The Effects of Alcohol on Short Term Memory in Drosophila Melanogaster Virginia Brodie, Paul Mascali, Jean Cheng Manhasset Science Research January 31, 2010 Abstract National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism has estimated that approximately 14 million people in the United States suffer from alcoholism. Alcohol consumption has long-term effects, including Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome, or Alcohol Dementia, which consists of impaired short-term memory. Alcohol is an organic compound that with consumption affects the nerve cell of the brain by preventing the production and propagation of electric impulses. When alcohol is dissolved in the membrane of memory cells, it disorganizes the membrane, which leads to changes in the physical and biochemical characteristics. Moore (98) exposed drosophila to ethanol using an inebrometer, and Miller, tested human’s short term memory and found that short term memory was weakened. The purpose of this research will be to show how alcohol consumption affects short-term memory. This will be tested by exposing drosophila to ethanol regularly and then using olfactory memory to test the short-term memory, by shocking flies 4 times for a minute over 40 minutes while simultaneously pumping them an odor. They will then be placed in a chamber where they have the choice to choose the familiar scent or a new scent, those choosing the new scent are said to have formed short term memory. Interest Alcohol is the most commonly used and widely abused psychoactive drug in the world. Alcoholism is a disease, which can some times be fatal. Nearly 14 million Americans meet diagnostic criteria for alcohol use disorders (Grant, 1994), and more than 100,000 U.S. deaths are caused by excessive alcohol consumption each year (Ericson ,2001). Knowledge Base Alcohol is an organic compound; its molecular structure is CH3CH2OH. Ethanol, one of the most common alcohols, is drinking alcohol its molecular formula is C2H6O. Alcoholism is the continuous or excessive use of alcohol with associated pathologic results, and affects nearly 8 million Americans. Alcohol affects the nerve cell by preventing the production and propagation of electric impulses along a network consisting of axons and synapses. In the brain, one nerve cell communicates information to many other cells, which in turn receive impulses from many other areas; alcohol enhances some of these impulses and blunts others. When alcohol is dissolved in the membrane of memory cells, it fluidizes or disorganizes the membrane, which leads to changes in the physical and biochemical characteristics. Chronic exposure alters the composition of the membrane as wells as its rigidity. Studies have found that lipid molecules, in the membranes of brain cells of animals fed alcohol for a prolonged time can confer alcohol resistance to brain cell membranes of animals that have not. Brain lipids are modified but also brain proteins, demonstrating that brain chemistry is modified by chronic exposure to alcohol (McGraw-Hill Encyclopedia, ‘97). Alcohol Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome, one of the long-term effects of alcohol consumption, is two disorders that can occur simultaneously or independently. Wernicke's disease involves damage to multiple nerve cells in the brain and spinal cord, it also includes symptoms caused by alcohol withdrawal. Korsakoff syndrome involves the impairment of memory and intellect skills, which include learning. Korsakoff psychosis involves damage to areas of the brain. Alcohol Withdrawal Syndrome is a set of symptoms that occur when one suddenly stops drinking after long term consumption of alcohol. The spectrum of alcohol withdrawal symptoms ranges from such minor symptoms as insomnia and tremulousness to severe complications such as withdrawal seizures and delirium tremens. In alcohol withdrawal syndromes, the brain maintains neurochemical balance through inhibitory and excitatory neurotransmitters. The main inhibitory neurotransmitter is gammaaminobutyric acid (GABA), One of the major excitatory neurotransmitters is glutamate, which acts through the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) neuroreceptor. Alcohol inhibits NMDA neuroreceptors, and chronic alcohol exposure results in up-regulation of these receptors. An abrupt stop to alcohol exposure results in brain hyperexcitability, because receptors previously inhibited by alcohol are no longer inhibited (Bayard, ’04). Alcohol has different affects based on different genetic makeup. Gender has an affect on the impaired. Women get impaired quicker then males while drinking the same amount of alcohol. This may happen because of the water levels in both are not equal. However, women are able to eliminate more alcohol per unit in an hour than men. Women are also more susceptible to alcohol’s long term effects. Heavy drinkers are mostly found in men. Only 2% of all women are alcoholics while 9% of all men are alcoholics (Mumenthaler, 