Title: Non-verbal behaviour of patients with schizophrenia in medical consultations – A comparison with depressed patients and association with symptom levels Running title: Non-verbal behaviour in consultations Authors: Sara Dimic¹, Christiane Wildgrube¹ ², Rosemarie McCabe¹, Imren Hassan ¹, Thomas R. E. Barnes ³, Stefan Priebe¹ ¹ Unit for Social and Community Psychiatry, Queen Mary University of London ² North Essex Partnership Foundation Trust ³ Imperial College London Correspondence: Dr Sara Dimic Queen Mary University of London Unit for Social and Community Psychiatry Newham Centre for Mental Health London E13 8SP Tel. 0 (+44) 20 8821 0300 e-mail sara.dimic@eastlondon.nhs.uk Key words Non-verbal behaviour, communication, praecox feeling, schizophrenia, medical consultations ABSTRACT Background: The Dutch psychiatrist Rümke introduced the notion that patients with schizophrenia could be recognised intuitively by an experienced psychiatrist, naming the process as the 'praecox feeling'. This feeling may reflect aspects of the non-verbal behaviour of patients as shown in a medical consultation. The aim of this study was to test whether aspects of the initial non-verbal behaviour of patients with schizophrenia and depression in medical consultations differ and are associated with symptom levels in each group. Sampling and Method: The first two minutes of videotaped outpatient consultations with 78 patients (39 each with schizophrenia and depression) were analysed by researchers who were masked to the diagnosis and symptom levels of the patients. Non-verbal behaviour was assessed using a modified Ethological Coding System for Interviews (ECSI). Symptom levels were rated by independent researchers on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). Results: The two diagnostic groups showed significant differences on one out of six aspects of non-verbal communication, with patients with schizophrenia showing more ‘flight’ behaviour, i.e. more looking down or away, crouching and freezing. Within the group of patients with schizophrenia, more ‘flight’ and less ‘assertion’ were associated with higher symptom levels. Conclusion: Initial ‘flight’ behaviour of patients in medical consultations may be specific for schizophrenia and associated with the symptoms of that illness. Such behaviour might indicate an avoidance of potentially adverse communication, and future research should investigate whether it can also be identified in everyday social interactions other than medical consultations. BACKGROUND In 1941 the Dutch psychiatrist Rümke introduced the notion that a patient with schizophrenia could be recognised intuitively by an experienced psychiatrist, naming the process as the 'praecox feeling'. According to Rümke [1941] the praecox feeling of the psychiatrist is based on the facial expression, demeanour, motor behaviour and changing expressions of patients with schizophrenia, that are characteristic for those patients and immediately give the condition away. Rümke suggested that the praecox feeling can be noticed in the “shortest assessment” and is “frequently generated before any words have been exchanged with the patient” (both quotes Rümke 1941, p. 168, translation by the authors). He explains that the feeling does not reflect the commonly assessed clinical symptoms of schizophrenia and rather occurs when the clinician attempts to establish contact with the patient. Gaebel [1990] suggested that Rümke intended to base the diagnosis of schizophrenia largely on specific impressions made on the psychiatrist by the behavioural characteristics of the patient. Such behavioural aspects may not be captured in the symptom definitions that determine current diagnostic categories and assessment instruments. Nevertheless they might be relevant for a better understanding of the characteristics of the disorder and possibly help explain the frequent difficulties of patients with schizophrenia in interpersonal communication [Bellack at al., 1990]. Using audio-visual recording, the behaviour of patients can be independently assessed. Extensive research has assessed some aspects of patients’ non-verbal behaviour within studies focussing on social cognition and emotional processing [Feldman-Barret and Salovey, 2002; Gross, 2002; Goldsmith and Davidson, 2004]. Several such studies have found a diminished facial expression of patients with schizophrenia [Schneider et al., 1990; Berenbaum and Oltmanns, 1992; Blanchard et al., 1994; Mattes et al., 1995; Geabel and Wolwer, 2005]. Yet, this research has used experimental settings in laboratories or role plays rather than real-life, everyday situations to assess patients’ behaviour. For various reasons, it remains unclear to what extent findings generated in