Organizational Paranoia 1 Social Uncertainty and the Fragility of Trust: Origins and Dynamics of Organizational Paranoia Roderick M. Kramer Graduate School of Business Stanford University Running head: Organizational Paranoia First Draft – Prepared for Research in Organizational Behavior (Editors: Barry Staw & Robert Sutton) Author Information Early versions of these ideas were presented at the Bellagio Conference on Distrust, Russell Sage Foundation Workshop on Trust in Organizations, and Tutzing Conference on Trust in Society and Organizations. Comments from participants at those conferences are gratefully acknowledged. I am greatly indebted also to Karen Cook, Diego Gambetta, Russell Hardin, James March, Joanne Martin, Jeffrey Pfeffer, Robert Sutton, Bernard Weiner, and Phil Zimbardo for their insightful comments at various stages of this work. Correspondence regarding this chapter should be addressed to the author care of: Graduate School of Business, Stanford University, Stanford CA 94305 or via e-mail at Kramer_Roderick@GSB.Stanford.edu. Organizational Paranoia 2 Abstract Distrust and suspicion are common and recurring problems within many organizations. Our understanding of the antecedents and consequences of such distrust and suspicion, however, remains far from complete. The present chapter addresses this issue by articulating an original framework for conceptualizing the origins and dynamics of a heightened and exaggerated form of distrust and suspicion termed organizational paranoia. Drawing on recent social psychological theory and research, the framework identifies social cognitive processes that contribute to the development of organizational paranoia. The framework also explicates psychological, behavioral, and organizational consequences of such paranoia. Evidence in support of the framework is reviewed. Approaches to reducing organizational paranoia are considered. Organizational Paranoia 3 Social Uncertainty and the Fragility of Trust: Origins and Dynamics of Organizational Paranoia "I just knew my enemies were plotting against me . . . I was beginning to have a vague sense of isolation. Things were happening without my knowledge. I was beginning to be excluded from meetings . . . I started to hear about deals being made that I knew nothing about, projects put into development that had never been discussed with me . . . I felt surrounded by people plotting against me." -- Hollywood Studio President, Dawn Steel (1993, pp. 222-223) "I believe in the value of paranoia." -- Intel CEO, Andrew Grove (1996). Organizational theorists have long recognized the importance of trust, noting the central role trust plays in the development of more cooperative and productive relationships among organizational members (e.g., Arrow, 1974; Barber, 1983; Zucker, 1986). Within the past few years, there has been a renewed appreciation of trust and interest in exploring more precisely how trust influences a variety of important organizational processes and outcomes (Brown, 1994; Carnevale, 1994; Lane & Bachman, 1998; Mayer Davis & Schoorman 1995, McAllister 1995, Sitkin & Roth 1993; Sitkin, Rousseau, Burt, & Camerer, 1998; Whitney, 1994). The ascension of trust as a major focus of recent organizational theory and research reflects, in no small measure, accumulating evidence of the substantial and varied benefits that accrue when high levels Organizational Paranoia 4 of trust exist within complex social systems (Fukuyama, 1995; Hollis, 1998; Putnam, 2000; Sztompka, 1999). Although recognizing the myriad benefits that flow from trust, organizational scholars have acknowledged also that trust tends to be a fragile and elusive resource (Arrow, 1974). However desirable and valuable the bonds of trust may be, they are often hard to establish and easily disrupted (Fox, 1974; Nye, Zelikow, & King, 1997; Pew, 1998; Sitkin & Roth, 1993). Although the fragility of trust has been widely noted (Gambetta, 1988; Meyerson, Weick, & Kramer, 1996; Slovic, 1993), it has been the subject of surprisingly little systematic theory or research. For example, although there exist numerous models that elaborate the dynamics of trust development (e.g., Lewicki & Bunker, 1995; Lindskold, 1978; Sztompka, 1999), models that systematically explicate the barriers or impediments to trust are virtually nonexistent (see Footnote 1). As a consequence, the conditions that contribute to the fragility of trust remain only poorly understood. Why is trust often so hard to establish and so easily disrupted, even when its benefits so individually and collectively transparent to organizational members? The primary aim of this chapter is to address this important question by advancing a framework for conceptualizing the origins and dynamics of a peculiar form of heightened and exaggerated distrust termed organizational paranoia. Such paranoia can arise at virtually every level of organization from the interpersonal to the collective. For example, employees of an organization can be paranoid about what they think their fellow co-workers are thinking or doing “behind their backs.” Subordinates can feel paranoid about the extent to which their supervisors or managers are treating them fairly or have their long-term interests at heart. Members from one group or department in an organization can be paranoid about what other groups or departments are doing. Even Organizational Paranoia 5 organizational leaders are vulnerable to the development of paranoid-like fears and preoccupations. As these examples illustrate, organizational paranoia can take many forms, depending on the nature of the relationship between paranoid perceivers and the target of their paranoia (see Footnote 2). Conceptualizing Organizational Paranoia To more formally develop the notion of organizational paranoia, and to help situate it within a wider nomological net, it may be helpful to begin with an overview of some of the more prominent images of distrust and suspicion encountered in the social science literature. Social scientists have generally conceptualized distrust as an active psychological state characterized by a specific constellation of negative expectations and beliefs regarding the lack of trustworthiness of other persons, groups, or institutions (Deutsch, 1958, 1973; Gambetta, 1988; Rotter, 1980). For example, Govier (1993) defined distrust in terms of the "absence of confidence in the other, a concern that the other may act so as to harm one, that he does not care about one’s welfare or intends to act harmfully, or is hostile. When one distrusts, one is fearful and suspicious as to what the other might do” (p. 240). As Govier's definition suggests, suspicion constitutes one of the important components of distrust. Fein (1996) has characterized such suspicion as a cognitive state in which a social perceiver “actively entertains multiple, plausibly rival, hypotheses about the motives or genuineness of a person's behavior” (p. 1165). Fein and Hilton (1994) have noted further that suspicion entails a “belief that the actor's behavior may reflect a motive that the actor wants hidden from the target of his or her behavior" (p. 169). To explain distrust and suspicion, researchers have often noted the critical role that prior experience plays in shaping individuals’ beliefs about others’ trustworthiness or Organizational Paranoia 6 lack of it. Models of trust development typically posit, for example, that individuals’ judgments about another’s trustworthiness or lack of trustworthiness can be construed largely as history-dependent processes (e.g., Deutsch, 1958; Lindskold, 1978, 1986; Pilisuk & Skolnick, 1968; Rotter, 1980). According to such conceptions, individuals’ trust in others "thickens" or "thins"--increases or decreases--as a function of their cumulative history of interaction with them. In noting the formative role that experience plays in the emergence of trust and distrust, these models imply individuals' judgments about another’s trustworthiness or lack of it are anchored, at least in part, on their a priori expectations about the other’s behavior and the extent to which subsequent experience affirms or discredits those expectations. Boyle and Bonacich’s (1970) analysis of trust development is representative of such arguments. Individuals’ expectations about trustworthy behavior, they propose, tend to change, “in the direction of experience and to a degree proportional to the difference between this experience and the initial expectations applied to it" (p. 130). In support of such arguments, numerous empirical studies have shown that interactions that reinforce individuals’ expectations about other’s trustworthiness increase trust, while interactions that violate those expectancies tend to undermine it (Lindskold, 1978; Pilisuk & Skolnick, 1968; Rotter, 1980). In addition to showing that individuals’ a priori expectations influence their judgments about a target person’s trustworthiness, other research has shown that such judgments are influenced also by the a posteriori attributions they make about another's actions (Gabarro, 1978; Kruglanski, 1970, 1987; Strickland, 1958). According to these studies, such attributions influence the inferences social perceivers make about others’ trust-related motives, intentions, and dispositions. Organizational Paranoia 7 Related research has demonstrated that the existence of uncertainty or doubt regarding others' trustworthiness induces fairly vigilant appraisal of others' actions, in an attempt to discern their presumably concealed intentions and motives. Experiments by Fein and his associates (Fein, 1996; Fein & Hilton, 1994; Hilton, Fein, & Miller, 1993) have shown, for example, that induced suspicion triggers a sophisticated analysis of others' intentions and motives, an analysis "characterized by active, careful consideration of the potential motives and causes that may influence [their] behaviors" (Fein, 1996, p. 1167). The findings from these experiments further suggest that suspicion is evoked under at least three conditions. First, suspicion is often evoked when perceivers recognize situational cues or are in possession of contextual information suggesting others might have ulterior motives for their actions. Second, it can be evoked when they have forewarnings others might be insincere or untrustworthy. Third, it can be evoked when their expectations about others have been violated. The portrait of the social perceiver that emerges from these diverse streams of research is compatible with the idea that, when trying to make judgments about others' trustworthiness, decision makers often behave much like "intuitive social auditors” or “mental accountants” who closely monitor or "track" their trust-related transactions in the hope of discriminating more accurately who among them can be trusted, how much and under what circumstances (cf., Bendor, Kramer, & Stout, 1991; Kramer, 1996). According to this intuitive auditor model, social perceivers tend to be vigilant and mindful social information processors, especially when the benefits of trust are perceived to be substantial and the costs of trust mistakes steep (Kramer, Meyerson, & Davis, 1990). The intuitive auditor model begins with two assumptions. First, it assumes that peoples' judgments regarding others' trustworthiness reflect intendedly rational and Organizational Paranoia 8 adaptive (i.e., systematic, orderly and efficacious) processes of social inference and hypothesis testing. Second, it assumes that, like "good" Bayesians, they try to update their social “mental accounts” on the basis of their ongoing experiences and accumulating evidence with others. Thus, people distrust when the evidence warrants it. Although recognizing that judgments about others' trustworthiness sometime resemble this sort of mindful and intendedly rational assessment process, a number of researchers have noted that other variants of distrust and suspicion appear to be far less rational with respect to both their origins and their judgmental consequences (Barber, 1983; Deutsch, 1973; Luhman, 1979). On the basis of such evidence, Deutsch (1973) argued it is useful to distinguish between rational and nonrational variants of distrust. The critical feature of rational distrust, he suggested, “is that it is flexible and responsive to changing circumstances” (p. 170). In contrast, nonrational forms of distrust are characterized by an “inflexible, rigid, unaltering tendency to act in a . . . suspicious manner, irrespective of the situation or the consequences of so acting" (p. 171). The pathology of such irrational distrust, Deutsch went on to argue, is reflected in "the indiscriminateness and incorrigibility of the behavioral tendency” it engenders (p. 171). Paranoid cognition constitutes a prototypic example of such irrational distrust. If social cognition is, as Kunda (1999) proposed, the study of how social perceivers “make sense" of themselves and other people, then paranoid social cognition can be viewed as a sensemaking process gone seriously awry. Colby (1981) provided one of the first, and most complete descriptions of paranoid cognition, characterizing them in terms of “persecutory delusions and false beliefs whose propositional content clusters around ideas of being harassed, threatened, harmed, subjugated, persecuted, accused, mistreated, wronged, tormented, disparaged, Organizational Paranoia 9 vilified, and so on, by malevolent others, either specific individuals or groups" (p. 518, emphases added). Subsequently, Robins and Post (1997) elaborated on this characterization, noting that the essential cognitive components of paranoia include suspicions “without sufficient basis, that others are exploiting, harming, or deceiving them, preoccupations “with unjustified doubts about the loyalty, or trustworthiness, of friends or associates,” and a reluctance “to confide in others because of unwarranted fear that the information will be used maliciously against them” (p. 3). Drawing on these general characterizations of paranoid cognition, organizational paranoia can be defined as a form of heightened and exaggerated distrust that encompasses an array of beliefs, such as organizational members’ perceptions of being harassed, threatened, harmed, subjugated, persecuted, accused, mistreated, wronged, tormented, disparaged, vilified, and so on, by malevolent others, either specific individuals or groups within their organization. These perceptions include suspicions that others are exploiting, harming, or deceiving one, along with preoccupations and doubts about their loyalty or trustworthiness. At the behavioral level, organizational paranoia is manifested in terms of a variety of intendedly self-protective behaviors. The aim of these defensive behaviors is to minimize the perceived risk of vulnerability or harm arising from misplaced trust (i.e., trusting the wrong people or trusting too much). Such behaviors include the reluctance to confide in or self-disclose to other individuals in the organization, a hesitancy to engage in spontaneous forms of cooperative behavior when interacting with other organizational members, and avoidance of informal social contact (Kramer, 1994). Paranoid cognition thus fosters both behavioral inhibition and social constriction. Tracing the Origins of Organizational Paranoia Organizational Paranoia 10 In trying to understand the origins of these rather peculiar, and in many respects, quite striking cognitions, social scientists have turned most often to the clinical psychology literature, invoking a panoply of psychodynamic constructs (Cameron, 1943; Colby, 1975). Colby (1981), for example, posited that paranoid cognition was the end product of a “causal chain of strategies for dealing with distress induced by the affect of shame-humiliation” (p. 518). The strategy of blaming others for one’s difficulties, he postulated, functions “to repudiate the belief that the self is to blame for an inadequacy” (p. 518). Such conceptions presumed, it should be emphasized, that the primary causes of paranoid cognition were located entirely “inside the head” of the social perceiver, rather than somehow connected to the social context within which such cognitions were embedded, and to which they might have reflected some sort of attempted adaptation (see Footnote 3). In contrast to these clinical conceptions, recent social psychological theory and research has advanced a very different perspective on the nature of paranoid social cognitions, and one that affords considerably more attention to their social and situational origins. Research in this vein has taken as a starting point two suggestive observations. The first observation is that, in milder forms, paranoid cognitions seem to be quite prevalent even among normal individuals. As Fenigstein and Vanable (1992) commented in this regard, it is not unusual for ordinary people “in their everday behavior [to] manifest characteristics--such as self-centered thought, suspiciousness, assumptions of ill will or hostility, and even notions of conspiratorial intent--that are reminiscent of paranoia” (p. 130). There are many social occasions, they go on to note, where people may think they are “being talked about or feel as if everything is going against them, resulting in suspicion and mistrust of others, as though they were taking advantage of Organizational Paranoia 11 them or to blame for their difficulties” (p. 133). On the basis of this evidence, Fenigstein and Vanable concluded that the concept of paranoia needs to be broadened to “include the thought processes that characterize everyday life” (p. 133). The second observation pertains to the emergent and often transient nature of these milder, more ordinary forms of paranoid cognition. As Robins and Post (1997) observed colloquially, “Who among us, in reaction to a preemptory request to come immediately to the front office, has not wondered, ‘What did I do now?’ ” (p. 3). Along similar lines, Siegel (1994) commented that “almost everyone has had a mild experience such as the vague suspicion that something is out there just waiting to get us . . . Many people experience it when they are alone in the house at night or walk down an unfamiliar street. Others may have the vague feeling that their life paths are being jeopardized by jealous persons known and unknown” (p. 7). Findings from several recent survey studies provide support for such observations, suggesting that paranoid-like perceptions, including the perception that others are out to get one or that one has been the victim of a conspiracy involving a romance, friendship, school or work are far from uncommon (Butler, Koorman, and Zimbardo, 1995; Goertzel, 1994; Pew, 1996; Pipes, 1997; Melley, 2000). These observations converge on two general propositions that are central to the conceptual argument I wish to advance in this chapter. The first proposition is that these more ordinary variants of paranoid cognition can be construed as forms of social misperception better characterized by misplaced or exaggerated--rather than necessarily false or delusional--distrust and suspicion of others. In this respect, such cognitions can be construed as not unlike other forms of judgmental bias widely documented in the social cognition literature (cf., Gilovich, 1991; Kunda, 1999). Second, as with other forms Organizational Paranoia 12 of social misperception, paranoid cognitions can be viewed as the logical end products of an interaction between the social information processing strategies social perceivers use to make sense of perplexing or threatening situations they encounter, and the features of the social contexts in which those perplexing or threatening situations arise. Stated another way, these ordinary and more benign forms of paranoid cognition can be viewed as intendedly adaptive coping responses to disturbing situations rather than necessarily manifestations of disturbed individuals (see Footnote 4). As I argue next, situations in organizational life characterized by sudden, unexpected or persisting forms of social uncertainty constitute an important class of such situations. To advance this argument, it is useful to elaborate on the relationship between social uncertainty and the disruption of presumptive trust. Social Uncertainty and the Disruption of Presumptive Trust In many situations that arise in organizational life, trust in others is implicit, having acquired over time a tacit or taken-for-granted quality (Fine & Holyfield, 1996; Garfinkel, 1963). In routine transactions with other organizational members, for example, trust is often conferred rather automatically: members both trust others with whom they interact and expect to be trusted by them in return. As Brothers (1995) aptly observed in this regard, “Trust rarely occupies the foreground of conscious awareness. We are no more likely to ask ourselves how trusting we are at a given moment than we are to inquire if gravity is still keeping the planets in orbit” (p. 3). Within organizational settings, individuals are likely to experience this sort of presumptive trust in others when they are confident or assured of their place in the social order (Garfinkel, 1963). In a perceptive analysis of the role such certainty plays in social relationships, Hogg and Mullin (1999) posited, "People have a fundamental need to feel Organizational Paranoia 13 certain about their world and their place in it" (p. 253). Subjective certainty, they go on to note, "gives one confidence about how to behave, and what to expect from the physical and social environment within which one finds oneself (p. 253). Consistent with this view, there is substantial evidence that, when social certainty is high, individuals experience what Edmondson (1999) has aptly termed "psychological safety" when relating to other members of a group or organization. This term, she emphasized, is meant to suggest "neither a careless sense of permissiveness, nor an unrelentingly positive affect but, rather, a sense of confidence that others will not embarrass, reject, or punish someone for speaking up” (p. 354). On the basis of such certainty, individuals can adopt a sort of diffuse or "depersonalized" (Brewer, 1981) trust in other organizational members--both trusting others and expecting to be trusted in return (Kramer, Brewer, & Hanna, 1996). In contrast, when individuals lack a sense of certainty regarding their relationship with other group members, then such presumptive trust may not be so easily or readily forthcoming. For such individuals, confidence in their footing in the social landscape of the organization is less secure. In the absence of psychological safety, trust acquires a more tenuous quality, and presumptive trust is replaced by presumptive distrust and wariness. A impressive body of theory and research can be adduced in support of this general proposition that people experience social uncertainty as an aversive, threatening psychological state and that they will work hard to reduce it (Ashford, 1989; Ashford & Cummings, 1985; Baumeister, 1991; Baumeister & Tice, 1990; Frank, 1985; Hogg & Mullin, 1999; Swann & Read, 1981). This proposition is supported, additionally, by equally strong evidence regarding the deleterious effects of loss of social certainty on psychological and interpersonal functioning, including the disruption of presumptive trust Organizational Paranoia 14 in other people (e.g., Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996; Baumeister & Tice, 1990; Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Kramer, 1994; Zimbardo, Andersen, & Kabat, 1981). Although indicative of a general relationship between social uncertainty and the loss of trust in social settings, the question remains, “Under what conditions are individuals likely to experience such social uncertainty in their intra-organizational relations?” In particular, does social uncertainty contribute in any way to the development of organizational paranoia? To develop answers to these questions, it is useful to briefly examine an evocative study by Zimbardo, Andersen, and Kabat (1981). When in Doubt: Perceiving and Coping with Social Uncertainty Zimbardo, Andersen, and Kabat's (1981) now classic study on the origins of social paranoia was inspired by a rather provocative, although at the time poorly understood, clinical observation. Clinicians working within institutional settings had often noted a correlation between gradual hearing loss and the onset of paranoid cogitation. For example, it was noted that elderly hospitalized patients diagnosed as paranoid often had significantly greater deafness compared to patients with other affective disorders. Moreover, paranoid perceptions appeared to be more prevalent among those hard-ofhearing individuals whose deafness had a gradual, and therefore undiagnosed, onset and course. Drawing on such observations, Zimbardo et al. theorized that when hearing loss is unexpected and gradual, patients are likely to remain relatively oblivious of their hearing loss and its impact on their social relationships. This leads to the emergence of paranoidlike thinking as the hard-of-hearing individual tries to explain the “perceptual anomaly" of not being able to hear what people around them are apparently saying (p. 1529). Judging others around them to be whispering, the individual begins to ask, “About Organizational Paranoia 15 what?” These initial responses by the hard-of-hearing person can be construed as reasonable attempts to make sense of a discomforting, but apparently quite genuine, social experience. Consistent with the view described earlier of social perceivers as “social auditor” motivated to monitor and make sense of others’ behavior, the hard-ofhearing person ponders, “Why are people suddenly whispering?” Such ruminations inevitably turn self-ward: “Are they talking about me -- and, if so, why?” Attempts to find an answer to this question are frustrated, however, by those around the hard-of-hearing person, who vehemently deny they are whispering. As Zimbardo et al. note, the persistent denial by others that they are whispering is likely to be construed by the hard of hearing person as an intentional lie since it is so clearly at odds with the evidence seemingly provided by their own senses. Thus, although intended to be reassuring, these protestations of innocence by others that nothing is amiss only spur the hard-of-hearing person to press more vigorously to find out what’s really going on. This increasingly persistent behavior of the hard-of-hearing person creates, in turn, a disconcerting sensemaking predicament for those around the hard-of-hearing person. As Zimbardo et al. reason, “Without access to the perceptual data base of the person experiencing the hearing disorder, [innocent bystanders and observers] judge these responses to be bizarre instances of thought pathology” (p. 1529). As a consequence, they actually do begin to whisper about the hard-of-hearing person, and do increasingly shun them, as the persistent suspicions and increasingly aggressive accusations regarding their alleged whispering become more and more upsetting. These reciprocal misattributions, and the behaviors they engender, lead persons experiencing hearing loss and those around them to become locked in a spiraling pattern of mutual wariness and escalating hostility, punctuated by periodic social withdrawal. As Organizational Paranoia 16 a consequence, “the individual experiences both isolation and loss of the corrective social feedback essential for modifying false beliefs (p. 1529). The system assumes a life of its own, becoming, as Zimbardo et al. (1981) aptly put it, a “self-validating, autistic system” in which “delusions of persecution go unchecked” (p. 1529). Zimbardo et al’s perceptive analysis of the evolution of social paranoia illustrates just how readily the disruption of presumptive trust within a social group can occur when situational or contextual factors weaken or tear an individual’s connection to other group members. Construed broadly, and with some interpretive license, it also raises the provocative question as to whether there might be similar dynamics at play in the development of organizational forms of such paranoia. Are there, for example, social or structural "equivalents" of the sensemaking predicament confronted by Zimbardo et al.’s hard of hearing patients found within organizational settings? If so, what might they be? Playfully engaging this question, I argue next, generates some useful insights into the social and structural antecedents of organizational paranoia. In particular, I propose that certain "locations" within organizations are especially likely to foster the sort of delibitating social uncertainty experienced by Zimbardo et al.’s "hard of hearing" patients, setting in motion a similar chain of cascading paranoid-like cogitation. Organizational Analogues of the “Hard-of-Hearing" Dilemma As Salancik and Pfeffer (1978) once noted, in order to explain human behavior, it is essential to examine the “informational and social environment within which [that] behavior occurs and to which it adapts” (p. 226). One reason context is so consequential, they argued, is that it selectively directs individuals’ attention to certain information, making that information more salient and thereby increasing its impact on their expectations about and interpretations of both their own and others’ behavior. This Organizational Paranoia 17 general social information processing argument has received substantial support from both experimental research (e.g., Fenigstein & Vanable, 1992; Fiske, 1993; Fiske, Morling, & Stevens, 1993; Taylor & Fiske, 1975; Vorauer & Ross, 1993) and field research (e.g., Ashford & Cummings, 1985; Burt & Knez, 1995). One implication of this social information processing perspective is that, when trying to assess others’ trustworthiness, our intuitive social auditor’s efforts to monitor and calibrate trustworthiness will be influenced by the particular features of the organizational contexts within which such judgments are embedded. In particular, some “locations” will facilitate such auditing, while others will sharpen constrain opportunities to sample behaviors relevant to assessing such trustworthiness. In the following sections, I review some of this evidence, using it to explicate a social information processing perspective on the origins of organizational paranoia. In particular, I review several streams of research that implicate some of the cognitive components and social antecedents of organizational paranoia. Social Uncertainty and Paranoia Within Hierarchical Relationships: Asymmetries in the View from "Atop" and "Below" Hierarchical relationships are obviously among the most important and prevalent form of intra-organizational relationship. Among the hallmarks of such relationships are significant asymmetries in power, status, access to information, and evaluative scrutiny (Fox, 1974; Kanter, 1977a). From the standpoint of individuals located on the proverbial “bottom” of such relationships (i.e., those who occupy positions of low power or status), trust in those above them is critical because of their vulnerability and dependence. Those on the bottom depend on those above them for a variety of important organizational resources. These include tangible resources, such as promotions, pay increases, attractive Organizational Paranoia 18 assignments, desirable office space, staff support, and other resources needed to get their work done. They may also depend on those above them for less tangible, but no less important psychological resources, such as positive reinforcement, mentoring, social support, and empathy. At the same time, such trust is problematic because individuals in such situations often experience considerable uncertainty about the extent to which those above them genuinely care about them and support them behind their back. In other words, it is hard to know with confidence how they are regarded and where they stand in their relationship. As a result of these uncertainties, individuals in such subordinate roles lack precisely the kinds of information that is crucial for making informed judgments about the trustworthiness of those on whom they depend. The hierarchical relationship between graduate students and their faculty advisors embodies many of these dilemmas of trust. The doctoral experience unfolds in a social context that embodies high levels of perceived evaluative scrutiny (Zanna & Darley, 1987; Taylor & Martin, 1987; Kramer & Martin, 1995). Graduate students progressing towards the doctoral degree are dependent on their faculty advisors for a variety of critical resources, including financial support, opportunities for co-authorship on scholarly papers, letters of recommendation, and approval of their dissertation research. They rely on their advisors to serve as advocates in departmental meetings in which they are evaluated, and hope they will buffer them from departmental politics that might otherwise adversely affect their graduate careers. Moreover, they depend upon their faculty members for many less tangible, although not less important, psychological resources, such as intellectual encouragement and emotional support. All of these dependencies, and the perceived vulnerabilities associated with them, are embedded in a highly evaluative interpersonal and institutional context. Organizational Paranoia 19 For all of these reasons, the relationship between graduate students and their faculty advisors provides a rich context in which to investigate the cognitive underpinnings and judgmental consequences of paranoid social cognition within a hierarchical organizational context. Accordingly, I recruited doctoral candidates and their faculty advisors for a study ostensibly concerned with social perception and interpersonal behavior in collaborative research relationships (see Kramer, 1996 for a more complete description of the methods and findings). In actuality, the primary focus of the study was perceptions of trustworthiness in these relationships. Twenty-six individuals (thirteen students and their faculty advisors) volunteered for the study. To produce the raw data for this study, autobiographical narratives were generated by asking students and their faculty advisors to recall and describe all of the significant incidents and behaviors that had occurred between them and that they felt affected the level of trust in their relationship (see Footnote 5). The twenty-six narratives produced in this fashion provided detailed developmental trust “histories” for the thirteen dyads. These