1 1 Summary of Laboratory Experiments on Mating Disruption 2 3 4 Effect of pheromone on male behaviour Experiment 1 evaluated the mating success of females in the presence or absence of 5 pheromone or pheromone analogues in Erlenmeyer flasks maintained in laboratory conditions, 6 using 10 males and 10 females per flask (Schmidt et al. 1980). Hercon hollow fibers were used 7 as a pheromone release device, at a rate of ca. 25 ng / h / fibre. The presence of pheromone 8 suppressed mating of females, and the level of suppression increased with the concentration of 9 pheromone for a range of 100 g to l mg AI or 125 ng / h to 1 g / h. Both components of the 10 spruce budworm pheromone blend, E-11 tetradecenal and Z-11 tetradecenal, were as effective 11 when tested alone as the natural blend (95:5 E:Z-11 tetradecenal), a finding that is somewhat 12 surprising considering that neither component alone is attractive to males. Of different 13 pheromone analogs tested, the only one as effective as the natural sex pheromone in suppressing 14 mating was 10-(Cyclopent-1-en-1-yl)-decanal. 15 Experiment 2 was conducted in a wind tunnel with two calling females at the upwind end 16 of the tunnel (Sanders 1982). The wind tunnel was either left untreated (control) or treated with 17 pheromone at a range of 10 to 10 000 pg / m3; for each pheromone concentration, two modes of 18 pheromone release were used, uniform permeation of the wind tunnel or nine discrete plumes 19 uniformly spaced in the tunnel. The males were enclosed for 3 h in the wind tunnel, and the 20 number of individuals wing fanning or flying to the calling female was recorded for all 21 treatments. The level of response of males declined with the concentration of pheromone, and 22 the orientation toward pheromone sources was particularly suppressed when the pheromone was 23 released from discrete plumes rather than uniformly in the atmosphere – with the exception of 1 2 24 the highest concentration of pheromone. These results suggest that, at least for concentrations of 25 pheromones that can be realistically achieved in the field, competition of synthetic pheromone 26 sources with calling females is more likely to underlie mating disruption than camouflage of 27 calling females by synthetic pheromone, and the release of pheromone from a few high release 28 sites than from numerous low release sites may improve the efficacy of mating disruption. 29 Experiment 3 was conducted in a wind tunnel and used different amounts of pheromone 30 placed at the upwind end of the tunnel (PVC pellets with 0.0003 to 0.03 % AI on a wt / wt basis), 31 corresponding to release rate of 0.1, 1 and 10 ng / h (Sanders 1985). One-day old males were 32 either pre-exposed or not (experienced or naive moths, respectively) to the pheromone for a 33 period of 3 h. The proportion of males that responded by wing-fanning or by orienting toward 34 the pheromone sources was recorded. The lack of difference among naïve and experienced 35 males in terms of orientation toward the pheromone source, even at the highest dose, suggests 36 that sensory habituation is not the predominant mechanism underlying mating disruption. 37 Experiment 4 was conducted in 3 L mating chambers maintained in laboratory conditions 38 (Ponder et al. 1986). Mating of females was suppressed by the presence of pheromone, and the 39 level of suppression increased with the concentration of pheromone from 1 to 320 g, although 40 an average of 20% females were mated even at the highest concentration. In untreated cages, the 41 mating probability of females increased with the ratio of males per female for a range of 1:1 to 42 1.5:1, whereas the density of females had a relatively minor effect on female mating success for 43 a range of 6 to 18 females. In pheromone-treated cages, in contrast, the mating success of 44 females increased significantly with density. As expected, the proportion of mated females 45 increased with the duration of the experiment from 20 to 68 h in both control and treated cages. 46 Two analogs of the spruce budworm pheromone, 11,13-tetradecadienal and 12-tetradecenal (1:1 2 3 47 E:Z ratio), were perceived by the male antenna and reduced captures of moths in traps when 48 added to the sex pheromone, but only the first one suppressed mating of females. 49 Experiment 5 was conducted in a flight tunnel with an array of 35 rubber septa 50 impregnated with pheromone or pheromone analogs (E-11-tetradecenyl acetate or E-11- 51 tetradecenol) (Sanders 1995). Two calling females were placed at the upwind end of the tunnel, 52 and one male released at the other end. The analogs had no significant effect on the response of 53 males for a dose of 10 g per septa, either in terms of wing-fanning or orientation toward 54 females. Comparing different ratios of E:Z-11 tetradecenal (95:5, 80:20, and 50:50) and 55 pheromone doses (0.1 and 1 g per septa) revealed that orientation of males toward females was 56 most suppressed at the highest dose of the 95:5 ratio. Most males flew toward the pheromone- 57 impregnated septa, which suggests false trail following as a mechanism of mating disruption; no 58 evidence of sensory fatigue or habituation was detected. It was concluded that mating disruption 59 may not suppress mating to near zero level, because a vast majority of males eventually reached 60 the calling females when they were kept in the wind tunnel for a period of 3 h. 61 Experiment 6 was conducted in a flight tunnel with 16 rubber septa baited with 62 pheromone and pinned equidistantly on the top part of the tunnel, using concentrations of 63 pheromone that ranged from 1 to 1 000 g per septa (Sanders 1996). Two one-day old females 64 were placed at the upwind end of the tunnel. Males were kept in cages in the upper and lower 65 section of the tunnel for 3 h before the onset of the experiment (experienced and naïve males, 66 respectively), and their response recorded (wing-fanning, orientation toward calling females) 67 over a four day period. Most naïve males wing-fanned independently of the pheromone 68 concentration, whereas the incidence of wing-fanning declined with concentration for 69 experienced males. The proportion of males that reached calling females declined with the 3 4 70 concentration of ambient pheromone; the response of naïve and experienced males was similar, 71 and no daily variation in response was observed during the course of the four day experiment. 