experimental situations help understand patients’ behaviour in real-life situations. The medical consultation, i.e. the routine meeting between a patient and a psychiatrist, is an everyday social interaction. The situation is of relevance to the patient since important decisions about future treatment are negotiated and agreed on [Priebe and McCabe, 2008]. It is also the context in which the psychiatrist observes the patient’s behaviour and reaches a diagnosis. It therefore lends itself as a setting in which to study the patient’s communication behaviour, including its non-verbal aspects, and test the findings for an association with the clinical diagnosis of the psychiatrist. However, investigating the medical consultation as a real-life situation has the major limitation that the methods of recording and documenting the patient’s behaviour must not change the nature of the consultation and not interfere with the behaviour normally shown by patients in such situations. This requires a cautious use of technical equipment (e.g. cameras) in the consultation room and assessment procedures that accommodate the wide range of behaviour as it occurs in routine practice. Over the last 25 years, the techniques of audio-visual recording of human interactions have been advanced and become widely available. This has facilitated an increasing body of research on non-verbal behaviour of patients with mental disorders in clinical consultations. Such ‘naturalistic’ studies have used different theoretical frameworks and explored various aspects of communicative behaviour in clinical settings without yet establishing standard procedures and assessment methods. A review on professional–patient communication in the treatment of mental illness [Hassan et al., 2007] found that existing studies used different underlying models. The authors concluded that there was little methodological consistency and findings were disparate. This study used naturally occurring consultations of psychiatrists with patients with schizophrenia and depression [e.g. McCabe et al., 2002], and assessed patients’ nonverbal behaviour during the first two minutes. This focus on the initial phase of the interaction was to reflect the assumption of Rümke that the praecox feeling is immediately noticeable, have a consistent timeframe across all consultations, investigate behaviour that is less influenced by the actions of the psychiatrist than at later stages of the consultation, and reduce the likelihood that diagnostically specific information is revealed in the verbal communication and thus bias the ratings of the observers. Following Rümke’s notion on the praecox feeling, we tested whether patients with schizophrenia and depression differed in their non-verbal behaviour. We also explored whether features of the non-verbal behaviour were associated with symptom levels in each group. METHOD Setting, sample and procedure The study analysed the outpatient psychiatric consultations of 78 patients with diagnoses of schizophrenia or depression, who had pre-arranged routine appointments with a consultant psychiatrist. All consultations took place in East London and in the normal consultation offices of the consultants, where a digital video-camera was placed. All aspects of the consultation (including time, length, venue, content and style) were intended to follow the usual routine and not be influenced by the recording in any way. All patients were between 18 and 65 years of age and, prior to the consultation, provided written informed consent for the session to be videotaped. After the consultation, patients were interviewed by an independent researcher and structured questionnaires were administered to capture socio-demographic and clinical characteristics and symptom levels of the patients. The first two minutes of the recorded consultations were analysed by two independent psychiatrists (SD and CW), both of whom were fully qualified with more than 10 years of clinical experience. They were not involved in the patients’ care in any way and masked towards the clinical diagnosis as well as all other characteristics of the patient including all assessments made by the researcher after the consultation. Each of the consultations was assessed by both researchers. Assessment methods The diagnosis was the primary clinical diagnosis according to ICD 10, which was taken from the patients’ records. Each patient’s age, gender, employment, living status (with partner or not), and length of illness were assessed using a structured questionnaire. Symptoms were assessed on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale [PANSS: Kay, Fiszbein and Opler, 1987] in the group of patients with schizophrenia and on the Beck Depression Inventory [BDI: Beck