histories were impressive in terms of both the variety and range of trustrelated behaviors they encompassed. To get a deeper understanding of these narratives, the data in these accounts were first analyzed in terms of the frequency of behaviors and incidents that respondents identified as having critically influenced the development of trust or distrust in their relationship. The first set of theoretical expectations I examined pertained to the hypothesized implications of heightened vigilance of those in the more vulnerable or dependent position in the relationship. Because of their greater dependence and vulnerability, I expected that trust concerns would be more consequential for students and that, consequently, they would evince greater vigilance about detecting and processing trust-relevant information. Organizational Paranoia 20 One implication of this heightened scrutiny and vigilance is that graduate students will develop more elaborate and differentiated “mental accounting” systems for tracking and coding trust-related transactions. According to this cognitive elaboration hypothesis, students would be expected to be able to recall more trust-related incidents and behaviors compared to their superordinate counterparts. The results of the study support this hypothesis. Students recalled significantly more behaviors and incidents that they construed as having impacted the level of perceived trust in their relationship compared to faculty members. This pattern of findings, it should be noted, parallel recent arguments by Fiske (1993) that people are vigilant and careful information processors when they need to be, but often rely on stereotypes and other poorly articulated schemata of others when they can get away with it. “The powerless need to try to predict and possibly alter their own fates” (p. 624). This general conclusion is supported also by imaginative and provocative work on asymmetries in victim-perpetrator accounts and memories (Baumeister, Stillwell, & Wotman, 1990; Stillwell & Baumeister, 1997). Additional analyses revealed several other patterns of interest. First, students and faculty both tended to recall more things the other person had done that had adversely impacted the level of trust between them, compared to things they had done, consistent with self-serving recall biases observed in other studies (e.g., Messick & Sentis, 1983), but the tendency was significantly greater among students. Thus, trust-building is arguably more fragile and easily disrupted for them. Based upon other research (Spranca, Minsk, & Baron, 1991), I expected that there might also be differences in the salience (availability in memory) of acts of commission versus omission, especially as a function of the perceivers’ position in the relationship. In other words, because students are more dependent and vigilant, they will “pay” more Organizational Paranoia 21 attention both to things done and not done. To evaluate this expectation, the data were coded in terms of whether the trust-relevant incidents or behaviors that students and faculty described reflected something the focal actor had actually done (an act of commission) versus something they had failed to do (an act of omission). With respect to the domain of trust-decreasing incidents, significantly more acts of omission than acts of commission were described in student narratives compared to those of their faculty. Thus, failures of action seemed to be especially salient to students. Stated differently, students "noticed" (or cognitively coded) many more things that their faculty advisors failed to do compared to their faculty counterparts. If students are motivated to pay greater attention to their faculty’s behavior than their faculty are motivated to pay to them, then it seems reasonable to argue also that acts of omission and comission might invoke especially intense ruminative activity among students. Because of their greater dependence and vulnerability, perceived violations of trust are not only likely to loom larger (be more salient), but also prompt more retrospective sensemaking activity for students. If this interpretation is correct, differential evidence of such tendencies should emerge in respondents’ estimates of how much time they spend thinking about such behaviors, incidents, and/or their relationship in general. Student and faculty estimates of the time they spent ruminating about the relationship support this conjecture. Specifically, students reported ruminating significantly more than faculty. A comparison of respondents' estimates of the amount of time they spent ruminating about their relationship and their predictions regarding their counterpart’s rumination suggested, moreover, a strong pattern of ego-centric projection (Goethals, 1986). Specifically, students’ estimates of faculty member's ruminative activity tended to Organizational Paranoia 22 be anchored on estimates of their own rumination, causing them to overestimate the amount of time their faculty spend thinking about them. Conversely, faculty estimates of students’ ruminative activity appeared to be anchored on the amount of time they report ruminating about the relationship, causing them, in effect, to underestimate the extent to which students are ruminating about the relationship. These data thus provide suggestive, even if somewhat indirect, indications of students' paranoid-like perceptions of the degree to which they are under evaluative scrutiny even when not in the presence of faculty: although they may be out of sight, students seldom think they are completely out of their advisors’ minds. This initial study serves as a documentation of some of the cognitive underpinnings of asymmetries in the processing of trust-related information tied to individuals' comparative position or location within their organizational relationships. Much like Zimbardo et al.’s hard-of-hearing patients, those on the proverbial bottom of a hierarchical relationship can easily come to think and feel that those above them are always figuratively whispering about them behind their back. But are there other circumstances in organizations that produce similar dynamics? Another factor that can contribute to the development of organizational paranoia, I argue next, is the perception of being a socially distinctive or atypical organizational member. Perceived Social Distinctiveness, Social Uncertainty, and Paranoid Cognition Perceptions of social distinctiveness reflect the relationship between individuals’ social identities and those who surround them. All human beings possess membership in multiple social identity groups. As a consequence, people can categorize themselves--and be categorized by others--in a variety of different ways. These include categorizations Organizational Paranoia 23 based upon physical attributes (such as age, race, or gender), as well as categorizations based upon social attributes such as religion, social class; and organizational identities. Recognizing their importance, researchers have afforded a great deal of attention in recent years to exploring how such social categorization processes influence social perception and behavior within social systems (e.g., Kanter, 1977a; Tsui, Egan, & O’Reilly, 1992; Turner, 1987; Wharton, 1992). Several conclusions emerge from such research. First, social categories influence how individuals define themselves in a given social situation, a process that Turner (1987) characterized as self categorization. Specifically, research on self categorization has shown that individuals often categorize themselves in terms of those attributes that happen to be distinctive or unique in a given setting (Cota & Dion, 1986; Kanter, 1977a,b; Swan & Wyer, 1997; Taylor, 1981). For example, if an individual is the only female member of a group, her distinctive gender status may be afforded disproportionate emphasis when she tries to explain her own behavior and the reactions of others to it. From a cognitive standpoint, the distinctiveness of this category makes gender-based attributes more available during social information processing. As a result, distinctive categories tend to “loom larger” during social interaction, influencing the social inferences made by both actor and observer (Taylor, Fiske, Etcoff, & Ruderman, 1978). Extrapolating from such evidence, it can be argued that individuals who belong to distinctive social categories are likely, all else equal, to be feel greater uncertainty and more self-conscious than those belonging to less socially distinctive categories. Because they feel that they are different from others or "stand out" in the group, they tend to overestimate the extent to which they are under evaluative scrutiny by other group members. This argument that self-categorization on the basis of distinctive or Organizational Paranoia 24 exceptional status can contribute to the perception of being under evaluative scrutiny and dysphoric self-consciousness is consistent with a considerable body of evidence regarding the cognitive and social consequences of merely “being different” from other members of a social group (Brewer, 1991; Kanter, 1977a,b; Tsui, Egan, & O’Reilly, 1992; Taylor, 1981). Such evidence suggests that such distinctiveness is often experienced, moreover, as an aversive state (Frable, Blackstone & Scherbaum, 1990; Lord & Saenz, 1985). As Brewer (1991) noted along these lines, “being highly individuated leaves one vulnerable to isolation and stigmatization” (p. 478).. In social information processing terms, one consequence of individuals’ uncertainty derived from simply feeling “being different” from others is that it prompts intense sensemaking activity. We might expect such sense-making activitiy to be especially pronounced when individuals are uncertain about precisely how much “being different” from others really matters (e.g., its impact on whether they will be accepted by others and the kind of treatment they will receive from them). In many respects, so-called "token" members of social systems exemplify this quandry. Token status in groups is based upon “ascribed characteristics (master statuses such as sex, race, religion, ethnic group, age, etc.) or other characteristics that carry with them a set of assumptions about culture, status, and behavior that are highly salient for majority category members” (Kanter, 1977b, p. 966). In her now classic discussions of the effects of token status on social perception and interpersonal relations, Kanter (1977a,b) noted that individuals who are members of token categories are likely to attract disproportionate attention from other groups members, particularly those who enjoy dominant status in terms of their greater numerical proportion. For example, she argued that females in many American corporations often Organizational Paranoia 25 feel as if they are in the "limelight" compared to their more numerous male counterparts. In support of these observations, Taylor (1981) demonstrated that observers often do allocate disproportionate amounts of attention to individuals who have token status in groups, especially when making attributions about group processes and outcomes. Lord and Saenz (1985) subsequently provided an important extension of this early work by showing how token status affects the cognitive processes of tokens themselves. Based on their findings, they concluded that, “Tokens feel the social pressure of imagined audience scrutiny, and may do so even when the ‘audience’ of majority group members treat them no differently from nontokens” (p. 919). On the basis of such evidence, it can be argued that individuals who have token status in a social system will be more self-conscious and perceive themselves to be under evaluative scrutiny to a greater extent than nontoken members. As a consequence, tokens will tend to overestimate the extent to which they are the targets of other’s attention. Relatedly, they will be more likely to overconstrue their interactions with other group members--especially interactions involving nontokens--in personalistic, self-referential terms. Evidence in support of this overly personalistic construal of social interaction hypothesis was provided in a study of social perception among incoming Masters of Business Administration (MBA) students at a major business school during the first term of their two-year program (Kramer, 1994). The MBA classes at most business schools are highly competitive groups. For example, individuals within these groups generally care a great deal about their relative standing or “positional status” (cf., Frank, 1985) with respect to their academic performance. At the same time, they also care a great deal about their social standing in the group. In particular, students attach considerable value to “fitting in” well and being accepted by the group (in part because they believe that they Organizational Paranoia 26 are forming life-long associations, and that their classmates represent a potentially invaluable network of several hundred “future contacts” on which they will draw throughout their professional life). From the standpoint of shaping how individuals’ construe their token or nontoken status, business schools are interesting organizations also because, rather than conceal or deemphasize social category-based information, such information is highlighted and actively disseminated. At the Stanford University Graduate School of Business, for example, detailed statistics as to the proportional representation of numerous social categories within the group are e-mailed to each incoming MBA student. The "diversity" of the group is a central topic during the formal orientation and socialization process. The group’s diversity is frequently showcased as a theme in discussions about the organizational culture. Thus, individuals are bombarded with precise information about how typical they are along a variety of categorical distinctions, including their age, gender, the prestige of the undergraduate institutions they attended, and the status of the employers for whom they worked just prior to coming to business school. Ostensibly, all of this information is provided to students in order to document and foster appreciation for the impressive level of diversity within the group, a characteristic that is highly valued by the organizational culture. Its psychological and social impact, however, may go beyond these noble institutional motives. For example, publicity about diversity also unintendedly draws attention to, and heightens the salience of, these categorical distinctions--especially for those individuals who happen to be statistically “deviant” from the population means. As a consequence, categorical outlyers may feel less certain about where they stand in the social order. They both notice how they are being treated—and, equally important, may feel especially noticed. Organizational Paranoia 27 Because of this heightened vigilance and self-consciousness, they are more likely to experience a more paranoid style of social perception and attribution compared to their nontoken counterparts. This general idea was evaluated by comparing the level of paranoid cognition among tokens and nontokens within the MBA culture. Token status was defined in terms of several dimensions: gender, race, sexual orientation, and age. Specifically, the “token” sample included black male, black female, “openly” gay students, and unusually old and young students within the first-year MBA population. The nontoken comparison group consisted of white male students. To assess the levels of trust and distrust within these two groups, data were collected on a variety of measures, including individuals’ level of perceived social certainty and uncertainty, self-consciousness, level of social discomfort, perceived status, and trust and distrust in the group. Several findings from this study are particularly noteworthy. First, with respect to their self-perceptions, token group members reported significantly higher levels of social uncertainty and self-consciousness compared to their nontoken counterparts, both in classroom discussions and in social gatherings outside of class. They also felt less comfortable raising and discussing personal issues compared to nontokens. Moreover, they perceived themselves as fitting less well into the MBA culture compared to nontokens. As a second source of data, I used an event recall procedure, similar to that used in the graduate student-faculty study described above, to elicit information regarding how tokens’ construed their trust-related interactions with other group members. These data were generated by asking MBAs to recall and briefly describe any and all experiences they could think of that had in any way adversely affected their level of trust in either a particular group member, or the group as a whole. The results are interesting in several Organizational Paranoia 28 respects. First, tokens recalled significantly more instances of behaviors they felt had adversely affected their trust in the group compared to nontokens. Inspection of these lists reveal that the kinds of behaviors tokens construed as violations of trust were often related to behaviors that appeared to call into question or invalidate their perceived standing in the group. For example, one student described