72 However, experienced males were slower in initiating flight and took significantly longer to 73 reach calling females, especially as they got older. At release rates characteristic of female 74 emission rate i.e., 10 to 100 g loadings, males were commonly attracted by the rubber septa, 75 indicative of competitive attraction of pheromone sources with calling females. Sensory fatigue, 76 indicated by a lack of upwind flight toward calling females or pheromone sources, occurred for 77 loadings of 1 000 g in rubber septa, which corresponds to approximately ten times the natural 78 release rate of females. Altogether, the results suggest that continuous exposure to around 20 ng 79 / m3 is necessary to achieve effective mating disruption in the field. 80 Experiment 7 was conducted in a flight tunnel with a movable patterned ceiling that served 81 to sustain the flight of males over prolonged periods (Sanders 1998). Two virgin females were 82 placed at the upwind end of the wind tunnel before a male was introduced at the opposite end of 83 the tunnel. All males reached the females in the absence of pheromone in the tunnel, whereas 84 the orientation of males toward females declined with the concentration of pheromone (septa 85 loaded with 0.1 to 100 g of AI). Some males flew for long durations (up to 53 min) toward 86 calling females when the atmosphere was permeated with pheromone, which suggests that 87 complete mating disruption will be difficult to achieve in the field because some males are likely 88 to eventually locate a calling female. 89 90 Effects of pheromone on female behaviour 91 Experiment 8 was conducted using three day old virgin females maintained in Erlenmeyer flasks 92 with an airflow consisting of either pheromone-impregnated air or “clean” air (Palaniswamy and 4 5 93 Seabrook 1978). The presence of pheromone significantly increased the rate of walking, 94 antennal grooming, extension of the ovipositor, and oviposition. Electroantennograms revealed 95 that females perceived their conspecific pheromone at an amplitude of approximately two-thirds 96 that of males. 97 Experiment 9 reported a physiological response of females to their pheromone using the 98 electroantennogram response technique (Palaniswamy et al. 1979). The response of females 99 increased with age and was suppressed by a juvenile hormone analog. 100 Experiment 10 was conducted in Erlenmeyer flasks containing females that were 101 subjected to an air flow with or without sex pheromone; the pheromone was released from 102 cartridges with 20 l of pheromone and replaced every 30 min (Palaniswamy and Seabrook 103 1985). Exposure to the pheromone resulted in a larger number of females actively releasing 104 pheromone (calling) and an earlier onset of calling. In other experiments, however, no effect of 105 either pheromone or conspecific calling females on the calling behaviour was detected (Sanders 106 and Lucuik 1972; Sanders 1995) 107 Experiment 11 evaluated the response of females enclosed in 27 L screen cages with one 108 shoot of balsam fir; the cages were treated with 14 pheromone dispensers pinned on the outer 109 surface of the cage (corresponding to a release rate of 100 to 200 ng / h), or left untreated 110 (Sanders 1987). The flight activity of females was recorded for a three day period using an 111 electrostatic charges amplifier. The incidence of flight was significantly increased by the 112 presence of pheromone for both mated and virgin females, and for some days the amplitude of 113 difference was greater than ten. Virgin females remained inactive for two days following 114 emergence, even in the presence of pheromone. 115 5 6 116 Summary of Field Experiments on Mating Disruption 117 118 Trials based on pheromone applied from the ground 119 Experiment 1 was conducted in 1975 north of Sault St.-Marie, ON, in a stand of spruce 120 and fir infested with a moderate density of spruce budworm (Sanders 1976). The experimental 121 design consisted of a grid of nine traps (3 x 3) baited with virgin females in 40 x 40 m plots. 122 Eight equidistantly spaced PVC lures releasing an unspecified amount of pheromone were placed 123 between the traps. The number of males captured per trap was recorded one day before and after 124 pheromone application, and the experiment was replicated six times in space and time. Female- 125 baited traps captured an average of about 50 males per night before treatment. On the night of 126 treatment, trap catches were reduced by 60% and 97% at the periphery and centre of plots, 127 respectively. 128 Experiment 2 was conducted in 1978 near St-Quentin, NB, in a mature stand of balsam 129 fir – spruce (Schmidt and Seabrook 1979). The experimental design consisted of 16 square 130 meter circular plots with cylindrical cages (61 cm diameter x 61 cm high) placed at the centre 131 and surrounded by eight sources of pheromone positioned on tree stumps. Adult moths were 132 confined in the cages at different densities and sex-ratios for 5 days, after which the females 133 were dissected to determine their mating status. In control plots, the mating success of females 134 was independent of the density of adults and increased with the relative abundance of males. For 135 a release rate of 1 800 mg / ha / h, the mating success of females in treated plots was low (4%) at 136 densities of one or three mating pairs (four of 89 mated females) and intermediate (23-27%) at a 137 density of ten mating pairs per cage (24 of 89 mated females for a sex-ratio of one male per 6 7 138 female; 11 of 47 mated females for a sex-ratio of three males per female). The mating success of 139 females was similar for release rates of 330 and 1 800 mg AI / ha / h. 140 Experiment 3 was conducted in 1981 near Fredericton in New Brunswick (Palaniswamy 141 et al. 1982). The experimental design included 20 treated sites and four control sites; the 142 distance between treated sites was > 500 m, whereas treated and control sites were > 3 km apart. 