The Consequences of Language Chapter 10.What Is the Relationship Between Language Structure and Communication? This chapter presents the wide range of approaches made by structuralists in the analysis of texts.(Each section introduces a perspective and an example of an analysis). 1. Introduction Each of the perspectives presented in this chapter has contributed to the analysis of language and culture by proposing a specific perspective on the nature of language and then a demonstration of this perspective through the analysis of a text, dialog or component of language. The chapter does by no means exhaust the various approaches taken in this vein, but does provide a representative sampling of the types of approaches. 2. French Structuralism Claude Lévi-Strauss has taken a structuralist approach to the analysis of culture and applied it both to the analysis of kinship systems and the grammar of myth. His approach is not easy to grasp, which has lead to a wide range of criticisms of them (Leach, Geertz, and Harris), but it is useful because an understanding of this approach helps to clarify the basic principles underling a structural approach and in the process reveals the potential of structural analysis and its limitations and it. Fundamental Tenets of Structuralism The approach used by Claude Lévi A semiological system consists of contrasting signs Strauss is often referred to simply as in a closed system. The operation of this structural system is below the structuralism for several reasons. First it is level of parole ( discursive or declarative) knowledge, but not the level of awareness. structuralist because it adheres to the The goal of structuralism is the recovering of the fundamental tenets of structuralism. That is, underlying system and ignoring of the analysis of the parole. it approaches cultural systems from a The name given to the methodology involved in semiological perspective - that underlying a uncovering the properties of a semiological system is “reconstructive science” (Habermaas) which differs cultural phenomenon like kinship or myth is from positivist science. a common underlying (closed) system of oppositions. For Claude Lévi-Strauss, these oppositions are binary, though this need not be the case for other semiological systems. Second, It is called structuralism because many are unaware of its affinity with other semiological systems like phonology, the lexicon or syntax. We prefer the term French Structuralism for Claude Lévi-Strauss’ approach both to distinguish it from other structuralist approaches and to show its similarity to them. 2.1 The framework. In this regard, I find his approach to myth, as opposed to kinship, more revealing, more interesting, and more understandable. We all recognize that in the elements of language (phonology, lexicon and syntax) go into the construction of a myth The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 2 The science of the concrete. Levi-Strauss proposes that humans do not ordinarily think in a rational linear sense associated with western science with its abstract concepts like gravity, intermolecular force and the like. The science of the concrete, CLS says, does not involve abstract concepts but can manipulate such concepts when they are embodied in a physical object. In this way of thinking, slyness could be portrayed by a fox, doom by a vulture, reliability by the plodding tortoise1, and so forth. Thus, even though CLS draws a distinction between the science of the concrete and the science of the abstract, he does not value one higher than the other. The significance of the embodiment of abstract concepts is that it allows for the manipulation of these abstract concepts through the telling of a story about the objects in which they are embodied. This kind of relationship is essentially the working of metaphor. Such stories constitute, form CLS’ perspective, myth. Thus a myth tells a story at two levels, one at the level of parole and the other at the level of langue. The level of awareness. When we read or listen to a myth at the level of parole, we encounter a story with actors, a plot and a resolution. When we encounter the same myth at the structural level we are not conscious of what is going on, but sense, according to CLS, a set of relationships ( to be found in the gross constituent units.) being juxtaposed. Furthermore, these relationships are not the product of the conscious mind, but the workings of the mind at the structural level, or as CLS put it, people don’t think in myths, but rather myths think themselves out through people.2 Research Issues for the Science of the Concrete. The relations that recur in myth are concerns about the nature of human beings that concern us all. The most important of these is the Culture v. Nature opposition, and that concerns the uniqueness of human beings and what makes us different from other animals. We are aware that we are animals (nature) but very unique animals because we have culture. Incest and Marriage. As cultural beings we, unlike animals, have (extended) families and therefore practice exogamy to avoid incest and build alliances with our in-laws’ families through the exchange of our children through marriage. Born from one v. born from two. As cultural beings we are also concerned about our origins. Most societies have a myth3 about how humans came into being. This usually involves a first man who reproduced to populate the world. This myth creates a problem in that the first 1 We note that in Cameroon in West Africa, the turtle is regarded as a trickster. 2 Nous ne pretendons donc pas montrer comment les hommes pensent dans les mythes mais comment les mythes se pensent dans les hommes, et à leur insu." We don't suggest therefore that men think in myths but that myths work themselves out in the minds of men unconsciously. 3 We are using the term myth in CLS’ sense of the word, that is a text that contains both a parole level and a structural level of meaning. CLS is not concerned with the truth value of a myth, only the sorts of meaning that it contains. The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 3 form of reproduction was asexual unlike the current mode which is sexual.4 Note also that even if a second sex is introduced, it is hard to escape the likelihood of incest in the early stages of populating the world.5 Often one finds in myths that the first couple are brother and sister, in which case their reproduction involves incest. Death v. Eternal Life. As cultural beings we are aware of and terrorized by the thought of our own death. Most societies have developed “myths” to deal with this which usually involve eternal life. This opposition also finds its way into the structural component of myth. Each of these topics represents a serious (subconscious) concern for humans and it is these concerns that CLS claims are embedded into the structural component of a myth. Furthermore, the research questions posed by the science of the concrete are essentially the same as those posed by (western) scientists of the abstract. The Units (signs). Every semiological system consists of a specific type of sign within a closed system. In the lexical system it is the word. In the representational system, it is the phoneme or letter. In the structural system it is the gross constituent unit. Like other semiological signs the gross constituent unit is below the level of discursive knowledge, but not the level of awareness. Now the concepts of culture, nature, incest and the like are abstract notions, and inaccessible to the science of the concrete. Rather, this science requires that these concepts need to be embedded into real objects and hence the fox for slyness mentioned above, but also the juxtaposition of two entities such as a domestic and a tame animal or a cultivated plant and a wild plant or a cooked and a raw fish to illustrate the culture/nature opposition. From this perspective it is easy to see why totemism, the metaphoric use of animals, is so common in myth. 2.3 The methodology. In a reconstitutive science, such as in linguistics, the goal is to “reconstitute” the grammar of a given language that is in the heads of the users. In such a science, according to Habermaas, there are two interdependent levels of theory: 1) the theory or grammar of a language; and 2) the theory of language, also known as the metatheory. The metatheory states what is common to all languages (also know as linguistic universals and universal grammar). A grammar of a language states how a sentences are produced and understood for a given language. These levels are interdependent so that the grammar of each language must be consistent with the metatheory. If it is not, then either the grammar or the metatheory needs to revised. The interdependence of grammar and metatheory derives from the rationalist presumption (chapter 5?) that language is acquired by an existing structured mind predisposed to language (the universal grammar) which 4 We note in passing that the “Western scientific” view is strikingly parallel in that the first life forms reproduced asexually. 