an incident where another student “didn’t bother to return a phone call to me during finals week.” Similarly, another noted that other students “ignored me at a party." As a way of assessing differences in the seeming paranoia of tokens' and nontokens' construal of their social interactions, second-year MBA students, who were blind to the purpose of the study, were asked to read these recall lists and evaluate the person who made the list along a number of dimensions. Raters knew only that they were reading the accounts of new MBA students about their first-year experience in the program. They were asked to form a global impression of the person who had generated the list. They were asked to rate also, along with a variety of other measures, the general level of “paranoia” of the person, using as a guideline the definition of paranoid cognition presented earlier in this paper. Several noteworthy results emerged from this procedure. First, tokens were rated as angrier, more unhappy, and less trusting than nontokens. They were also more likely to be rated as “whiners,” “ complainers” and “social losers" (defined as “people who just naturally seem to have trouble fitting in”) compared to nontokens. Most importantly, they were viewed as significantly more paranoid. These results suggest how social observers may make fairly harsh judgments about others’ cognitions and perceptions when the social context and interactional histories that give rise to those cognitions are not readily available or weighted appropriately. Organizational Paranoia 29 In putting these results in perspective, it is important to highlight several points. First, although the data reveal a consistent pattern of difference between the construals of token and nontoken group members that is in accord with the theoretical expectation, it is necessary to emphasize that the claim here is not that tokens’ perceptions of being “over observed” or treated differently are necessarily and/or completely in error. Previous theory and research would lead one to expect that, in all likelihood, the behaviors of these tokens are scrutinized more intently and that they are treated differently than other group members. But, it is important to note, it is logically possible for individuals to both actually be disproportionately observed relative to other group members and at the same time to overestimate the extent to which they are being overobserved. In other words, the perceptions of tokens may contain a "kernel of truth" and yet still reflect a systematic cognitive bias or distortion. Similarly, it is possible for nontokens to actually pay more attention, or afford different kinds of attention, to tokens and to nonetheless systematically underestimate such overattention. In conjunction, these two biases lead to a “spreading apart” of their estimates of social scrutiny and beliefs about the social reality obtaining between them. The fragility of trust in such situations thus derives, it should be noted, not only from the token’s social uncertainty, but also the uncertainty for nontokens about how to treat the token and how to act around them. It should also be emphasized that the argument here is not that all individuals in token roles in organizations experience paranoid cognitions, or that token status per se leads to paranoid cognition. For example, individuals whose token status is predicated upon more positive social stereotypes may view the disproportionate attention they attract as a positive form of social distinctiveness in the group. Consistent with this point, one young female Japanese MBA student in the study sample felt that she received extra Organizational Paranoia 30 “social credits” for her status. Because she was Japanese, rather petite, and quite softspoken, she felt people assumed she was not only exceptionally bright, but also modest, sincere, hardworking, and a good team player. But she still felt as if she were regarded specially and treated differently because of her gender and race. Even excelling on positively valued dimensions, as Brewer (1991) noted, generates the perception of social distance and fear of potential rejection. Thus, whether one “basks” or “bakes” in the limelight of others’ perceived scrutiny clearly depends upon how they construe their distinctiveness and the kind of social attention it is perceived to invite. Social Uncertainty and the Sinister Attribution Error: Paranoia Among Organizational Newcomers Another factor that can contribute to social uncertainty and the onset of situationally-induced paranoia is an individual’s status with respect to their tenure in a social system. There are several reasons why tenure status--defined as the length of time an individual has been a member of a group or organization--might be correlated with their susceptibility to paranoid social cognitions. First, newcomers to a group are likely to have greater uncertainty and insecurity regarding their standing in the group. Because their place in the social order is still being negotiated, they are likely to be concerned with such things as how well they will fit into the group’s culture, and also whether or not they will be accepted by other group members. They may be anxious as well about how well they will perform in their expected roles. As a result of such uncertainties, newcomers are likely to be relatively self-conscious during their social interactions. They may, consequently, experience a form of intra-group anxiety akin to, and no less distressing than, the sort of inter-group anxiety described by Stephan and Stephan (1985). For them, life is not déjà vu, but as Weick (1995) aptly put it, vuje de. Organizational Paranoia 31 In contrast, as tenure increases, individuals gain considerable information regarding their standing in a group. As experienced group members, they become knowledgable about, and comfortable with, group norms and routines. In effect, such “oldtimers” know where they stand because they have successfully negotiated their place in the group or organization (cf., Fine & Holyfield, 1996; Moreland, 1985; Moreland & Levine, 1989). As a result, their uncertainty--and the social anxieties that attend such uncertainty--are likely to be relatively low. Because their social uncertainty, and the motivation to reduce it, are both high, newcomers to a group should, all else equal, be fairly vigilant and proactive when seeking diagnostic information regarding their standing (Ashford, 1986, 1989; Ashford & Cummings, 1985; Morrison, 1993). Accordingly, they will find salient, and actively process, information about how they are treated during their interactions and exchanges with other members as clues to their standing in the group. From an attributional standpoint, unfortunately, the diagnostic value of such information is often problematic because the correct construal or interpretation of many social interactions among group members is often ambiguous, and especially so for newcomers. The cause of another person’s behavior is almost always subject to multiple interpretations, making the attribution process quite difficult even for the most motivated intuitive scientist. For example, the fact that a faculty colleague in a university department fails to say hello to another faculty member as they pass each other in the hallway on a Monday morning may reflect a personalistic cause (the colleague is angry at the faculty member for something said or did at the last faculty meeting). However, there are also many nonpersonalistic causes that can be invoked to explain this same behavior Organizational Paranoia 32 (e.g., he was preoccupied by a particularly nasty faculty meeting earlier that morning and did not even notice the other person as they passed each other). From the perspective of normative attribution models, individuals should discount the validity of any particular explanation for another person's behavior when multiple, competing explanations for that behavior are available (Kelley, 1973; Morris & Larrick, 1995). Thus, even when individuals suspect they are the target or cause of another’s behavior, they should discount this personalistic attribution until more conclusive evidence is available. However, as already noted, and despite the logical inappriopriateness of doing so, people often make overly personalistic attributions of others’ actions even when competing explanations for those actions are readily available (Fenigstein, 1979; Fenigstein & Vanable, 1992; Greenwald, 1980; Vorauer & Ross, 1993). Extrapolating from this research, it might be expected that newer members of an organization will be, all else equal, more self-conscious and more likely to construe their interactions with other organizational members in overly personalistic terms, especially compared to those with longer tenure in the group. This tendency may be especially pronounced, moreover, with respect to unequal status interactions (i.e., interactions between lower and higher status group members), because such interactions will be particularly salient to newcomers, and processed more intensively by them. For example, because of their greater evaluative anxiety, to the newly hired assistant professor in a department, interactions involving senior colleagues will be more noticeable and processed more extensively than comparable interactions with other assistant professors. Thus, an ambiguous act such as passing one by in the hallway without saying hello may be attributed to simple rudeness, arrogance, or social awkwardness if done by another new assistant professor. The same behavior by a senior Organizational Paranoia 33 colleague, however, may be construed more ominously as evidence that the person doesn’t like one or did not one’s hiring in the first place. According to this logic, the tendency to make overly personalistic attributions about anothers' behavior should be particularly pronounced with respect to interactions involving newcomers and those with longer tenure. Support for this sinister attribution bias or error was provided in a recent study (Kramer, 1994) using a vignette methodology similar to that employed by Fenigstein (1984) but adapted to an organizational context. Participants in this study were all MBA students from the Stanford Business School. They were told they would be participating in a study that was concerned with how MBAs perceive their interactions with other MBAs within the school. They were asked to read a series of eight vignettes, each of which described a hypothetical interaction between the participant in the study and another MBA. In each vignette, study participants found themselves in the role of ‘target’ of a transgression which called into question their standing or which entailed a potential violation of trust. The status of the perpetrator in the scenarios was varied, so that in half the experimental conditions the offense was committed by a first-year MBA student, and in the other half it was committed by a second-year student. For example, in one vignette, students were asked to imagine the following situation, “You are having lunch with a group of first- and second- year students that you have just met. You are telling a joke that you consider quite funny. Suddenly, right in the middle of your telling the joke, one of the [first/second] year students in the group gets up and leaves.” For each vignette, two explanations for the perpetrator’s behavior were provided. One explanation suggested a personalistic (target-relevant) attribution, implying the perpetrator’s behavior was intentional and directed at the target. For example, it was Organizational Paranoia 34 suggested that the reason the student stood up and left is that he or she “thought your joke was uninteresting.” The other suggested a nonpersonalistic (target-irrelevant) attribution. For example, it was stated the reason the person got up and left is that he or she “suddenly remembered they had an appointment.” Paranoid social cognition was assessed by comparing individuals’ judgments regarding the plausibility of the personalistic versus nonpersonalistic attributions for the perpetrator’s behavior. Analysis of these data revealed a number of effects supportive of the theoretical expectations. First, there was a main effect for tenure of target, such that first year students were significantly more likely to make personalistic attributions regarding a perpetrator’s behavior compared to second year students. More importantly, a significant interaction between target and perpetrator tenure was also observed. Specifically, first year students were particularly likely to make personalistic attributions when the perpetrator was a second year student compared to when the perpetrator was another first year student. In contrast, second year students’ attributions reflected little sensitivity to the tenure of the perpetrator. In further support of the argument, correlations revealed that newcomers were more self-conscious on average than second years. Additionally, first-year students who scored high on a scale measuring dispositional public self-consciousness were more likely to make overly personalistic attributions compared to those scoring low on the scale. Technologies That Foster Organizational Paranoia: The Fragile Trust Between Auditors and Those They Audit Another factor that can contribute to the development of organizational paranoia is the use of technologies to monitor employee behavior. Organizations typically adopt such technological remedies when doubts about employee trustworthiness are present or Organizational Paranoia 35 in the hope of deterring anticipated untrustworthy behavior. Enthusiasm over such remedies to managerial trust problems has been considerable, as evidenced by the rapid infusion into the workplace of surveillance systems and f electronic monitoring of employee performance. For example, according to Aiello (1993), over 70,000 U. S. companies purchased surveillance software between 1990-1992, at a cost of more than $500 million dollars. Although reliable data regarding the overall impact and efficacy of these systems is still hard to come by, there is some evidence that such systems can foster paranoid-like reactions among employees. Ironically, they may even elicit the vary behaviors they are intended to suppress or eliminate. In a recent discussion of this evidence, Cialdini (1996) identified several reasons why monitoring and surveillance can disrupt trust within an organization. First, when people think their behavior is under the control of extrinsic motivators, their intrinsic motivation to engage in that behavior may be reduced. Thus, surveillance may actually undermine individuals’ willingness to engage in the very kinds of more trustworthy behaviors such monitoring is intended to induce or ensure. For example, some organizations have begun to use compulsory polygraphs, drug testing, and other forms of mass screening to deter such behavior. Innocent employees forced to undergo these procedures, however, may become less committed to internal standards of honesty and integrity in the workplace, even though they may have never used drugs or cheated. Moreover, honest employees may begin to assume there must be a problem with others’ trustworthiness. After all, why would a company be spending thousands of dollars on surveillance if there wasn’t a problem? Thus, although intended to detect or deter the few who might cheat, such systems unintendedly weaken beliefs about collective trustworthiness within the organization. Organizational Paranoia 36 Cialdini (1996) has noted also that monitoring and surveillance systems communicate to employees that they are not trusted, which potentially breeds mistrust and resentment in return. Thus, individuals may begin to be suspicious about those implementing such systems. What are they after? Why don’t they trust us? Does that tell us something about their view of human nature? Monitoring systems designed to improve the quality and reliability of service can also foster paranoia. For example, Hochschild (1983) described how fear of monitoring adversely impacted organizational trust and customer service among flight attendants at a major airlines. The airline implemented a policy whereby passengers were encouraged to provide feedback about the quality of flight attendant service by writing letters to the company. From the Flight Attendant's perspective, no matter how justified or unjustified the complaint, such letters would automatically be placed in their file. The resultant climate of resentment and suspicion created by this policy was further intensified because flight attendants feared that “passengers” they were serving might not even really be passengers at all. Instead, they might be airline supervisors working undercover to monitor their performance. Thus, a system intended to produce more trustworthy (reliable) and friendly service encounters unintendedly produced fearful, suspicious ones. Other recent evidence suggests that the corrosive effects of surveillance extend to those who are actually doing the surveillance, breeding a form of supervisor paranoia about those they watch. Several studies have shown, for example, that the act of surveillance increases surveillant distrust over those they monitor (Kruglanski 1970, Strickland 1958). This