143 Each plot consisted of a 12 m diameter shaded clearing surrounded by conifers. Four cylindrical 144 screen cages (61 cm diameter x 61 cm high) were deployed at the center of each plot. Adult 145 moths were confined in the cages at different densities and sex-ratios for 5 days, after which 146 period the females were dissected to determine their mating status. In control plots, the mating 147 success of females was independent of the density of mating pairs or sex ratio, and 228 of 566 148 (41%) of females mated during the 5 day experiment. Mating success of females in pheromone- 149 treated plots was extremely low (1.5%) at a low density of adults (one mated female among 67 150 females in cages with three males and three females), low (8.9%) at intermediate density (22 151 mated females among 250 females in cages with six males and six females), intermediate 152 (14.1%) at high density (24 mated females among 170 females in cages with 12 males and 12 153 females), and relatively high (20.7%) when the sex-ratio was male-biased (149 mated females 154 among 720 females in cages with 12 males and six females). The level of mating suppression 155 increased linearly with the release rate of pheromone for a range of 8 to 84 mg AI / ha / h. 156 Experiment 4 was conducted in 1981 near Fredericton, NB, in a 10 ha mixed stand of 157 balsam fir and white spruce with a density of ca. 0.3 late instars per terminal shoot (Alford and 158 Silk 1983). Eight 10 x 10 m plots were established, with a distance between plots > 25 m. Two 159 hundred Hercon flakes impregnated with pheromone were applied by hand in six plots (10 mg 160 AI / ha / h) and the two remaining plots served as control. The effect of mating disruption on 7 8 161 mating success was assessed using 480 females tethered near the tip of a branch. Mating success 162 of females was higher in control plots than in pheromone-treated plots (62 vs 21%). 163 Experiment 5 was conducted in 1982 near Durham, NB, in a 100 ha mixed stand of 164 balsam fir and white spruce with a density of ca 0.5 late instars per terminal shoot (Alford and 165 Silk 1983). Sixteen 20 x 25 m plots were established, with a distance between plots > 50 m. 166 Two thousand Hercon flakes impregnated with pheromone were applied by hand in 12 plots (21 167 mg / ha / h) and the four remaining plots served as control. The effect of mating disruption on 168 female mating success was evaluated using sticky traps baited with virgin females (four traps per 169 plot) and recording the number of males captured in each trap daily. The pheromone treatment 170 reduced trap catches by about 80%, although the effect was minimal during the peak period of 171 adult emergence. The level of disruption was higher when pheromone sources were clumped 172 rather than uniformly distributed. 173 Experiment 6 was conducted in 1989 near Noonan, NB, in two 1 ha sites composed of 174 balsam fir and spruce (Kipp et al. 1990). One site was treated with 110 g AI / ha of pheromone 175 and the other site left untreated. The effect of mating disruption on male orientation was 176 evaluated using traps baited with virgin females or synthetic pheromones. Several males were 177 captured in traps located in the control plot but none in the treated plot. 178 179 180 Trials based on pheromone applied from the air Experiment 7 was conducted in 1977 near Sault St.-Marie, ON, in two 12 ha white spruce 181 stands heavily infested with budworms (20 to 25 third to fifth instars per 45 cm branch tips); the 182 two stands were located 1.6 km apart (Sanders 1976). The control stand was treated with 183 insecticide when larvae were in the sixth instar, but the mortality attributable to the insecticide 8 9 184 was only 30%. The other stand was treated with pheromone applied from the air at a rate of 7.4 185 g AI / ha in the middle of the flight season. Twenty traps baited with one virgin female per trap 186 were deployed in each plot, and the females were replaced every second to fifth day. Following 187 the application of the pheromone, the captures of males declined by > 90% from more than 80 188 males per trap per night to less than five; captures of males in control plots remained consistently 189 high (> 40 males per trap per night) over time. The effect of pheromone treatment was sustained 190 for a period exceeding 10 days. The relatively high captures of males in female baited traps on 191 the windward side of the pheromone-treated plot suggest that mating disruption is most effective 192 in large stands with a low periphery to area ratio. 