1 Review of the Literature in Literacy Development and Instruction in Students who are Deaf and Hard of Hearing Susan R. Easterbrooks Professor Department of Educational Psychology and Special Education Georgia State University January, 2005 2 Special Recognition Special recognition for their participation goes to Kathleena Whitesell, Lenoir Rhyne College, and Elaine Gale, City College of New York, for their reviews of 15 articles each, and to Melody Stoner, doctoral student, Georgia State University, for her review of several of the articles. Recognition also goes to Gay Su Pinnell, Ohio State University, who reviewed the draft outline of the project and offered her expert suggestions. 3 Literacy Literature Review Background Children who are deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) learn to read across a continuum of stimulus sources. Some children have sufficient residual hearing and powerful amplification that allow them to develop literacy through the auditory pathway (Izzo, 2002); some require visual support from English based sign systems (LuetkeStahlman & Nielson, 2003); and still others learn to read English as a second language based on competence in their natural language of American Sign Language (Musselman, 2000) or another native language such as Spanish (Walker-Vann, 1998). Some of the practices commonly used today, such as Guided Reading, may be used both as auditory means and as visual means of instructing DHH children in literacy. Some practices, such as phonemic awareness (e.g., /k/ sound as in cat or kite), vizeme awareness (e.g., a visual symbol represents each letter shape on the lips), or cheremic awareness (e.g., the index finger shape as in think or me) tend to be language-specific (i.e., spoken English, signed English, American Sign Language) and require modifications depending on whether they are being used with primarily auditory learners or primarily visual learners. The multiplicity of procedures and strategies that may be appropriate for an individual student requires teachers of students who are deaf and hard of hearing to have a broadly based set of skills to apply in the classroom, especially since the majority of students with hearing loss fall below the basic skills level (Traxler, 2000). This literature review looks at the categories identified below and, whenever possible, does so from several perspectives, which are described first in this document. Perspectives The following skills are identified as critical to literacy learning by the National Reading Panel (2000): Phonemic awareness Phonics (traditional decoding and encoding) Vocabulary Comprehension Text Comprehension (reading strategies) Fluency (spoken) Motivation To the above we add Visual Fluency Visual decoding and encoding (e.g., use of Cued Speech, Visual Phonics) Code-Switching (dealing with dual languages) Reading in the content areas 4 In addition, the Laurent Clerc Center of Gallaudet University) identifies nine practices that are critical to literacy development. Where information is available, these are reviewed (http://clerccenter.gallaudet.edu/Literacy/programs/literacy.html). Reading to Students Language Experience Shared Reading and Writing Guided Reading and Writing Writer’s Workshop Research Reading and Writing Dialogue Journal Journals and Logs Independent Reading Some of the above categories were collapsed or not reviewed due to a lack of literature available. References for Background Izzo, A. (2002). Phonemic awarenss and reading ability: An investigation of young readers who are deaf. American Annals of the Deaf, 147(4), 18-28. Luetke-Stahlman, B. & Nielsen, D.C. (2003). The contribution of phonological awareness and receptive and expressive English to the reading ability of deaf students with varying degrees of exposure to accurate English. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 464-484. Musselman, C. (2000). How do children who can’t hear learn to read an alphabetic script? A review of the literature on reading and deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 9-31. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidencebased assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH Publication No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Traxler, C.B. (2000). The Stanford Achievement Test, 9th Edition: National norming and performance standards for deaf and hard-of-hearing students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(4), 337-348. Walker-Vann, C. (1998). Profiling Hispanic deaf students: A first step towards solving the greater problems. American Annals of the Deaf, 143(1), 46-54. 5 Review of the Literature We have attempted to identify appropriate research-based literature for the literacy actions identified above. A brief annotation is also given. The evidence is identified by the following coding system so that the reader will have a sense of the rigor and reliability of the data: 1= true experimental research containing quantitative methods, an actual treatment, and a matched control group 2= quasi-experimental research where qualitative or quantitative methods are used but no control group is available but where a comparison group may have been used. 3= narrative information regarding cases studies, overall program data, or other descriptive means of identification of information; theoretical or literature review articles; testomonials. I. Phonemic Awareness Izzo, A. (2002). Phonemic awareness and reading ability: An investigation with young readers who are deaf. American Annals of the Deaf, 147 (4), 18-28. Rating: 2 Results: For 29 severe to profoundly deaf elementary school children in the midsouth who used ASL as a primary mode of communication, there were four interesting findings. 1. Language ability was significantly correlated to reading ability. 