Brian Wang

advertisement
Brian R. Wang
Nov. 18, 2005
Biology 112
Literary Analysis of a Classic Ecology Paper
In the classic ecology paper, “Theory of Feeding Strategies”, Thomas W.
Schoener examined how the specific feeding strategies of different animals influence the
behaviors and morphologies of these animals in a manner that allows them to optimize
the amount of food energy in their environment. Schoener believed that feeding
strategies are affected by four focal aspects, which include the optimal diet, the optimal
foraging space, the optimal foraging period, and the optimal foraging-group size
(Schoener, 1971). By constructing the various components of the feeding strategies,
Schoener produced a series of cost-benefit formulations that show how the behavioral
strategies affect the overall feeding outcomes of animals.
According to his research, Schoener thought that the optimal diet of an animal
was a function of the caloric value of the food source, the amount of time spent searching
for the food, and the handling time involved for storage or consumption of the energy
source (Schoener, 1971). Using previously conducted research for the basis of his theory,
he came to the conclusion that the amount of food and its caloric value is related to the
size of a predator, with larger predators requiring a greater amount of food with a higher
amount of energy. Also, Schoener came to the conclusion that the amount of time and
energy a predator would be willing to spend on its prey increases as the nutritional value
of the prey increases. Furthermore, he applied his theory to two different optimal kinds
of feeders, generalists and specialist. Schoener thought that a larger animal was more
likely than a smaller animal to develop into a generalist feeding pattern, due to the fact
that larger animals had higher nutritional needs which could be met by obtaining a wider
variety of food (Schoener, 1971).
With respect to optimal foraging space, Schoener believed that the home range
site of an animal increased with the body weight of the animal. Since animals of
increasing size have higher average nutritional requirements, he concluded that animals
with larger body weights were more willing to establish a larger foraging space in order
to obtain the necessary resources for their survival (Schoener, 1971). Through a series of
cost-benefit calculations, he also found that the variety of food items in a home range was
more important than the overall food density. He proved this theory by referencing
previously conducted research, which showed that squirrels were more likely to inhabit a
territory that had a larger variety of sparsely distributed tree cones than a habitat that
contained a more densely distributed amount of homogenous tree cones.
Although Schoener was not able to produce a formal theory that predicted the
relationship between the behavior of animals and their optimal feeding periods, he was
able to make a number of assumptions based on a set of well-known animal observations.
For example, he proposed that optimal feeding periods were greatly effected by climate,
in a way that animals gathered for food more vivaciously during seasons that produced
more profitable amounts and types of food sources (Schoener, 1971). Through his
formation of the cost-benefit calculations, Schoener also discovered the three main
determinates of the optimal foraging-group size: the measure of foraging efficiency, the
probability of predation, and the defendable area per unit cost of defense (Schoener,
1971). After investigating several studies that looked at the different effects that foraging
group-size had on feeding strategies and outcomes, he found that in most instances
groups allow animals to increase their foraging efficiency and that animals participating
in group foraging significantly increased their detection of predators.
At the time that Thomas W. Schoener’s ecology paper “Theory of Feeding
Strategies” was published, the science community had not previously focused on animal
feeding behaviors in such an in-depth and comprehensive manner. Schoener’s
groundbreaking theory about optimal foraging behavior opened new areas of ecological
research. A couple years after “Theory of Feeding Strategies” was published, Reed
Haisworth and Larry Wolf conducted research that applied some of the principle concepts
of Schoener’s study towards the feeding strategies of hummingbirds. Their research
paper, entitled “Nectar Characteristics and food selection by hummingbirds”, examined
how hummingbirds maximize their feeding efficiencies by choosing the nectars in their
natural habitat that were highest in sugar concentration. The hummingbirds that were
observed in the experimental trials consistently chose the nectars that had the highest
concentrations of sucrose, even if they had to travel further distances (Haisworth and
Wolf, 1976). Also, Haisworth and Wolf found that the hummingbirds were willing to
spend more time extracting and consuming food from its source if it resulted in a larger
nutrient pay-off. This study relates back to Schoener’s concept of the optimal diet, in
which he states that individuals were more likely to expend more energy and time for
food sources of higher nutritional content than they were for food sources that were at a
closer location but lower nutrient value. It shows that hummingbirds were willing to
incorporate feeding trade-offs into their feeding strategies in order to maximize the
amount of net resources they can obtain from their surroundings.