193 Experiment 8 was conducted in 1977 in Ontario at two sites, one located near 194 Searchmont and the other near Black Sturgeon Lake (Sanders 1979). The density of budworms 195 was considerably higher at Searchmont (32 late instars per 45 cm branch tip) than Black 196 Sturgeon Lake (< 1 late instar per branch tip). In Searchmont, a 250 ha white spruce stand was 197 treated from the air with 10 g AI / ha of pheromone-treated Conrel hollow fibers applied from 198 the air; a slightly larger stand 1.5 km to the west of the treated area served as a control. In Black 199 Sturgeon Lake, two 10 ha stands located 1 km apart were treated with either 3 or 30 g AI / ha of 200 pheromone, and one control plot was established > 10 km from the treated areas. In order to 201 minimize the effect of female dispersal on the assessment of mating disruption in treated plots at 202 Black Sturgeon Lake, all the estimates were obtained in 300 x 300 m plots surrounded by a 100 203 m buffer area. The effect of mating disruption on the orientation of males toward pheromone 204 sources was evaluated in each plot using > 25 traps baited with either synthetic pheromone or 205 virgin females. At both locations, the density of males captured in pheromone- or female-baited 206 traps was > 97% lower in treated plots than in control plants. The effect of mating disruption on 9 10 207 female mating success was evaluated in each plot using 10 traps baited with virgin females for 208 one day; the traps were replaced periodically depending upon the availability of females. In the 209 control plots, the proportion of females that attracted males for mating was higher at the site with 210 a high budworm density (Searchmont: 32 of 67 females, or 47.8%) than at the site with a low 211 density (Black Sturgeon Lake: 12 of 65 females, or 18.5%). Mating disruption greatly reduced 212 the mating success of females at the two locations: in total, only three of 127 females (2.4%) 213 attracted males in pheromone-treated plots. The effect of mating disruption on the abundance of 214 eggs could not be rigorously assessed at Black Sturgeon Lake due to the very low population 215 density. In Searchmont, the number of egg masses did not significantly differ in control and 216 treated plots, whereas the number of overwintering larvae was about 50 % lower in pheromone- 217 treated plots than in control plots (26 vs 39 larvae per 10 m2 of foliage). 218 Experiment 9 was conducted in 1978 near Saint-Quentin, NB, and near Amherst, NS, in 219 mixed stands of fir and spruce (Miller 1979). The experimental design consisted of five 100 ha 220 plots that were treated with 0, 0.1, 1.0, 10, and 20 mg AI / ha / h (New Brunswick) and 0, 0.1, 1, 221 10, and 13 mg AI / ha / h (Nova Scotia). The pheromone was applied from the air using 222 Conrel® hollow fibers footnote 2. We follow herein the recommendations of the authors and 223 discard results in plots with 0.1 or 1 mg AI / ha / h, because the effect of the pheromone was 224 minimal and inconsistent at these concentrations. The number of adults emerging in the 225 experimental plots was similar in pheromone treated plots and control plots (New Brunswick: 58 226 and 83 pupae pupae per 10 m2 of foliage; Nova Scotia; 226 vs 103). The effect of mating 227 disruption on the orientation of males toward pheromone sources was evaluated using 15-40 228 sticky traps baited with synthetic pheromone per plot. For traps placed at the mid-crown level 229 (1.5 m above ground), the pheromone reduced male captures in traps by > 94 % in New 10 11 230 Brunswick (70 vs 1226 males captured in 10 traps over 22 nights) and Nova Scotia (477 vs 10 231 731 males). The pheromone also reduced captures of males in traps located in the upper canopy 232 but the effect was slightly lower than that observed in the mid-crown (New Brunswick, 1 164 vs 233 6 390 males; Nova Scotia, 3 620 vs 25 623 males). The latter result was attributed by the authors 234 to a low level of mating disruption in the upper canopy toward the end of the flight season. The 235 effect of mating disruption on female mating success was assessed using a variety of methods: 236 (1) Laboratory-reared spruce budworms were confined in cylindrical screen cages (61 cm 237 diameter x 61 cm high) at different densities of mating pairs (1, 3, and 10). The pheromone 238 treatment reduced the proportion of mated females from about 73 to 40% early in the emergence 239 season, but the reduction was weaker late in the season. The mating success of females 240 increased with the density of mating pairs in the cage in the pheromone-treated plots, but was not 241 affected by density in the control plots. (2) Females that had been collected in the field as pupae 242 were tethered on branches in the mid-crown and upper canopy of balsam fir for a period of 24 h. 243 A total of 166 tethered females were recovered from plots treated with > 10 mg AI / ha / h, 54 of 244 which were mated (32.5%). For the females tethered in the mid-crown of trees, the proportion of 245 mated females was lower in the plots treated with pheromone (six of 77, or 7.8%) than in the 246 control plots (26 of 42, or 61.9%). Females tethered in the upper tree canopy experienced a 247 relatively high mating success (22 of 47 mated females, or 46.8%). (3) Feral females were 248 collected from trees sprayed with pyrethrin in all plots, with the exception of control plots. The 249 low proportion of virgin females in plots treated with a low (< 1 mg AI / ha / h) concentration of 250 pheromone (45 of 859 virgins, or 5.2%) compared with plots with a high (> 10 mg AI / ha / h) 251 concentration of pheromone (139 of 972 females, or 14.3%) suggests some level of mating 252 disruption among feral females. (4) Females that died from natural causes were collected in 11 12 253 trays placed at the bottom of trees to determine their mating status. No virgin females were 254 collected in a sample that included > 1 000 females. (5) In plots treated with a high 255 concentration of pheromone, only two of 831 females collected in Malaise traps were virgin. 256 The number of egg masses was slightly higher in pheromone-treated plots than in control plots in 257 New Brunswick, whereas the opposite trend was observed in Nova Scotia. 258 Experiment 10 was conducted in 1980 near Machias, ME, in mixed spruce-fir stands. 