5 In the Judeo-Christian origin, for example, with whom did the children of Adam and Eve beget their children? The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 4 enables the acquisition of a grammar of a language, which is, as a consequence, consistent with the universal grammar. The operation of French Structuralism follows the same methodology. The metatheory holds that all myths contain an underlying structural opposition of GCUs which contain elements of the research issues. Work on the metatheory will involve an elaboration of the research issues, the nature of the GCUs and their insertion into myths. Work on the individual myth involves separating the relevant (at this level) from that is irrelevant. (necessary v contingent) by identifying specific (pairings and patternings of) GCUs and showing how they relate to the metatheory and by comparing these with a set of related myths, looking for the same patterning of (pairs of) GCUs. The GCUs will often be paired and the parings will often be analogous to the other myths, though this does not mean involving the same totemic devices. One of the best examples of this is his paper on Four Winnebago Myths. 2.4 The Thebesian legends 1. a. b. 2. 3. a. b. 4. a. 5. a. 6. a. 7. 8. 9. 10. Kadmos seeks sister (Europe) who has been ravaged and carried off by Zeus. [Zeus disguised as white bull. Makes Europe queen of Crete.] [Kadmos told by oracle in Delphi to forget sister, she's ok. Also told to follow white cow and found city. (Thebes. Kadmos needs water for sacrifice, sends soldiers one after the other to fetch water. The dragon son of god Ares eats all the soldiers] Kadmos kills the dragon. Spartoi arise from dragon's teeth and kill one another. (Kadmos told to throw stone among Spartoi to causes the Spartoi to fight among themselves.) [ Five Spartoi survive and they help Kadmos build Thebes]. [Oedipus sent to another family grows up without knowing prophesy or who real parents are.] Oedipus unknowingly kills his father Laios, King of Thebes, on a mountain pass. [Later Oedipus comes to Thebes where a Sphinx is guarding and imprisoning the city of Thebes.] Oedipus kills Sphinx (riddle of the Sphinx). (The Sphinx commits suicide once Oedipus answers riddle) [Oedipus is welcomed as king into Thebes.] Oedipus unknowingly marries his mother Jokaste (Queen of Thebes). [Oedipus learns of the truth of the prophesy. Jokaste commits suicide and Oedipus blinds himself.] Eteokles (son of Oedipus) kills his brother Polyneikes Antigone (daughter of Oedipus) buries her brother despite prohibition 9. Labdakos (father of Laios = 'lame' Laios (father of Oedipus = 'left footed' Oedipus = 'swollen foot' 11. The following account is drawn from Edmund Leach’s (19xx) account of CLS’ analysis of the Greek collection of myths known as the Thebsian legends which contains the well known story of Oedipus. In the following synopsis, the numbered lines are GCUs identified by CLS. Those not numbered were ruled out on the basis that a) they did not form a pattern within the myth and b) they did not fit other myths. They have been included to help tell the parole level of the story. Analysis Having identified the GCUs, CLS then noted that they could be assigned to one of two sets of oppositions, one dealing with incest (the over v. undervaluation of kinship) and one dealing with humans and monsters. The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? I Overvaluation of Kinship 1 Kadmos' (over) concern for his sister (Europe) 6 Oedipus' incest with his mother 8 Antigone's (over) concern for her brother. II Undervaluation of Kinship 3 Spartoi (brothers) kill themselves. 4 Oedipus kills father. 7 Eteokles kills his brother Polyneikes III Culture over Nature 2 Kadmos kills dragon (half god half animal). 5 Oedipus kills sphinx (half woman half lion) Chapter 10, Page 5 IV Nature over Culture 9 Labadakos (grandfather of Oedipus) = lame. 10 Laios (father of Oedipus) = left-sided. 11 Oedipus = Swollenfooted To understand the last column, CLS notes that many human origin theories involve autochthonous (coming from the earth) first humans. Such is the case, for example with the Judeo-Christian view. He adds that it is generally the case that autochthonous people walk with a limp and that it is not a mere coincidence that Oedipus, his father and grandfather all have names that reflect this fact. From this persistence of lameness, reflecting autochthony, that CLS can say that the GCUs in this column reflect the persistence of nature over culture in contrast to the preceding column where humans have conquered nature in the form of mythical monsters. I Overrating of Kinship <is to> II its Underrating Here CLS sees GCUs representing the < as > culture/nature question in two ways: in the III Denial of autocho<is to> IV the persistence thonous origin of autochthony domain of kinship (columns I and II) and in the domain of autochthony (columns III and IV). Since the order of the appearance of the GCUs is not a significant aspect of CLS’ theory, the presentation of the question does not lead to a resolution of the problem, but it does constitute a statement of it. "how to find a satisfactory transition between this theory and the knowledge that human beings are actually born from the union of man and woman. Although the problem obviously cannot be solved, the Oedipus myth provides a kind of logical tool which relates to the original problem -- born from one or born from two -- to the derivative problem: born from different or born from the same. By a correlation of this type, "the overrating of blood relations is to the "underrating of blood relations" is to the "impossibility to succeed in it". Although experience contradicts theory, social life validates cosmology by its similarity of structure. Hence, cosmology is true. (CLS, Structural Anthropology: 216) Commentary. As we have noted, numerous writers have offered criticisms of CLS and perhaps the reader shares some of these reservations. Nevertheless, the approach is quite useful in pointing out the basis of a structural approach. These are: A semiological system consists of contrasting signs in a closed system. The operation of this structural system is below the level of parole ( discursive or declarative) knowledge, but not the level of awareness. The goal of structuralism is the recovering of the underlying system and ignoring of the analysis of the parole. The name given to the methodology involved in uncovering the properties of a semiological system is “reconstructive science” (Habermaas) which differs from positivist science in that it involves the interaction of two levels of analysis, the theory and the metatheory. The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 6 3. Semiotics Another structuralist approach to the study of language was initiated by Charles S. Peirce, a brilliant mathematician with an eclectic approach to the world who wrote at about the same time as Saussure, but whose eight volumes of papers were published much later. Peirce added to the analysis of the sign the concept of a referent and a recognition of three different types of signs, 1) iconic, those which bear some resemblance to their referent (a drawing), 2) indexical, those which indicate their referent (smoke is an Pierce, C.S. . Collected Papers. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. 1965-6. index of fire) and 3) symbolic, those which have no recognizable association with their referent. Sebeok, Thomas. The Structure of Cheremis Charms. In Hymes 1964: 356-71. This line of thinking has been picked up and developed by Thomas Sebeok and applied to areas of poetry. In an analysis of Cheremis charms, Sebeok (1964) pointed out that the structural design, including their syntax, was substantially different from ordinary language. 4. Cognitive Anthropology 1. Goodenough a. Goodenough: a description of a culture should properly specify what it is that a stranger to a society would have to know in order appropriately to perform any role in any scene staged by the society. b. [Note parallel to Chomsky.] (87) The Intended objective of these efforts is eventually to provide the ethnographer with public, non-intuitive procedures for ordering his presentation of the people he is studying. To order ethnographic observations solely according to an investigator's preconceived categories obscures the real content of culture: how people organize their experience conceptually so that it can be transmitted as knowledge from person to person and from generation to generation. The principles by which people in a culture construe their world reveal how they segregate the pertinent from the insignificant, how they code and retrieve information, how they anticipate events, how they define alternative courses of action and make decisions among them. Consequently a strategy of ethnographic description that gives a central place to the cognitive processes of the actors involved will contribute reliable cultural data to problems of the relations between, language, cognition, and behavior; it will point up critical dimensions for meaningful cross-cultural comparison; and finally, it will give us productive descriptions of cultural behavior, descriptions which, like the linguist's grammar, succinctly state what one must know in order to generate culturally acceptable acts and utterances appropriate to a given social- ecological context. From the Ethnographic Study of Cognitive Systems Frake SEGREGATES CONTRAST SETS TAXONOMIES ATTRIBUTES 2. Keesing The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? 