Another study that employed some of the principles of Schoener’s ecological
research, “Optimal foraging in bumblebees and co-evolution with their plants”, looked at
how the movements of bumblebees have co-evolved with the spatial distribution of the
plants and flowers from which they use as food sources. In his analysis of bumblebee
feeding strategies, Graham Pyke studied how bumblebee movement patterns were
kinetically optimized in such a way that they resulted in the maximum net rate of energy
gain from their food. Pyke observed how the bumblebees in his experiment used a large
array of different food extraction techniques, each of which were plant specific, in order
to obtain their food more efficiently, while minimizing the amount of energy they expend
during the process (Pyke, 1978). He also found that the bumblebees had modified their
foraging space in such a way that they had the largest variety of plants and flowers
available to them for consumption. This study further examines two key aspects from
“Theory of Feeding Strategies”, the optimal diet and the optimal foraging space. Pyke
relates back to the concept of optimal diet by showing how bumblebees purposefully
calculate their body movements in such a way that they are able to acquire the greatest
amount of resources from their habitat while reducing their energy expenditure to the
bear minimum. He also refers back to Schoener’s theory of optimal foraging space by
exhibiting how bumblebees construct their home range site in a way that offers them the
greatest variety of food resources in the smallest amount of space.
The paper “Optimal foraging and community structure”, by Gary Belovsky,
attempts to further the development of Schoener’s concept of the optimal diet. In his
ecological investigation, Belovsky examined the relationship between the body sizes of
herbivores and their specific foraging behaviors and habits. He found that herbivores
maximized their nutritional intake by obtaining foods that were easy to harvest and foods
that required the least amount of energy to digest (Belovsky, 1985). Also, Belovsky
looked into how the body size of herbivores affected their physiological digestibility of
plants. He found that body size was one of the key determinates of an herbivore’s
feeding strategy, because their bodies could only physically and chemically digest a
limited variety of plants in an efficient manner. The study also looked at how the
existence of competition could influence the optimal foraging space of an animal. By
showing that herbivores of similar physiologies were more likely to consume the same
types of plants, Belovsky proposed that herbivores with comparable physiologies and
similar nutritional constraints were more likely to live in territories that did not
completely overlap (Belovsky, 1985).
Current research in Schoener’s ecological scope of feeding strategies is directed
towards a major aspect of the optimal diet theory that has been overlooked in the past.
Over the past couple of decades, a lot of studies have been conducted on the strategies
that foragers use to obtain immobile prey and the majority of these studies have been able
to successfully apply the optimal diet theory to their specific experimental situations. But
now researchers are currently interested in the application of the theory to foragers that
rely on mobile prey as their sole nutritional sources. The study “Optimal diet theory:
when it does work, and when and why does it fail?” published in 2001, applied the
optimal diet theory to a number of different foragers whose main source of food was
some type of mobile prey. The researchers found that the optimal diet theory worked very
well to predict the feeding behaviors of foragers who acquired their food from stationary
food sources; however they found that the theory failed to effectively predict the diet and
behaviors of foragers that consume mobile prey (Christensen and Sih, 2001). The
researchers discovered that one of the chief reasons why the optimal diet theory did not
work well with mobile prey was because the variations of vulnerabilities in prey were
more important in these instances than the behavior of the predators (Christensen and Sih,
2001).
Many more questions still remain unanswered on Schoener’s “Theory of feeding
strategies”. In the future, more studies should be conducted on the two less acknowledged
concepts of Schoener’s theory, the optimal foraging period and the optimal foraging
group-size. Even though researchers know that the optimal foraging period is somewhat
influenced by the seasonality and climate of an animal’s habitat (sources), they can
investigate further into the field by examining how an animal adapts its behavior
according to the changes in its environment. For example, they can observe to see if
animals extend their foraging space during some seasons and decrease it during others,
and they can also look to see if any changes in the food extraction techniques or kinetic
body movements occur in the animals during the different seasons. With respect to the
theory of the optimal foraging group-size, researchers could delve deeper into the concept
by monitoring the influence of group size on animals that prey on stationary food
resources, and compare this with animals that prey on mobile food. Furthermore, they
could look at how disturbances and variances in the environment affect the optimum
number of individuals for various groups of animal species.
Works Cited
Belovsky, G.E. 1985. Optimal foraging and community structure: implications for a guild
of generalist grassland herbivores. Oecologia 70:35 – 52.
Christensen, B. and Sih, A. 2001. Optimal diet theory: when does it work, and when and
why does it fail? Animal Behaviour 62:379-390
Haisworth, R. and Wolf, L. 1976. Nectar Characteristics and food selection by
hummingbirds. Oecologia 25:101 – 113.
Pyke, G. 1978. Optimal foraging in bumblebees and co-evolution with their plants.
Oecologia 3:281-293.
Schoener, T.W. 1971. Theory of Feeding Strategies. Annual Review of Ecology and
Systematics 2:369 – 404.
Overall good discussion, but could use some fleshing out/filling in of gaps.
23/26
Good job of projecting future development of the general area of research. But, I
would have appreciated a bit more on the real impact of the paper. What with in excess
of 1600 citations since 1980, this paper is really driving a lot of science. Your explain
well the papers you discuss, but I am wondering about the 1600+ you did not discuss and
the major direction among them.
19
Download