1999). The gene that affects the addiction to alcohol is ADH. The alcohol dehydrogenate gene is mainly meant to metabolize alcohol. Without it people are much more susceptible to becoming addicted to alcohol. Different types of the fruit fly Drosophila Melanogaster have this gene as well (Winberg, 2004). Studies by Heberlein have shown Drosophila has increases of speed, loss of motor control, and when exposed to the ethanol gas for over 20 minutes, they become sedated (Heberlein, 2000). Short-term memory is formed when an outside stimulus is presented to the brain; the CREB gene, located in the nucleus, is then activated which induces transcription factors. The RNA enters the nucleus of the cells and copies the CREB gene, and the messenger-RNA then leaves the cell and executes the genes instructions. The cyclic AMP cycle is initiated and sodium is flushed into the brain. This sodium causes the spaces between synapses in the brain to temporarily shrink, allowing for the formation of short-term memory. Short-term memory in drosophila can last up to 24 hours. Memories are thought to be due to lasting synaptic modifications within the brain. The rutabega adenylyl cyclase, an enzyme that is expressed in the Drosophila brain and mediates synaptic plasticity is needed in cells of the mushroom bodies for a component of olfactory short-term memory (Zars 2000). The cyclic AMP systems plays a crucial role in olfactory learning in drosophila; calcium ions enter vertebrate cells through a glutamate receptor know as the NMDA receptor. This neural stimulation leads to a movement of calcium ions into the cells leading to activation of adenyl cyclase and a rise in cyclic AMP levels. Cyclic AMP then activates protein kinase A, which causes a phosphorylation cascade leading to the induction of genes involved in learning (Davis 1995). Protein kinase A, also known as the cyclic-AMP dependent protein kinase, is an enzyme that supplies proteins with phosphate groups. This protein kinase A activity is regulated by fluctuating cyclic-AMO within cells and is ultimately responsible for all the cellular responses due to the cyclic-AMP second messenger system (Bowen 2003). Literature Review Moore, 1998, exposed drosophila to ethanol and found that drosophila displayed behaviors that are similar to behaviors of rodents and humans who are intoxicated, including lack of balance, and impaired motor coordination. An inebriometer, a 4 ft long glass column containing multiple oblique mesh is baffles in which ethanol vapor is circulated, was used to measure ethanol- induced loss of posture control. An inebriometer is a tube in which the flies are placed in towards the top and are meant to “stick” to the level, as the intoxication increases, the fall layer and layer. To begin a trial flies are introduced into the top of the inebriometer. With time, flies lose their ability to stand on the baffles and gradually tumble downward. As they fall out of the bottom of the inebriometer, a fraction collector is used to gather them at 3 min intervals, at which point they are counted. The elution profile of wild-type control flies follows a normal distribution Cheapdate a mutant drosophila with enhanced sensitivity to ethanol was used. It was found that it is an allele of the memory mutant amnesiasic, a fly that has been postulated to encode a nueropeptide that activates the cAMP pathway. It was found that cheapdate can be reversed by treatment with stimulators that increase cAMP levels or PKA activity. Miller tested human’s short-term memory a week after alcohol consumption, in which one group of people were consuming alcohol throughout a week and another group was given a placebo. After testing short-term memory he found that alcohol treated subjects displayed significantly poorer short-term memory compared to those without alcohol consumption, demonstrating that short-term memory is affected by alcohol consumption. Farr allowed mice to become dependednt on alcohol by adding it into their food and exposing them to it constantly since birth. He then abruptly stopped giving them alcohol for a three-week withdrawl period. The learning and long-term memory tests included olfactory footshock avoidance and shuttlebox active avoidance. His findings included permanent defects of neurochemical changes in the brain, demonstrating permanent, irreversible, effects on the brain. Sullivan, 2000, developed a scale of 0-7 on how Drosophila reacts to ethanol and cocaine. When Drosophila have normal behavior they are a 0 on the scale; a 1 is intense nearly continuous grooming; a 2 is a loss of negative geotaxis and flight; a 3 is slow locomotion in a circular motion; 4: sideways or backward locomotion; 5: hyperkinetic behaviors including wing bussing, erratic activity with flies often bouncing off the walls; 6: sever body tremors, no locomotion; 7: death. Heberlein, 2004, demonstrated the effects of ethanol concentration on the sedation times of Drosophila. There were three different concentrations