1 CHAPTER 9 OVERVEIW OF THESIS This thesis explored the subjective quality of life of people with MS, their partners, and a control comparison group. The unique combination of physical symptoms and the unpredictable, fluctuating disease course of MS provided an interesting context within which to explore subjective quality of life. Also, given the limited availability of treatments for MS, research with such a focus was needed to aid people with MS to live full and satisfying lives within the unique constraints of their condition. An overview of the thesis is now presented. Implications for future research and interventions are discussed, as are the strengths and limitations of the thesis. Finally conclusions are drawn. Background to the Thesis A model proposed to explain the process whereby psychological factors maintain subjective quality of life in an adaptive range was generated and tested by the thesis. This model was based on the presupposition that life circumstances exert only a limited influence on subjective quality of life (Argyle, in press). This is evidenced by the consistent of life satisfaction evaluations across a range of life situations that have resulted in the estimation of a life satisfaction ‘gold standard’ at 75+/-2.5 per cent SM (Cummins, 1995). In the first five chapters of the thesis literature was reviewed that linked psychological variables to subjective quality of life. Cummins and Cahill (in press) have proposed a model that links personality, positive cognitive biases of self-esteem, optimism and primary control, and subjective quality of life. The thesis additionally integrated perceived social support and secondary control into this model. It was hypothesised that these two variables contribute to subjective quality of life in a consistent and predictable way. The inclusion of perceived social support into the 2 model was based on previous research that has consistently demonstrated a positive association between perceived social support and subjective quality of life (Abbey et al., 1985; Holahan et al., 1996; Revenson et al, 1991; Schaefer, Coyne, & Lazarus, 1981). Furthermore, the effect of perceived social support was hypothesised to be subject to a threshold effect such that people under stress feel the absence of social support more acutely (Cohen & Wills, 1985). Secondary control was hypothesised to mediate between positive cognitive bias and subjective quality of life. The inclusion of a mediating role of secondary control in the model was based on Wiesz et al.’s (1984) definition of secondary control as accepting or adjusting to one’s situation. It was proposed that life situations can challenge positive cognitive biases and that secondary control acts to accommodate life situations and maintain subjective quality of life. It was hypothesised that the model is valid for all types of people, including those with MS, but that the variables of the model interact differently between different groups of people. This model was illustrated in Figure 5.2 in Chapter 5. Summary of Studies The model was explored through two linked studies presented in chapter 7 and 8. In the Study One a technique called ‘photo voice’ was utilised to obtain qualitative information to inform the materials of the second study. This study used photographs taken by the participants to explore, in an interview, the factors that effected their quality of life and the ways in which they coped with everyday difficulties. People took photographs of both positive and negative influences on their quality of life including family members, pets, homes, wheelchairs and medications. They also reported a wide range of ways they coped with everyday difficulties including two novel approaches: ‘ventilation’, which included yelling and crying, and ‘give up’. The qualitative information obtained from Study One valuably informed the materials for Study Two. Specifically, two new items were added to the Primary and Secondary Control Scale, a new domain was added to the Comprehensive Quality of Life Scale and potentially biased items were identified in the NEO-Five Factor 3 Inventory and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory. The Perceived Social Support Scale was also modified to include varied sources of support, and a subscale was designed to assess support from pets. The second study employed a quantitative methodology comprising a self-report questionnaire designed to measure the variables of the model. This study involved a larger group of people with MS, their partners and a control comparison group. The findings of Study Two supported the proposed model for each group. The variables of the model accounted for a significant portion of the subjective quality of life for each group (45 % for controls, 59% for people with MS and 58% for partners of people with MS). Personality, specifically neuroticism, predicted subjective quality of life. Positive cognitive bias and perceived social support accounted for a significant portion of subjective quality of life after personality was controlled. Also, secondary control mediated between positive cognitive bias and subjective quality of life for each of the three groups. Consistent with predictions of the thesis, the variables of the model interacted differently between the three groups and people with MS reported a lower subjective quality of life than the other two groups. The subjective quality of life of the three groups was similarly negatively effected by neuroticism. The three positive cognitive biases differently predicted the subjective quality of life of the three groups. Selfesteem was highly positively associated with the subjective quality of life of all three groups. However, people with MS reported a low self-esteem. Additionally, optimism positively predicted the subjective quality of life of controls, and primary control negatively predicted the subjective quality of life of people with MS and positively predicted for partners. As hypothesised, perceived social support consistently positively predicted the subjective quality of life of the three groups and was subject to a threshold effect. People with MS felt the absence of support more acutely than the other two groups. 