259 The experiment included blocks of varying size (9 to 145 ha), two of which served as control and 260 the remaining three were treated with pheromone applied from the air in Hercon flakes at 261 different concentrations (50, 100, 500 g AI / ha) (Dimond et al. 1984). The density of spruce 262 budworm was not reported, but the experiment was conducted at the peak of an outbreak. The 263 effect of mating disruption on the orientation of males toward pheromone sources was evaluated 264 using 20 sticky traps baited with synthetic pheromone and five unbaited traps per plot; due to the 265 high density of budworms, the traps saturated with moths very quickly, thus the observations 266 were conducted over 2 or 3 h periods. Captures of males exceeded 20 individuals per hour for 267 several days over a 20 day interval in July. The reduction of male captures in pheromone-treated 268 plots relative to control plots was more obvious at the high pheromone concentration (500 g AI / 269 ha: >95 % reduction) than at the low pheromone concentrations (< 50 g AI / ha: ca. 50% 270 reduction). The effect of mating disruption on female mating success was assessed using two 271 approaches. (1) Virgin females were tethered on the upper tree canopy and their mating status 272 recorded later. Sample sizes were low overall, but a trend suggested that mating disruption 273 reduced female mating success early but not late in the season. (2) Feral females were collected 274 either using Malaise traps suspended between the tree crowns or a gasoline powered vacuum 275 device. The mating success of females was very similar in pheromone-treated and control traps, 12 13 276 independent of the sampling device. The authors reported a high proportion of feral females 277 were virgin (up to 55% in some plots) through the entire period of adult emergence, but provided 278 no actual data. The relatively high incidence of virgin females was not due to a shortage of 279 males per se because the ratio of males per female exceeded 10:1 at most sites. Counts of egg 280 masses on the foliage failed to reveal any effect of mating disruption on either the abundance of 281 egg masses or the fertility of eggs. 282 Experiment 11 was conducted in 1981 near Thessalon, ON, in white spruce stands 283 (Sanders and Silk 1981). The experiment included one 7.5 ha control plot and one 30 ha plot 284 treated with 100 g AI / ha applied as Hercon laminated flakes from the air, with a distance of 10 285 km between plots. The density of budworm was moderate (ca 0.3 emerged pupae per 45 cm 286 branch section). The effect of pheromone treatment on male orientation was evaluated using 20 287 synthetic pheromone baited traps in each plot, half of which were suspended in the tree canopy 288 and the other half two m above ground. The traps in the control plots became saturated with 289 moths during the peak period of flight (> 50 males per day), but the data clearly indicated lower 290 captures of males in pheromone treated plots than in control plots; the reduction in trap catches 291 appeared to be more pronounced for traps located near the ground than those in the tree canopy. 292 The effect of mating disruption on mating success was evaluated by confining laboratory-reared 293 moths in 1 m3 screen cages hauled into the canopy about 8m above ground, using six cages per 294 plot; the experiment was repeated over time. The proportion of mated females was lower in 295 pheromone-treated plots (102 of 222, or 45.9%) than in control plots (173 of 218, or 79.4%). 296 However, all 18 feral females captured by net in the upper crown of spruce in pheromone-treated 297 plots were mated, and the density of egg masses was not different in control and pheromone- 298 treated plots (0.75 and 0.78 masses per 45 cm branch section). 13 14 299 Experiment 12 was conducted in 1983 in Ontario using two large screen cages (9.4 x 5.6 300 x 3 m) erected around live balsam fir trees at the centre of 3 ha plots, before the application of 301 pheromone treatment (Seabrook and Kipp 1986). One plot was treated by air with Hercon flakes 302 impregnated with pheromone (148 g AI / ha) and the other left untreated. A total of 2 300 moths 303 (1 450 males: 850 virgin females) were confined in each cage for a period of 4 days. A reduction 304 in mating success of 30-75% in the pheromone-treated plot relative to the control plot was 305 reported. 306 Experiment 13 was conducted in 1985 using the same protocol as in experiment 10. The 307 body size (wing length) of mated and unmated females was assessed upon completion of the 308 experiment (Kipp et al. 1987). The proportion of mated females was two times higher in the 309 control plot (326 of 517 females, or 63.1%) than in the treated plot (147 of 458, or 32.1%). 310 Mated females were larger than unmated females in the treated plot but not in the control plot. 311 Experiment 14 was conducted in 1987 near Dunphy, NB, in a 25 ha plot treated with 312 pheromone applied from the air as ICI Microcaps at a concentration of 96 g AI / ha, and in two 313 control plots of unspecified area (Seabrook and Baskerville 1988). The density of spruce 314 budworm at the onset of the experiment was lower in the treated plot than in the control plots 315 (ca. 4.5 vs 9.0 empty pupal cases per m2 of foliage). The effect of mating disruption on male 316 orientation was assessed using traps baited with synthetic pheromone or left unbaited (three traps 317 per plot for each treatment); the traps were replaced periodically to prevent trap saturation. The 318 data reported below are in terms of males per trap per day. The pheromone-baited traps captured 319 > 10 times more males in the control plots than in the treated plot early in the emergence season 320 until the 7 July (9.2 vs. 0.8); few males were captured in blank traps in either the control or 321 treated plots (0.6 vs. 0.1). The level of mating disruption declined thereafter (17.1 vs. 3.5 in 14 15 322 control and treated plots), which was attributed by the authors to a heavy rainfall “washing off” 323 the pheromone from the foliage. Surprisingly, the blank traps captured a relatively high number 324 of males, especially in control plots (2.5 vs 0.5). The effect of pheromone treatment on female 325 mating success was assessed using adults collected in Malaise traps deployed on scaffold towers 326 (six traps per plot). All the sampled females were mated. The results were difficult to interpret 327 because only eight adults were captured in treated plots compared with 1 289 in the control plots; 328 the authors noted that the traps may not have been deployed properly in treated plots. The ratio 329 of egg mass per pupal case was three times higher in the control plots than in the treated plot, but 330 data on the number of egg masses in different plots were not reported. It was concluded that the 331 data were difficult to interpret due to the variation in adult densities between plots and the 332 potential adverse effect of rainfall on mating disruption. 