used, 56% alcohol, 50% alcohol, and 43% alcohol. The higher concentrations caused more drosophila to become intoxicated LaFerriere, 2008, studied the correlation between ethanol sensitivity and learning mechanisms in Drosophila. The learning phenotypes of 10 new strains of drosophila were examined, and 5 were found to have increased ethanol sensitivity. They trained the files using place and olfactory memory; individual flies were trained in a heat box to avoid part of a small chamber by associating one half with a temperature that they avoid, 41o, with a contrasting temperature of 24o, In this test, one-half of the chamber is heated to the same temperature as that used for conditioning while the other half of the chamber is kept at 24°, a temperature that flies normally prefer. After being trained for 20 minutes the memory was tested for 3 minutes, directly following the training, once the high temperature was removed. In the other, more classic test, olfactory conditioning paired one of two odorants (4-methylcyclohexanol and octanol) with electric shock (100 V). Memory tests were performed 3 min after training, where changed olfactory preferences were tested in a t-maze. Besides finding new memory mutant flies, they found no significant correlation between ethanol sensitivity and place memory in the 10 lines tested. She found that the 3-54 mutant memory remains elevated relative to wild-type performance even 3 hr after training. Furthermore, the slopes of the memory decay curves in the two lines are roughly parallel. Purpose Therefore the purpose of this experiment is to demonstrate the effects of alcohol exposure on short-term memory in drosophila. Hypothesis The null hypothesis states that the ethanol exposure will have no effect on the short term memory of Drosophila melanogaster. The alternate hypothesis states the flies exposed to ethanol before training will have impaired memory and the flies exposed to alcohol after training will not have damaged memory. Methodology Drosophila Melanogaster were the model organism in this study. This model organism was divided into 5 groups. The first group was a control in which no drosophila will be exposed to alcohol. The next group was the organism that was exposed to 200proof Ethanol before training and then a group that was exposed after training. The flies were placed in a training container lined with a 1-milimeter flexible circuit board, All-Flex, which gave the flies a tingling sensation in their feet. For 1 minute the flies were shocked while simultaneously being pumped an odor 3-octonal which has been found to not be preferable in flies. The shock intervals consisted of 1.5 square pulses with 1.5 seconds of rest, which was repeated for 60 seconds. They were times to complete the training protocol. The scent was administered using an air pump while the shock was supplied with a Grass Telefactor S48 Square Pulse Stimulator. The flies were then returned to the vial with the supplemented food. 5 hours after training, which is sufficient short term memory for flies, the flies were tested by being lowered in an elevator into two scent chambers between two scents, the familiar scent 3-octanol, which they were previously shocked with, as well as a new unfamiliar scent 4-methycyclihexanol, which is found to not be preferable in flies. The flies that moved away from the 3-octanol scent, which they were previously shocked with, were said to have formed short term memory, since they had linked the odor with the foot shocking, while those that remained in the middle or went to the old scent did not form short-term memory, since they could not make the connection. Alcohol was exposed to Drosophila through respiration. Drosophila were placed in a testing chamber which was connected through a tube to a beaker. 200-proof ethanol was placed in the beaker and an air pump was connected in to the beaker. The alcohol was evaporated by an air pump allowing a steady stream of ethanol to follow into the chamber where the drosophila are held. The drosophila were exposed for 5 minutes either before or after training. Discussion: The purpose of this experiment is to demonstrate the effects of alcohol exposure on short term memory in drosophila. The data supported part of the alternate hypothesis, the files exposed to ethanol before training will have impaired memory, because the control group demonstrated short-term memory formation but the flies exposed to ethanol before and after training did not. The independent variables in the graph were the control, alcohol before and the alcohol after. The dependent variable was the performance index. The Performance Index is a way to measure if learning occurred. Since there were 3 groups, learning occurred if the performance index was above 33%. The three groups for the most part had a variety of results. The control group had a high performance index where learning occurred. The two groups given alcohol were below the 33% because they did not use olfactory memory, or