4 The value of support from pets for people with MS suggested by Study One was also confirmed. The adaptive outcome of primary control was in accordance with the Discrimination Model of control that claims that the use of primary control is adaptive when the situation is controllable (Folkman 1984; Thompson et al., 1998). No support was found for either the Primacy/Back-Up model that claims that primary control is more adaptive than secondary control regardless of the controllability of the situation (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995), or Heep’s (2000) assertion that balanced levels of high primary and secondary control are positively associated with well-being. The effect of secondary control on subjective quality of life varied between the three groups. The mediating effect of secondary control was most for people with MS, less for partners, and least of all for controls. People with MS utilised a high level of avoidant secondary control that was negatively associated with subjective quality of life. Thus, people with MS were using secondary control in maladaptive ways. The adaptive outcome of secondary control evident in the study did not support any of the three models of control. ‘Ventilation’ commonly negatively predicted subjective quality of life. Thus, the utility of the inclusion of this novel form of secondary control was confirmed. However, the effect of the other four types varied between the groups. Most notably, the five types of secondary control were only negatively associated with the subjective quality of life of people with MS, had a mixed effect for partners, and a negligible impact for controls. Thus, secondary control efforts had the most impact on well-being in the most stressful life circumstances. The analysis of the adaptive outcome of secondary control highlighted the value of analysing the five type secondary control separately. This suggested limitations to the current models of control (i.e. The Discrimination Model (Folkman, 1984; Thompson et al., 1998), the Primacy/Back-Up Model (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995)) 5 that compare only average primary and secondary control and therefore do not accurately represent the varied functions and outcomes of secondary control. Implications of the Findings The implications of the findings of the thesis for interventions aimed at improving subjective quality of life and for future research have been discussed previously and are now summarised. Implications for Interventions The thesis aimed to inform interventions aimed at improving the subjective quality of life of people with MS and their partners. The following recommendations were made based on the findings of the thesis: Extraversion and neuroticism are traits with a strong genetic link (Tellegen et al., 1998), as such they are not likely to be appropriate targets for intervention. However, other variables and processes identified by the model and confirmed by this thesis are potentially malleable and could be effective targets for interventions The findings suggested that improvement to self-esteem will likely benefit subjective quality of life for people with MS, partners and controls. This is especially relevant to people with MS who reported lowered self-esteem. Previous research hypothesises that positive cognitive biases are maintained by failure experiences (Cummins & Nistico, in press). Thus, interventions could focus on reducing the experience of failure, or conversely increasing successful experiences of people with MS. Additionally, this research found that secondary control mediates between positive cognitive bias and subjective quality of life. Secondary control was proposed to maintain the lower boundary of positive cognitive bias. However, as the adaptive outcome of secondary control could not be determined by this thesis, no interventions regarding secondary control for people with MS were made. However, situation bias was positively associated with the subjective quality of life of partners. Thus, 6 partners would likely benefit from assistance to see the positive in their situation. Interventions may include counselling and the promotion of the positive aspects of MS. Further research needs to explore potential positive aspects of MS, these may include the opportunity for a renewed perspective on life or the sense of achievement gained through caring for a partner. Primary control may also be an effective target for interventions. Partners would benefit from increased feelings of personal competence. For example, interventions could provide partners with access to resources, such as life skills training or educational materials, so that they feel better equipped to deal with potential stressors. If these resources already exist, then interventions could focus on raising awareness of them to partners. Because, as Thompson (1981) asserts, control does not need to be exercised for it to be effective and control does not need to be real, just perceived, for it to influence the aversiveness of a stressful encounter (Thompson, 1981). While primary control positively effected the quality of life of partners, it negatively effected that of people with MS. Therefore, people with MS could be assisted to decrease levels of primary control. For example, it may be more adaptive for people with MS to relinquish personal control to other people such as partners or health-care professionals. However, previous researchers (Rothbaum et al., 1982) suggest that this may not be an easy task as control is so valued that the quest for it is rarely abandoned. Individuals are instead likely to shift from one method of control to another. Therefore, interventions may be more effective if they assisted people with MS to re-focus their primary control on controllable situations. Interventions could provide people with MS opportunities for primary control. For example, people with MS could be taught new skills in order to develop feelings of personal mastery. Further research needs to explore this possibility. Improvements in perceived social support will also likely benefit the subjective quality of life of both people with MS, partners and controls. This is especially 7 important to people with MS who felt the absence of social support more acutely and to partners who reported low perceived support in Study Two. As discussed, when designing and implementing interventions both the positive and problematic aspects of social support need to be considered (Holahan et al., 1996; Revenson et al, 1991). Thus, interventions need to focus not only on increasing social contact, but