333 Experiment 15 was conducted in 1988 near Bathurst, NB, in three 25 ha stands 334 comprising > 40% of mature balsam fir (Seabrook 1989). Two plots were treated with 335 pheromone (20 and 90 g AI / ha) and the other plot was left untreated. Sampling was conducted 336 in the 10 ha at the center of each plot; the distance between plots was > 5 km. The study is 337 unique in that the mating status of males was recorded in the first half of the emergence period 338 (before ca 50% of females had emerged) using the method developed by Bergh et al. (1988); no 339 difference was observed in control plots (69 of 425 mated males, or 16.2%) and treated plots (58 340 of 423, or 13.7%). The effect of mating disruption on male orientation was assessed using traps 341 baited with synthetic pheromone or virgin females, or left unbaited. The high density of spruce 342 budworm resulted in very high captures of males (>800 individuals per trap for some days) thus 343 the traps in control plots rapidly became saturated with moths; data compiled during one day by 344 replacing the traps every two h indicated a high level of mating disruption (captures of males in 15 16 345 traps baited with pheromone and females in control plots: 835 and 260; in treated plots: 80 and 346 62); captures of males were high even in blank traps (average of 37 males per trap per day). 347 Female mating success was not assessed. The ratio of egg mass per pupal case was similar in 348 control and treated plots and reached a plateau above 3.5 late in the season. 349 Experiment 16 was conducted in 1996 near Fredericton, NB, in two plots, one of which 350 was 1.9 ha in size and treated twice during the season with pheromone applied in 3M microcaps 351 at a concentration of 100 g AI / ha; the second plot was left untreated (Lonergan et al. 1997). 352 The effect of pheromone treatment on female mating success was assessed by confining adults in 353 large screen cages (five to 15 mating pairs in 1 980 L cages), intermediate-sized screen cages 354 (two to three mating pairs in 7.8 L cages) and small screen cages (1 mating pair in Petri dish 355 sized cages); the experiment was repeated for each cage design over time. The difference in the 356 proportion of mated females between control and treated cages was higher early rather than late 357 in the season (Before 25 August in treated plots, 53 of 727 mated females, or 7.35%; in control 358 plots, 645 of 648 mated females, or 99.5%. After 25 August in treated plots, 61 of 168 mated 359 females, or 36.3%; in control plots, 158 of 160 mated females or 98.8%). No apparent effect of 360 cage size on mating success was detected. 361 Experiment 17 was conducted in 2000 near Fredericton, NB, in two plots, one of which 362 was 4 ha in size and treated with 50 g AI / ha of pheromone in 3M microcaps, and the other left 363 untreated (Silk and Kettela 2001). The effect of mating disruption on female mating success 364 was assessed by confining two mating pairs in 7.8 L screen cages; the experiment was repeated 365 several times during the season both low in the canopy and in the upper crown. The proportion 366 of mated females was considerably lower in treated plots (12 of 122 mated females, or 9.8%) 16 17 367 than in control plots (90 of 116 mated females, or 77.6%). The level of mating suppression was 368 more pronounced early in the season, but no apparent effect of canopy height was detected. 369 Experiment 18 was conducted in 2001 near Fredericton, NB, in three plots, two of which 370 were 5 ha in size and treated with either 25 or 75 g AI / ha of pheromone applied from the air in 371 3M microcaps; a control 1 ha plot was left untreated (Silk and Kettela 2002). The effect of 372 pheromone treatment on female mating success was assessed by confining two mating pairs in 373 17.5 L screen cages; the experiment included 12 cages per plot and was repeated over time. The 374 mating success of females was low in the high pheromone dose plot (15 of 165 mated females, or 375 9.1%), intermediate in the low pheromone dose plot (36 of 165 mated females, or 21.8%), and 376 high in the control plot (105 of 145 mated females, or 72.4%). 377 Experiment 19 was conducted in 2004 in Balsam Lake Provincial Park in southern 378 Ontario in three white spruce stands, two of which were treated with pheromone at a 379 concentration of 20 g AI per ha (20 ha plot) and 50 g AI / ha (46 ha plot); a third plot located 1 380 km away from the treated plots was left untreated and served as a control (Kettela and Silk 381 2005). The density of spruce budworm at the onset of the experiment averaged 4.8 late instars 382 per 50 cm branch section. The effect of pheromone treatment on male orientation was assessed 383 using traps baited with synthetic pheromone (30 traps per plot); the traps were checked daily. 384 The number of moths per trap was consistently lower every day through the emergence season in 385 the plot treated with 50 g AI / ha than in the control plot. Male captures were also reduced in the 386 plot treated with 15 g AI / ha, yet to a considerably lesser extent than for the high dose treatment. 