Annual Review of Critical Psychology Copyright © 2000 Discourse Unit Vol. 2, pp. 3-16 (ISSN: 1464-0538) EDITORIAL Critical psychology and action research Dan Goodley and Ian Parker Abstract. ‘Critical Psychology’ and ‘Action Research’ are terms that look, at first glance, to be easily linked. But the relationship between the two is not that simple. In fact, what each of them is on their own is not simple either. We need to worry away at the complexity of both critical psychology and action research, to ask questions about where they have come from and how they might develop in such a way as to make the complex relations between the two more visible. Or perhaps, more accurately, to ask the kind of questions that create those complex relations, that make it possible to develop action research in critical psychology and arguments for critical psychology in action research. To do that we need to look first at the cultural and historical context for this kind of endeavour, review some examples of good practice, suggest ‘transitional’ strategies for working critically in psychology and laying the basis for a ‘critical psychological praxis’. Keywords: Critical psychology, action research Mainstream psychology in the English-speaking world is still strongly wedded to empiricist research. You only trust what you can observe and measure. Most research has been underpinned by the crassest positivism, in which you steadily accumulate what you have measured into a corpus of ‘facts’ about human beings. And that is why it has also been, in the main, quantitative. That does not mean that all quantitative research is necessarily positivist. The longstanding existence of the Radical Statistics Group reminds us of that (Dorling and Simpson, 1999). But what it does mean is that positivism is the reason here why psychology is predominantly quantitative. The historical context is that we had varieties of alternative research in the 1970s and 1980s, which seemed to break from that positivist image of what psychology, as a science should be like. The ‘new paradigm’ movements looking at roles and rules in little social worlds and forms of discourse analysis looking at patterns of language use smuggled in bits of sociology into the discipline. But it turns out that the effect of these alternatives has been paradoxical. Perhaps it would be more accurate to say that the fate of the alternative research movements has been dialectical. For on the one hand, they, the advocates of the alternative research perspectives, were caught redhanded; it was clear that they often had a bigger agenda than just ‘better methods’. What was most radical was confiscated, and much ‘qualitative’ research in psychology now has been press-ganged into mainstream empiricism and positivism. Dan Goodley and Ian Parker A qualitatively different critical space On the other hand, the reason why some psychologists turned to these alternatives was because they realised that something was deeply wrong with the politics of psychology, with the politics of what psychologists do to people either in professional practice or as researchers. It has been feminist psychologists actually, and to an extent Lesbian and Gay psychologists, who have been largely responsible for keeping politics on the agenda in alternative research debates. Sometimes they too have been recruited into ‘qualitative research’ or ‘discourse analysis’ and forced to speak that kind of language which is a very risky thing to do. You could not say that Marxist or other leftist psychologists have been able to organise in the same kind of way, though there has been a little of that. There was also humanist research, which seems to be at odds with mainstream psychology. So, we have a space, or contradictory spaces, emerging inside the discipline of psychology. That makes it possible to think about critiques of psychology and the politics of research. There is sensitivity to the ethics of interviewing, to the process of accountability, for example. Those working in that space, in those spaces, often look to sociology; again, to show the way forward, but they are often unclear where exactly the arguments go in relation to psychology. And psychology does not quite know what to do about these things happening inside it. Sometimes it responds by demands that criteria for qualitative research be clearly defined, defined in line with the way it already conducts its research. Sometimes it contents itself with gratuitous attacks on Lesbians and Gays in the new section in the British Psychological Society for being ‘nihilist’, or being in favour of ‘social constructionism’ or, a new charge now, for links with ‘critical psychology’. Critical psychology What is critical psychology? We are not entirely sure. It has picked up on some of the 1960s radical psychology and anti-psychiatry critiques and it has short-circuited some strange connections with 1990s radical beryline social constructionism or postmodern theory. It is friendlier than mainstream psychology to feminism, and some qualitative researchers and discourse analysts hang around it sometimes looking for approval from it in odd moments when they think the discipline is not watching them. For sure critical psychology is broad. Broad enough, for example, for us to agree that the ‘Status Quo’ is bad and the ‘Good Society’ is good; and broad enough for us to disagree about what exactly each of those terms mean (e.g., Fox and Prilleltensky, 1997). Here is another paradox. For some reason Britain is seen as a hotbed of critical psychologists. However, we could not have developed critical psychology 2 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker here without an international context of debate, arguments and examples from colleagues in different countries. We would go so far as to say that critical psychology would not have been possible without email, for it allows a speed of communication and organisation of networks that have not been possible up to now. But we have developed something here that seems quite specific, even parochial, disconnected from the experience of radical psychologists in other places. Critical psychology here, for example, is quite different from the tradition of German Kritische Psychologie or from Central American Liberation Psychology. What have we learnt from those examples? What can we learn? Family resemblances and critical relations We need to be working in and with and questioning three different kinds of relations, dare we say dialectical relations: first, a relation between theory and practice (and it would be too simple to say that this means that we need a theory that is practical and a practice that is theorised); second, a relation between the inside and the outside (and this isn’t just to say that those inside psychology need to reach out and those outside need to have a voice in the discipline); and third, a relation between different traditions of work in different places (and the task is more complicated than admiring them as a differentiated assortment or squashing them together to create a perfect blend). The only thing we can be sure of is that what we think critical psychology ‘is’ is limited, culturally located, historically situated. We start here because we have to start somewhere. We will give a sharper definition of what critical psychologists are trying to do though. Maybe you will disagree with this, but we are doing this because it is essential to give some kind of definition of key issues in order to pose questions to any kind of research practice, including action research. Critical psychology stands at the margins of psychology, philosophy, sociology, politics, history, literary theory, anthropology, education and social policy. Proponents are brought together under this counter-paradigm rubric to challenge the Western psy-complex. This complex at best trivializes human experience (through, for example, adhering to the taken-for-granted assumptions of a positivistic approach to research) and at worst controls and oppresses (through, for example, conceptualizing mental distress as the product of a disorder of the unitary individual subject). In this sense then, critical psychology is inherently political, though it is often presented as yet another postmodern and anti-foundationalist intellectual project. Critical psychology needs to include the following four points of focus: first, the way psychology as a discipline privileges its own accounts over all of the other explanations people give about what they do; second, the way particular 3 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker everyday psychologies may either challenge or confirm mainstream psychological accounts; third, the way psychology operates throughout culture to regulate people as efficiently as the discipline of psychology does; and fourth, the way the discipline of psychology, for good or ill, rests on commonsense assumptions about action and experience (Parker, 1999). Of course these points of focus are contradictory. Human beings are contradictory, culture is contradictory. That is why some critical psychologists turn to dialectics and some to deconstruction to develop theoretical accounts which are as fragmented and multi-sided as the realities they want to understand and change. Critical theory, Marxism, feminism and humanism have historically been resources for ‘critical’ psychologists and action researchers. The question critical psychology asks now is whether those resources are able to take the power of the psy-complex seriously, and by psy-complex we mean that dense network of theories and practices which make up the apparatus of psychology inside and outside colleges and clinics, including its power to define what people think about themselves and their own personal resources for change. That is, we insist on asking whether humanism (but we are only singling this out first as an example so you get the point about the problem of taking at face value individual experience and psychological explanations and selfresponsibility for change) along with feminism and Marxism and Critical Theory generally and any other pretender to be a radical resource reproduce the worst of psychology when they seem to be alternatives to it. Whatever critical psychology is exactly, the discipline of psychology does not quite know what has hit it, for we put questions of politics and power on the agenda in theoretical debates about what psychology is. It could be incorporated as another loyal opposition, maybe even as section 497 of the American Psychological Association, maybe as an academic movement of self-critique in the history and philosophy of psychology - and we already have a section for that here in the British Psychological Psychology - and that incorporation could spell the death of radical work. But it could radicalise those other spaces of resistance inside psychology. Action research To do that it must turn to action research. Well, maybe not. At any rate critical psychology must move from the sphere of academic argument into the methodological process and into the realm of praxis. We use the word ‘praxis’ here to capture the dialectical interlinking of theory and practice, a theory which has its roots in practice and a practice that is always already theorised. In fact the relation between critical psychology and action research also needs to involve that kind of dialectical interweaving. 