also on improving the quality of social contact. Thus, potential interventions include organised social activities, support groups, and relationship and individual counselling. Interventions could also focus on support received from pets. Interventions may include promoting the value of pets as important sources of support and assistance to people in the care of their pet, such as the provision of volunteers to exercise pets, or respite care for pets during times of hospitalization. The interventions recommended have so far focussed on individuals, however interventions aimed at improving the subjective quality of life of people with MS and their partners may also need to extend beyond the individual to the context surrounding the individual. The capacity of the individual to change their subjective quality of life may be limited by life circumstances. For example, the maladaptiveness of primary control evident in this study may lie not in the individual, but in a world that results in uncontrollable situations for people with disabilities to such an extent that reduced subjective quality of life is inevitable. Thus, interventions could focus on providing a more controllable world for people with MS and other disabilities. For example, by lobbying local government to ensure that new buildings and infrastructure are disability friendly. Implications for Future Research The findings of the study and consideration of the application of these findings to interventions aimed at improving subjective quality of life have highlighted some avenues for future research. These are now summarised. Further research is needed to inform the proposed interventions recommended to improve the subjective quality of life of people with MS and their partners. For 8 example, research is needed to explore the support needs of people with MS, and the reasons why partners perceived less support than people with MS. Research needs also to determine ways that partners can feel more personally competent and how people with MS can develop more self-esteem. Research also needs to consider the contextual issues regarding subjective quality of life to determine to what extent the subjective quality of life of partners and people with MS is restrained by life circumstances. For example, while beliefs of personal competence are beneficial to partners, there may be very little capacity for development of this belief in partner’s lives. Also, their ability to maintain satisfying social support networks may be constrained by their caregiving responsibilities. The inconsistency of some of the findings of the thesis with previous research also warrants further investigation. In particular, further research needs to explore the previously asserted stable nature of personality (Tellegen et al., 1998) especially in relation to people with chronic illness. In the meanwhile, this thesis recommends that any further research exploring personality and chronic illness undertake a process, similar to that employed in this thesis, in which measures are assessed for suitability to the population in question. The utility of the inclusion of self-esteem in a model to explain subjective quality of life was questioned by the findings of this thesis. Self-esteem was highly correlated with the subjective quality of life of all three groups and served to weaken statistical analysis. The inclusion of self-esteem in the model may be superfluous and needs to be research further. The relationship between self-esteem and subjective quality of life needs further investigation. The magnitude of optimism in people with MS in the second study was unexpected. The unpredictable and fluctuating disease course of MS would intuitively result in lowered optimistic. However, Robinson and Ryff (1999) claim that conditions of uncertainty and high motivation, likely present in MS, are ideal conditions for exaggerated optimism. This thesis proposed that a combination of positive and 9 negative influences serve to maintain optimism at normal levels for people with MS. Further research needs to explore the influences on the optimistic bias especially in relation to chronic illness. A new secondary control item ‘ventilation’ was identified in Study One and confirmed in Study Two. It was proposed that this represents a new type of secondary control. Future research needs to confirm the utility of this type of control and to determine whether any similar responses to stressful life circumstances exist. The findings of the thesis suggested that current models of control that focus on average primary and secondary control (i.e. The Discrimination Model (Folkman, 1984; Thompson et al., 1998), the Primacy/Back-Up Model (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995), and Heep’s (2000) assertion regarding relative levels of control) are too simplistic and do not adequately describe the function or adaptive outcome of secondary control. Therefore, research is needed to explore, and generate models regarding, the adaptive outcome of secondary control. This could be achieved through a meta-analysis of the many studies that have explored the function of secondary control in different groups of people (e.g. Aikens et al., 1997; Chipperfield et al., 1999; Pakenham, 1999; Warren et al., 1991). Further research needs to explore the possibility that the adaptiveness of primary control may be constrained by situational factors. This thesis did not explicitly measure the controllability of life circumstances for each group. Rather it was assumed, based on a review of literature regarding the psychosocial impact of MS (e.g. Larsen, 1990; Pakenham, 1998, Schofield et al., 1997), that MS provides frequent uncontrollable situations to people with MS and, to a lesser extent, partners. The findings of the two studies were interpreted based on this assumption regarding the uncontrollability of life for people with MS, partners and controls. Future research would benefit from assessments of the frequency with which people experience life stressors and uncontrollable situations. These assessments could then be related to the functioning of primary and secondary control. 