Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution Running head: LOCUS OF CONTROL AND CONFLICT RESOLUTION Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution: A Comparison of Early Adolescents versus Young Adults Shannon M. Saville and Robert A. Swoap Warren Wilson College 1 Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution Abstract This study represents the first attempt to compare locus of control (LOC) and conflict resolution in adolescents and young adults. Participants ages 14-15 and 21-22 (N = 196) were recruited from 2 high schools and 3 colleges. The participants completed the Adult Nowicki-Strickland Internal-External Scale and the Conflict Resolution Questionnaire to measure their LOC and conflict resolution tactics. It was found that LOC was more internal (p = .001) and compromise was used more (p < .000) by young adults than adolescents, females and males used overt anger equally (p = .001), and LOC was a predictor of compromise (R2 = .17, p < .000). The current research suggests that LOC, age, and gender are reliable predictors of conflict resolution tactics, namely compromise. 2 Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 3 Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution: A Comparison of Early Adolescents versus Young Adults The ages of adolescence and young adulthood are rife with numerous challenges and new life experiences. Individuals vary in the way they perceive and manage these challenges; some may change the way they resolve conflicts as they mature. For example, there have been numerous studies concerning differences found in locus of control (LOC) between genders and ages (Blanchard-Fields & Irion, 1988; Chubb & Fertman, 1997; Gomez, 1997; Nunn, 1994; Riechard & Peterson, 1998; Ross & Mirowsky, 2002), as well as studies pertaining to adolescent conflict resolution tactics (Bjorkqvist, Lagerspetz, & Kaukiainen, 1992; James & Owens, 2004; Feldman & Gowen, 1998; Laursen, Finkelstein, & Betts, 2001; Lindeman, Harakka, & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, 1997; Owens, Daly, & Slee, 2005). However, these constructs have never been compared in the same study. The primary aim of the current study is to see whether LOC could be an additional predictor of conflict resolution tactics. If so, LOC could provide another helpful indicator as to how adolescents and young adults are likely to resolve conflicts, and therefore guide interventions and education in populations at a higher risk for using less desirable resolution tactics. Conflict Resolution Conflict resolution, as defined by Owens and colleagues (2005), is an action required to terminate an exchange of mutual opposition. Developmental research has found differences in conflict resolution tactics between adolescents and young adults. The use of less socially desirable tactics, such as overt anger, avoidance, distraction, and withdrawal, are thought to be employed by adolescents more frequently than by young adults; this may be due to decreased self-control, increased peer influence, and an increased concern with the opposite sex associated Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 4 with this stage of development (Feldman & Gowen,1998; Bjorkqvist et al., 1992; Lindeman et al., 1997; Gamble, 1994). Young adulthood is marked by a decrease in withdrawal and an increase in direct problem solving and compromise, in part attributable to the emergence of prosocial behavior through late adolescence and an increase in moral reasoning (Lindeman et al., 1997; Gamble, 1994; Owens et al., 2005; Feldman & Gowen,1998). A meta-analysis of peer conflict resolution, conducted by Laursen et al. (2001), compared studies evaluating adolescents (ages 11-18) and young adults (ages 19-25). Researchers divided conflict resolution into compromise, third party intervention, withdrawal, standoff, and submission. These five subsets could be combined into three categories: negotiation (compromise and third party), disengagement (withdrawal and standoff), and coercion (submission). Adolescents used disengagement most and negotiation least while young adults used negotiation most and coercion least. The majority of studies show a progressive developmental trend toward negotiation and away from coercion (Laursen et al., 2001). Gender differences in conflict resolution have also been found. Males have been shown to exhibit more direct, physical aggression while females have been shown to inflict indirect, mental discomfort on others (Bjorkqvist et al., 1992). Bjorkqvist et al. (1992) suggested that a possible reason for these findings is that females usually develop more quickly than males. This developmental difference often involves verbal and social skills, which are needed for the utilization