4 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker What is action research? There are many overlapping characterisations of action research and participatory action research. We will briefly set out eleven key points from one account from Colombia (Granada, 1991, pp. 176177). Each point is, from certain angles in critical psychology, problematic. But we do need some definitions in order to pose questions from action research to critical psychologists. Each of these points does conflict with certain lines of work in ‘critical’ psychology, let alone mainstream psychology. the construction and constitution of knowledge is social; agents who intervene have particular skills and experience; it is important for researchers and participants to share mutual perceptions about what well-being is; the researcher’s and community’s ‘knowledge’ and the knowledge process needs to be made explicit so that values may be examined; researchers must adapt modes of communication and discussion to the needs of local communities; interdisciplinary self-help develops through a concept of the community as an active subject (rather than as a target group); the participatory process also involves informal education in which community and researcher learn, and this facilitates the ‘expansion and democratisation of knowledge’; its philosophy is centred on community development and improvement of quality of life, with certain political and social values as central; the focus on problems and projects contributes more effectively to an inter- and multi-disciplinary perspective; knowledge is ‘not found or taught. It is created by means of a dialectical interaction’; ‘there is a move to create projects that are increasingly organic, interinstitutional, interdisciplinary’ with an effect on official policies 5 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker Actionable research Research is often accounted for in ways that reflect the deliberations of researchers upon research projects that they have initiated themselves, at their discretion where they have had ultimate control (Swain, 1995). Accordingly, research that attempts to adhere to the principles of action research alongside recognition of the importance of radical critical theory creates dilemmas. Take for example research in the disability studies field in Britain. A major theoretical development that has emerged from the activism of disabled people has been the social model of disability (see UPIAS, 1976). In one way this model celebrates radical theorising (picking at the contradictory social conditions that exclude people with impairments), though in other ways it questions the rights to authorship and ownership of the theorist (where are disabled people in the doing of research that impacts positively on their lives?) Recent writings by key contributors in the field (Oliver, 1996; Oliver and Barnes, 1997; Barnes and Mercer, 1997) have further highlighted the tensions between being theoretically radical and being accountable to disabled people (and their organisations) in the doing of disability research. Showing parallels with feminist research of the 1970s and 1980s (e.g., Oakley, 1981; Stanley and Wise, 1993), the social model calls for an all-embracing though contradictory relationship between theoretical developments and political ambitions of the disabled people’s movement. First, often heard are critical voices against the role of the non-disabled (academic) researcher. Exactly why and how do these researchers contribute to the development of an alternative disability culture that breaks down, deconstructs and eradicates disabling society (see Finkelstein and Stuart, 1996)? Moreover, why are so few disabled people involved in disability research, particularly when they boast ontologically privileged access to incidents of disablement and resistance (Barnes and Oliver, 1997)? Second, and in contrast, (non-disabled) researchers working alongside disabled people have drawn attention to the critical distance and theoretical skills that they bring along to disability research (Goodley, 1999). Indeed, researchers involved with people with the label of learning difficulties have argued for the authority they hold in exposing disablement and encouraging resistance in the lives of people whose own ambitions have been consistently suppressed (Booth and Booth, 1994; Gillman et al., 1997). Research projects capture as, societal microcosms, ideological and interpersonal constructions of disability, intervention, policy and practice. Interrogating the conditions of research production contributes to a critique of wider disabling practices. 6 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker Commodification, parody and false domains of research Research that attempts to adhere to the radical potential of the social model of disability sits uneasily with the current climate of research production that mirrors the modes of production of late capitalism. In particular, the commodification of different research domains separates research communities. One consequence is the distinction created between different ‘theory’ and ‘action’ research commodity domains. Lost is praxis (where theory and practice are dialectically linked) and in its place are separated marketable fields of knowledge and expertise. Disability studies are not exempt from this commodification. Indeed, it is possible - through parody - to represent these created social spaces to consider the challenges facing disability theorists