5. Ethnomethodology 1. Garfunkel and the theory and its Contributions 2. Cicourel Chapter 10, Page 7 Towards Ethnographies of Communication: The Analysis of Communicative Events (Excerpts from D. Hymes, `Introduction: toward ethnographies of communication'. American Anthropologist, 1964 88.6-2:12-25 6. The Ethnography of Communication Dell Hymes approached the topic of language and culture from the perspective of the ethnography. Here we understand the ethnography as a report by an anthropologist upon return from the field about what he or she has seen there. Underlying the ethnography are the theoretical orientation (materialist, idealist, ...), the principle of participant observation and the idea of providing an emic (insider’s) as opposed to an etic (outsider’s) account of what was seen in the field. 6.1 Emics and Etics. An emic account is one in terms of features relevant in the behavior in question; an etic account, however useful as a preliminary grid and input to an emic (structural) account, and as a framework for comparing different emic accounts, lacks the emic account's validity. As an ethnography, the ethnography of communication identifies the communicative event as its focus, bearing in mind that the event is the product of an activity in a context by agents with a purpose. This approach breaks with the structuralist tradition which focuses on the structures which contribute to the construction of the event In addition, Hymes points out “a message implies the sharing (...) of (1) a code or codes in terms ow which the message is intelligible to (2) participants, minimally an addressor and addressee (..), in (3) an event constituted by its transmission and characterized by (4) a channel or channels [e.g. graphic, acoustic, visual], (5) a setting or a context, [a classroom, restaurant, dormitory room] (6) a definite form or shape to the message [often called a genre or a style], (7) a topic and comment [i.e., saying something about something. Hymes 1972:26 In this account, the first component, the codes, represents the main focus of structural linguistics to which Hymes has expanded the domain of interest. Hymes adds that while any of these components could be the focus of an ethnography of communication, the components are often interrelated: the setting, such as a dark room may favor an acoustic channel; certain types of participants will be more likely to appear in some social settings than others; and some forms will be more appropriate in some social settings than others. 6.2. Frake. Like other ethnographies, ethnographies of communication differ in a variety of ways. I have chosen Frake’s How to ask for a drink in Subanum, an Filipino community, as a good The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 8 representative of such ethnographies. In describing his approach to the ethnography of speaking he notes that one “cannot provide rules specifying exactly what message to select in a given situation.... The task of an ethnographer of speaking is to specify what the appropriate alternatives are in a given situation and what the consequences are of selecting one alternative over another.” (1979:88) Frake begins by establishing a context of drinking where he focuses on the festive as opposed to the informal gathering. The festive gathering is marked by a festive meal (which requires a meat side dish), an occasioning event, multi family participation and beer. The drinking of beer, unlike the consumption of any other beverage, occurs only during a festivity and must occur as part of any festivity. It occupies a crucial position as a focus of formal social gatherings. The period of festivity includes all relevant events between and including arrival and departure of participants. Part of the festive gathering involves beer-drinking sessions. During these sessions people sit around one or more beer jars. Individuals take turns drinking beer from the jar through a straw. Because water is added to the jar after each turn, the beer weakens with each round. Frake observed that the drinking sessions consisted of several discourse stages: Invitation-Permission: Provider of jar initiates by inviting someone to taste. The invited speaker squats before the jar and asks for permission to taste and addresses others in gathering (order of addressing and form of address have implications. The taster can invite next person to drink (but normally is the host. Jar Talk: During this phase the focus on discussing the quality of the beer provided by the host, emphasizing the quantity and quality of the responses that the speaker can elicit from his comments of the beer. Some invitees will be will be encouraged and some will be out. Discussion: The discussion phase begins with trivial gossip but the topics gradually become more serious as community concerns are introduced. These matters are deliberated by the participants and important factor of a successful outcome is based on verbal skill. Display of verbal art. Allows individuals to display their verbal art and helps to ensure that the festivity will end with good feelings. Frake points out that the discussion carries a social function, for it provides an opportunity for inter-family communication on matters of mutual concern. To understand point, we need to understand Subanum social organization where there are no positions of official authority like mayor or judge. Subanum society consists of a series of families (or lineages) where authority usually resides in the elders recognized for their wisdom. In such societies, decisions are carried out, not through threat of force, but through a consensus developed within the family, or in the case of inter-family matters during the drinking sessions are brought up for discussion. The development of a consensus in the resolution of these matters is highly dependent on one’s The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 9 verbal skills, not only with regard to logic and reason, but with respect to the speaker’s esteem, established significantly through verbal skills and the speakers argued position in the matter, which leads Frake (1979:93) to conclude, “the most skilled in 'talking from the straw' are de facto leaders of the society.” During the jar talk stage, individuals may be indirectly encouraged to stay or leave ostensibly because of their verbal performance, but more importantly because they are involved in the matter to be discussed. In addition, according to Frake, “The strategy of drinking talk is to manipulate the assignment of role relations among participants so that, ...,one can maximize his share of the encounter resources (drink and talk), thereby having an opportunity to assume an esteem-attracting and authority-wielding role.” In this ethnography, Frake provided most of the detailed on the event situated in the setting of Subanum festive drinking, but in the process he has brought in several other components: the participants, the form of the language (genre) used, especially with respect to verbal art, and topic and comment, though little was said in this area. Frake also illustrated the importance of the interrelationship between these components, something that Hymes regards as a key component of the ethnography of speaking. Interestingly, nothing was said about the code - the grammar of Subanum (or the channel) which would have been the sole topic of a structuralist linguist analysis. But it is also important to recognize that this ethnography did not go into the analysis of any specific interaction and concentrated mainly on the structure of Subanum festive drinking. Thus, for example, while one learned that verbal performance was central to Subanum life, one did not get a clear idea of how this was accomplished. 7. A Discourse Oriented Approach to Language and Culture I VIEW CULTURE as symbolic behavior, patterned Also within the structuralist organizations of, perceptions of, and beliefs about the world in symbolic terms. 285 LANGUAGE includes grammar, but framework, but attempting to break from goes beyond grammar. As a sign system, language has the it comes the “discourse oriented approach interesting property of being both unmotivated and arbitrary (purely symbolic in semiotic terms) and motivated (iconic and to language and culture” from Joel indexical in semiotic terms). 