finding usually has been explained in terms of self-perception theory. According to self-perception theory, peoples’ attitudes and beliefs are often shaped by observations of their own behavior. Thus, if I find myself watching others for Organizational Paranoia 37 signs of untrustworthiness, I may come to believe that people in the organization obviously need to be watched (after all, why would I be investing so much time and energy in watching them if watching them wasn't necessary or prudent?). Moreover, I may come to the conclusion my surveillance is working, even when in fact it is having no effect at all. If I detect no one cheating, I might infer that my surveillance has successfully deterred the temptation to cheat. Note that the rival hypothesis that no cheating would have occurred even without monitoring is less likely to seem plausible, given the “base-rate” suspicion that monitoring is needed. Less obvious, but no less insidious in terms of their consequences, are the behaviors that surveillants are less likely to engage in when surveillance and other substitutes for trust are utilized in organizations. When managers rely on technological solutions to reduce their uncertainty about others' trustworthiness, they may stop doing other things, such as taking other more constructive measures to assess and build trust. As one executive who had implemented a computer monitoring system called Overview mused, “If I didn’t have the Overview, I would walk around and talk to people more . . . I would be more interested in what people are thinking about” (quoted in Kipnis 1996, p. 331). Thus, systems intended to guarantee trust may, ironically, prevent it from building. They not only make it more difficult for employees to demonstrate their trustworthiness, but also for managers to detect or discern that trustworthiness. Once in Doubt, Always in Doubt: The Self-Sustaining and Self-Entrapping Character of Paranoid Social Cognition All else equal, it might seem as if the various misperceptions and judgmental distortions described thusfar would be rather difficult for social perceivers to sustain. Organizational Paranoia 38 After all, as noted earlier in this paper, there is a considerable body of empirical research suggesting that, when making judgments about others’ trustworthiness, people tend to be fairly vigilant social auditors, paying close attention to others' behavior and mindfully evaluating that evidence for signs of trustworthiness or its absence. From the standpoint of such research, even if we can understand the initial circumstances that give rise to paranoid cognitions, we might expect such misperceptions and judgmental errors to be self-correcting over time. For example, as individuals acquire evidence that their fears and suspicions regarding other organizational members turn out to be exaggerated or groundless, those fears and suspicions should diminish. Perceptions, in other words, should eventually converge on reality. Yet, in contrast to this expectation, paranoid perceptions seem to enjoy what Pruitt (1987) aptly characterized as a “flinty” persistence. The persistence of such perceptual errors invites consideration of some of the dynamics that help create and sustain them. Research suggests a number of cognitive, behavioral, and social dynamics that may contribute to the resilience of paranoid cognition in organizations. These self-sustaining and self-entrapping characteristics of paranoid perceptual systems reflect two difficulties that suspicious social perceivers routinely confront. The first pertains to the paranoid perceiver’s difficulty in generating usefully evidence for making judgments about others’ trustworthiness. The second concerns difficulties such perceivers have in trying to learn from those experiences they do generate. As I will suggest, the paranoid social auditor is neither good at collecting data or drawing the right lessons from it. Difficulties in Generating Trust-relevant Experience As with other forms of social learning, learning about others’ trustworthiness requires amassing relevant evidence regarding others’ trustworthiness, and extracting Organizational Paranoia 39 reasonable inferences and generalizations from that evidence. For example, in order to learn the true prevalence or distribution of trustworthiness within a given population, individuals must sample widely and representatively from that population. Put another way, individuals must have both sufficient and representative personal experiences with social trust in order to generate veridical or faithful theories about other people’s trustworthiness. To generate such experiences, individuals must be willing to engage in appropriate forms of risky “experiments” with respect to trusting others. They must then monitor the consequences of those experiments in order to probe the limits or boundaries of trust (Hardin, 1992). Obviously, individuals are most likely to engage in such experiments in situations where they hope for or expect a positive payoff. However, such experiments in risk-taking require that individuals expose themselves to both the prospect of misplaced trust and misplaced distrust. In any words, one must be willing to be risktolerant enough to experience mistakes in both directions. Any systematic bias in the generation of experimental data samples runs the danger of leading to a skewed or truncated portrait of the distribution, pushing individuals toward inferences riddled with one of two errors (too much trust or too little). As Hardin (1992) and Gambetta (1988) have argued along these lines, differences between low and high trusters’ ex ante presumptions about others’ trustworthiness (or lack of trustworthiness) can differentially impact the frequency with which they are likely to generate these crucial learning opportunities. As Gambetta (1988) noted, distrust is very difficult to invalidate through experience, because it “prevents people from engaging in the appropriate kind of social experiment” (p. 234, emphasis added). Moreover, he argues, “it leads to behavior which bolsters the validity of distrust itself” (p. 234). As a consequence, presumptive distrust Organizational Paranoia 40 tends to become perpetual distrust. Findings from computer simulations (Bendor, Kramer, & Stout, 1991) and laboratory experiments (Fenigstein & Vanable, 1992; Kramer, 1994) support this argument. An instructive parallel to this argument can be found in research on the dynamics of the development and persistence of patterns of hostile attribution among aggressive children (Dodge, 1985). Such children, Dodge found, tend to approach social interactions “pre-offended.” They enter social transactions with a state of heightened vigilance and prepared for the worst in their social encounters. Because of this stance, they end up, ironically, eliciting through their own pre-emptive and defensive behaviors the very outcomes they most dread. In much the same fashion as these overly aggressive boys, the paranoid perceiver is perceptually vigilant to detecting signs of lack of trustworthiness in others. They are prepared for the prospect of trust abuses and prone, as a result, to code even ambiguous encounters as further evidence of tainted trustworthiness. Because the paranoid perceiver’s behavior is grounded in presumptive wariness, it ends up eliciting the very sort of uncomfortable, distant interactions that reinforce mutual or reciprocated wariness, suspicion and discomfort. Thus, a dubious set of assumptions and nagging doubts serve as a foundation for an equally questionable set of social strategies for dealing with them. Along related lines, Gilovich (1991) has elaborated another reason why people may be able to sustain beliefs in questionable beliefs and counter-productive interpersonal influence strategies. Because a given social orientation or strategy is initially thought to be effective, he notes, only that orientation or strategy is likely to be employed when similar-seeming situations are encountered. As a consequence, a person never learns what would have happened had a different interpretation of the situation given or Organizational Paranoia 41 approach taken. As a consequence, the individual cannot (or, more accurately, simply does not) assess either the true adaptiveness of the orientation or effectiveness of the strategy it seems to dictate. This pattern of dysfunctional social interaction can be aided and abetted, Gilovich goes on to note, by a self-fulfilling prophecy. Like someone who believes that the only way to get ahead is to be competitive or come on strong when dealing with other people, such a person will consistently push too aggressively for what he or she wants. The occasional success will “prove” the wisdom of the rule, and the individual never learns, as a result, that an alternative strategy might have been even more effective. As Gilovich noted further, “Because no single failure serves to disconfirm the strategy’s effectiveness (after all, nothing works all the time), the only way it can be shown to be ineffective is by discovering that the rate of success is lower with this strategy than with others” (p. xx). Given that alternative strategies are not employed and their consequences not systematically evaluated, the person never discovers the strategy they are using is suboptimal, or that the theory behind it in error. Equally important, they never come to realize that the model of self, others, and the world on which the rule is predicated is in error. As Kelley and Schmidt (1989) noted in an analysis of the dynamics of overly aggressive boys, “with their apparent belief that persistence in coercion finally works, aggressive boys are likely to develop sustained exchanges of aggression,” fully believing their experiences bears out the necessity for such belief (p. 256). In elaborating on this idea, it should be emphasized also that, because of their other-focused orientation, paranoid perceivers are also unlikely to discern how their own actions end up eliciting behavior from others that ironically sustain and justify their view that the world around them as populated by unreliable and untrustworthy others. An Organizational Paranoia 42 analogy can be drawn, in this regard, with the experience of freeway drivers who are either much faster than, or much slower than, other drivers in their environment. Consider the case first of the very fast driver. For these extremely impatient, hurried, time-urgent drivers, the daily experience on the freeway is one of persistent annoyance and frustration as countless slow, incompetent drivers are continually found to be in their way. Each press of the pedal and each lane change generates yet another interaction with yet another incompetently slow and inconsiderate driver, thwarting their progress and hindering them from reaching their destination in a timely fashion. Over time, such drivers are likely to develop a view of the world as populated by terrible drivers. On average, their experience is that the people around them are bad (overly cautious, stupid, etc.) drivers relative to their own beliefs about efficient and prudent driving speeds. Consider, on the other hand, the experience of the very slow and extremely cautious driver navigating on the same freeways. From the standpoint of these drivers everyone else on the freeway seems always to be in a hurry. Other drivers aggressively honk and gesture rudely as they pass by. The world of the slow driver is a world populated by reckless, angry, and impatient people. In both instances, however, it is the simple fact that these social actors remain “out of synch” with the rest of the world that leads to their self-generated and selfsustaining theories regarding others’ basic hostility, unreliability and lack of trustworthiness (see Footnote 6). Note also that, because of the positive illusions that social perceivers so easily sustain about themselves, they are unlikely to realize fully how their own behavior might even elicit some of the behavior from other drivers that they find so annoying. Thus, the fast reckless driver may prompt greater caution and slowing Organizational Paranoia 43 down of those around them, while the slow driver may actually cause people to speed up and go around more than might otherwise happen. Of course, social scientists trying to develop and test theories about trust and trustworthiness in the experimental laboratory tend to be mindful of such threats to the internal and external validity of their inferences about human nature. Ordinary social perceivers, however, are less likely to think spontaneously in such terms. Instead, they are caught up in a fluid, dynamic world of purposeful action. They are likely to think of their actions as moving them toward goals, rather than opportunities to test hypotheses about human nature. Thus, beliefs about others’ trustworthiness tend to develop incidentally and only after the fact. Such beliefs are residues of social experience, rather than intended products of it. Difficulties in Learning From Experience Not only do paranoid social perceivers confront significiant barriers with respect to generating representative and diagnostic samples of others’ behavior, they also face formidable difficulties in drawing appropriate inferences and lessons from the experiential samples they do manage to generate. Because of their presumption that others around them lack trustworthiness, the perceived diagnostic value of any particular social cue or bit of potentially diagnostic data is, from the outset, tainted. As Weick (1979) noted in this regard, because all social cues are potentially corruptible, it is easy to assume that they are in actuality corrupted. To illustrate this point, he cites an interesting historical example. The day before the Japanese attack on Pearl Harbor, an American naval attache had informed Washington that he did not believe a surprise attack by the Japanese was imminent. To justify his prediction, he cited the ‘fact’ that the Japanese fleet was still stationed at its home base. The clear and compelling evidence for this Organizational Paranoia 44 conclusion, he noted, was that large crowds of sailors could be observed happily and casually strolling the streets of Tokyo. Without sailors, the fleet obviously could not have sailed. If the sailors were still in port, therefore, so was the fleet. Q. E. D. What the attache did not know—and, more importantly, failed to even imagine-was that these “sailors” were in actuality Japanese soldiers disguised as sailors. They had been ordered to pose as sailors and stroll the streets to conceal the fact that the Japanese fleet had, in fact, left port and was currently steaming towards Pearl Harbor. From the perspective of the Japanese, this ploy was a brilliant example of what military strategists call "strategic disinformation". The purpose of strategic disinformation is, of course, to mislead an adversary about one’s true capabilities, intentions or actions (Kramer, Myerson, & Davis, 1990). In this instance the deception worked beautifully. In elaborating on the implications of this incident, Weick noted that the very desire and determination of the attache to find a “fool-proof” cue about Japanese intentions, made him, ironically, more vulnerable to manipulation about the nature of those intentions. Quoting a passage from Goffman (1969), Weick reasoned that, “the very fact that the observer finds himself looking to a particular bit of evidence as an incorruptible check on what is or might be corruptible, is the very reason he should be suspicious of this evidence” (p. 172). After all, Weick notes, “the best evidence for him is also the best evidence for the subject to tamper with (p. 173) From the perspective of the paranoid social perceiver, of course, the attache’s experience provides dramatic proof of what happens when individuals allow their vigilance to become too lax. Innocence regarding others’ trustworthiness can be too readily assumed, and with fatal consequences. In a world presumed to be sinister, social cues are always corrupted and always in a predictably dangerous direction. As Weick Organizational Paranoia 45 (1979) pointedly observes, “When the situation seems to be exactly what it appears to be, the closest likely alternative is that the situation has been completely faked” (p. 173). From the standpoint of the paranoid social perceive, the fleet, figuratively speaking, has always stealthily sailed and sailors are never just sailors. Ironically, even the complete absence of any cues at all can be construed as a powerful form of confirmatory evidence to the paranoid social perceiver. Dawes (1988) has provided a nice illustration of this possibility in his discussion of the debate over the necessity of internment of Japanese-Americans at the beginning of the Second World War. The question, of course, was the safety of American society given the presence of a sizable contingent of Japanese-Americans in its midst in the middle of a war. Where would their loyalties lie? Could they be trusted? When the late Supreme Court Chief Justice Earl Warren (then Governor of California) testified before a congressional hearing regarding the wisdom of internment, one of his interrogators noted that absolutely no evidence of espionage or sabotage on the