387 The effect of mating disruption on female mating success was assessed in ten L screen cages 388 with two males and one virgin female; 18 cages were deployed in each plot and the adults 389 replaced every third day. The proportion of mated females was low at the high pheromone dose 17 18 390 (6%), intermediate at the low pheromone dose (48%), and high in the control plot (82%). The 391 mating success of wild females was also determined for individuals captured with a sweep net. 392 All the females collected in the control plots after the 23 June were mated; in contrast, the 393 proportion of mated females varied between days from 30 to 80% at the high pheromone dose, 394 and from 55 to 85% at the low pheromone dose. The density of egg masses remainded 395 consistently low at the high pheromone dose (1.16 to 1.32 masses per 50 cm branch section) and 396 steadily increased over time at the low pheromone dose (3.98 to 13.20 egg masses per 50 cm 397 branch section between 23 June and 11 July); the data expressed in relative terms (egg masses 398 per pupal case) was 0.22 for the control plot, 0.18 for the low pheromone concentration, and 0.12 399 for the high pheromone concentration. 400 Experiment 20 was conducted in 2005 near Craighurst, ON, in two white spruce stands, 401 one of which was 8.2 ha in area and treated with 50 g AI / ha of pheromone; the other plot was 402 25 ha in area and served as a control (Kettela et al. 2006). The density of pupa at the onset of 403 the experiment was approximately 1.7 individuals per 50 cm branch section. The effect of 404 mating disruption on male orientation was assessed using Multipher traps baited with synthetic 405 pheromone and replaced at intervals of 3-6 days (15 traps in control plots and 30 traps in treated 406 plots). The average number of males per trap was 85% lower in treated plots (2.2) than in 407 control plots (14.8). The effect of pheromone treatment on female mating success was assessed 408 using 10 L screen cages with two males and one female placed in the field for a period of three 409 days. The proportion of mated females was considerably lower in the treated plot (37 of 98 410 mated, or 37.8%) than in control plots (97 of 105 mated females, or 92.3%). A total of 26 feral 411 free-flying females were captured, all of which were mated; the authors did not specify if these 412 females were captured in the treated or control plots. The number of egg masses sampled on the 18 19 413 foliage was considerably lower in the treated plot than in the control plot (five and 38 egg masses 414 in total, respectively). 415 Experiment 21 was conducted in 2008 on the North Shore in Québec in a stand with > 416 40% of fir infested with budworms (20 to 25 overwintering larvae per branch) (Trudel et al. 417 2009). The strength of the study was the replicated structure of the experiment (factorial design 418 including all combinations of Bt and/or pheromone application at a dose of 35 g AI / ha, 419 corresponding to four treatments repeated four times each, for a total of 16 50 ha blocks). The 420 distance between experimental blocks was not specified by the authors. The interpretation of the 421 results is difficult because the application of Bt before adult emergence had no effect on any of 422 the demographic parameters reported in the study. The captures of males in traps baited with 423 synthetic pheromone were reduced by 80 to 99% in blocks treated with pheromone, and the 424 reduction was consistent over a 39 day period. Traps baited with virgin females were used as a 425 proxy to assess the mating success of females. Although the presence of pheromone reduced the 426 percentage of cages that captured at least one male and the overall number of males per trap, the 427 proportion of mated females within the cages was independent of the pheromone treatment. No 428 difference in reproduction by female budworms could be detected as a function of pheromone 429 treatment, either in terms of egg or larval densities. 430 431 References 432 433 Alford, A.R., and Silk, P.J. 1983. Effect of pheromone-releaser distribution and release rate on 434 the mating success of spruce budworm (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae). Journal of Economic 435 Entomology, 76: 774-778. 19 20 436 Bergh, J.C., Eveleigh, E.S., and Seabrook, W.D. 1988. The mating status of field-collected male 437 spruce budworm, Choristoneura fumiferana (Clem.) (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae), in 438 relation to trap location, sampling method, sampling date, and adult emergence. Canadian 439 Entomologist, 120: 821-830. 440 Dimond, J.B., Mott, D.G., Kemp, W.P., and Krall, J.H. 1984. A field test of mating-suppression 441 using the spruce budworm pheromone. Maine Agricultural Experiment Station, 442 University of Maine at Orono, Technical Bulletin 113. 21 pp. 443 Kettela, E., and Silk, P. 2005. Development of a pheromone formulation for use in early 444 intervention pest management strategies of the spruce budworm Choristoneura 445 fumiferana (Clem.). SERG-1 Project 614, 36 pp. [Reference 1 in 446 http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/.] 447 Kettela, E., Holmes, S.E., and Silk, P.J. 2006. Results of aerially applied Disrupt® microflakes 448 on spruce budworm mating success, Ontario, 2005. [Reference 2 in 449 http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/]. 450 Kipp, L., Bergh, J.C., and Seabrook, W.D. 1987. A spruce budworm mating bias in two- 451 component pheromone environments. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata, 45: 139- 452 144. 453 Kipp, L.R., Lonergan, G.C., and Seabrook, W.D. 1990. Spruce budworm-related research, final 454 report, 1989. Department of Biology and Chemistry, University of New Brunswick, 455 Fredericton, NB [Reference 3 in http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/]. 456 Lonergan, G, Silk, P., and Kettela, E. 1997. Development of a 3M microencapsulated sex 457 pheromone formulation for use in spruce budworm IPM programs. [Reference 4 in 458 http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 20 21 459 460 Miller, C.A. 1979. Report of spruce budworm pheromone trials, Maritimes. 