296 Sherzer (1987:297) who notes that, “increasingly, contemporary research in linguistic anthropology takes discourse as its starting point, theoretically and methodologically.” CONTEXT is to be understood in two senses here: In Sherzer ‘s view discourse is a level first the social and cultural backdrop, the ground rules and assumptions of language usage and second, language use, “related to, but distinct from the immediate, ongoing, and emerging actualities of grammar” and is akin to Wharf’s “fashions of speech events. speaking in that it encompasses patternings like the Hopi conceptualization of time given in chapter five. The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 10 IT IS DISCOURSE which creates, recreates, modifies, and fine tunes both culture and language and their intersection, and it is especially in verbally artistic discourse such as poetry, magic, verbal dueling, and political rhetoric that the potentials and resources provided by grammar, as well as cultural meanings and symbols, are exploited to the fullest and the essence of language-culture relationships become salient 296 7.1 An example from Kuna (Panama) poetic and magical discourse. Sherzer illustrates his approach by showing how a grammatical category can be used to achieve a magical effect To understand this point, Sherzer points out four possible (optional) verbal suffixes. Suffix Meaning Example Translation -kwici standing, in a vertical position sunmak-kwici `talking-standing' -mai lying, in a horizontal position kan-mai `sleeping-lying' -sii sitting) maskun-sii `eating-sitting' -nai perched, in a hinging position ua so-nai `fishing-perched' These suffixes are used in a magical curing chant. In this ritual, the chanter addresses the spirit of a dangerous snake while holding a real snake. As the says the words of the chant, the chanter guides the real snake through the motions described in the chant, with the understanding that the spirit snake is doing the same. At a crucial point in the chant the snake, euphemized as a vine: kali mokimakke-mai-ye `the snake is dragging in horizontal position (mai) kali piknimakkewa-mai-ye `the snake is turning over in horiz pos' (mai) During the next line (a magical formula): the snake is raised in the air. unni na pa onakko' anti sokekwiciye ‘simply indeed I raise` I am saying.’ Followed by a repetition of the earlier couplet but with the replacement of the suffix -mai with nai. Not only is this the substitution of a single morpheme, but when the morphemes are compared, by a single phoneme, and when the phonemes are compared, a single feature (the replacement of a bilabial nasal with an alveolar nasal). kaliti mokimakken-nai-ye kali piknimakken-nai-ye `the snake is dragging in vert pos' `the snake is turning over in vert pos' Thus with a minimally perceptible change in the chant’s poetry (an n for an m) the meaning has changed dramatically and according to Sherzer (299) “through this packing of a maximum of meaning into a minimum of form, grammar becomes poetry and poetry becomes magic. 299 7.2 An Example from Kuna political discourse. The speeches associated with the inauguration of a new community leader, are “typically bristling with intersecting and overlapping metaphors.” Sherzer recorded one such speech which used the same positional suffixes reported above, “ in conjunction with a complex of metaphors, The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 11 largely drawn from the Kuna plant and animal world, in order to represent Kuna political structure. CHIEFS ARE -nai hanging because they are perched in their hammocks in the center of the gathering house where they chant myths in public performances. CHIEF'S SPOKESMEN are -kwici standing, because they stand when they make speeches or sii (sitting) because they sit on benches surrounding the chiefs.. ORDINARY VILLAGERS are -sii (sitting) because they sit on benches behind the chiefs and spokesmen Both examples illustrate how a grammatical feature of a language can be used by a community to achieve meaningful, even magical effects. But rather than seeing this as an effect of the language on the thought of the Kuna people, Sherzer sees this grammatical property as a resource which the Kuna have been able to use successfully for their own purposes. 7.4 Kuna oral narrative. Among the other examples, Sherzer reports on an example of a Kuna narrative style in which the organization by describing one individual’s experiences and then another until the story is told. This kind of narrative is somewhat disorienting to the westerner who is accustomed to a timely progression of events.6 As a result, the narrative seems to jump back and forth in space and time. “One feature of this narrative that non-Kuna find particularly strange is the fact that the pepper plant is growing before the boy dies and that the boy is later found buried under the plant, as if his burial had caused the plant to grow.” 305 7.5 Summary. On the basis of these examples and others, Sherzer concludes that: discourse and especially the process of discourse structuring is the locus of the languageculture relationship” Furthermore, “discourse is also an embodiment of language. Grammar provides a set of potentials. Since these potentials are actualized in discourse, they can only be studied in discourse. (306). discourse and especially the process of discourse structuring is the locus of the languageculture relationship. In other words, discourse for Sherzer is in the domain of parole, and as such introduces actions, agents, creativity, though these remain unexplored. Furthermore, he draws on the langue/parole dialectic by showing that significance of specific examples of parole (discourse), such as the magical chant or the narrative by seeing how the users draw creatively from the resources provided by the language structure. His statement that “discourse is the is the broadest and most comprehensive level of linguistic form, content, and use” (306 ), however, suggests that the langue/parole relationship is not fully distinguished. 6 There are exceptions, such as Kurt Vonnegut’s Slaughter House Five which follows a narrative pattern quit similar to that reported by Sherzer. The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 12 8. Pragmatics 8.1 Austin’s How to do things with words. In 19xx, John Austin a philosopher at xxx, delivered a series of lectures at xxx with the deceptively simple title of “How to do things with words.” These essays ushered in the field of pragmatics, when Austin noted that not all language statements are descriptions (locutionary acts) which add new information to the sentence. (saying something about something) but are acts which accomplish something as a result of saying them. These statements, Austin called performatives.7 Performatives are words, which when found in statements known as illocutionary acts, actually accomplish something, given of course that the certain conditions are met. Locutionary Acts Illocutionary Acts The book is red. John saw that his dog needed to go for a walk. The man got in his car and sped off. I now pronounce you man and wife. I bet you a dollar that I can do this. We the jury find the defendant innocent. Illocutionary acts are neither true nor false. Rather they work or don’t work or in Austin’s words are ‘happy’ or ‘unhappy.’ This of course depends on the conditions surrounding the statement. For example, not anyone can name a boat; one has to be asked by the owner of the boat to do this. We also need a boat. Austin provided a more detailed list of things necessary for an illocutionary act to go right. , must be an accepted conventional procedure particular persons must be appropriate for the invocation of the act procedure must be executed correctly and completely person must have those thoughts and feelings requisite of the act must actually conduct themselves subsequently. The violating of these conditions (the bride and groom are already married, the speaker does not have the authority to marry people) results in an infelicity which makes the performance unhappy. Types of Acts Illocutionary acts: By bringing our attention to giving or answering a question, performatives, Austin made us aware of a giving some information or an assurance or a warning new dimension of language. First, we see announcing a verdict or an intention pronouncing a sentence, something of the magic of language in making an appointment or an appeal or a criticism making an identification or giving a description. that words actually have power. As such they have a connection to spiritual events such as the magic described above or the biblical “Let there be light.” Second Austin made us aware of a much richer conception of communication in which people were not simply describing the events around them but actually getting things 7 Austin also added a third type of speech act which he termed ‘perlocutionary.’ This is the act of “attempting or purporting or successfully achieving an illocutionary act” and is typically marked by verbs such as: convince; persuade; urge; surprise; humiliate. The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 13 done. Bourdieu (19xx) noted that Austin’s conditions, almost always are associated with the institution (chapter 2), an observation which promises to enrich our understanding of the institution as the central unit of social analysis. 