et al., 1961] in the patients with depression. The PANSS is a 30-item questionnaire measuring positive, negative and other symptom dimensions on the basis of a formal semi-structured clinical interview. The scores for each symptom range from 1=absent to 7=extreme. Of the 30 items, seven are grouped to form a subscale for positive symptoms, seven form a subscale for negative symptoms, and the remaining 16 items generate a score for general psychopathology. The BDI is a self-rating scale with 21 items for measuring the presence and degree of depression. For the analysis of the videotaped, non-verbal behaviour of patients during the first two minutes of the consultation, a modified version of the Ethological Coding System for Interviews [ECSI: Troisi, 1999] was used. The instrument is based on an ethological background which aims to integrate an understanding of the individual‘s interaction with the natural environment into a clinical perspective of mental disorders (Troisi, 1999). Fourteen behavioural aspects, including facial expression and hand movements, were used out of an original thirty-seven ECSI patterns. Items were grouped according to their meaning, comprising ‘prosocial behaviour’ (look at the interviewer, smile, nod), ‘flight’ (look down, crouch, look away, freezing), ‘assertion’ (lean forward) ‘gesture’ (variable hand and arm movement used during speech), ‘displacement’ (hand–face movements, fumbling) and ‘relaxation’ (fold arm across chest, laugh, neutral face). The other 23 aspects of the ECSI were not rated because they were difficult to assess with sufficient accuracy given the limited quality of the recordings. The quality was compromised as a consequence of attempting to assess routine consultations with minimal interference from the recording procedures; the video camera was placed discreetly in a corner of the office, and therefore not all aspects of non-verbal behaviour could be assessed in sufficient detail, although the patients’ face, trunk and legs could always be fully seen. The first two minutes of each videotaped consultation were analysed for the presence of each of the fourteen behavioural items in four successive 30-second intervals. The end of each 30s sampling period was marked by a beep. At the end of each 30s period, the researchers recorded the frequency of each behavioural aspect. A simple threepoint scoring system was used: 0=behaviour not present, 1=behaviour present on only one occasion, 2=behaviour present more than once. The scoring with three options rather than two as in other studies was intended to increase the variance of scores based on only two minutes. Researchers first assessed each tape independently. They then discussed any differences in ratings and reached an agreement. Statistical Analysis The sample size was intended to be more than 30 for each group to achieve a representative distribution in line with the central limit theorem [Rice, 1995]. The study aim was to find substantial differences between the diagnostic groups, rather than identifying small or medium effect sizes. We therefore selected a relatively small sample in each diagnostic group despite a limited statistical power to detect effects of small and medium size. Differences of socio-demographic characteristics between the two groups were tested using t-tests, Mann-Whitney and Chi square tests. The inter-rater reliability for assessing the defined aspects of non-verbal behaviour was assessed calculating the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient. Given the non-normal distribution of the features of non-verbal behaviour, non-parametric tests were computed to compare the behaviour in the two groups of patients (Mann-Whitney test) and test the associations with symptom levels within each group (Spearman-Rho coefficient). The same methods were used to compare non-verbal behaviour in male and female patients [Troisi, 1999], and in patients in consultations with a clinician of the same or a different gender [Hall et al 1994; Sandhu et al., 2009]. The analysis was performed using SPSS for Windows, version 13.0. RESULTS The sample consisted of 78 patients, i.e. 39 with the diagnosis of depression and 39 with the diagnosis of schizophrenia. Their characteristics are summarised in Table 1. Insert table 1 about here In both groups the mean age of patients was about 44 years, most patients were without employment and lived alone. The schizophrenia group had significantly more males (χ2 =8.8, p< 0.05) and a significantly longer mean duration of illness (U=250.5, p<0.001). There were no other statistically significant differences between the two groups. In 67 out of the total of 78 consultations the clinician was male and in 11 consultations female. In 40 consultations