and activists. These parodies aim to capture a particularly cynical view of the distinctions between ‘theory’ and ‘practice’, a cynicism that is augmented by and a reflection of in the current climate of research in late capitalism. First, a parody of the ‘disability theorist’ and their involvement with ‘nonparticipatory research’. Here disability theory delves into abstracted examinations of the complex relations of disablement. While the social model can be viewed as the disability movement’s ‘big idea’ (Hasler, 1993), much of the literature of the social model fits neatly into an academic scholastic tradition (Marx, 1845). Here the academy is exploited for its space and relative intellectual freedom. Theories of disability are brought above ground, from crude activism to intellectual knowing, to assessment from the top-down. While the movement boasts its organic intellectuals (e.g., Oliver, 1990, 1996; Barnes, 1990; Morris, 1991, 1993; Shakespeare, 1993), dangers exist in research moving away from action and activism, with the consequent loss of disabled people in a grand narrative. As Marxist critiques dominate then accusations abound of the structuralist victimisation of disabled people (Corker, 1998). Abberley (1987) suggests that physically impaired people, in contrast to the non-impaired proletariat, are surplus to revolutionising practice in the work of Marx and Engels. People with impairments are therefore either conceptualised as products of exploitative, dangerous working conditions (as argued by Engels) or considered to be unable to offer non-alienated value as workers in socialist societies. Elsewhere, interpretive research that engages with the ‘lived realities’ of disabled people - ‘giving voice’ - can so easily slip back into an objectifying gaze as these ‘voices’ are swallowed up in analysis (e.g., Ferguson, Ferguson and Taylor, 1992). Here, as the researcher enjoys a free reign in relation to analysis, the individualised, rational, bounded individual survives in positions of subject and object. In search of ‘voice’, the researcher moves nearer and nearer to the therapeutic subject, playing around with a variety of strategies to gain the individual’s authentic voice (e.g., Goodley 7 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker 1998a). In contrast, a rejection of pseudo-humanistic attempts to ‘empower’, has invited theorists to ‘turn to the text’. Postmodernists start to enjoy a fashionable position in research, where ‘voice’ is redundant and deconstruction is employed to pick at the ‘psy-complex’ that pathologises and creates abnormality (e.g., Corker, 1998, Shakespeare, 1998, Hughes and Paterson, 1997). Judith Butler, Michel Foucault and Jacques Derrida occupy almost ‘Third Way’ Blairite positions here in social scientific discussion, palming us off in response to our requests for political alternatives. The character of this paralysis, ‘never knowing’, standing in the naive landscape between individualism and society promises us everything but provides nothing. Second, a parody of the ‘disabled activist’ and their attempts to employ ‘emancipatory disability research’. Disability activism resonates with the materialist destruction of micro and macro relations of disablement. The social model was and is the disability movement’s ‘big idea’ (Hasler, 1993), though disability studies literature so often loses touch with the daily experiences of oppression and resistance (Barnes, 1997). Here in the ‘real world’, outside of academia, spaces of resistance are demonstrated and the disability movement’s organic intellectuals and activists are thrown together as comrades in order to self-organise. Practice and politics are thrown forward, theories denounced for their fictional and abstracted nature. A model of emancipatory disability research is espoused, where disabled people take the reigns and direct the research process in ways that contribute to emancipation. It is far more than ‘giving voice’. Instead, research is about doing, acting, changing and contributing - activism masquerading as research. Often there appears to be little to distinguish action, politics and research. Where discussions of disability theorists centre around postmodernism or Marx, here action researchers consider questions put forward by participants: alternative frameworks of meaning are drawn together and celebrated (Vincent, 1998); service provision is re-appraised (Whittaker, Gardner and Kershaw, 1993) and professional practice is critiqued and informed (e.g., Goodley, 1998b). While emancipatory research has its own vocabulary and style of presentation, a lot of emphasis is placed on accessibility, meaning and relevance. People First groups, for example, have carried out their own appraisals of professionalism and engage in educating the public about the rights of people with learning difficulties. Here a new ‘third way’ is created, in between revolutionary and party politics, a politics of positive identity. The position of the theorist in all of this becomes opaque: as Marx (1845) would have it, the theorists’ work is the stuff only of the scholar and not the revolutionary, though it is to the latter we should always turn. 8 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker These parodies do a disservice to the participants of each camp who attempt to bridge the divide and engage in ‘praxis’. However, parody is never far from the truth, directly reflecting the false separation of ‘theorists’ and ‘practitioners’ as commodities. This separation leads to acrimony between the two camps, one arguing for the virtues of theory over practice, the other the