of indirect aggression. Females have also been found to be more prosocial and less aggressive than males (Lindeman et al., 1997). In contrast, one study found that adolescent females used more overt anger tactics combined with compromise when resolving conflicts, while males had a higher rate of distraction (Feldman & Gowen, 1998). A study in Australia found anger to be the same between sexes, but the females used more obliging, avoidance, and Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 5 compromise methods. This may indicate that females are more peaceful and constructive when resolving conflicts (Owens et al., 2005). The gender findings for withdrawal as a conflict resolution strategy have been mixed (Bjorkqvist et al., 1992; Lindeman et al., 1997). In addition to age and gender differences found among conflict resolution tactics, the type of relationship requiring resolution and whether or not the conflict studied is hypothetical or real also influences which tactic is used. Voluntary relationships, such as those between romantic partners and friends, are more conducive to negotiation tactics, while coercion is used more for sibling and acquaintance relationships (Laursen et al., 2001). Laursen et al. (2001) also found that negotiation was preferred more in hypothetical conflict scenarios, while coercion was used more in actual conflicts. This disparity may be due to the social desirability of certain tactics such as compromise and negotiation, making these tactics what most people would like to use, even if in reality other methods of resolution are employed. Locus of Control Gomez (1997) describes locus of control (LOC) as the degree to which people think they or other factors control events in their lives. Internal locus of control (ILOC) is characterized by the belief that people have control over their own life events, while external locus of control (ELOC) is characterized by the belief that outside sources control what happens to them, whether it be powerful others, chance, or fate (Gomez, 1997). LOC is a continuous, rather than a dichotomous variable, and can vary between situations (Chubb & Fertman, 1997). ILOC has been examined in many age-comparison studies, including comparisons between children, adolescents, young adults, middle age adults, and elderly adults (BlanchardFields & Irion, 1988; Chubb & Fertman, 1997; Gomez, 1997; Nunn, 1994; Riechard & Peterson, 1998; Ross & Mirowsky, 2002). Chubb & Fertman (1997) found an interaction between gender Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 6 and grade for LOC in adolescents. Adolescent boys were found to first have a decrease in internalization between 9th and 10th grades, and then have an increase in internalization between 10th grade and one year post-high school, with the biggest shift occurring between the 10th and 11th grades. Girls had their largest increase in internalization between 9th and 10th grades and maintained an overall ILOC throughout adolescence. In general, as adolescents grow older, LOC becomes more internal (Chubb & Fertman, 1997; Gomez, 1997; Nunn, 1994; Riechard & Peterson, 1998). This internalization is theoretically related to the increased responsibility, independence, and self-control associated with the transition to young adulthood. The observed gender differences may be related to sex differences with the onset of puberty. Some gender LOC comparisons in adults show women to be more external (Nunn, 1994; Ross & Nirowsky, 2002), while still others find men to be more external (Blanchard-Fields & Irion, 1988). Gender differences have been found in LOC when comparing older adults (over the age of 65), with men being more internal. These findings are potentially explained by generational differences, such as older women’s increased economic dependence and decreased career opportunities; this disparity has not been found in younger subjects (Ross et al., 2002). According to Chubb & Fertman (1997), most adult studies do not find a significant difference in LOC between genders. The aim of the current research is to compare male and female adolescents to young adults using the constructs of conflict resolution and locus of control. It is theorized that the progression toward independence that occurs during these developmental stages will result in LOC and conflict resolution differences. It is hypothesized that: 1) LOC becomes more internalized as a person grows older; 2) conflict resolution relies more on compromise as age Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 7 increases; 3) males show more direct forms of resolution (overt anger) than females; 4) there will be a significant correlation between ILOC and compromise. Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 8 Method Participants One hundred and ninety-six students from North Carolina volunteered to participate in this study (132 female, 63 male, and 1 unspecified gender). There were 110 high school participants (14-15 years old) and 86 college participants (21-23 years old). Participants were obtained through random sampling (via the primary researcher contacting every fifth teacher/professor from the selected schools’ websites) and through convenience sampling. The ethnic distribution of participants was as follows: 85.2% White European American, 6.1% African American, 4.6% Hispanic, 3.1% Asian American, and .5% Native American. Participants were not compensated for their participation. Measures Locus of control. The Adult Nowicki-Strickland Internal-External Control Scale (ANSIE; see Appendix A) is a widely used measure for LOC and has strong test-retest reliability (r = .83) and construct validity (r = .68). It is a 40-item “yes or no” scale that measures a participant’s degree of either internal (own behavior) or external (fate or chance) LOC. Higher scores indicate a more external LOC. Sample questions are as follows: “Are some people just born lucky?” and “Most of the time, do you find it useless to try to get your own way at home?” Conflict resolution. The Conflict Resolution Questionnaire (see Appendix B) was adapted from Feldman and Gowen (1998) and Charlton (2001) by James and Owens (2004). It is a 28-item scale that measures six styles of conflict resolution: overt anger, compromise, avoidance, social support, obliging, and distraction. Answers to conflict resolution behaviors are on a five-point Likert Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 9 scale ranging from “Never” (0) to “Very Often” (4). Reliability of the subscales range from α = .61 (distraction) to α = .85 (overt anger). Examples of resolution behaviors included: “Try to reason,” “Clam up and hold my feelings inside,” and “Get angrier the more I talk.” Procedure Approval for research was first obtained through the researcher’s college Institutional Review Board. Permission from schools and educators was then requested in a letter sent to principals, teachers, and professors. The primary researcher visited three colleges and two high schools, which had given permission, to administer the research packets. For the high school participants, informed consent documents were sent to teachers prior to test administration. Both parents and students signed and returned the consent forms to the school before the researcher’s arrival. College participants were handed the informed consent document upon the researcher’s arrival to review and sign if they wished to participate. The informed consent was reviewed in every class before the research packets were handed out. The research packets consisted of an instruction form, a demographics form, the ANSIE, and the Conflict Resolution Questionnaire and took approximately 15 minutes to complete. Time was given during class to complete the packets. For the students who did not want to participate and/or did not have their informed consent, a brief and voluntary reading was provided discussing interesting psychological phenomena. When all students were finished with their packets, a debriefing form was distributed which further explained the nature of the research and provided a website where the results of the study would be posted. Results Questionnaires were scored and then entered and analyzed using SPSS (15th ed.). An alpha level of .05 was used to analyze the following hypotheses. Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 10 Hypothesis 1 A 2 X 2 (Gender X Age) factorial ANOVA was conducted to determine whether or not participants differed in locus of control scores. ANSIE scores showed a significant main effect for age F(4, 191) = .81, p = .001, with adolescents exhibiting a more external LOC (see Table 1). Neither gender nor the interaction between age and gender showed a significant effect. Hypothesis 2 A 2 X 2 (Gender X Age) factorial ANOVA was conducted to determine if participants varied on the usage of compromise. A significant main effect was found for both gender F(4,191) = 2.65, p < .05, and age F(4,191) = 2.65, p <.001, but not for the interaction between the two. Young adults and females used compromise more often than did adolescents or males (see Table 2). Hypothesis 3 A 2 X 2 (Gender X Age) factorial ANOVA was conducted to determine the degree to which participants used overt anger as a conflict resolution tactic. In contrast to the hypothesis, there was not a significant difference between male and female use of this tactic. There was, however, a significant main effect for age F(4, 191) = 3.48, p = .001, with adolescents using overt anger more often than young adults (see Table 3). There was not a significant interaction effect. Hypothesis 4 A linear multiple regression analysis was conducted to analyze the degree LOC, age, and gender predict compromise. Using stepwise method, LOC entered the equation first and accounted for 17% of the variance within compromise (p < .000). When combined with gender and age, 23% of the variance within compromise was accounted for (see Table 4). Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 11 Table 1 Mean ANSIE Scores by Gender and Age Adolescents Young Adults Female M = 14.07 (SD = 4.71, n = 81) M = 11.39 (SD = 4.21, n = 51) Overall average by gender M = 13.04 (SD = 4.69, n = 132) Male M = 13.03 (SD = 4.59, n = 29) M = 10.88 (SD = 4.43, n = 34) M = 11.87 (SD = 4.60, n = 63) Overall average by age M = 13.80* (SD = 4.68, n = 110) M = 11.12* (SD = 4.30, n = 86) M = 12.62 (SD = 4.70, n = 196) Note. Maximum score = 40. Higher scores indicate more external LOC. *p = .001. Table 2 Mean Compromise Scores by Gender and Age Adolescents Young Adults Female M = 2.74 (SD = 0.69, n = 81) M = 3.28 (SD = 0.55, n = 51) Overall average by gender M = 2.95* (SD = 0.69, n = 132) Male M = 2.58 (SD = 0.85, n = 29) M = 2.88 (SD = 0.69, n = 34) M = 2.74* (SD = 0.78, n = 63) Overall average by age M = 2.70** (SD = 0.73, n = 110) M = 3.12** (SD = 0.63, n = 86) M = 2.88 (SD = 0.72, n = 196) Note. Maximum score = 4. *p < .05. **p < .001. Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 12 Table 3 Mean Overt Anger Scores by Gender and Age Adolescents Young Adults Female M = 1.86 (SD = 0.75, n = 81) M = 1.45 (SD = 0.59, n = 51) Overall average by gender M = 1.70 (SD = 0.72, n = 132) Male M = 1.87 (SD = 0.84, n = 29) M = 1.53 (SD = 0.47, n = 34) M = 1.69 (SD = 0.68, n = 63) Overall average by age M = 1.86* (SD = 0.77, n = 110) M = 1.48* (SD = 0.54, n = 86) M = 1.70 (SD = 0.70, n = 196) Note. Maximum score = 4. *p = .001. Table 4 Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis for Predicting Compromise R R square LOC .42** LOC & gender LOC, gender, & age * p < .05. ** p < .000. .17 Adjusted R square .17 Standard error of the estimate .659 .46* .21 .20 .646 .49* .24 .23 .633 Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 13 Discussion The current study marks the first attempt to analyze both locus of control and conflict resolution tactics in adolescents and young adults. The results indicate that LOC is a predictor of compromise for these populations, providing further support for predicting and encouraging desirable conflict resolution tactics. Consistent with previous literature, the hypotheses that LOC would be more internal for young adults than for adolescents and that young adults would rely on compromise more than adolescents were supported. These findings form a logical progression: if young adults exhibit a more internal LOC, in part due to increased independence, etc., and internal LOC predicts compromise, then young adults should be more likely than adolescents to use compromise in conflict situations. It is interesting to note that males and females did not differ in their usage of overt anger, which is in opposition to what was hypothesized. The current findings are also in opposition to previous studies that have found differences between genders. One possible explanation for this may be attributed to the common definition of overt anger as direct, physical aggression, a trait that is commonly attributed more to males. Bjorkqvist and colleagues (1992) note that “women [are] not less aggressive if the readiness to inflict mental pain [is] considered” (p. 118). The lack of a statistical difference between genders in overt anger in this study may indicate equal usage between genders, but perhaps in different forms. One possible limitation to this study concerns the sample sizes among the age and gender groups. Due to the nature of random sampling, obtaining equal representations of adolescent/young adult and male/female participants could not be controlled. This led to sample sizes ranging from 29 (adolescent males) to 81 (adolescent females), which could have Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 14 potentially had a negative consequence for effect sizes. Perhaps increased control for equality in sampling variance in the future could lead to stronger significance for research findings. Future research should focus on finding additional factors relating to conflict resolution tactics. The three independent variables used in this study (LOC, gender, and age) only accounted for 23% of the variance within compromise, leaving much room for additional predictors. Suggestions for these additional factors include: socio-economic status, ethnicity, cultural differences, type of conflict, type of relationship in conflict, and degree