clinician and patient were of the same gender (male clinician + male patient = 37, female clinician + female patient’=3), while in 38 consultations there was a gender difference (male clinician + female patient’= 30, female clinician + -male patient’= 8). The average Intraclass Correlation for the inter-rater reliability for the two researchers assessing the six aspects of non-verbal behaviour was 0.973 (95% C.I.: 0.968 - 0.975, using 1092 ratings). The scores on the six aspects of non-verbal behaviour assessed in the two groups are shown in Table 2. Insert table 2 about here Patients with schizophrenia showed significantly more flight behaviour [U = 495.5, p < 0.05], whilst other differences between the two groups were not statistically significant. Across the two groups, there were no statistically significant differences between male and female patients or between those in consultations with a clinician of the same or a different gender. The correlations between the scores of non-verbal behaviour and symptom levels in each group are shown in Table 3. Insert table 3 about here In the group of patients with schizophrenia, higher levels of positive and negative symptoms and global psychopathology were associated with more flight behaviour and less assertion, although the correlation failed to reach statistical significance for the association between positive symptoms and assertion. In patients with depression, the higher BDI sum scores were correlated significantly with more displacement and less prosocial behaviour. DISCUSSION Main findings Patients with schizophrenia and depression showed differences in their non-verbal behaviour during the first two minutes of routine medical consultations. Patients with schizophrenia displayed more ‘flight’ behaviour, i.e. they more often looked down or away, crouched, and froze in their movements. Such behaviour is understood as cutting off social stimuli that are perceived as adverse [Troisi, 1999]. In patients with schizophrenia more flight behaviour was significantly associated with higher levels of psychopathological symptoms. Flight behaviour and symptom levels were not associated in patients with depression, suggesting that flight behaviour is unlikely to be a non-specific sign of severity of mental illness. Thus, flight behaviour may be specific for patients with schizophrenia (at least as compared with patients with depression) and associated with symptom severity. The fact that significant correlations in schizophrenia patients were found with positive and negative symptoms as well as global psychopathology may indicate that flight is not particularly linked with any of the established symptom dimensions, but to aspects that are captured on all three dimensions. Strengths and limitations The study has several methodological strengths. It succeeded in studying patient behaviour in a real-life situation and analysed routine medical consultations, which closely reflect the context in which psychiatrists may perceive a praecox feeling. All consultations were in a similar setting to reduce a potential source of variance. The analysis took only the first two minutes of the consultation, to minimise any confounding of the ratings through information revealed later in the consultation or by patient behaviour influenced by the psychiatrist. The assessment was rigorous and raters were masked towards the diagnosis and symptom assessments of the patients. The inter-rater reliability in assessing non-verbal behaviour was very high. The study also has limitations. Only the first two minutes of each consultation were analysed. Whilst routine medical consultations are everyday situations, there may still be specific aspects of the institutional context that influence a patient’s behaviour, and it is unclear to what extent a patient would show the same behaviour in other everyday social interactions. The study used wide diagnostic categories based on clinical diagnoses and did not employ standardised diagnostic interviews. Symptoms were inconsistently assessed in the two groups, rated by an observer in patients with schizophrenia and self-rated in patients with depression. Not all aspects of the ECSI could be rated because of the technical limitations of videotaping real medical consultations. The analysis was exploratory and not hypothesis driven. It identified a significant difference between the two groups on only one out of six tested scores, although the result remains significant after a Bonferroni adjustment for multiple testing. Patients with schizophrenia were compared only with patients with depression, and there were no other diagnostic groups and no healthy control sample. Finally, the two diagnostic groups differed in their gender distribution