8.3. Searle What is a Speech Act. John Searle expanded the frame work of Austin by exploring the formal make up of illocutionary acts using How to Promise S = Speaker; H = Hearer; p = proposition; T = Statement; A = Future Act 1. Normal input and output conditions obtain. 2. S expresses that p in the utterance of T 3. In expressing that p, S predicates a future act A of S 4. H would prefer S's doing A to his not doing A, and S believes H would prefer his doing A to his not doing A 5. It is not obvious to both S and H that S will do A in the normal course of events. 6. S intends to do A. 6'. S intends that the utterance of T will make him responsible for intending to do A. 7. S intends that the utterance of T will place him under an obligation to do A 8. S intends that the utterance of T will produce in H a belief that conditions (6) and (7) obtain by means of the recognition of the intention to produce that belief, and he intends this recognition to be achieved by means of the recognition of the sentence as one conventionally used to produce such beliefs. 9. The semantical rules of the dialect spoken by S and H are such that T is correctly and sincerely uttered if and only if conditions obtain (1)- (8). what he called constitutive rules. Like Austin, Searle noted that a sentence may contain an optional proposition and an obligatory illocutional component, though the illocutional component may be implied by the context. state, assert, describe, remark; comment; Searle then focused on the illocutional (speech act) warn; promise; aspect of the sentence. For example, Searle would take a command, order, request; specific verb like “to promise” and ask what are the criticize, censure, approve; apologize; necessary conditions that are involved in making the act welcome. possible, some of which are specific to the verb to promise and others more widely distributed. What is so striking about this line of investigation is the revealed complexity of performatives. The analysis of to promise should be seen as part of a larger project to examine the “differences between the various types of illocutionary acts could be simply resolved through a structural analysis...” It is tempting to think of the task of learning vocabulary as simply one of learning a signifier and a signified, however, when looking at the constitutive rules for a verb like to promise, we have to recognize that much more is involved, especially in the area of if - then statements and one’s social relationships. That is the study of how children learn performatives might provide important insights into the process of language learning and into the nature of performatives. But the most important aspect of studying performatives is to recognize that they represent a new domain of human interaction, that they introduce a type of symbolic communication that makes things happen by saying them. 8.3. The Conversational Implicature of H.P. Grice. (cf. H.P. Grice Logic and Conversation ) The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 14 H. P. Grice explored an area of language known as conversational implicature. This has to do with how people come to understand statements which are note fully explicit. Take for example the following hypothetical example offered by Grice (19xx). A: How is C getting on in his job [at the bank]? B: Oh quite well, I think; he likes his colleagues, and he hasn’t been to prison yet. The implicature in B is that C is likely to or is doing something that, if caught, will land him in prison. Although some ways of speaking are more direct than others, almost all involve some degree of implicature. Grice then proposed the cooperative principle which states that speakers expect their messages to honestly represent their intentions and that the other can comprehend the intent from the message.8 Grice notes that the CP is broader than just This line of thinking needs to be expanded. For example it may relate to intentionality, and for that matter the basis of customary law. language and is part of human interaction. What I find so fascinating is that the CP appears to arise as a logical necessity of having to live with and interact with others. If we were not to agree to the CP then it would follow that implicature would not be possible and communication would be quite limited, or in Grice’s words, “if you expect to get something out of the conversation, you expect the CP to be in place.” Given that the CP is universal to humans, it thus appears to be a constructed as opposed to an innate principle. Specif Maxims: that can be derived from the CP Because the CP is quite general, Grice 1. Quality: make contribution 1) as informative and 2) not more informative than required. proposed several maxims containing more 1. Quality: don’t say 1) what you believe to be false specific statements that can be derived from the and 2) that for which you lack adequate evidence. CP. These maxims can be used to account “for 2. Relation: Be relevant any implicatures that might depend on the 3. Manner: 1) avoid obscurity of expression; 2) avoid ambiguity; 3) be brief; 4) be orderly. presence of one rather than another of these singular terms in the sentence utters.” Needless to say, there may be other maxims, but uses these to show how implicature works. Returning to our example, we ask how the CP helps us to derive the implicature q: © is likely to steal money.) from B’s statement p: © hasn’t been to prison yet.)? First we note that, following the CP, both A and B believe that B has said something that statement q is relevant to their discussion about C. Furthermore, maxim 1 states that p is informative; 2 that it is true and informed; 3) that it is relevant and 4) that it is comprehensible by A. Given this permits A to address the question of “what does C’s work at the bank have to do 8 Grice stated the Cooperative Principle as follows: Make your conversational contribution such as is required, at the stage at which it occurs, by the accepted purpose or direction of the talk exchange in which you are engaged. The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 15 with going to prison?” Given the common background which A and B share, A should easily come up with the implicature. To calculate a conversational implicature is to calculate what has to be supposed in order to preserve the supposition that the CP is being observed, and since there may be various possible specific explanations, a list of which may be open... Variations. The normal situation or course would be to converse without violating any of the conversational maxims as was the case in our first example (and A in the sidebar). B's statement must (A) No maxims violated A: I am out of petrol [gas]. B: There is a garage around the corner. (B) Two maxims are in conflict and one has to be broken. A. Where does C live? B: Somewhere in the south of France. (C) A maxim is violated for the purpose of producing a conversational implicature that the sender does not wish to say directly. Letter of recommendation: Dear A, ... C’s command of English is excellent, and his attendance at tutorials has been regular. Yours, etc. B. be relevant to A's statement so that A concludes that one can buy petrol at the garage. However sometimes maxims are broken for good reason. Grice mentions a common situation (B) when two maxims are in conflict and one has to be broken. B doesn’t have the requested information and can either provide the wrong information or be vague. B chooses vagueness as the best option. A third example (C) involves breaking a maxim to lead to an implicature which the sender does not want to say directly. By saying nothing about Mr. X's ability, the writer is conveying the implicature that X has no other positive attributes. This type of white lie violates a maxim but saves face (see Goffman below) for it allows B. to respond honestly to A while not directly saying anything negative about C, thus preserving their relationship.. These maxims can be further violated as in the outright lying, but it is important to remember that even lying cannot be successful were the CP not in place and that were lying to dominate messages that the CP would be undermined and communication more difficult cases like B and C rely on the existence. 9. Face work and politeness 9.1. Erving Goffman’s Face Work. The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 16 Along the lines of Grice’s work Definitions Wrong face: when information is brought forth in some way that with the cooperative principle cannot be integrated... Out of face: when participant is without “having a ready line of the having to do with the necessary kind of participants are supposed to take. conventions of living together, Poise: capacity to conceal any tendency to become shamefaced during encounters with others. Goffman introduced the Save face: process of sustaining an impression that face has not been lost. concept of face which he Give face: (Chinese usage) helping someone else improve face. defines as “the positive social Line: a specific type of face in a specific situation. value a person effectively claims for himself by the line others assume he has taken during a particular contact.” 