opposite. At times, guilt is expressed by those theorists working from the ‘topdown’ - ‘theorising the oppressed’ - though offering no direct political involvement or alternative to discriminatory practices. Meanwhile, those working from the ‘bottom-up’ - ‘working with the oppressed’ - appear unable to move beyond common-sense understandings of the world or to stand outside of their very work in a critical-reflexive manner. The separation of camps in contemporary disability studies threatens to simplify and deny the cross-fertilisation of ideas. However, this area of research raises questions for all other areas of ‘action research’ trying to go critical in psychology. Strategies for action research for psychologists We want to set out some different strategies for change in psychology that follow the activity of critical reflection. This is schematic, but we think it is useful in thinking through the ways we conceptualise what we are up to when we complain about the oppressive nature of psychology, and when we try to do something about it. You will recognise some of these strategic moves, perhaps, from the language of politics. We do not want to pretend that psychology is like the State apparatus, and that we can simply transpose ways of engaging with Politics with a big P into the micro politics and personal politics of coping with and challenging the psy-complex. However, these approaches might be useful for thinking through how we might want to tackle the discipline. Maximum action The first strategy would be to attack everything about psychology, and to repudiate the whole enterprise. People do sometimes leave the discipline in disgust and decide that there are better things to do with their lives, and better ways to help other people. There is a warning in foucauldian writing about the ways in which such strategies lead to the ‘perpetuation of the present’ that we do not think we can just write off. This strategy of maximum opposition can lead advocates into something that pretends to be an absolute alternative but is really an absolute reproduction, sometimes in caricature of what it tried to escape. Those who thought they could find a complete progressive alternative in Soviet psychology, for example, often ended up doing psychology which was just as oppressive as Western bourgeois varieties. Those who turn to spirituality as an alternative to psychology, as another example, often end up with a system of concepts just as mystifying and disempowering. 9 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker Minimum hopes The second strategy is at the other extreme, as a most cautious minimum process of debate and refinement of concepts and methods. The most grandiose wishes for a more humane and better society in which psychology was entirely unnecessary might fuel this strategy, but worry at the counterproductive effects of destructive criticism leads to the most careful possible expression of it. At some safe moments we might confide in each other what our hopes are, but we must recognise, so this narrative for change goes, that we will not be taken seriously unless we speak psychology’s language. We may even have sympathy with the new foucauldian line that the best strategy is no strategy, that open challenge defines power structures all the more sharply and that it in turn defines us as mirrors of what we oppose. Neither of these first two strategies works. The middle way Let us turn to a third strategy. It does seem like the most obvious and sensible middle course. The parameters are very broad, and we might adopt different stances of defiance or consensus depending on the situation and what we can personally bear in terms of conflict. Different committee meetings call for a certain level of give and take, public meetings for rhetorical flourishes, and seminars and academic papers allow some margin for reasoned polemic. Much of what we do will look as if it lies in this third middle way between immediate revolution and far off reforms. Tactics We can anticipate two responses to what we have said so far, and perhaps we should address these before turning to a fourth strategy. We leave aside the standard positivist reaction that simply claims that we need to be more scientific and wipe away all traces of ideology from the discipline. That is a rather optimistic hope, and is itself a deeply ideological view. The illusion that there is value-free knowledge about human beings and the stories they tell about themselves is responsible for much harm in psychology. The first response comes from colleagues who have some political commitment to change in psychology, but who are worried about the relativism of discursive approaches. An emphasis on critical reflection upon the way psychology is structured is, to them, dangerous, for it encourages psychologists to abandon any hope of using psychological knowledge helpfully or progressively. We have heard discourse analysis, for example, described as a virus that is eating psychology away from the inside. The worry is that it only eats. It gives nothing in return. We need to address that seriously. The second response comes from colleagues in discourse analysis 10 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker who would like discourse analysis to be taken seriously in psychology, and hope that this can be achieved if the politics is screened out. We know we are on the edge of engaging in what some colleagues in discourse analysis see as ‘finger-wagging discourse’ here, so we will try and be a little more constructive. We want to argue that there is a particular dynamic that drives the type of critical reflection we