part of any Japanese-Americans had been presented or was available to the committee. Thus, there was absolutely no objective evidence of danger at all. Warren’s response as to how best to intrepret this absence of evidence is revealing. “I take the view that this lack [of evidence] is the most ominous sign in our whole situation. It convinces me more than perhaps any other factor that the sabotage we are to get, the Fifth Column activities we are to get, are timed just like Pearl Harbor was timed” (p. 251). He then went on to add, “I believe we are just being lulled into a false sense of security (p. 251). When a lot is perceived to be at stake, the absence of reassuring information can be regarded as just as informative and compelling as the presence of threatening information. Organizational Paranoia 46 Recent research suggests other cognitive “toeholds” for the development of paranoid cognition in organizations. As several scholars have noted, it is easier to destroy trust than to create or sustain it (Barber 1983; Janoff-Bulman, 1992; Meyerson, Weick, & Kramer, 1996; Slovic, 1993). Slovic (1993) has argued that there are a variety of factors that contribute to difference in trust-building versus trust-eroding processes. First, negative (trust-destroying) events are more visible and noticeable than positive (trustbuilding) events. Second, trust-destroying events carry more weight in judgment than trust-building events of comparable magnitude. As evidence for his asymmetry principle, Slovic evaluated the impact of hypothetical news events on people’s trust judgments and found negative events had more impact on trust judgments than positive events. Slovic noted further that asymmetries between trust and distrust may be reinforced by the fact that sources of bad (trust-destroying) news tend to be perceived as more credible than sources of good news. Other related evidence suggest that violations of trust tend to "loom larger" than confirmations of trust. For example, studies of individuals’ reactions to trust betrayals suggests that violations of trust are highly salient to victims, prompting intense ruminative activity, and evoking greater attributional search for the causes of the violation (Bies, Tripp, & Kramer, 1997; Brothers, 1995; Janoff-Bulman, 1992). To the extent that violations of trust are coded as losses, they should loom larger than "mere" confirmations of trust of comparable magnitude. For example, failure to keep a promise should have more impact on judgments about trustworthiness than "merely" keeping a promise. This general argument is supported as well by evidence that cognitive responses to positive and negative events are often highly asymmetrical (Peeters & Czapinski, 1990). In aggregate, such asymmetries imply that information supportive of a stance of distrust Organizational Paranoia 47 should be weighted and evaluated differently than information of the same magnitude that is supportive of trust. For the paranoid social perceiver, such asymmetries are likely to be even more salient and pronounced because of the perceptual readiness to detect bad (trust-destroying) versus good (trust-affirming) evidence. There are also a number of social dynamics that can contribute to the emergence and maintenance of organizational paranoia. The first concerns the role of gossip in the diffusion of paranoia. Recent research indicates that information that provides grounds for distrust may diffuse through a social system more rapidly than information pertaining to trust. For example, Burt and Knez (1995) showed how network structures, and the social dynamics they create, differentially affect rates of diffusion of trust-relevant versus distrust-related information. Using a sample of managers in a high tech firm, they investigated the influence of third-parties on the diffusion of trust and distrust within managers’ networks. Burt and Knez argued that third parties are more attentive to negative information, and often prefer negative gossip to positive information and gossip. Consistent with their general theoretical expectation, they found that, although both trust and distrust were amplified by third-party disclosures, distrust was amplified to a greater extent than trust. As a result, judgments about distrust had, as Burt and Knez put it, a “catastrophic” quality to them. A Model of Organizational Paranoia: How the Intuitive Social Auditor Goes Astray In the preceding pages, I have attempted to lay a foundation for the general argument that social uncertainty plays a central role in the development of organizational paranoia. To advance this argument, I have invoked a number of theoretical constructs and empirical findings from a number of recent and disparate streams of social cognitive theory and research. It may be useful, accordingly, to attempt to pull the various strands Organizational Paranoia 48 of theory and data more tightly together. Accordingly, Figure 1 is offered as a schematic representation of a social information processing or “auditing” model of organizational paranoia. [Insert Figure 1 about here] As noted earlier, a prominent assumption of such models is that a complete understanding of organizational paranoia--or any form of social cognition, for that matter-requires recognition of the influence that the context in which cognition is embedded exerts on social judgment and inference. From the perspective of such a framework, what the intuitive social auditor sees and infers with respect to trust depends quite literally on where he or she stands in the organization. As the figure indicates, social uncertainty is presumed to play a central role in initiating a cycle of social appraisal and coping that fosters paranoid-like perception and behavior. Specifically, social uncertainty induces several psychological states, including heightened self-consciousness, heightened perceptions of evaluative scrutiny, uncertainty about one’s status in the group, and reduced perceptions of psychological safety. Because social uncertainty and the psychological states it engenders are aversive states, individuals are motivated to make sense of, and attempt to reduce or eliminate, this uncertainty. The more discomforting and aversive the uncertainty, the greater the motivation to do so and the more earnest the effort at uncertainty reduction. The model assumes that these initial attempts at sensemaking and uncertainty reduction reflect intendedly adaptive appraisal and coping responses. In other words, an assumption of the model is that individuals experiencing social uncertainty engage in what they construe as reasonably vigilant appraisal and mindful action. If these initial attempts at uncertainty reduction are successful, feelings of psychological safety are Organizational Paranoia 49 enhanced, self-consciousness diminished, and the perception of being under evaluative scrutiny reduced. Thus, trust can begin to build and the individual's attention can be claimed by other emergent concerns. However, when individuals' efforts at uncertainty management are unsuccessful, social vigilance and appraisal are characterized by a hypervigilant mode of social auditing and a tendency to engage in dysphoric rumination about one’s perceived problems. As suggested by the figure, hypervigilance and rumination are posited to enjoy a circular causal relationship. Specifically, hypervigilant appraisal of social information tends to generate additional "raw data" about which the paranoid social perceiver is likely to ruminate. Such rumination helps generate, in turn, additional and ever more sinister "hypotheses" that push the already hypervigilant social auditor towards ever closer scrutiny of others' actions. Hypervigilance and dysphoric rumination contribute to several distinct models of paranoid-like social misjudgment and misperception. Specifically, they foster the cognitive elaboration of individuals' trust-related "mental accounts," the overly personalistic construal of social interaction and sinister attribution bias. These cognitive distortions and biases result in heightened concerns about others’ trustworthiness, fostering self-protective or defensive behaviors. These behaviors include behavioral inhibition (e.g., reluctant to engage in personal self-disclosure) and social withdrawal. Unfortunately, these behaviors tend to elicit responses from others that exascerbate the paranoid perceiver’s difficulties. Specifically, they foster a tendency for those around the individual to attribute paranoia to them and a reluctance to interact with the individual. These responses contribute to further social uncertainty on the part of the paranoid perceiver. Thus, the cycle has a self-entrapping or autistic character to it. Organizational Paranoia 50 The model does not, it should be emphasized, provide a complete account of all of the causal factors that influence the development of paranoid cognition within organizations. In this respect, the model is offered as a “middle range” account that serves to organize and integrate a fair amount of what is currently known about the cognitive underpinnings of organizational paranoia and their origins (see Footnote 7). The model is offered as a heuristic device for stimulating further research, rather than intended as a final statement. Reducing Organizational Paranoia: Rendering Trust Less Elusive and Less Fragile The model of organizational paranoia developed in this chapter and the evidence presented in support of it suggests how readily organizational paranoia can gain a toehold and flourish within organizational situations where high levels of social uncertainty exist. The analysis identifies a variety of factors that contribute to the emergence of organizational paranoia, and that help explain the resilience of such paranoia even in situations where only the flimsiest and most ambiguous evidence exists to support it. The model also makes clear some of the deleterious consequences associated with such cognition. In light of the persistence of organizational paranoia and the perverse consequences it imposes, it is important to consider the question of how the problem of organizational paranoia might be addressed. Can organizational paranoia be reduced, and if so how? The general problem of reducing or eliminating organizational paranoia can be approached from a number of vantage points, including thinking about organization-level interventions and remedies that are designed to lessen the climate of collective distrust and suspicion among organizational members. The problem can be viewed, at least in part, as one of uncertainty reduction. Concretely, such uncertainty-reducing measures Organizational Paranoia 51 include providing sufficient reassurance to individuals that the doubts and concerns they possess are, in actuality, exaggerated or ungrounded. One way such reassurance can be provided is through means of structural solutions that promote more information flows and/or contact between individuals or groups that distrust each other. If paranoia within organizations is partly about individuals’ uncertainty regarding the trustworthiness of other organizational members, then both explicit and tacit understandings regarding transaction norms, obligations, and exchange practices provide an important basis for addressing such concerns. Particularly useful is information indicating that others in the organization are likely to behave in a trustworthy fashion, even in the absence of individuating knowledge about their trustworthiness. Such generalized knowledge can be viewed as a source of depersonalized trust described earlier (Brewer, 1981). Organizational rules, both formal and informal, capture much of the depersonalized knowledge and generalized expectations that organizational members possess about each other and the organization as a whole (March, 1995). Rule-based trust is thus predicated on members' shared understandings regarding the system of rules regarding appropriate (allowed) and inappropriate (disallowed) behavior. To be most effective, adherence should be perceived as voluntary and readily forthcoming because of internalized values, commitments, and a sense of obligation (Fine & Holyfield, 1996). As March and Olsen (1989) put it, rule-based trust is sustained within an organization “not [by] an explicit contract . . . [but] by socialization into the structure of rules" (p. 27). When reciprocal confidence in members' socialization into and continued adherence to a normative system is high, mutual trust can acquire a taken-for-granted quality. Trust becomes tacit. Organizational Paranoia 52 Fine and Holyfield (1996) provide a nice illustration of how such explicit rules and tacit understandings function to create and sustain high levels of mutual trust within an organization. Their study examined the bases of trust in the Minnesota Mycological Society, an organization that consists of amateur mushroom afficionados. This organization provides a rich setting in which to study the bases of presumptive trust for several reasons. First, the costs of misplaced trust in the organization can be quite severe: eating a mushroom that someone else has mistakenly declared safe for consumption can lead to serious illness and even, in rare instances, death. Given such risks, Fine and Holyfield note, credibility is lost only once unless a mistake is reasonable. Consequently, members are likely to be fairly vigilant about assessing and maintaining high levels of mutual trust and trustworthiness. Second, because membership in the organization is voluntary, exit is comparatively costless. If doubts about others’ trustworthiness become too great, therefore, members will take their trust elsewhere and the organization will die. Thus, the organization’s survival depends upon its ability to successfully instill and sustain perceptions of mutual trustworthiness among its members. Fine and Holyfield identified three important bases of trust within this organization, which they termed awarding trust, managing risk, and transforming trust. One way trust is created, they observed, is to award trust to others even when confidence in them may be lacking. For example, considerable social pressure is exerted on novices to consume dishes at banquets prepared by other members. As Fine and Holyfield put it, there is an insistence on trust. Thus, even if members remain privately anxious, their public behavior connotes high levels of trust. Collectively, these displays constitute a potent form of social proof to members that their individual acts of trust are sensible. Organizational Paranoia 53 An insistence on trust is adaptive, of course, only if collective trustworthiness is actually in place within the organization. The difference between blind, foolish trust and mindful and constructive commitment to an organizational order is obvious. Accordingly, a second crucial element in the creation of trust within this organization occurs through practices and arrangements that ensure individual competence and due diligence with respect to all of the routines and activities that the collective life of the group depends on. This result is achieved partially through meticulous socialization of newcomers to the organization. Novices participate in these socialization processes with appropriate levels of commitment because it helps them manage the personal risks of mushroom eating and also because it helps them secure a place in the social order of the group. In turn, more seasoned organizational members teach novices out of a sense of obligation, having themselves benefitted from instruction from those who came before them. This repaying of their own instruction constitutes an interesting temporal (transgenerational) variant of depersonalized trust. This requirement that oldtimers socialize newcomers has a second and less obvious benefit: it continually forces them to articulate the rules, reminding them as well which attitudes and behaviors are critical to the maintenance of collective trust. Thus, this form of trust is not simply trust in the expertise and motivation of specific individuals, but more importantly, trust in a social system characterized by collective expertise and high motivation. Over time, Fine and Holyfield argue, as members acquire knowledge about the organization, the nature of trust itself becomes transformed. Early on, the organization is simply a “validator” of trust for new members. Eventually, however, it becomes an “arena in which trusting relations are enacted and organizational interaction serves as its own reward” (p. 29). Organizational Paranoia 54 Sometimes organizations and their members are even willing to accept suboptimal rules as solutions to their trust dilemmas, rather than live with the fear or even mere suspicion they are being exploited or taken advantage of by others in organization. Gambetta (1993) provided a very nice illustration of such a structural solution that emerged among Palermo taxi drivers, where a climate of collective paranoia had resulted in the collapse of cooperation among them. A radio-dispatcher service was initially instituted in Palermo to help drivers locate prospective fares. Each driver contributes to provide the central dispatcher, so the dispatcher is, in this sense, a public good. The dispatcher, on receiving a