306 Although the definitions of “positive social value” will differ from community, Goffman claims that the concept of face and the desire to maintain one’s own face are universal and terms the effort one gives to maintaining face he terms “facework..” The motivating force behind facework is “the desire to maintain one’s own self respect,” which because of the strong emotions that are associated with matters of face, represents a real need. The desire to maintain self respect involves the assessment of others, much of facework has to do with maintaining what Goffman calls a ritual equilibrium with others. The word ritual not emphasizes the almost spiritual obligation behind facework and that “one’s face is a sacred thing.” Maintaining this ritual equilibrium helps to explain why we feel obliged to “save the feelings and face of others present.” For example given another’s loss of control, we find that “others may protectively turn away from him to give him time to assemble himself.” The ritual equilibrium is disrupted by the face threatening act: something that lowers the face of the other. Maintaining ritual equilibrium involves two types of techniques which 1) aim to avoid face-threatening encounters or 2) measures to restore the equilibrium after it has been disturbed . Goffman termed these techniques “avoidance” and “corrective measures.” Avoidance involve techniques of avoiding events that will damage face Total avoidance: Avoiding those who might damage face. Defensive measures: With respect to topics that might force the individual to make statements detrimental to one’s face, one can attempt to avoid such topics, shift the topic or suppress or hedge on opinions which are detrimental to face in the situation. Protective maneuvers: The individual can show respect and politeness; show discretion about feelings on topics that might embarrass others; use euphemisms and white lies; be courteous; joke; neutralize offending activities by explaining them in advance. The Corrective Process: A threat to face occurs when someone does something to disrupt the ritual equilibrium. This is often a very emotional process. The normal corrective process involves the following stages. the challenge where the participants call attention to the misconduct; the offering: whereby a participant, typically the offender, is given a chance to correct for the offence and reestablish the expressive order (an apology or an excuse, ..) the acceptance (or not) by the offended of offering; gratitude by the offender which reestablishes the ritual equilibrium. The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 17 The final reestablishment of equilibrium depends on the agreement of both the offended and offender that the face threat has been resolved. Of course, corrections are not always made, but these alternatives rarely restore the equilibrium. have high costs. For example: the offender ignores the challenge and continues the face-threatening activity - this can be seen as face-saving to the offender but it does not restore the equilibrium; the offender denies challenge - this increases the face threat by challenging the other’s claim; the offender challenges the offended, by storming off - this also further disrupts the equilibrium. In contrast to the sacred ritual of face-saving, Goffman introduces a profane version of face work whose aim is self-serving rather than equilibrium restoring. Nevertheless, the successful working of this secular version depends on the existence of the ritual version. For example, one can ‘fish for complements’ by pretending modesty, knowing full well that ritual face work will lead the other to restore equilibrium by countering the modesty with praise. One can do more, like arrange for favorable events to appear or falsely claiming injury to face, which calls for the other to engage in corrective measure. Insights. Goffman’s face provides two important perspectives on the theory of subject, 1) the characterization of a dimension of self as an image to be presented and 2) that the role of the subject as an agent or player with the responsibility to maintain ritual equilibrium. The first perspective is similar to the concept of an inner and outer self suggested by Mead and Berger & Luckmann in that there is the real me inside which I reveal only as I see fit (when I am in control) and then there is the public me, consisting of my face, and which I attempt to manage. The existence of self, Berger and Luckmann argue, is made possible by the process of (symbolic) objectivization, whereby through the use of objects (primarily language signs) we can project ourselves into the external world for others to appropriate and interpret. But it also opens up an interesting connection with Grice’s cooperative principle. As social beings we have a need to be together and apparently a need to maintain a ritual equilibrium of face. The second perspective elaborates the nature of agency and points out that it is often focused on things like maintaining equilibrium and not totally free to do what it wants. Both of these perspectives go beyond the structuralist view of presenting and analyzing static structures. The concept of face and the desire to maintain equilibrium also helps us to understand why we have greetings and leave takings. As Goffman notes, “greetings of course serve to serve to clarify and fix roles that the participants will take during the occasion. This is well brought out in our presentation of the greeting system used by the Wolof. Leave takings can also do this, but in addition leaving somebody is a face-threatening act and leaves one to wonder if in the process of leaving the equilibrium has been broken. This may help to explain why the two most common ways of taking leave are either the assurance that this rift is temporary (see you later, au revoir, The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 18 hasta mañana) or a blessing to show that the ritual equilibrium is still en effect (Farewell (literally ‘travel well’, via con Dios (go with God)). 9.2. Brown and Levinson’s Politeness. Penelope Brown and Steven Levinson have taken Goffman’s concept of face and modified it in two ways: first inserting it into structuralist framework which involves both the emphasis of language universals and language rules as the basis of explanation; and secondly, dividing the concept of face into two components, negative face and positive face. Positive Face is the same as Goffman’s face, while negative face consists of the wish by individuals to act without the constraints imposed by society. We noted earlier that Goffman’s (positive) face resembled G.H. Mead’s me which is the public, socialized self. To this, we can add the parallel of Brown and Levinson’s negative face resembling Mead’s I, the private, impulsive self. Unlike Goffman, Brown and Levinson do not emphasize the interpersonal dimension of face and the importance of maintaining the ritual equilibrium, but they do emphasize the importance of the face threatening act, the FTA, but now both one’s positive and negative face can be threatened.. First Distinction: Kinds of face threatened Speaker threaten’s Hearer’s Negative Face Speaker threaten’s Hearer’s Positive face [imposition] Speaker’s negative evaluation of hearer Speaker puts pressure on hearer to act (orders, (disapproval, criticism, disagreement) requests); Speaker’s indifference to hearer’s positive face; Speaker offers suggestions and advice (remindings, Speaker’s irreverence toward hearer; threats and warnings); Speaker introducing bad news about hearer or Speaker puts hearer in debt (offers, promises) good news about Speaker; Speaker expresses desire or envy of hearer’s Speaker’s raising divisive topics (politics); possessions which lead hearer to think that he has to Speaker’s non-cooperation; wrong terms of protect them: complements, envy, expressions of address strong emotion (hatred, anger, lust). Second distinctions: Threats to Hearer’s face versus threats to Speaker’s Those that offend Speaker’s negative Face Those that damage Speaker’s positive face Speaker expressing thanks; Speaker’s apologies; Speaker acceptance of hearer’s thanks; Speaker’s acceptance of a complement; Speaker’s excuses; Speaker’s breakdown of physical control:, Speaker’s acceptance of offers; Speaker-humiliation Speaker’s response to hearer’s faux pas; unwilling Speaker’s confessions promises and offers An important contribution from Brown and Levinson is the recognition that FTAs are often necessary. All of use have encountered situations where we have had to tell others that they have behaved badly, that their work is inferior, that we no longer love them, or in other words, to consider doing a face threatening act. Brown and Levinson have provided an account of the strategy involved in doing these FTAs. Our reluctance to do these things is the