are arguing for here, and that it leads us toward a political engagement with psychological institutions. There are good reasons for not adopting any particular model, method or approach. We want to keep in mind strategic issues, but foucauldian discourse analysis leads us, in addition, to think tactically against power. It does this on two counts. First, no model of psychological activity is necessarily reactionary. Take the example of social psychological theories of collective behaviour. Mainstream theories of crowd behaviour treat it as a pathological hypnotic regression to animal impulses and herd instincts, and so any collective activity is seen as suspect (Le Bon, 1896). It has been important to argue in response to this, and to demonstrate through good empirical qualitative research, that people do not lose their minds in crowds, but that they act with a purpose, achieving things that are denied in everyday life where experience is separated from others and individualised (Reicher, 1984). However, when Black South Africans were charged under Apartheid for the crime of ‘common purpose’ for murder after riots, simply for being a member of the group in question, it was necessary to wheel out the standard social psychological accounts in court as mitigating factors to save their lives. There were some sharp debates around the values and dangers of this for stereotypical images of the crowd, but the issue boils down to the tactical use of psychological theory. The other side of the coin is that no model of psychological activity is necessarily progressive. Take the example of phenomenology, which might be thought to provide a good basis for a respectful humanist approach to the different psychological realities of people’s life-worlds. This view was connected to the rhetoric throughout the crisis in social psychology that claimed that we needed to find more social conceptions of psychology. There was, in fact, a very strong very social psychology in Germany in the 1930s, which developed a notion of the person as, rooted in an organic relationship with their community, and talk of the Volk was underpinned by social theories of goodness of fit between an individual and their racial and cultural group. In South Africa, Afrikaaner psychology drew upon the phenomenological tradition to reinforce the idea that distinct groups had quite separate psychological realities, and good psychology would involve an exploration of these other life-worlds so that they could be supported. The use of phenomenology during the crisis critiques of experimentation did, as a 11 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker consequence, jar on the sensibilities of radical psychologists in South Africa, and they had good reason to feel happier with positivism, which did at least reveal some hard truths about inequalities in the country (Dawes and Donald, 1994). Perhaps it is not surprising that these tensions between reactionary and progressive uses of psychology should be brought to the fore in a charged political climate. Maybe it is only what is going on outside psychology that will force us to think though what psychology does in terms of power and ideology. Maybe the biggest ideological problem that we face is when people generally give up on politics, and see anything other than reflexivity in psychology as fanatical, manic or otherwise pathological. At any rate, we should be reflecting on how psychology functions in particular places, rather than looking to replace the whole lot with something which will then stand us in good stead for all occasions. As well as working strategically and tactically, we have keep to context. This does not mean that we simply adopt a postmodern ironic stance toward psychological techniques and carry on using them as we did before. Some social constructionists have sent out this reassuring message when they lurch from adopting the role of virus in the discipline to that of critical friend because they fear being heard as being destructive. One of the dangers of relativism, and of reflexivity without some critical standpoint is precisely that of folding back into the arms of psychology because if anything goes, then psychology can go on with business as usual too. A transitional strategy We want to suggest that there is a fourth strategy, which can move us along in and against psychology. A theoretical assumption and a political question together guide this fourth approach. The assumption is that there are structural limits to change, and these structural limits are reflected in, and reinforced by patterns of discourse. Or, better to say that discursive practices are in place that prevent us from thinking and acting against power, and that encourage us to think and act with it. The political question is how to bring people up against the limits so that they feel those patterns of discourse, those discursive practices as coercive, so that they are then able to shake them and break them. The most extreme maximum position misses the point that people can critically reflect on the limits and must themselves challenge power for progressive change to occur, and the most cautious minimum position fails to bring people to the limits. Most initiatives that simply work in between, as a middle path, accomplish some change, but without people being brought into an awareness of how those changes also accomplish, to borrow a situationist term, the ‘recuperation’, that is the neutralisation and absorption of resistance. This fourth path is one that is, of necessity, conscious of its line of attack, and 12 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker that marks it as a variety of critical reflection, of course. It is tactical as well as strategic. Critical Psychological Praxis In our research and teaching, we have often been faced with charges from peers that critical psychology only offers epistemological, theoretical, methodological and analytical alternatives. While we recognise these alternatives as essential to challenging what may be called the ‘psy-complex’ (and its associated applications), it is our contention that critical psychological praxis has a long history in a variety of local and global contexts. Our task is creating interventions that straddle critical theory and practice in ways that aim for social and individual change. This change may be defined as revolutionising in the sense that various individuals and collectives have worked together towards different ways of theoretically and practically conceptualising the role of psychology in the lives of societal members from a variety of cultures. Finding praxis We may suffer from being so closely connected to the academy and in its attempts to provide a scholarly, some could argue, idealistic alternative to the psy-complex as evidenced in action research. In Theses on Feuerbach (1845), Marx challenges the contradictory separation of contemplation (idealism) from activity (materialism). We are reminded that idealism is particularly apparent in scholarly bourgeois writing; celebrating itself over practice, the latter which is conceptualized as outside, alien to thought. Marx notes that as all social life is practical, then those idealistic or contemplative materialist positions that fail to recognize this fact or always prone to only contemplate individuals. Much space is given in critical psychology to assessing the actions and stories of ‘people’ or ‘subjects’ and readers of critical accounts are often taken through a bizarre journey from pathologised objects (noting how people are often excluded by a variety of social barriers enforced through practices of the psy-complex), to resilient subjects (demonstrating their resilient tales and actions through, for example, qualitative description) back to subjectified and objectified objects (phenomena to be analyzed and understood: back to the psy-complex). Research so often re-creates the very difficulties it is mean to challenge (Clough and Barton, 1998). However, to conclude so pessimistically is to leave a victimized image of the human subject. ‘Co-researchers’ necessarily complicate matters, by providing ways of bridging the artificial divide between theory and practice, conceptualization and activism, idealism and materialism. Their actions violate the distinction between commodities, stressing how theory and activism emerge together in places where one would 13 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker perhaps not expect such actions to take place: putting forward ideas for future research agendas. For some this has led to a process of deconstruction, where the role of the psychologist and psychology have been so consistently undermined that their position is considered to be one of occasional ally. In this sense then, critical psychological praxis aims to reject many (if not all) of the advances made by psychology into the lives of participants involved. Instead, the expertise of activists reign supreme and reignite interest in (and feed into the development of) critical theory. For others, engaged in a process of reconstruction, a critical psychology is an essential part of the interventions that are made. Here, critical psychological praxis attempts to offer change from the inside, to revamp existing practices in ways that are sensitive to critical theory and critical activism. Conclusions The relationship between action and ideas is crucial, and the dialectical formation of theory and practice enable activists, practitioners and researchers to reconsider their often seemingly contradictory and counter-productive positions. In conclusion we need to raise the following questions: What are the practical consequences of critical theory in academic psychology? What are the theoretical stakes in progressive practice in the real world? What connections between radical theorists, practitioners and activists might be forged to understand and change oppressive conditions? How are attempts to connect theory and practice played out a local level in specific cultural conditions? The argument which frames critical psychological praxis is that (i) critical theory must be practical, (ii) that critical practice is always theoretical and (iii) that specific cultural conditions mediate the development of forms of critical theory and critical practice and the connections between the two. ‘Critical psychological praxis’, then, is an historically and culturally specific endeavour that takes distinctly different shapes in different conditions. Due to the commodification and splitting off of various forms of work, in late capitalism, critical theorists and activists are faced with a constant problem of separation. In particular, ‘academic theory’ is often contrasted with ‘atheoretical practice’, with members of each distinct group at best misunderstanding and at worse rejecting one another. It is our contention that there is much overlap between progressive theorising and radical practice. Working with this overlap dialectically in a critical psychological praxis may be the way we can connect critical theory and action research. 