and mean length of illness, for which the statistical comparison was not adjusted because of the small sample size. Comparison with the literature Whilst this study identified a difference between patients with schizophrenia and depression, several ethological studies have shown that patients with schizophrenia can be distinguished from healthy subjects on the basis of their non-verbal communication, particularly by their lack of behaviour encouraging ongoing social interaction during the interview [Brüne et al., 2009]. In an Italian study of young men with a diagnosis of recent-onset of schizophrenia, nonverbal behavioural patterns clearly distinguished schizophrenia patients from control subjects, with a global restriction of non-verbal expressiveness in the patient group [Troisi et al., 1998]. However, our findings of an association of more flight behaviour with a diagnosis of schizophrenia and higher symptom levels within the group of schizophrenia patients are inconsistent with a study by Brüne et al. [2008]. They studied a heterogeneous group of acute adult inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders and reported less flight behaviour in patients as compared to healthy subjects. In the patient group, they found more flight behaviour to be correlated with lower scores of negative symptoms, as well as higher levels of excitement. Whether this inconsistency is due to differences in patient characteristics between the two studies or the different context of the behaviour – especially arranged research interviews in the study of Brüne et al. [2008] and real-life consultations in this study – remains unclear. Yet, clarifying such inconsistencies appears important for the use of similar assessment methods in future research. An association between conventional psychopathology measures and non-verbal behaviour has also been found in other studies on patients with schizophrenia [Trémau et al. 2005; Troisi et al. 2007; Brüne et al. 2008]. These studies analysed videotaped interviews that were specifically arranged for the research and not real-life situations. The study of Troisi [1998] reported a weak correlation between nonverbal behaviour and psychiatric symptoms, whilst newer studies found a strong correlation between less facial expressivity, measured on the ethological prosocial score, and more negative symptoms, in particular anergia [Trémau et al. 2005; Troisi et al. 2007]. In addition, Brüne et al. [2008] reported multiple associations of psychopathology, particularly negative symptoms with non verbal behaviour in patients with schizophrenia spectrum illness. The association between flight and symptom levels in patients with schizophrenia in this study was moderate, yet found only in patients with schizophrenia. The combination of the main results of his study, i.e. the difference in flight between the diagnostic groups and the association between flight and symptom levels in patients with schizophrenia, suggest that flight behaviour may indeed be specifically linked with the symptoms of schizophrenia. Flight is a non-verbal behaviour that can only be identified in the context of interpersonal communication and involves movements of the whole body. Future research may investigate whether there is a link between flight behaviour and the diminished facial expression of patients with schizophrenia that has been documented in social cognition research [Schneider et al, 1990; Berenbaum and Oltmanns, 1992; Blanchard et al. 1994; Mattes et al.1995; Geabel and Wolwer, 2005]. The results can be seen as being consistent with Rümke’s notion of the praecox feeling and the assumption that disturbed communication is an important criterion for this. Our findings are also similar to those of Grube [2006], who concluded that psychopathological features including reduced communication skills are associated with the intensity of 'praecox feeling'. Identifying potential correlates of the praecox feeling in a real social interactions is of particular interest since Rümke suggested that the feeling does not reflect “a disturbance of the affect” but “something that concerns the relationship between people” (Rümke 1941, p 168, translation of the authors). The patients with depression were analysed only as a comparison group in this study. We found correlations of aspects of the patients’ non-verbal behaviour, i.e. pro-social and displacement, with symptom levels within this group [also see Troisi 2002]. Patients with higher symptom levels tended to show less smiling, nodding and looking at the interviewer, and had more hand-face-movements and fumbling. Although a previous study [Troisi et al., 1999a] did not find an association between non-verbal behaviour and severity of depression, the