recognition that these are FTAs Positive Redress The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 19 Don’t do the FTA Brown and Levinson have introduced a taxonomy Without redressive action of strategies which people use in these situations. Do On the Record Of course, it is sometimes possible not to do the FTA With positive FTA, or at least postpone it. Usually, this option redressive politeness leads to other complications that are even more action negative distasteful so that usually we do decide to do the politeness FTA, how here we have a choice of doing it Off Record directly (on record) or indirectly (off record) using the mechanism of implicature described by Grice. An off record FTA is indeed less threatening, but it runs the risk of not being understood, or even if it is, the act is more easily ignored, for the other can claim that no such FTA was issued. The on record FTA has the advantage of clarity, and because of this it is more threatening.9 Accompanying the on-record strategy is the possibility for redress, that is to show deference to Hearers positive face (honor) or negative face (privacy). Redress need not be undertaken if the social distance between the self and other (boss/worker; teacher/student) is such that FTAs are seen as offered as a matter of course. Computing the Weightiness of an FTA. The preceding paragraph suggested that part of the A. Wx = D(S,H) + P (H,S) + Rx B. Weight of the FTE C. Social Distance between S and H for the purposes of that act and as determined by such things as the frequency of interaction and the kinds of material and nonmaterial goods exchanged.... D. Power differential (Weber’s sense). Degree to which H can impose his own plans and own face at the expense of S’s plans and face. E. Ranking of imposition of the act. decision of how to carry out a FTA depends on the degree of seriousness (weight) of the FTA). Brown and Levinson offer a tentative formula for determining the weight of any FTA and note that the variables D, P and R can vary not only for different acts, but for different individuals and for different social settings. Politeness, like the concept of face it is built on offers a real insight about the ways in which people interact and why. It opens the potential for studying concepts of face cross culturally. I am reminded of a FTA common to the Bandi people in Liberia for whom the FTA “Look at you.” is regarded as an extremely serious FTA and could lead to a fine. Interestingly this FTA is off record, at least formally, because it requires an implication to arrive at the meaning of the FTA. In fact, I am not completely certain what the implicature is, but I think it is something like, 9 A case can be made for the on-record strategy, because unlike the off-record strategy it can be said to be non-manipulative. “Look, let me be straight with you - talk with you as a friend.. You really messed up this job.” The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 20 “Look at the fool you are making of yourself.” There are also indications that there may be different weights according to the dimensions of positive and negative face (Li Qing, 2000). Finally, following Bourdieu’s observation that Austin’s and Searle’s work with speech acts might be even more insightful were the conditions of felicities be associated with the institution, we note the same for face. That is, the weighting of face, may be more easily understood in the context with specific institutions than with a general formula. 10. Narrative Structure. William Labov, whom we encountered earlier in the formulation of sociolinguistics, has also been an innovator in other areas of linguistics as well, including his contribution to narrative structure. This development arose in part from Labov’s work with children in the inner city, currently known as African Americans. Part of the ideology (see chapter xx) at that time was concept called the culture of poverty (Bereitter and Engleman) who claimed that these children spoke poorly and did poorly in school because this culture of poverty held them back. Labov, as wee shall see in chapter yy, was able to demonstrate using his analysis of narrative structure that the speech of these children was as well organized and articulate (if not more so) than their suburban counterparts. The Overall Structure of narrative 2. Abstract: Summary of the whole story at the beginning. Optional 3. Orientation: Identification of the time, place, persons and the activity or situation. (Can be at the beginning or interspersed in the text.) 4. Complicating Action: 5. Evaluation: the means used by the narrator to indicate the point of the narrative why the story is being told and what the narrator is getting at. 6. Result or resolution 7. Coda: Signaling that the narrative is finished. May also contain general observations or show the effects of the events of the narrator. Bring the narrator and the audience back to the point where they entered the narrative. 8. This narrative structure is illustrated in the following new story. Story Structure Abstract Orientation Evaluation US troops ambushed in Honduras TEGUCIGALPA UNITED STATES troops in Honduras were put on high alert after at least six American soldiers were wounded, two seriously, in a suspected leftist guerrilla ambush yesterday, United States officials said. Six or seven soldiers were wounded when at least three men, believed to be leftist guerrillas, used high-powered weapons in an ambush of bus carrying 28 passengers 20 kilometers north of the capital Tegucigalpa United States embassy spokesman Terry Kneebone said. The bus was carrying the soldiers from a pleasure trip at a beach on the Atlantic Coast. Time Structure The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Complicating Action Resolution Coda Chapter 10, Page 21 “It was a surprise attack" Southern Command spokesman Captain Art Haubold said in Panama City. "The US forces did not return fire. They kept going to get out of the area as quickly as possible." A Teguicigalpa radio station said an unidentified caller said the leftist group Morazanista Patriotic Liberation Front claimed responsibility for the attack. NZPA-Reuter 10. Interactional Analysis: Deborah Tannen does “anthropologically oriented.. discourse analysis” and “sociolinguistics” at Georgetown University. She traces her theoretical orientation to Robin Lakoff’s work on (conversational style) and John Gumpers work on (interactional sociolinguistics) which maintains that: roles are not given but are created in social interaction; context is not given but is constituted by talk and action; nothing that occurs in interaction is the sole doing of one party but rather is a “joint production,” the result of the interaction of individuals’ ways of speaking... linguistic features (such as interruption, volume of talk, indirectness, and so on) can never be aligned on a one-to-one basis with interactional intentions or meanings, in the sense that a word can be assigned a meaning. No language has meaning except by reference to how it is “framed” (Bateson 1972, Goffman 1974) or contextualized (Becker 1979, 1984; Gumperz 1982a). At the heart of Tannen’s work is the question dominance and power and its relationship to differences in style that work to the disadvantage of members of groups that are stigmatized in our society and to the advantage of those who have the power to enforce their interpretations. Again, following Gumperz, she considers an important part of her work “to confront and counteract the social inequality that results form negative stereotyping of minority cultural groups.”p9 Because dominance and power are closely associated with issues of gender, Tannen has conducted much of her research in that area and published both popular and professional books on this topic because she argues that “What is required to effect change is an understanding of the patterns of human behavior as they exist today, an appreciation of the complexity of these patterns, and a humane respect for human beings” She describes her methodology, derived from Gumpers, as beginning with the taping of naturally occurring conversations, 2) then identifying segments in which trouble is evident, then 3) looking for culturally patterned differences in signaling meaning that could account for the trouble. She will often playing the recording, or segments of it back to participants in order to solicit their spontaneous interpretations and reactions, and also, perhaps later, soliciting their responses to the researcher’s interpretations; and play segments of the interaction for other The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 22 members of the cultural groups represented by the speakers in order to discuss patterns of interpretations. 