 1959-1961. JAMA. 294(5):557-562. PMID: 16077049 86. Susser, E. and Schwartz, S. (2006) Psychiatric Epidemiology: Searching for the Causes of Mental Disorders. 37 New York: Oxford University Press. 87. Susser, M. and Susser, E. (1996) Choosing a future for epidemiology: I. Eras and paradigms. American Journal of Public Health. 86(5):668-73. PMID: 8629717 PMCID: PMC1380474 88. Susser, M. and Susser, E. (1996). Choosing a future for epidemiology: II. From black box to Chinese boxes and eco-epidemiology. American Journal of Public Health. 86(5):674-77. PMID: 8629718 PMCID: PMC1380475 89. Susser, E., Valencia, E., Conover, S., Felix, A., Tsai, W.Y., and Wyatt, R.J (1997) Preventing recurrent homelessness among mentally ill men: a "critical time" intervention after discharge from a shelter. American Journal of Public Health. 87(2):256-62. PMID: 9103106 PMCID: PMC1380803 90. Susser, E., Brown, A., and Matte, T. (2000) Prenatal antecedents of neuropsychiatric disorders over the life course: Collaborative studies of US birth cohorts. . In: Rappoport J, ed. Childhood Onset of “Adult” Psychopathology: Clinical and Research Advances. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.; 2000: 121146. 91. Swanson, J.W., Swartz, M.S., Van Dorn, R.A., Elbogen, E.B., Wagner, H.R., Rosenheck, R.A., Stroup, T.S., McEvooy, J.P., and Lieberman, J.A. (2006) A national study of violent behavior in persons with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 63(5):490-499. PMID: 16651506 92. Tyrka, A.R., Price, L.H., Kao, H.T., Porton, B., Marsella, S.A., Carpenter, L.L. (2010) Childhood maltreatment and telomere shortening: preliminary support for an effect of early stress on cellular aging. Biol Psychiatry. 67(6):531-4. PMID: 19828140 PMCID: PMC2853238 93. Veling W, Susser E, van Os J, Mackenbach JP, Hoek HW, Incidence of psychotic disorder in immigrants correlates with ethnic density of neighborhood. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd 154(15):A1767, 2010. PMID: 20456785 94. Wang, P.S., Lane, M., Olfson, M., Pincus, H.A., Wells, K.B., and Kessler, R.C. (2005) Twelve-month use of mental health services in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 62(6):629-40. PMID: 15939840 95. Wang, P.S., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M.C., Borges, G., Bromet, E.J., Bruffaerts, R., de Girolamo, G., de Graaf, R., Gureje, O., Haro, J.M., Karam, E.G., Kessler, R.C., Kovess, V., Lane, M.C., Lee, S., Levinson, D., Ono. Y., Petukhova, M., Posada-Villa, J., Seedat, S., and Wells, J.E. (2007) Use of mental health services for anxiety, mood, and substance disorders in 17 countries in the WHO world mental health surveys. Lancet. 370:841-50. PMID: 17826169 PMCID: PMC2847360 96. Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P., and Weissman MM. (2008) The role of fear and anxiety in the familial risk for major depression: a three-generation study. Psychological Medicine. 38(11):1543-56. PMID: 18275630 PMCID: PMC2904071 97. Weissman, M.M., Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P., Nomura, Y., Merikangas, K.R., Bruder, G.E., Tenke, C.E., and Grillon, C. (2004) Offspring at High Risk for Anxiety and Depression: Preliminary Findings From a ThreeGeneration Study. Fear and anxiety: The benefits of translational research. Washington, DC, US: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc. 65-83. 98. Weissman, M.M., Gross, R., Fyer, A., Heiman, G.A., Gameroff, M.J., Hodge, S.E., Kaufman, D., Kaplan, S.A., and Wickramaratne, P.J. (2004) Interstitial cystitis and panic disorder: a potential genetic syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry. 61(3):273-9. PMID: 14993115 99. Weissman, M.M. and Olfson, M. (2009) Translating intergenerational research on depression into clinical practice. JAMA. 302(24):2695-6. PMID: 20040558 PMCID: PMC2904067 100. Weissman, M.M., Wickramaratne, P., Nomura, Y., Warner, V., Pilowsky, D., and Verdeli, H. (2006) Offspring of depressed parents: 20 years later. Am J Psychiatry. 163(6):1001-1008. PMID: 16741200 101. Weissman, Myrna, Alan Brown, and Ardescheer Talati. (In Press) Translational Epidemiology in Psychiatry: Linking Population to Clinical and Basic Sciences. Archives of General Psychiatry. 102. WHO (2000) Cross national comparisons of the prevalence and correlates of mental disorders. Bulletin of the World Health Organisation. 78(4):413-426. PMID: 10885160 PMCID: PMC2560724 38 103. Xu, M.Q., Sun, W.S., Liu, B.X., Feng, G.Y., Yu, L., Yang, L., He, G., Sham, P., Susser, E., St. Clair, D., and He, L. (2009) Prenatal malnutrition and adult schizophrenia: further evidence from the 1959-61 Chinese famine. Schizophr Bull 35(3):568-76. PMID: 19155344 39