Developmental Change in Infants’ Use of Cues to Word Meanings Carmel Houston-Price School of Psychology, University of Reading Kim Plunkett Dept. of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford Paul Harris Graduate School of Education, Harvard University Hester Duffy Dept of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford Infants’ rapid vocabulary acquisition suggests that they are equipped with a range of strategies for learning the meanings of new words from a very young age. This research investigates 15- and 18-month-olds’ ability to use two different types of cues to discover the meanings of words in ambiguous situations. A series of studies use the intermodal preferential looking paradigm to explore whether, when two potential referents are available, infants will preferentially attach a new label to (a) a moving referent versus a stationary referent, (b) the referent towards which an adult gazes, (c) a moving referent towards which an adult also gazes, and (d) a gazed-upon versus a moving referent. Results suggest that by 15 months infants are highly sensitive to the gaze direction cue, even in the presence of a conflicting movement cue. Use of the movement cue is not robust, however, until infants reach 18 months of age. The philosopher, Quine (1960), described the difficulty faced by the young child word learner as follows: On hearing an adult use a new word (“gavagai”) as a rabbit runs past, how can the child know whether the word refers to the category of rabbits, to some part or property of the rabbit, to the manner of its movement or to its name? The child clearly faces a further problem, that of knowing whether the word refers to the rabbit at all, or instead to some other aspect of the scene, or indeed to something that is not even present. Will the child assume that the word refers to the rabbit because the speaker is looking at it, or because the rabbit is moving, thereby attracting her attention? It is this second aspect of the child’s dilemma that is the focus of this research. Previous research has shown that young children are sensitive to the intention of the user of a new word; the adult’s gaze direction, pointing and emotional expression towards potential referents are all noticed and exploited by the child in her attempt to establish the most likely meaning of the word. The learning environment itself also provides clues to word meanings; for example, interesting objects and events are more likely to be talked about. If the child is sensitive to this correlation, she might employ the strategy of attaching words to more salient items in her environment. Hollich, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff et al. (2000) suggest that these strategies and others come into use in a two-phase process where salience cues, such as object movement, are used early in vocabulary acquisition while social cues, such as gaze direction, come into use later. However, Moore, Angelopoulos and Bennett (1999) found the opposite pattern of acquisition; while 24-month-olds used both cue types successfully, 18month-olds were limited to using the social cue. This research uses an adaptation of the intermodal preferential looking paradigm (Golinkoff, Hirsh-Pasek, Cauley & Gordon, 1987) to explore the utility of social and salience cues for infants aged 15 and 18 months. Infants were presented with a series of trials. On each trial, two potential referents were presented on screen while a novel label was heard. The target to be associated with the label was indicated either (a) by its movement, (b) by the gaze direction of a face presented on screen, or (c) by both cues in combination. In a fourth condition (d), the two cues provided conflicting information about the reference of the novel word; the face looked at one image, while the other moved. Infants’ learning of associations between words and images was assessed by comparing their looking times to each image in the absence of any cue other than the word heard. Longer looks towards the target were interpreted as evidence of learning. Results showed that the 15-month-olds were highly sensitive to the social cue, reliably attaching new labels to gazed-upon images even in the presence of the conflicting movement cue. A trend suggesting that these infants were sensitive to the salience cue was also evident, but only the 18-month-olds reliably used this cue to determine reference. These findings therefore provide some support for Moore et al.’s (1999) claim that social cues are used before salience cues in the discovery of word meaning (cf. Hollich et al., 2000). Interestingly, the older group showed no sensitivity to the gaze direction of the adult, which might be explained in terms of their greater understanding about the intentional nature of the cue, and their resulting unwillingness to accept the disembodied face as having communicative intention. Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K. M., & Gordon, L. (1987). The eyes have it: Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14, 23-45. Hollich, G.J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R.M., Brand, R.J., Brown, E., Chung, H.L., Hennon, E., Rocroi, C. & Bloom, L. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65 (3, Serial No. 262). Moore, C., Angelopoulos, M., & Bennett, P. (1999). Word learning in the context of referential and salience cues. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 60-68. Quine, W.V.O. (1960). Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.