suggestion that more depressed patients may smile less and fumble more appear clinically plausible. Conclusion The study did not aim to validate the concept of the praecox feeling. There are more precise and reliable methods than the praecox feeling for establishing a diagnosis of schizophrenia. However, the notion of the praecox feeling guided this study because it might help develop a better understanding of the particular difficulties of patients with schizophrenia that prevent them too often from effective interpersonal communication and good functioning in the community [Priebe, 2007; Priebe et al., 2008a and b]. The praecox feeling may reflect an interactional problem that hinders patients also in other areas of life. The ‘flight’ behaviour they exhibit may function to reduce adverse stimuli in the communication with a psychiatrist and also reduce potentially adverse contacts with other people. Alternatively, flight behaviour may merely reflect a lack of expressivity [Mandal et al., 1998; Pinkham et al., 2003] and no intention to avoid further communication. Such explanation would be consistent with research showing a disjunction between the expression and experience of emotions in patients with schizophrenia [Berenbaum and Oltmans, 1992; Earnst and Kring, 1999] In any case, if flight behaviour occurs throughout the real life of the patients, it might reduce interpersonal encounters and contribute to social isolation and poor functioning. More studies in other everyday social situations are required to decide whether flight behaviour is linked to the problems patients with schizophrenia encounter in their attempts to function in the community, and whether this may lead to new therapeutic approaches. Longitudinal studies may establish to what extent flight behaviour fluctuates along with other symptoms or is relatively stable over time. Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank Mary Laville for her help in the literature search and data collection. References: 1. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J: An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1961; 4: 561–71 Bellack AS, Morrison RL, Wixted, JT, Mueser, LT: An analysis of social competence in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 156: 809-818. 1. Berenbaum H, Oltmanns TF: Emotional experience and expression in schizophrenia and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1992; 103: 474-481 2. Blanchard JJ, Kring AM, Neale JM: Flat affect in schizophrenia: a test of neuropsychological models. Schizophrenia Bulletin 1994; 20: 311-325. 3. Brune M, Sonntag C, Abdel-Hamid M, Lehmkaemper C, Juckel G, Troisi A: Nonverbal behaviour during standardized interviews in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 2008; 196: 282-288. 4. Brune M, Abdel-Hamid M, Sonntag C, Lehmkamper C, Langdon R : Linking social cognition with social interactions: Non – verbal expressivity, social competence and ‘metalising’ in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Behavioural and Brain Functions, 2009, 5:6. DOI:10.1186/17449081-5-6. 5. Earnst KS, Kring AM: Emotional responding in deficit and non-deficit schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 1999; 88; 191-207. 6. Feldman –Barrett L, Salovey P: The Wisdom in Feeling. Psychological Processes in Emotional Intelligence. New York Guilford Press 2002. 7. Gaebel W: Verhaltensananalytische Forschungsansätze in der Psychiatrie. Nervenarzt 1990; 61: 527-535. 8. Gaebel W, Wolwer W: Facial expressivity in the course of schizophrenia and depression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical neuroscience 2004; 254: 335-342. 9. Gross JJ: Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive and social consequences. Psychopathology 2002; 39: 281-291. 10. Goldsmith HH, Davidson RJ: Disambiguating the components of emotion regulation. Children Development 2004; 75:361-365. 11. Grube M: Towards an empirically based validation of intuitive diagnostic: Rümke's 'Praecox Feeling' across the schizophrenia spectrum: preliminary results. Psychopathology 2006; 39: 209-217 12. Hall JA, Irish JA, Roter DL, Ehrlich CM, Miller LH: Gender in medical encounters: an analysis of physician and patient communication in primary care setting. Health Psychol 1994; 13; 384-92. 13. Hassan I, McCabe R, Priebe S: Professional – patient communication in the treatment of mental illness: A review. Communication & Medicine 2007; 4: 141-152. 14. Jones IH, Pansa M: Some non-verbal aspects of depression and schizophrenia occurring during the interview. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 1979; 169: 402-409. 15. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LR, Lindenmayer JP: Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale [PANSS]. Schizophrenia Bulletin 1987; 13: 261-71. 