149 pp. [Reference 6 in http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 461 Palaniswamy, P., and Seabrook, W.D. 1978. Behavioral responses of the female eastern spruce 462 budworm Choristoneura fumiferana (Lepidoptera, Tortricidae) to the sex pheromone of 463 her own species. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 4: 649-655. 464 Palaniswamy, P., Sivasubramanian, P., and Seabrook, W.D. 1979. Modulation of sex pheromone 465 perception in female moths of the eastern spruce budworm, Choristoneura fumiferana by 466 Altosid. Journal of Insect Physiology, 25: 571-574. 467 Palaniswamy, P., Ross, R.J., Seabrook, W.D., Lonergan, G.C., Wiesner, C.J., Tan, S.H., and 468 Silk, P.J. 1982. Mating suppression of caged spruce budworm (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae) 469 moths in different pheromone atmospheres and high population densities. Journal of 470 Economic Entomology, 75: 989-993. 471 Palaniswamy, P., and Seabrook, W.D. 1985. The alteration of calling behaviour by female 472 Choristoneura fumiferana when exposed to synthetic sex pheromone. Entomologia 473 Experimentalis et Applicata, 37: 13-16. 474 Ponder, B.M., Kipp, L.R., Bergh, C., Lonergan, G.C., and Seabrook, W.D. 1986. Factors 475 affecting spruce budworm (Choristoneura fumiferana) (Clem.) mating and mating 476 disruption with pheromone in the laboratory. Canadian Entomologist, 118: 797-805. 477 478 479 Sanders, C.J. 1976. Disruption of sex attraction in the Eastern spruce budworm. Environmental Entomology, 5: 868-872. Sanders, C.J. 1979. Spruce budworm mating disruption trials using synthetic attractant in Conrel 21 22 480 fibres (Ontario, 1977). Canadian Forestry Service, Department of the Environment, Great 481 Lakes Forest Research Centre, Sault-Ste Marie, ON. Report 0-X-285. 32 pp. [Reference 9 482 in http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 483 484 485 Sanders, C.J. 1982. Disruption of male spruce budworm orientation to calling females in a wind tunnel by synthetic pheromone. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 8: 493-506. Sanders, C.J. 1985. Disruption of spruce budworm, Choristoneura fumiferana (Lepidoptera: 486 Tortricidae), mating in wind tunnel by synthetic pheromone: role of habituation. 487 Canadian Entomologist, 117: 391-393. 488 489 490 Sanders, C.J. 1987. Flight and copulation of female spruce budworm in pheromone-impregnated air. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 13: 1749-1758. Sanders, C.J. 1995. Disruption of male spruce budworm orientation to female moths by 491 pheromone and pheromone analogues in a wind-tunnel. Physiological Entomology, 20: 492 71-80. 493 Sanders, C.J. 1996. Effects of prolonged exposure to different concentrations of synthetic 494 pheromone on mating disruption of spruce budworm moths in a wind tunnel. Canadian 495 Entomologist, 128: 57-66. 496 497 498 Sanders, C. 1998. Effect of pheromone permeation on sustained flight of male spruce budworm. Canadian Entomologist, 130: 539-544. Sanders, C.J., and Lucuik, G.S. 1972. Factors affecting calling by female eastern spruce 499 budworm, Choristoneura fumiferana (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae). Canadian Entomologist, 500 104: 1751-1762. 501 Sanders, C.J., and Silk, P.J. 1981. Disruption of spruce budworm by means of Hercon plastic 22 23 502 laminated flakes, Ontario 1981. Great Lakes Forest Research Centre, Information Report 503 O-X-335, Canadian Forest Services. 22 pp. [Reference 10 in 504 http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 505 506 507 Schmidt, J.O., and Seabrook, W.D. 1979. Mating of caged spruce budworm moths in pheromone environments. Journal of Economic Entomology, 72: 509-511. Schmidt, J.O., Seabrook, W.D., Lonergan, G., Oda, T., Darvesh, S., and Valenta, Z. 1980. 508 Effects of pheromone, pheromone components, and pheromone analogues on mating of 509 the spruce budworm (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae). Canadian Entomologist, 112: 605-608. 510 Seabrook, W.D. 1989. Spruce budworm pheromone project: 1988 progress report. Department of 511 Biology, University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, NB. 54 pp. [Reference 11 in 512 http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 513 Seabrook, W.D., and Baskerville, G.L. 1988. The development of a larvicide and pheromone 514 based integrated pest management strategy for the control of the spruce budworm. 515 Department of Biology, University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, NB. 91 pp. 516 [Reference 12 in http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 517 Seabrook, W.D. and Kipp, L.R. 1986. The use of a two component blend of the spruce budworm 518 sex pheromone for mating suppression. In Proceedings of the International Symposium 519 on Controlled Release of Bioactive Materials, Chandry, Q.A., and Thjeophilus, C. (eds). 520 Controlled Released Society, Norfolk, VA. pp. 128-129. [Reference 13 in 521 http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 522 Silk, P.J., and Kettela, E.G. 2001. To develop and test pheromone formulations for early 523 intervention management strategies of the spruce budworm. SERG Report # 2000 / 06. 524 17 pp. [Reference 14 in http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 23 24 525 Silk, P.J., and Kettela, E.G. 2002. To develop and test pheromone formulations for use in early 526 intervention strategies of the spruce budworm – year 2. SERG Report, 13 pp. [Reference 527 15 in http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 528 Trudel, R., Dupont, A., and Bélanger, A. 2009. Experimental pheromone applications using 529 Disrupt Micro-flakes SBW® for the control of the spruce budworm populations: Québec 530 mating disruption trials 2008. Proceedings of the SERG International 2009 Workshop, 531 Winnipeg, Manitoba, 140-145. [Reference 16 in 532 http://atl.cfs.nrcan.gc.ca/sprucebudworm/] 533 24