of personal responsibility. As mentioned previously, voluntary relationships in conflict, such as romantic partnerships, are usually resolved using negotiation (Laursen et al., 2001). Additionally, hypothetical conflicts, as were used in this study, tend to elicit socially desirable responses, such as negotiation. Actual conflicts are reported to be resolved using coercion most often (Laursen et al., 2001). Social desirability effects should be researched further in the future, as they were not addressed in the current study. Locus of control is just one of potentially many factors contributing to the use of different conflict resolution tactics. It may be helpful for conflict mediators and school officials to be cognizant of individual variances in adolescents and young adults and the effects these differences may have on which conflict resolution tactics are used. Knowing that having a more internal LOC is related to compromise, mediators and school officials could attempt to provide opportunities that would encourage internalization of LOC, such as increased opportunities for decision making. This internalization could make it more likely that individuals will use more socially desirable conflict resolution tactics, such as compromise, leading to improved social interactions in the future. Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 15 In addition, schools and community programs could provide workshops and resources to teach students socially desirable conflict resolution tactics. This learning process may also have the effect of internalizing LOC, furthering the chances of students using compromise to resolve conflicts. Teachers and administrators may be in a unique position to alter interactions between students through education and modeling that would lead to a more harmonious and safe learning environment. Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 16 Appendix A ANSIE – Form C YES NO ___ ___ 1. Do you believe that most problems will solve themselves if you don’t fool with them? ___ ___ 2. Do you believe that you can stop yourself from catching a cold? ___ ___ 3. Are some people just born lucky? ___ ___ 4. Most of the time, do you feel that getting good grades means a great deal to you? ___ ___ 5. Are you often blamed for things that just aren’t your fault? ___ ___ 6. Do you believe that if somebody studies hard enough, he or she can pass any subject? ___ ___ 7. Do you feel that most of the time it doesn’t pay to try hard because things never turn out right anyway? ___ ___ 8. Do you feel that if things start out well in the morning that it’s going to be a great day, no matter what you do? ___ ___ 9. Do you feel that most of the time parents listen to what their children have to say? ___ ___ 10. Do you believe that wishing can make good things happen? ___ ___ 11. When you get criticized, does it usually seem it’s for no good reason at all? ___ ___ 12. Most of the time do you find it hard to change a friend’s mind/opinion? ___ ___ 13. Do you think that cheering, more than luck, helps a team to win? ___ ___ 14. Do you feel that it is nearly impossible to change your parents’ mind about anything? ___ ___ 15. Do you believe that your parents should allow you to make most of your own decisions? ___ ___ 16. Do you feel that when you do something wrong there’s very little you can do to make it right? ___ ___ 17. Do you believe that most people are just born good at sports? ___ ___ 18. Are most of the other people your age and sex stronger than you are? ___ ___ 19. Do you feel that one of the best ways to handle most problems is just not to think about them? ___ ___ 20. Do you feel that you have a lot of choice in deciding who your friends are? ___ ___ 21. If you find a four-leaf clover, do you believe that it might bring good luck? ___ ___ 22. Do you often feel that whether or not you do your homework has much to do with what kinds of grades you get? ___ ___ 23. Do you feel that when a person your age is angry with you, there’s little you can do to stop him or her? Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 17 ___ ___ 24. Have you ever had a good luck charm? ___ ___ 25. Do you believe that whether or not people like you depends on how you act? ___ ___ 26. Will your parents usually help you if you ask them to? ___ ___ 27. Have you ever felt that when people were angry with you, it was usually for no reason at all? ___ ___ 28. Most of the time, do you feel that you can change what might happen tomorrow by what you do today? ___ ___ 29. Do you believe that when bad things are going to happen they just are going to happen no matter what you do to try to stop them? ___ ___ 30. Do you think that people can get their own way if they just keep trying? ___ ___ 31. Most of the time, do