14 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker References Abberley, P. (1987) The Concept of Oppression and the Development of a Social Theory of Disability. Disability, Handicap and Society, 2, 1, pp.5-21., Barnes, C. (1990) The Cabbage Syndrome: The Social Construction of Dependence. London: The Falmer Press. Barnes, C. (1997) Disability and the Myth of the Independent Researcher. In Barton, L. and Oliver, M. (eds.) Disability Studies: Past Present and Future. Leeds: The Disability Press. Barnes, C. and Oliver, M. (1997) All We’re Saying is Give Disabled Researchers a Chance. Disability and Society, 12 (5), 811-814. Barnes, C., and Mercer, G. (ed.) (1997) Doing Disability Research. Leeds: The Disability Press. Booth, T., and Booth, W. (1994) Parenting under Pressure: Mothers and fathers with Learning Difficulties. Buckingham: Open University Press. Campbell, J., and Oliver, M. (1996) Disability Politics: Understanding our Past, Changing our future. London: Routledge. Clough, P. and Barton, L. (1998) (eds) Articulating with Difficulty: Research Voices in Special Education. London: Paul Chapman Ltd. Corker, M. (1998) Disability Discourse in a Postmodern World. In T. Shakespeare (ed) The Disability Reader: Social Science Perspectives. London: Cassell. Dawes, A. and Donald, D. (1994) Childhood and Adversity: Psychological Perspectives for South African Research, Cape Town: David Philip Publishers. Dorling, D. and Simpson, L. (eds) (1999) Statistics in Society: The Arithmatic of Politics. London: Arnold. Ferguson, P. M., Ferguson, D. L., and Taylor, S. J. (eds) (1992) Interpreting Disability: A Qualitative Reader. New York: Teachers College Press. Finkelstein, V., and Stuart, O. (1996) Developing New Services. In G. Hales (ed.), Beyond Disability: Towards an Enabling Society. London: Sage / Open University Press. Fox, D. and Prilleltensky, I. (eds) (1997) Critical Psychology: An Introduction. London: Sage. Gillman, M., Swain, J., and Heyman, B. (1997) Life History or ‘Care History’: The Objectification of People with Learning Difficulties through the Tyranny of Professional Discourses. Disability and Society, 12(5), 675-694. Goodley, D. (1998a) Stories about Writing Stories. In L. Barton and P. Clough (ed.), Articulating with Difficulty: Research Voices in Special Education. London: Paul Chapman Ltd. Goodley, D. (1998b) Supporting People with Learning Difficulties in Self-advocacy Groups and Models of Disability. Health and Social Care in the Community, 6 (5), 438 - 446. Goodley, D. (1999) Disability Research and the ‘Researcher template’: Reflections on Grounded Subjectivity. In ethnographic research’. Qualitative Inquiry, 5 (1), 24 - 46. Granada, H (1991) Intervention of community social psychology: the case of Colombia. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 40 (2), pp. 165-180. Hasler, F. (1993) Developments in the Disabled People’s Movement. In J. Swain, V. Finkelstein, S. French, and M. Oliver (ed.), Disabling Barriers - Enabling Environments. London: Sage. Hughes, B., and Paterson, K. (1997) The Social Model of Disability and the Disappearing Body: Toward a Sociology of Impairment. Disability and Society, 12(2), 325-340. Le Bon, G. (1896) The Crowd: A Study of the Popular Mind, London: Ernest Benn. Marx, K. (1845) Theses on Feuerbach. In K. Marx and F. Engels, Selected Works. London: Lawrence and Wishart Morris, J. (1991) Pride Against Prejudice: Transforming Attitudes to Disability. London: 15 Dan Goodley and Ian Parker The Women’s Press. Morris, J. (ed.) (1996) Encounters with Strangers: Feminism and Disability. London: The Women’s Press. Oakley, A. (1981) Interviewing Women: A Contradiction in Terms. In H. Roberts (ed.), Doing Feminist Research. London: Routledge. Oliver, M. (1990) The Politics of Disablement. Basingstoke: Macmillan. Oliver, M. (1996) Understanding Disability: From Theory to Practice. London: Macmillan. Parker, I. (1999) Critical psychology: critical links, Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 1, pp. 3-18. Reicher, S. (1984) The St. Pauls’ riots: an explanation of the limits of crowd action in terms of a social identity model, European Journal of Social Psychology, 14: 1-21. Shakespeare, T. (1993) Disabled People’s Self-organisation: A New Social Movement? Disability, Handicap and Society, 8(3), 249-264. Shakespeare, T. (ed) (1998) The Disability Reader: Social Science Perspectives. London: Cassell. Stanley, L. and Wise, S. (1993) Breaking out again : feminist ontology and epistemology. London: Routledge UPIAS (1976) Fundamental Principles of Disability. London: Union of the Physically Impaired Against Segregation. Vincent, C. (1998) Class, Race and Collective Action: African Caribbean Parents’ Involvement in Education. Paper presented at the Policy, Failure and Difference Seminar, Ranmoor Hall, Sheffield. Whittaker, A., Gardener, S., and Kershaw, J. (1991) Service Evaluation by People with Learning Difficulties: Based on the People First Report. London: King’s Fund Centre. Ian Parker is Professor of Psychology at Manchester Metropolitan University: Address: Discourse Unit, Department of Psychology and Speech Pathology, The Manchester Metropolitan University, Elizabeth Gaskell Campus, Hathersage Road, Manchester, M13 OJA, UK. Email: I.A.Parker@mmu.ac.uk Dan Goodley is a member of the Disability Research Unit at the University of Leeds. A forthcoming book explores activism in the lives of people with the label of ‘learning difficulties’ (2000, Politics of Resilience, Open University Press) Recent research projects have drawn upon narrative, ethnographic and discourse analytic approaches. He is especially critical of clinical psychologists. Address: Disability Research Unit School of Sociology and Social Policy University of Leeds Leeds LS2 9JT, UK. Email: D.Goodley@Leeds.ac.uk 16