call, was supposed to give the call to the driver who happened to be nearest the caller. Unfortunately, knowing this decision rule, drivers had incentives to misrepresent (overstate) their proximity to the caller, because there was no obvious way for the radio dispatcher to verify whether cabbies were telling the truth or not regarding their location. As suspicions spread that others were cheating the system by strategically misrepresenting their location, individual drivers began opting out of the system, feeling why pay for a public good that’s gone bad. Individual drivers maintained that “a lot of cheating” was going on, even though objective evidence of such cheating was hard to come by (p. 221). As Gambetta noted, “If a driver were lucky enough to land a few extra runs in a day, the other drivers would almost automatically leap to the conclusion that he was cheating” (p. 224). Moreover, once this judgment had crystallized, there was little that could be done to undo it. Drivers had ample time to privately ruminate in their cabs about such “cheating.” Additionally, they could reinforce such conclusions through collective or social rumination, by 'comparing notes' when they took their breaks together. “In Sicily,” Gambetta wryly observed, “there is nothing as Organizational Paranoia 55 suspicious as luck” (p. 224). By 1987, despite a population of over a million inhabitants, this system of radio-dispatched taxi-service in Palermo had almost collapsed entirely. The solution to this trust dilemma—and one that emerged only after ten years of chronic and bitter failure, was to create a new allocation system where all cabs would go to a central area, que up, and take incoming calls in order. The system readily allowed visible verification of location. Thus, all drivers could remain assured that the system was working fairly. Ironically, this solution did mean that passengers had to wait longer and pay more on average for taxi service because the meter starts running once a call is taken by the radio dispatcher and, additionally, passengers have to wait for the cab to traverse from the central location to their destination, even if other cabbies happen to be much closer to their calling point. As Gambetta wrly observed, however, even clearly inferior service dominates no service at all for people whose mobility is dependent on cab transportation. In the case of interventions intended to create positive collective identities and expectations of mutual trustworthiness, these prompts make salient and reinforce the perception that organizational members, as a group, are on the whole trustworthy. Miller (1992) provides an excellent example of this kind of socially constructed and ultimately self-reinforcing dynamic. In discussing the underpinnings of cooperation at HewlettPackard, he noted that, "The reality of cooperation is suggested by the open lab stock policy, which not only allows engineers access to all equipment, but encourages them to take it home for personal use" (p. 197). From a strictly economic perspective, this policy simply reduces monitoring and transaction costs. However, from the standpoint of a rulebased set of expectations about trust-related interactions, its consequences are more subtle and far-reaching. As Miller (1992) observed, "the open door symbolizes and Organizational Paranoia 56 demonstrates management's trust in the cooperativeness of the employees” (p. 197). Because such acts are so manifestly predicated on trust in others, they tend to breed trust in turn. Another way in which rules foster trust is through their influence on individuals’ self-perceptions and their expectations about other organizational members. As March (1995) observed in this regard, organizations function much like "stage managers" by providing "prompts that evoke particular identities in particular situations” (p. 72). Rulebased practices of this sort can exert subtle influences, not only on individuals’ expectations and beliefs about the trustworthiness and honesty of other organizational members, but also their self-perceptions of their own honesty and trustworthiness as well. As Miller notes in this regard, by eliminating time clocks and locks on equipment room doors at Hewlett-Packard, the organization built a “shared expectation among all the players that cooperation will most likely be reciprocated" creating "a shared 'common knowledge' in the ability of the players to reach cooperative outcomes" (p. 197). By institutionalizing trust through practices at the macro-organizational level, trust becomes internalized at the micro level. Thus, rule-based trust becomes a potent form of expectational asset (Knez & Camerer, 1994) that facilitates spontaneous coordination and cooperation among organizational members. Putting Paranoia in Perspective: Toward a Positive Theory of Doubt Up to this point, the analysis has emphasized almost exclusively the dysfunctional or deleterious consequences of organizational paranoia. The paranoid perceiver has been portrayed largely as a social misperceiver who exaggerates others' malevolence and indifference, and who tends to overestimate their lack of trustworthiness. In certain respects, this emphasis is appropriate. After all, to the extent that paranoid cognitions Organizational Paranoia 57 represent systematic patterns of misperception in organizations, they obviously constitute a maladaptive mode of social information processing and should be analyzed much like other forms of social misperception found in social psychology (Kunda, 1999). However, I would argue that the prevalence and ubitiquousness of such paranoia invites contemplation of the possible functional or adaptive roles such cognitions might play in organizational life. There are several observations that prompt consideration of such roles. First, distrust and suspicion are not always irrational. In highly competitive or political organizational environments, for example, an individual may have quite legitimate cause for suspicion and concern about others’ trustworthiness. In such environments, the costs of misplaced trust can be quite costly—and sometimes even fatal-to one’s career. Thus, in ecologies populated by individuals who lack trustworthiness, a propensity towards vigilance with respect to others’ lack of trustworthiness may be quite prudent and adaptive (Bendor, Kramer, & Stout, 1991). Even though the fears and suspicions of paranoid individuals may seem exaggerated or inappropriate to others who are in more advantage positions, this does not mean that their distrust is entirely misplaced or unwarranted. As Intel President and CEO Andrew Grove (1996) likes to reminds his employees, “Only the paranoid survive” (p. 3). In expounding on what he means by such adaptive paranoia, Grove notes, “I believe in the value of paranoia. Business success contains the seeds of its own destruction. The more successful you are, the more people want a chunk of your business and then another chunk and then another until there is nothing left. I believe that the prime responsibility of a manager is to guard against other people’s attacks and to inculcate this guardian attitude in the people under his or her management” (p. 3) Organizational Paranoia 58 A better appreciation of the adaptive functions of even seemingly irrational forms of distrust and suspicion might help us, moreover, “back into” a richer and more robust theory of organizational trust. By understanding more completely the conditions that give rise to such irrational forms of distrust and suspicion, and that render trust more fragile, we may develop a better understanding of the requirements for building more resilient systems of collective or reciprocal trust. Along these lines, the theory of organizational paranoia developed in this chapter helps direct attention to the rather mundane or banal “social information processing” origins of wariness of others and the reluctance to give them the benefit of the doubt. In commenting on the fragile and elusive character of trust in organizations, March and Olsen (19xx) onced observed, “The greatest threat to innocence is the awareness that not everyone else is innocent” (p. xx). As the results of the research reviewed in this paper suggest, even the mere suspicion that others’ lack innocence may be sufficient to tilt individuals preciptiously toward presumptive suspicion and wariness. Organizational Paranoia 59 Footnotes 1. This comparative lack of attention to distrust as a distinct topic may reflect, at least in part, the assumption that trust and distrust are merely opposite poles of a single continuum and are symmetric (see, e.g., Gambetta, 1988 and Stzompka, 1999). This assumption has been questioned on a variety of theoretical and empirical grounds (see, e.g., Slovic, 1993). 2. More extensive treatments of these different forms of organizational paranoia can be found elsewhere. Interpersonal paranoia is discussed in Butler, Koopman, & Zimbardo, 1995. Discussions of intergroup paranoia can be found in Kramer & Messick, 1997 and Pruitt, 1987. Analyses of leader paranoia can be found in Kramer (2000) and Robins and Post (1997). 3. The emphasis placed upon intrapsychic dynamics in these early theories is not altogether surprising, given that these theories were based largely upon clinical observations made in institutional settings in which the ‘disturbed’ individual had been removed from the social contexts for which it may have represented some sort of psychological adaptation. 4. This is not to argue that clinical forms of paranoia are not indicative of any acute intrapsychic disturbance. Rather, it is to argue that extant clinical theories may underappreciate the extent to which ordinary or mundane social and situational factors contribute to the emergence and maintenance of such paranoia. 5. As recent studies have shown, autobiographical narratives of this sort are rich sources of naturalistic data and can provide important insights regarding how individuals construe different facets of their past experiences, as well as the significant lessons they extract from those experiences (see, e.g., Baumeister & Newman, 1994 and Baumeister, Stillwell, & Wotman, 1990). 6. Space does not permit a detailed explication of the behavioral manifestations and consequences of paranoid cognition. However, a general taxonomy of forms of selfdefeating behavior can be found in Baumeister and Scher (1988), and their linkages to paranoid behavior found in Kramer (1995). 7. Experimental evidence for many of the specific theorized linkages in the model can be found elsewhere, including Buss and Scheier (1976). A review of this social cognitive evidence can be found in Kramer (1998). 8. This view is consonant with recent perspectives on the constructive consequences of mindful deliberation and mental self-control (Langer, 19; Taylor, Pham, Rivkin, & Armor, 1998; Wilson & Kraft, 1993; Wyer, 1996). In such environments, it may be far better to be safe than sorry. Organizational Paranoia 60 9. Unfortunately, freeway driving also provides all too much time for the sort of hypervigilant scrutiny of other people, and dysphoric rumination about their lack of competence. For a variety of well-understood reasons, such individuals are likely to remain relatively oblivious to the possibility that their own reckless lane changes and frantic actions elicit more careful, defensive behavior from other drivers around them. Both the too fast driver and the too slow driver. The common positive illusion that one is a better than average driver insulates one further from any suspicion that it is one’s own location in a system of moving objects that generates the experience. Organizational Paranoia 61 References Aiello 1993 Computer-based monitoring: Electronic surveillance and its effects. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 499-507. Ashford, S. J., (1986). Feedback-seeking in individual adaptation: A resource perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 465-487. Ashford, S. J. (1989). Self-assessments in organizations: A literature review and integrative model. In L. L. Cummings and B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 11), pp. 133-174. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Ashford, S. J., & Cummings, L. L. (1985). Proactive feedback seeking: The instrumental use of the information environment. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 58, 6779. Barber, B. (1983). The logic and limits of trust. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Escaping the self: Alcoholism, spirtuality, masochism, and other flights from the burden of selfhood. New York: Basic Books. Baumeister, R. F., & Newman, L. S. (1994). How stories make sense of personal experiences: Motives that shape autobiographical narratives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 676-690. Baumeister, R. F., & Scher, S. J. (1988). Self-defeating behavior patterns among normal individuals: Review and analysis of common self-destructive tendencies. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 3-22. Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 5-33. Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A., & Wotman, S. R. (1990). Victim and perpetrator accounts of interpersonal conflict: Autobiographical narratives about conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 994-1005. Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1990). Anxiety and social exclusion. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 165-195. Bendor, J. Kramer, R. M., & Stout, S. (1991). When in doubt: Cooperation in an uncertain world. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 35, 691-719. Bies, R. J., Tripp, T. M., & Kramer, R. M. (1996). At the breaking point: Cognitive and social dynamics of revenge in organizations. In J. Greenberg & R. Giacalone (Eds.), Antisocial behavior in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Boyle, R., & Bonacich, P. (1970). The development of trust and mistrust in mixedmotives games. Sociometry, 33, 123-139. Brewer, M. B. (1981). Ethnocentrism and its role in interpersonal trust. In M. B. Brewer & B. E. Collins (Eds.), Scientific inquiry in the social sciences. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self-: On being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 475-482.. Brothers, D. (1995). Falling backwards: An exploration of trust and self-experience. New York: Norton. Brown, P. G. (1994). Restoring the public trust. Boston: Beacon. Burt, R. S., & Knez, M. (1995). Kinds of third-party effects on trust. Rationality and Society, 7, 255-292. Organizational Paranoia 62 Buss, D. M., & Scheier, M. F. (1976). Self-consciousness, self-awareness, and selfattribution. Journal of Research in Personality, 1976, 10, 463-468. Butler, J. (1991). Toward understanding and measuring conditions of trust: Evolution of a conditions of trust inventory. Journal of Management, 17, 643-663. Butler, L. D., Koopman, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1995). The psychological impact of viewing the film JFK: Emotions, beliefs, and political behavioral intentions. Political Psychology, 16, 237-257. Cameron, N. (1943). The development of paranoiac thinking. Psychological Review, 50, 219-233. Carnevale, D. G. (1994). Trustworthy government. San Franscisco: Jossey-Bass. Cialdini, R. (1996). The triple tumor structure of organizational behavior. In D. M. Messick & A. E. Tenbrunsel (Eds.), Codes of conduct. New York: Russell Sage. Colby, K. M. (1975). Artificial paranoia: A computer simulation of paranoid processes. New York: Pergamon Press. Colby, K. M. (1981). Modeling a paranoid mind. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515-560. Cota, A. A., & Dion, K. L. (1986). Salience of gender and sex composition of ad hoc groups: An experimental test of distinctiveness theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 770-776. Dawes, R. (1988). Rational choice in an uncertain world. New York: Harcourt Brace. Deutsch, M. (1958). Trust and suspicion. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2, 265-279. Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Deutsch, M. (1986). The malignant (spiral) process of hostile interaction. In R. K. White (Ed.), Psychology and the prevention of nuclear war. New York: New York University Press. Dodge, K. A. (1985). Attributional bias in aggressive children. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Acvances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy (Vol. 4). New York: Academic Press. Edmondson, A. (1998). Psychological safety and learning behavior in work teams. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, 350-383. Fein, S. (1996). Effects of suspicion on attributional thinking and the correspondence bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1164-1184. Fein, S., & Hilton, J. L. (1994). Judging others in the shadow of suspicion. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 167-198. Fenigstein, A. (1979). Self-consciousness, self-attention, and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 75-86. Fenigstein, A. (1984). Self-consciousness and self as target. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 860-870. Fenigstein, A., & Vanable, P. A. (1992). Paranoia and self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 129-138. Fine, G., & Holyfield, L. (1996). Secrecy, trust, and dangerous leisure: Generating group cohesion in voluntary organizations. Social Psychology Quarterly, 59, 2238. Fiske, S. T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotypes. American Psychologist. 48, 621-628. Organizational Paranoia 63 Fiske, S. T., Morling, B., & Stevens, L. E. (1996). Controlling self and others: A theory of anxiety, mental control, and social control. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 115-123. Fox, A. (1974). Beyond contract: Power and trust relations. London: Faber & Faber. Frable, D., Blackstone, T., & Scherbaum, C. (1990). Marginal and mindful: Deviants in social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 660-670. Frank, R. (1985) Choosing the right pond: The quest for status. New York: Oxford University Press. Fukuyama, F. (1995). Trust: The social virtues and the creation of prosperity. New York: Free Press. Gabarro, J. J. (1978). The development of trust and expectations. In A. G. Athos & J. J. Gabarro (Eds.), Interpersonal behavior: Communication and understanding in relationships, 290-303. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Gambetta D. 1988. Can we trust trust? In D. Gambetta (Ed.), Trust: Making and breaking cooperative relationships. Cambridge: Basil Blackwell. Gambetta, D. (1993). The Sicilian Mafia: The business of private protection. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Garfinkel, H. (1963). A conception of, and experiments with, ‘trust’ as a condition of stable concerted actions. In O. J. Harvey (Ed.), Motivation and social interaction: Cognitive determinants. New York: Ronald Press. Garske, J. P. (1976). Personality and generalized expectancies for interpersonal trust. Psychological Report, 39, 649-650. Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn’t so: The fallibility of human reason in everyday life. New York: Free Press. Goethals, G. R. (1986). Fabrication and ignoring social reality: Self-serving estimates of consensus. In J. Olsen, C. P. Herman, and M. Zanna (Eds.), Relative deprivation in social comparison: The Ontario Symposium (Volume 4). Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Goertzel, G. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731-742. Goffman, E. (1969). Strategic interaction. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvannia Press. Govier, T. (1993). An epistemology of trust. International Journal of Moral and Social Studies, 8, 155-174. Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603-618. Grove, A. (1996). Only the paranoid survive: How to survive the crisis points that challenge every career. New York: Doubleday. Hardin R. (1992). The street-level epistemology of trust. Analyse & Kritik, 14, 152-176. Harrison, A. A., & Thomas, J. M. (1997). The Kennedy assassination, unidentified flying objects, and other conspiracies: Psychological and organizational factors in the perception of ‘cover-up.’ Systems Research in the Behavioral Sciences, 14, 113-128. Hilton, J. L., Fein, S., & Miller, D. T. (1993). Suspicion and dispositional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 501-512. Hochschild , A. R. (1983). The Managed Heart. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Organizational Paranoia 64 Hogg, M. A. (2000a). Self-categorization and subjective uncertainty resolutions: Cognitive and motivational facets of social identity and group membership. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, & L. Wheeler (Eds.), The social mind: Cognitive and motivational aspects of interpersonal behavior. (pp. 323-349). New York: Cambridge University Press. Hogg, M. A. (2000b). Subjective uncertainty reduction through self-categorization: A motivational theory of social identity processes. European Journal of Social Psychology, 11, xx. Hogg, M. A., & Mullin. 1999 xx. Hollis, M. (1998). Trust within reason. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Janis, I.L. (1989). Crucial Decisions. New York.: Free Press. Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychology of trauma. New York: Free Press. Kanter, R. (1977a). Men and women of the corporation. New York: Basic Books. Kanter, R. (1977b). Some effects of proportions on group life: Skewed sex rations and responses to token women. American Journal of Sociology, 82, 965-990. Kelley, H. H. (1973). Causal schemata and the attribution process. American Psychologist, 28, 107-123. Kelley, H. H., & Schmidt (1989). The "aggressive male" syndrome: Its possible relevance for international conflict. In P. Stern, R. Axelrod, R. Jervis, & R. Radner (Eds.), Perspectives on deterrence, pp. 251-286. New York: Oxford University Press. Kipnis, D. (1996). Trust and technology. In R. M. Kramer & T. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations. Thousand Oaks, Ca: Sage. Knex, M., & Camerer, C. (1994). Creating expectational assets in the laboratory. Strategic Management Journal, 15, 101-119. Kramer, R. M. (1994). The sinister attribution error: Origins and consequences of collective paranoia. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 199-230. Kramer, R. M. (1995). Power, paranoia, and distrust in organizations: The distorted view from the top. Research on negotiation in organizations, 5, 119-154. Kramer, R. M. (1996). Divergent realities, convergent disappointments in the hierarchic relation: Trust and the intuitive auditor at work. In R. M. Kramer and T. R. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kramer, R. M. (1999). Trust and distrust in organizations: Emerging questions, enduring perspectives. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 569-598. Kramer, R. M. (2000). Political paranoia: Antecedents and consequences. In S. Bacharach & S. Lawler (Eds), Research in the Sociology of Organizations, xx. Kramer, R. M., Brewer, M. B., & Hanna, B. (1996). The decision to trust as a social decision. In R. M. Kramer & T. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations. Thousand Oaks, Ca: Sage. Kramer, R. M., & Martin, J. (1995). Transitions and turning points in faculty-doctoral student relationships. In P. Frost and S. Taylor (Eds.), Rhythms of academic life. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, forthcoming. Kramer, R. M., & Messick, D. M. (1997). Getting by with a little help from our enemies: Collective paranoia and its role in intergroup relations. In C. Sedikides, Organizational Paranoia 65 J. Schopler, & C. A. Insko (Eds.), Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Kramer, R. M., Meyerson, D., & Davis, G. (1990). How much is enough? Psychological components of “guns versus butter” decisions in a security dilemma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 984-993. Kramer, R. M. & Tyler, T. R. (1996) Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kruglanski, A. W. (1970). Attributing trustworthiness in supervisor-worker relations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 214-232. Kruglanski, A. W. (1987). Blame-placing schemata and attributional research. In C. F. Graumann & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Changing conceptions of conspiracy, pp. 219229.. New York: Springer-Verlag. Kunda, Z. (1999). Social Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Lane, C., & Bachmann, R. (1998). Trust within and between organizations: Conceptual issues and empirical applications. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Lewicki, R. J., & Bunker, B. B. (1995). Trust in relationships: A model of trust development and decline. In B. B. Bunker & J. Z. Rubin (Eds.), Conflict, cooperation, and justice. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Lindskold, S. (1978). Trust development, the GRIT proposal, and the effects of conciliatory acts on conflict and cooperation. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 772793. Lindskold, S. (1986). GRIT: Reducing distrust through carefully introduced conciliation. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of Intergroup relations. Chicago: Nelson Hall. Lord, C. G., & Saenz, D. S. (1985). Memory deficits and memory surfeits: Differential cognitive consequences of tokenism for tokens and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 918-926. Luhmann, N. (1979). Trust and power. New York: Wiley. March, J. G. (1995). A primer on decision making. New York: Free Press. March, J. G., & Olsen, J. (19xx). Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schmoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20, 709-734. McAllister, D. (1995). Affect- and cognition-based trust as foundations for interpersonal cooperation in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 38, 24-59. Melley, T. (2000). Empire of conspiracy: The culture of paranoia in postwar America. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Messick, D. M., & Sentis, K. P. (1983). Fairness, preference, and fairness biases. In D. M. Messick & K. S. Cook (Eds.), Equity Theory: Psychological and sociological perspectives. New York: Praeger. Messick, D. M., Wilke, H., Brewer, M. B., Kramer, R. M., Zemke, P. E., & Lui, L. (1983). Individual adaptations and structural change as solutions to social dilemmas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychhology, 44, 294-309. Meyerson, D., Weick, K., & Kramer, R. M. (1996). Swift trust in temporary groups. In R. M. Kramer & T. R. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Moreland, R. L. (1985). Social categorization and the assimilation of "new" group members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1173-1190. Organizational Paranoia 66 Moreland, R. L., & Levine, J. M. (1989). Newcomers and oldtimers in small groups. In P. B. Paulus (Ed.), Psychology of group influence. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Morris, M. W., & Larrick, R. P. (1995). When one cause casts doubt on another: A normative analysis of discounting in causal attribution. Psychological Review, 102, 331-355. Morrison, E. W. (1993). Newcomer information seeking: Exploring types, modes, sources and outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 557-589. Norem, J. K., & Cantor, N. (1986). Defensive pessimism: Harnessing anxiety as motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1208-1217. Nye, J.S., Zelikow, P. D., & King, D. C. (1997). Why people don’t trust government. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Peeters, G., & Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology, 1, 33-60. Pew Research Center for the People and the Press. (1996). Trust and citizen engagement in metropolitan Philadelphia: A case study. Washington, DC: The Pew Research Center for the People and the Press. Pew Research Center for the People and the Press (1998). Deconstructing distrust: How Americans view government. Washington, D.C.: Pew Press. Pilisuk, M. & Skolnick, P. (1968). Inducing trust: A test of the Osgood Proposal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 121-133. Pipes, D. (1997). Conspiracy. New York: Free Press. Pruitt, D. G. (1987). Conspiracy theory in conflict escalation. C. F. Graumann & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Changing conceptions of conspiracy, pp. 191-202. New York: Springer-Verlag. Putnam, R. (2000). Bowling alone. New York: Simon & Schuster. Robins, R. S., & Post, J. M. (1997). Political paranoia. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Rotter, J. B. (1980). Interpersonal trust, trustworthiness, and gullibility. American Psychologist, 35, 1-7. Salancik, G. R., & Pfeffer, J. (1978). A social information processing approach to job attitudes and task design. Administrative Science Quarterly, 23, 224-253. Shapiro, D. (1965). Neurotic styles. New York: Basic Books. Siegel, R. K. (1994). Whispers: The voices of paranoia. New York: Crown. Sitkin, S. B., & Roth, N. L. (1993). Explaining the limited effectiveness of legalistic ‘remedies’ for trust/distrust. Organizational Science, 4, 367-392. Sitkin, S. B., Rousseau, D. M., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Special topic forum on trust in and between organizations. Academy of Management Review, 23, whole issue. Slovic, P. (1993). Perceived risk, trust, and democracy. Risk Analysis, 13, 675-682. Spranca, M., Minsk, E., & Baron, J. (1991). Omission and commision in judgment and choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 76-105. Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social Issues, 41, 157-175. Stillwell, A. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). The construction of victim and perpetrator memories: Accuracy and distortion in role-based accounts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1157-1172. Organizational Paranoia 67 Strickland, L. H. (1958). Surveillance and trust. Journal of Personality, 26, 200-215. Sutton, R. I., & Galunic, D. C. (1996). Consequences of public scrutiny for leaders and their organizations. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 18), pp. 201-250. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Swan, S., & Wyer, R. S. (1997). Gender stereotypes and social identity: How being in the minority affects judgments of self and others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1265-1277. Swann, W., & Read, S. (1981). Self-verification processes: How we sustain our selfconceptions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 351-372. Sztompka, P. (1999). Trust: A sociological theory. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Taylor, S. E. (1981). A categorization approach to stereotyping. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior, pp. 83-114. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67-85. Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1975). Point of view and perceptions of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 439-432. Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T., Etcoff, N. L., & Ruderman, A. (1978). Categorical bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 778-793. Taylor, S. E., & Martin, J. (1987) The present minded professor: Controlling one's career. In M. P. Zanna & J. M. Darley (Eds.), The compleat academic. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., & Armor, D. A. (1998). Harnessing the imagination: Mental simulation, self-regulation, and coping. American Psychologist, 53, 429-439. Tsui, A. S., Egan, T. D., & O’Reilly, C. (1992). Being different: Relational demography and organizational attachment. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 549-579.. Turner, J. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Tyler, T. R. (1993). The social psychology of authority. In J. K. Murnighan (Ed.), Social psychology in organizations: Advances in theory and practice, pp. 141-160. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Vorauer, J. D., & Ross, M. (1993). Making mountains out of molehills: An informational goals analysis of self- and social perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 620-632. Weick, K. E. (1979). The social psychology of organizing. Reading, MA: AddisonWesley. Weick, K. E. (1995). Sensemaking in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Weiner, B. (1985). “Spontaneous” causal thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 74-84. Wharton, A. S. (1992). The social construction of gender and race in organizations: A social identity and group mobilization perspective. In (Eds.), Research in the Sociology of Organizations (Vol. 1), pp. 55-84. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Whitney 1994. Organizational Paranoia 68 Wilson, T. D., & D. Kraft (1993). Why do I love thee? Effects of repeated introspections about a dating relationship on attitudes towards the relationship. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 409-418. Wu, Jianzhong, & Axelrod, R. (1997). Coping with noise. In R. Axelrod (Ed.), The complexity of cooperation, pp. 33-39. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Wyer, R. (Ed.) (1996). Ruminative thoughts (Advances in Social Cognition, Vol. IX). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Yamagishi. Xx Zanna, M. P., & Darley, J. M. (1987). On managing the faculty-graduate student research relationship. In M. P. Zanna and J. M. Darley (Eds.), The compleat academic. New York: Random House. Zimbardo, P. G., Andersen, S. M., & Kabat, L. G. (1981). Induced hearing deficit generates experimental paranoia. Science, 212, 1529-1531. Zuboff, 1988 xx. Zucker, L. G. (1986). Production of trust: Institutional sources of economic structure, 1840-1920. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (vol. 8), pp. 53-111. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Organizational Paranoia 69 Social Uncertainty Induced Psychological States Heightened Self-Consciousness Heightened Perceptions of Evaluative Scrutiny Uncertainty about Status Reduced Perceptions of Psychological Safety Cognitive Appraisal and Coping Responses Hypervigilance Dysphoric Rumination Trust-related Mental Accounting Cognitive Elaboration Overly Personalistic Construal of Social Interactions Sinister Attribution Bias Heightened Concerns about Others’ Trustworthiness Defensive Behaviors Behavioral Inhibition Social Withdrawal Social Consequences Paranoid Attribution Bias Avoidant Responses Figure 1. A Social Auditing Model of Organizational Paranoia