you find it useless to try to get your own way at home? ___ ___ 32. Do you feel that when good things happen, they happen because of hard work? ___ ___ 33. Do you feel that when somebody your age wants to be your enemy, there’s little you can do to change matters? ___ ___ 34. Do you feel that it’s easy to get friends to do what you want them to do? ___ ___ 35. Do you usually feel that you have little to say about what you get to eat at home? ___ ___ 36. Do you feel that when someone doesn’t like you there’s little you can do about it? ___ ___ 37. Do you usually feel that it is almost useless to try in school because most other students are just plain smarter than you are? ___ ___ 38. Are you the kind of person that believes that planning ahead makes things turn out better? ___ ___ 39. Most of the time, do you feel that you have little to say about what your family decides to do? ___ ___ 40. Do you think it’s better to be smart than to be lucky? Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 18 Appendix B Indicate by circling the appropriate number (from 0 to 4) how often you do the following things during a disagreement or conflict with peers. Conflict Resolution Behaviors Never Seldom Sometimes Quite Often Very Often 1. Try to reason 0 1 2 3 4 2. Talk to a brother or sister 0 1 2 3 4 3. Get angry and yell 0 1 2 3 4 4. Put the other person’s needs first 0 1 2 3 4 5. Hurt other person’s feelings 0 1 2 3 4 6. Try to be funny and make light of it 0 1 2 3 4 7. Get sarcastic 0 1 2 3 4 8. Apologize to other person 0 1 2 3 4 9. Clam up and hold my feelings inside 0 1 2 3 4 10. Listen and try to understand 0 1 2 3 4 11. Get angry and walk away 0 1 2 3 4 12. Give in to what the other person wants 0 1 2 3 4 13. Walk away and discuss later 0 1 2 3 4 14. Bring in or try to bring in a friend 0 1 2 3 4 15. Get cool and distant/ Give cold shoulder 0 1 2 3 4 16. Talk to a friend 0 1 2 3 4 17. Tell myself it is not important 0 1 2 3 4 18. Try to avoid talking about it 0 1 2 3 4 19. Try to work out a compromise 0 1 2 3 4 20. Make other person feel bad 0 1 2 3 4 21. Talk to a parent 0 1 2 3 4 22. Watch TV or play video games 0 1 2 3 4 23. Get angrier the more I talk 0 1 2 3 4 24. Bring in or try to bring in someone (to help) 0 1 2 3 4 Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 19 25. Try to smooth things over 0 1 2 3 4 26. Stay angry a long time 0 1 2 3 4 27. Talk to a teacher 0 1 2 3 4 28. Distract yourself or the other person through entertainment or relaxation 0 1 2 3 4 Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 20 References Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K., & Kaukianen A. (1992). Do girls manipulate and boys fight?: developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117-127. Blanchard-Fields, F., & Irion, J. (1988). The relation between locus of control and coping in two contexts: age as a moderator variable. Psychology and Aging, 3(2), 197-203. Chubb, N., & Fertman, C. (1997). Adolescent self-esteem and locus of control: a longitudinal study of gender and age differences. Adolescence, 32(125), 113-121. Feldman, S., & Gowen, L. (1998). Conflict negotiation tactics in romantic relationships in high school students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 27(6), 691-718. Gamble, W. (1994). Perceptions of controllability and other stressor event characteristics as determinants of coping among young adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 23(1), 65-86. Gomez, R. (1997). Locus of control and Type A behavior pattern as predictors of coping styles among adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(3), 391-398. Laursen, B., Finkelstein, B., & Betts N. (2001). A developmental meta-analysis of peer conflict resolution. Developmental Review, 21, 423-449. Lindeman, M., Harakka, T., & Keltikangas-Jarvinen L. (1997). Age and gender differences in adolescents' reactions to conflict situations: aggression, prosociality, and withdrawal. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26(3), 339-53. Nunn, G. (1994). Adult learners' locus of control, self-evaluation and learning temperament as a function of age and gender. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 21(3), 260-266. Owens, L., Daly, A., & Slee P. (2005). Sex and age differences in victimization and conflict Locus of Control and Conflict Resolution 21 resolution among adolescents in a south australian school. Aggressive Behavior, 31, 1-12. Riechard, D., & Peterson, S. (1998). Perception of environmental risk related to gender, community socioeconomic setting, age, and locus of control. Journal of Environmental Education, 30(1), 11-21. Ross, C., & Mirowsky, J. (2002). Age and the gender gap in the sense of personal control. Social Psychology Quarterly, 65(2), 125-47.