6 One of the most exciting aspects of her work is this ethnological approach to discourse, namely that of actually observing what is going on in encounters by people of different social rank and power or of different cultural backgrounds. This methodology differs from pure sociolinguistic research which looks for sociolinguistic variables but not their meaning and from narrative structural analysis which looks for the structure of a text, for Tannen, analyzing a text is to answer the question of what is really going on? For example, in analyzing systematic differences in women’s and men’s characteristic styles (cf. You Just Don’t Understand) she found that these styles often put women in a subordinate position in their interactions with men. For example, she found that in lecturing/listening situations: men lecture, women listen; in conflict situations: women are inclined to avoid conflict which puts them at a disadvantage; in conversations, men often end up interrupting women because “men who approach conversation as a contest are likely to expend effort not to support the other’s talk but to lead the conversation in another direction perhaps one in which they can take center stage by telling a story or joke or displaying knowledge. She is also famous for her analysis of a thanksgiving dinner involving two 1. Topic: a) prefer personal topics, b) shift topics abruptly, c) introduce topics without hesitance, d) persist if new topic is not picked up, reintroduce it, repeatedly if necessary. 2. Genre: a) tell more stories, b) tell stories in rounds, c) internal evaluation is preferred over external. Evaluation < Labov’s Narrational analysis. Internal (in the story); external: stated explicitly) 3. Pacing: a) faster rate of speech, b) inner-turn pauses avoided (silence is regarded as lack of rapport), c) faster turn taking, d) cooperative overlap and participatory listenership. 4. Expressive paralinguistics: a) expressive phonology, b) pitch and amplitude shifts, c) marked voice quality, d) strategic within-turn pauses. different culture types, one from the East Coast, and the other from the west cost. She found that the discourse style from the New York area is more intense than the West Cost type (see sidebar). While it may not be too surprising to find that miscommunication was more likely to occur among speakers using different discourse styles, she also found that discourse features like interruptions and overlaps can be regarded as disruptive and rude by speakers of one style (West Cost) can actually be seen as facilitating and friendly to speakers of another style (East Coast). 11. Summary Questions for Study and Review The Consequences of Language: The relationship between Language and Communication? Chapter 10, Page 23 1. The following folktale was told by Wofo Govina Otto on January 22, 1979 in Lome Togo from Benin, and recorded by Zinta Contrad (19xx). As far as we know Mr. Otto has never heard of CLS. Nevertheless, the story lends itself rather nicely to a structural analysis. Heina, Leopard, and Goat 1. Listen to the tale. (Let the talk come.) Positive politeness Negative It politeness strategies: 2. The tale strategies: flew until it fell on Goat. (THE ONE WHO SAYS EEEEE)* fell on Hyena. It fell Leopard (THAT'S RIGHT!) They were there, theyBewere there until oneindirect Notice, attend to Hon (his/her interests, wants, needs, goods) direct/conventionally day Goat married GoatH) came and took Leopard in Question, marriage. Exaggerate (interest, approval,Leopard. sympathy with hedge 3. When Goat married Leopard, it was Goat who would go to the market to do the Intensify trading. interest to HWhen she returned from the market, she usually Be pessimistic brought back things for Use ingroup markers Minimize the size of in~position on H heridentity husband. 4. agreement Meanwhile, a friend craved Goat. Leopard's friend is Hyena. Hyena craved Goat Seek Give deference and wanted to kill her. In spite of Hyena's craving for goat meat, Hyena never Avoid disagreement Apologize could find a way to get any. Hyena thought a while, wondered how he was going to Presuppose/raise/assert common ground Impersonalize S and H:' avoid get Goat. Joke pronouns and place. 'you' 5. He went out, came back, and told Leopard he had seen Goat's shit '1' some shit very tasty. He <Hyena> wondered how hea take ... Assert or Goat's presuppose S'swas knowledge of and concern for H's wants aloud ... State thecould FTA as general rule how he take Goat's shit from her stomach? Leopard asked. "Really?" Hyena said, Offer, promise Nominalize "Yes!" Leopard said he <Hyena> was lying. Hyena said it was true, he saw it Be optimistic Go doubt on record incurring a debt, or as himself. Goat's shit is very tasty. Why did he <Leopard> himas<Hyena>? (LEOPARD ALREADY Include both S and HHAS in the activityBEEN TRICKED HYENA!) That's right. not indebting H 6. (or ask Hyena out and returned ... went out and returned after buying tiger nuts ... Give for)went reasons (THAT'S IT) the black nuts. He brought the black tiger nuts and gave them to Assume or assert reciprocity Leopard to eat. (TO EAT AND SEE FOR HIMSELF) Leopard ate the nuts and discovered Give giftshow to Hgood (goods, sympathy, cooperation) they tasted.understanding, Leopard said, "Ooooooh! Did I have these things here all this time and not know it? Well, okay we'll see." (LEOPARD ATE THE TIGER NUTS THINKING HE WAS EATING THE GOAT'S SHIT) Leopard said he was ready to eat Goat's shit. (LEOPARD ATE THE TIGER NUTS.) Leopard ate the tiger nuts. Off-record 7. Nowstrategies: then, soon afterward, Hyena returned to Leopard, asking him what they could do to get more nuts. Leopard asked, what did he usually do to make the shit so Those violating conversational see tasty? Grice's Hyena told him to get maxims, a club ... a Chapter big club 3~ ... a very big club. The first of nut he hits will notViolate be tasty. second one won't Violate be tasty either. It's Violate maxim Relevance maxim The of Quality maxim of Manner the third one that will beUnderstate tasty ... and the fourth one ...Be(THE THIRD ONE WILL Give hints/clues ambiguous KILL HER)!) Yes, that's the tasty one. So, Leopard went to carve a club - he Give association went toclues carve a club. He Overstate carved a hardwood club; he carvedBeitvague very well with a Presuppose Use tautologies Over~generalize good handle on it. 8. In the meantime, while Goat at the market, her childrenDisplace overheard Usewas contradictions H Hyena and Leopard conversing. Goat's children were in the house Be ironic Be incomplete; use ellipsis (LISTENING) listening to the conversation. They heard what was being said. (WELL, IS IT UseBAD metaphors NOT FROM CHILDREN YOU NORMALLY HEAR NEWS?) It's from children alright. (RIGHT!) The children got up, left their father in a hurry prrrr1 and went to tell their mother at Use rhetorical questions the market (GOAT), Goat who was at the market. Goat asked whether indeed, this was all true. Oh! Goat headed back but stopped to buy honey - buy honey. ... She poured it in a calabash, put the calabash on her head, and returned home. 9. When she arrived, her husband greeted her, saying "Agoo!" (HE WELCOMED HER.) He Suggested Readings welcomed her and she responded, "Yoooo." She asked him to help her remove the load from her head. Her husband, Leopard, helped her unload, but while he was helping her, the woman <Goat> shook the container and poured the contents on him Erving Goffman On Face-Work: An analysis of Ritual Social Interaction. Goffman, Interaction - all over him. He asked, what was it she poured on him? She told him not to worry.onIt was somthing very good.1967 She told him to lick it and see. He licked Ritual: Essays Face-to face Behavior. it and found it very tasty. He asked what it was. She said it was Hyena's urine. (OH! IT'S ALL OVER' <laughter> HE' GOING TO KILL HYENA NOW!) 10. Yes indeed! (OH!) She said it was Hyena's urine. He asked whether it was true that it's Hyena's urine? She said, "Yes indeed! Isn't it sweet?" She said she bought it as she was coming home from the market. He said, "Oh well, if Hyena's urine tastes this good, all is well." 11. Hyena hadn't arrived yet. He had gone home to prepare (FOR GOAT) ... for Goat herself. Leopard said to Goat. "Have you heard what Hyena said about you? He brought her some of the tiger nuts, what remained of them. 12. (THAT WAS GOAT'S SHIT!) Yes, her shit! And if he hit her, more shit would come out! She said, "is that so? Nonetheless, it is Hyena's urine that is sweeter than my shit." Goat and Leopard then conspired against Hyena (THEY WERE CONSPIRING AGAINST HYENA U-U-U-U-U). 13. They were talking when Hyena arrived. Hyena sat down and chatted for awhile. Leopard went behind Hyena - and then said yudi! (AI!) They started to beat Hyena. Goat lashed at him - she lashed at him! Urine was running out of his bottom while Leopard licked it, saying "It's not sweet!" Goat said, "Just continue. It'll get sweet." 14. Goat said it was not enough yet. They beat Hyena until he was dead kranananananananana! Finally, Hyena was dead. They took Hyena, singed his hair, cooked him and ate him. 15. That is why they say, in this world (DON'T SEEK EVIL FOR YOUR BROTHER ...) don't seek evil for your brother, just do things your own way and be mindful of what goes on around you. For if you wish evil on your friend, you will meet his end.