16. Mandal MK, Pandey R, Prasad AB: Facial expression of emotions and schizophrenia: a review. Schizophrenia Bulletin 1998; 24: 399-412. 17. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO: Implications for neural basis of social cognition for the study of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 2003; 160: 815-824. 18. Mattes, RM, Schneider F, Heimann H, Birbaumer N: Reduced emotional response of schizophrenic patients in remission during social interaction. Schizophrenic Research 1995; 17: 249-255. 19. McCabe R, Heath C, Burns T, Priebe, S: Engagement of patients with psychosis in the medical consultation: A conversation analytic study. British Medical Journal 2002; 325: 1148-1151. 20. Priebe S: Social outcomes in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 2007; 191 (suppl. 50): S15-S20. 21. Priebe S, McCabe R: Therapeutic relationships in psychiatry: The basis of therapy of therapy in itself? International Review of Psychiatry 2008; 20: 521526. 22. Priebe, S., Fakhoury, W.K.H., Henningsen, P: Functional incapacity and physical and psychological symptoms: How they interconnect in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Psychopathology 2008a; 41: 339-345. 23. Priebe S, Watzke S, Hansson L, Burns T: Objective social outcomes index (SIX): a method to summarise objective indicators of social outcomes in mental health care. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 2008b; 118: 57-63. 24. Rice J: Mathematical Statistics and Data Analysis. Duxbury Press, 1995. 25. Rumke HC: Das Kernsyndrom der Schizophrenie und das ‘Praecox-Gefühl’. Zentralblatt für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 1941; 102: 168–169. 26. Sandhu H, Adams A, Singleton L, Clark-Cater D, Kidd J: The impact of gender dyads on doctor-patient communication: A systematic review. Patient Education and Counseling 2009; 76:348-355. 27. Schneider F, Heimann H, Himer W, Huss D, Mattes R, Adam B: Computerbased analysis of facial action in schizophrenic and depressed patients. European Archives of Pcychiatry Clinical Neurology Science 1990; 240: 6776. 28. Tremau F, Malaspina D, Duval F, Correa H, Hanger-Budny M, Coin-Bariou L et al: Facial expressiveness in patients with schizophrenia compared with depressed patients and nonpatient comparison subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry 2005; 162: 92-101. 29. Troisi A, Spalletta G, Pasini A. Non-verbal behaviour deficits in schizophrenia: an ethological study of drug-free patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1998; 97: 109-115. 30. Troisi A: Ethological research in clinical psychiatry: the study of nonverbal behaviour during interviews. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 1999; 23: 905-13. 31. Troisi A, Moles A: Gender differences in depression: An ethological study on nonverbal behaviour during interviews. Journal of Psychiatric Research 1999a; 33: 243-250. 32. Troisi A. Displacement activities as a behavioural measure of stress in nonhuman primates and human subjects. Stress. 2002; 5: 47-54. 33. Troisi A, Pompili E, Binello L, Sterpone A: Facial expressivity during the clinical interviews as a predictor of functional disability in schizophrenia. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 2007; 31: 475-481. Table 1 Characteristics of patients with schizophrenia and depression Patients with Patients with schizophrenia depression N=39 N=39 29/10 16/23 Age in years (mean ± SD) 44.1 ± 9.8 44.7 ±10.1 Duration of illness (mean ± 17.0 ±10.2 8.4 ± 6.4 Employed/unemployed 9/30 13/26 Living with partner/living alone 12/27 14/25 Characteristics Gender male/female SD) Table 2 Mean ESCI scores (± SD) of patients with schizophrenia and depression Patients with Patients with Non-verbal schizophrenia depression behaviour N=39 N=39 Pro-social 8.41 ± 3.34 9.05 ± 2.53 Statistics (MannWhitney test) U = 654.5, p = 0.287 Flight 4.78 ± 3.32 2.85 ± 3.46 U = 495.5, p = 0.008 Gesture 3.52 ± 2.53 4.32 ± 2.13 U = 615.5, p = 0.146 Assertion 1.01 ± 1.13 1.41 ± 1.73 U = 726.0, p = 0.713 Displacement 4.17 ± 2.68 4.35 ± 3.61 U = 743.0, p = 0.861 Relaxation 8.66 ± 1.77 8.17 ± 1.95 U = 684.5, p = 0.385 Table 3 Correlations of ECSI scores with PANSS scores in patients with schizophrenia and BDI scores in patients with depression Patients with schizophrenia Patients with N=39 depression N=39 PANSS PANSS PANSS Non-verbal positive negative global behaviour symptoms symptoms psychopathology Pro-social 0.063 0.219 0.053 - 0.345* Flight 0.447* 0.448* 0.591** 0.007 Gesture - 0.050 - 0.228 - 0.193 - 0.132 Assertion - 0.318 - 0.409* - 0.370 ** 0.143 Displacement 0.121 - 0.087 - 0.004 0.399* Relaxation 0.027 - 0.144 - 0.055 - 0.136 * p<0.05, **p<0.01 BDI