their memory was impaired from the alcohol, to connect the scent with the shocking. There was variation between the control and the two alcohol groups. The control was higher, while the two alcohol groups were very similar in results, with the alcohol after being slightly higher. The flies were counted as those who avoided the scent, including the flies that stayed in the middle, and the flies that went towards the scent. In the individual trials, the flies had choice between the familiar scent that they were shocked with, and a new unfamiliar scent. Ideally, the files should have avoided the familiar scent and gone to the unfamiliar scent. For the most part, the flies choose a scent within the 4 minutes they were given, but in every trial there were also flies that stayed in the middle and did not decide. Although the flies did avoid the familiar scent, they did not pick the other scent. It is difficult to decide what to do with the flies in the middle because there are a variety of things that could affect them. They could still be drunk, they could be dead, or they could just not know what to pick. But they could also be avoiding the familiar scent. The results did not make sense because the performance index of the flies with alcohol was, with alcohol before, 7.8%, and alcohol after, 10.8%. They both seemed to defy the expected outcome because even if the flies had just guessed, the percentages would have been higher. Another data analysis was made excluding the middle flies. All of the groups increased. The control went from 50.5% to 75%, the alcohol before changed to 35% and the alcohol after became 37%. These results made more sense because the variables with the alcohol were higher and more likely. The alcohol before and after were still both very close, showing that they had a similar effect on the brain. Conclusion- The data supports the alternate hypothesis because the control group demonstrated short term memory formation, while the alcohol groups did not. Limitations: Limitations included possible human errors. Also more trials would’ve been helpful for the flies given alcohol. Future studies would include looking at the long term effects of alcohol on the brain. Bibliography "Alcohol." McGraw-Hill Encyclopedia of Science & Technology. 8th ed. Pennsylvania: McGraw-Hill, 1997. 396-401. “Alcohol Damages Day-to-Day Memory Function” 2009. The New York Times. <http://alcoholism.about.com/cs/dementia/a/blacer030617.htm> "Alcohol Dementia." 2009. The New York Times. <http://alcoholism.about.com/od/dementia/Alcohol_Dementia.htm> Bayard, Max. "Alcohol Withdrawal Syndrome - March 15, 2004 - American Family Physician." American Academy of Family Physicians. 2004. 25 May 2009 <http://www.aafp.org/afp/20040315/1443.html>. Bowen, R. “Protein Kinase A”. Biomedical Hypertexts. November 28, 2003. Davis, Ronald. John A. Kiger. “dunce Mutants of Drosophila melanogaster: Mutants Defective in the Cyclic AMP Phosphodiaterase Enzyme System”. The Journal of Cell Biology. Volume 90. July 1991. Ericson, Nels. "Substance Abuse:The Nation’s Number One Health Problem." 2001. U.S. Department of Justice. <http://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/ojjdp/fs200117.pdf>. Farr, Susan. "Drinking for just eight weeks impairs learning and memory in mice." Science, Industry and Business: the innovations-report. 15 May 2009 <http://www.innovations-report.com/html/reports/life_sciences/report-45402.html>. Grant B, Harford TC, Dawson DA, et al. Prevalence of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence, United States 1992. Alcohol Health & Research World 18(3). 1994. Heberlein Ulrike. “Molecular Genetic Analysis of Ethanol Intoxication in Drosophila Melanogaster.” Aug. 2004 Heberlein, Ulrike, Fred W. Wolf, Adrian Rothenfluh and Douglas J. Guarnier, “Molecular Genetic Analysis of Ethanol Intoxication In Drosophila Melangaster” Department of Anatomy and Program in Neuroscience. 2004. Laferriere, Holly, and Douglas J. Guarnieri. "Genetic Dissociation of Ethanol Sensitivity and Memory Formation in Drosophila melanogaster." (2008). Genetics. <http://www.genetics.org/cgi/content/full/178/4/1895/>. Miller, Loren L., and Michael P. Dolan. "SpringerLink - Journal Article." SpringerLink Home - Main. 1973. <http://www.springerlink.com/content/u0161w1781274g83/>. Moore, Monica, and Jim DeZazzo. "Ethanol Intoxication in Drosophila:Genetic and Pharmacological Evidence for Regulation by the cAMP Signaling Pathway." (1998). <http://www.northwestern.edu/neurobiology/faculty/allada2/gbbpapers/MooreHeberlein9 8.pdf>. Mumenthaler. “Martin Gender Differences in Moderate drinking effect alcohol Health & Research World” 1999. Sullivan Ashburner. “Hawley, Drosophila Protocols.” Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press. 1996. Windberg,Jan Olof “Drosophila Melanogaster Alcohol Dehydrogenase: PridyctInhibition.” 1994. Zars, T. M Fischer, R Schulz, and M Heisenberg. “Localization of Short Term Memory in Drosophila” Science Magazine. April 28, 2000