10 Strengths and Limitations of the Thesis The thesis was primarily limited by the characteristics of the sample. The sample of people with MS and partners was relatively small in both the first and second study despite extensive recruitment efforts. Also, people with MS seemed to be experiencing illness remission rather than exacerbation. A larger sample may have enabled more in-depth comparisons between the different types of MS and exploration of intra-couple processes. While recruitment difficulties are likely inherent to research with chronically ill people that may find it difficult to take part in research due to their illness, nevertheless future research would benefit from a larger and more representative sample of people with MS. This may be obtained through more active recruitment. For example, rather than relying on passive means of recruitment, researchers could attend self-help groups and other places were people with MS attend and minimise the effort required by participants to take part. The complex methodology, especially of the first study, may have precluded the involvement of some individuals. The sample excluded people with medical conditions other than MS. While, it was necessary to obtain a relatively homogenous sample for analyses, this nevertheless limits the generalizability and interpretability of findings. The research is unable to determine whether the effects of MS on subjective quality of life demonstrated in the studies is due specifically to MS or is instead characteristic of all chronic illness. In addition, the findings cannot be generalised to people with other chronic illness. Another shortcoming of the thesis is that only people with partners were included. Inclusion of people without partners would have enabled further exploration of the role of intimate relationships in subjective quality of life. Further research could therefore improve on this thesis by utilising a longitudinal methodology so that changes over time and causal relationships between the variables 11 of the model could be explored. Also, a larger sample would permit the use of more sophisticated data analysis techniques and the analysis of within group hypotheses regarding the effect of illness characteristics on subjective quality of life and the adaptiveness of couple coping. The major strength of the thesis was the use of a model for the maintenance of subjective quality of life that was valid for different groups of people. Utilisation of such a generic model enabled comparisons between different groups of people. The thesis benefited greatly from the use of three comparison groups. As discussed previously, many of the findings of the study were only apparent in comparisons between groups. Also, the control group provided a baseline comparison so that deficits in the other two groups could be identified. Another strength of the thesis was the combination of qualitative and quantitative methodology employed. The quantitative second study was greatly benefited by the qualitative first study. This information obtained in the qualitative study ensured that the materials in the second study were valid for people with MS and their partners. Validation of the questionnaire was important as the questionnaires were designed for use with a ‘normal’ healthy population. The validated questionnaires provided some findings that would not have been otherwise obtained. For example, the role of perceived social support from pets would not have been detected. Conclusions Findings from the two studies support the utility of a generic model for the maintenance of subjective quality of life. A model was generated based on previous research and then tested with three comparison groups: people with MS, partners of people with MS and controls. The model was supported for all three groups. This model was based on Cummins and Cahill’s (in press) proposition that personality and positive cognitive bias influence subjective quality of life, and on newly hypothesised links between these variables and perceived social support and secondary control. 12 Specifically, perceived social support was found to exert a consistent and strong influence on subjective quality of life, that was further subject to a threshold effect. Secondary control was found to mediate between positive cognitive bias and subjective quality of life. The adaptive outcome of primary control was supportive of the Discrimination Model of Control (Folkman, 1984; Thompson) that claims the primary control is adaptive when situations are controllable. However, the adaptive outcome of secondary control was not supportive of any of the models of control and varied across the three groups and between the five types. The findings suggest that secondary control had the most impact of subjective quality of life in stressful life conditions. This is consistent with the stress-reactive role of secondary control proposed by the thesis. The subjective quality of life of people with MS was less than both partners and controls. Consistent with their lowered subjective quality of life they reported lowered self-esteem, an exaggerated need for perceived social support, and the maladaptive use of primary and secondary control. Partners did not report a lowered subjective quality of life. They reported a lowered perceived social support that was strongly related to their subjective quality of life however, this was likely counterbalanced by the positive impact of primary control and situation bias secondary control. The findings of the thesis have implications for interventions aimed at improving the subjective quality of life of people with MS and their partners. Specifically, it was recommended that the self-esteem of people with MS be improved. Also, the quality of social support, including that from pets, would be an effective target for interventions for people with MS and their partners. Partners would benefit from improved feelings of personal competence and assistance in seeing the positive in situations. 13 The findings of the thesis also have implications for future research. Specifically future research could explore the stability of personality, the utility of self-esteem in models predicting subjective quality of life, the magnitude of optimism for people with chronic illness and the adaptive outcome of the five types of secondary control. Further research would benefit from a longitudinal design that could assess causality between variables, and from the use of comparison groups. This thesis benefited from the use of comparison groups, a generic model for subjective quality of life, and from the combination of qualitative and quantitative research techniques.