2. Age was significantly correlated to reading ability. 3. Phonemic awareness was not significantly correlated to reading ability. a. Most scores in phonetic ability were low. b. Scores in reading ability were widely distributed. 1. Suggested that language ability accounted for wide variance in reading scores. 2. Even students reading on grade level had low scores in phonemic awareness. 4. Phonemic awareness was not correlated significantly to any variable under investigation. The researcher suggested that perhaps skilled readers who are deaf are using strategies other than phonetics, such as orthographic, visual, and/or sign-based strategies. Considering the popularity of including phonetics in early education programs, perhaps we should look at teaching reading to students who are deaf via other methods as well. The suggestion is not to dump phonetics, but to include other strategies in reading instruction as well, or, more of a holistic approach to teaching reading rather than focusing only on the bottom-up approach of teaching phonemic awareness. Luetke-Stahlman, B., & Corcoran-Nielsen, D. (2003). The contribution of 6 phonological awareness and receptive and expressive English to the reading ability of deaf students with varying degrees of exposure to accurate English. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, (8), 4, 464-484. Rating: 2 Results: 31 students (ages 7:9 to 17:9) from three different educational programs strongly committed to the use of SEE were investigated. Three groups were made from the 31: one group of 31, one group of students exposed to SEE for 5 years or more, and one group of students exposed to SEE for 2 years or less. There were four research questions: given a battery of tests standardized on hearing children, which elements would most strongly correlate with reading comprehension ability; do any background or program variables affect reading comprehension scores; do any background or program variables differ for the students in 5 year SEE group versus students in 2 year SEE group; and, do students in 5 year SEE group score higher than students in 2 year SEE group? In response to the first question, it was determined that students in the 31 group who scored highest on reading comprehension also scored highest on word comprehension, word identification, and phoneme substitution. In response to the second question, it was determined that there was no difference between groups except for age of identification, only because this information was not available for all participants. In response to the third question, it was determined that there was no significant difference between groups. In response to the last question, it was determined that students in the group of 5 years or more scored higher on all 15 language and literacy measures investigated. Miller, P. (1997). The effect of communication mode on the development of phonemic awareness in prelingually deaf students. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research (40), 5, 1151-1163. Rating: 1 Results: Three groups of children (oral of hearing parents, signing deaf of deaf parents, and hearing control group) were compared on measures of phonemic awareness by splitting across communication mode (oral deaf, signing deaf, and hearing). It was determined that the two deaf groups scored lower on phonemic awareness, but that deafness does not preclude phonemic awareness development. It was also determined that there was no significant difference between deaf groups in phonemic awareness performance. Sterne, A., & Goswami, U. (2000). Phonological awareness of syllables, rhymes, and phonemes in deaf children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41 (5), 609625. Rating: 1 Results: Three experiments were completed. In the first, three groups of children in England were compared, a deaf group, a chronological age match group, and a reading 7 level match group. Children were asked to indicate whether words were the same or different in word length and were measured on correct answers as well as length of time to respond. Different sets of words were used, such as words that had the same number of letters, but different numbers of syllables, or, different number of letters, but same numbers of syllables. All groups scored above chance level. There was a significant effect of condition (congruent versus non-congruent word pairs), but this did not affect any of the three group’s performances to be better or worse than another group. Each group took longer to determine length for word pairs that were incongruent. Researchers concluded that deaf children can make decisions about word length on par with agematched hearing children, so are capable of representing syllables phonologically. In the second experiment, two groups of children were compared, a deaf group, and a reading level match group. Children were asked to pick which of two pictures rhymed with the target picture and were measured on correct answers as well as length of time to respond. Deaf children performed better with orthographic rhyme pairs than nonorthographic rhyme pairs, while hearing children performed equally well on both tasks. However, deaf children did perform above chance level on both tasks. Orthographic similarity was significant for deaf participants. For the hearing participants, number of correct rhyming judgments was significantly higher than for deaf participants. Researchers indicated that deaf children are able to determine rhyme pairs in a simple task. In the final experiment, two groups of children were compared, a deaf group, and a reading level match group. Children were asked to look at a picture and determine which of four “nonsense” words sounded most like the picture (i.e., “boiz” for “boys” versus “beiz,” “roiz,” or “boin”). The measurement was based on the number of correct nonsense words chosen. The deaf children performed higher than expected, significantly above chance level. In an analysis of the types of errors made by the deaf children, it was surprisingly found that only 9% of the errors were initial, versus 37% medial, and 55% final. The control group performed at ceiling. The researchers indicated that deaf children with an average reading age of 7:6 do have some phonological awareness at each of the three levels investigated, syllable, rhyme, and phoneme. Researchers also suggested that identification of onset seems more salient for these deaf children than identification of rime (which is the same as pre-reading hearing children). Other Non-Reviewed Sources Badian, N. A. (1998). A validation of the role of preschool phonological and orthographic skills in the prediction of reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31, 472-482. Ball, E. W., & Blachman, B. A. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly, 26(1), 49-66 8 Miller, P. (2002). Another look at the STM capacity of prelingually deafened individuals and its relation to reading comprehension. American Annals of the Deaf,147(5), 56–70. Torgesen, J. K., & Davis, C. (1996). Individual difference variables that predict response to training in phonological awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 1-21. II. Phonics, Visual Decoding and Encoding, and the Use of Cued Speech and Visual Phonics Harris, M. & Moreno, C. (2004). Deaf children's use of phonological decoding: Evidence from reading, spelling, and working memory. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 9: 253-268. Rating= 1 This study looked at the performance on three reading tasks by 29 deaf 7 and 8 year olds and 33 deaf 13 and 14 year olds as compared to a hearing control group matched by chronological age and a second control group matched by reading age. The three tasks measured short term memory, lexical legality, and spelling. The researchers found that deaf children performed as well as their hearing peer of the same reading age but lower that hearing peers of the same chronological age on tests of memory for pictures and that short term memory span predicted reading ability in the older group. On the orthographic awareness task, the deaf students scored as well as control group matched by reading age. On the spelling task both groups scored significantly lower on percentage of phonetically based spelling errors than either of the control groups. The authors argued that the data supported the notion that deaf students do not rely on phonological coding during literacy tasks. Leybaert, J. & Lechat, J. (2001). Variability in deaf children’s spelling: the effect of language experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 554-562 Rating= 1 Research: This study investigated deaf children’s use of phoneme-to-grapheme relationships. The authors believe cued speech entails accurate phoneme-tographeme relationship especially those who use cued speech at home because the exposure is early and intensive. The authors believe that the majority of spelling errors would be phonologically accurate in CS-home as in hearing children and not as phonologically accurate for CS-school children because of later exposure. The authors also compared spelling errors of signing children at home and school if early exposure to a fully accessible language would be the critical factor instead of exposure to early CS. Participants were 67 deaf children and 32 hearing children with hearing loss greater that 90dB in better ear. The 20 children in the 9 CS-home group (mean age 8 years 2 months) started CS at home with a mean age of 24 months, the 18 children in the CS-school group (mean age 10 years 10 months) started CS at school several hours a day inconsistently with a mean age of 49 months. The 14 children in the SL-home group (mean age 11 years 1 month) had deaf parents and the 10 children in the CS-school group (mean age 11 years 7 months) had hearing parents but used sign language at school. Participants were asked to write words suggested by a drawing or short sentence context in their test booklet. Sometime part of a word would be written and children had to fill in the correspondence. Findings: CS-home children like their hearing peers spelled dominant graphemes better than non-dominant graphemes. And CS-home children made phonologically accurate spelling errors like their hearing peers. In addition, CS-home children like their hearing peers applied dominant correspondences between phonemes and graphemes when they did not have a fully detailed orthographic representation. SL-home and SL-school children make more word errors than hearing and CShome children. Conclusion: Early acquisition of a natural language and language with phonological structure of spoken language is important for accurate use of phonome-to-grapheme relationships. Spelling production system of SL children seems more governed by orthographic knowledge. Miller, P. (2002). Communication mode and the processing of printed words: Evidence from readers with prelingually acquired deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 7(4), 312-329. Rating= 1 Since all alphabetic systems represent a spoken language, is spoken language necessary in order for students to decode vocabulary meaning? This is the question posed in this well-crafted empirical study of deaf students who were raised orally and deaf students, 68% of whom had deaf parents, who were raised using Israeli Sign Language. The 22 oral deaf students, 22 signing students, and 39 hearing controls were selected from 4th to 9th grade classrooms for mean grade levels of 6.9, 6.9, and 6.6, respectively. Preliminary data on the groups indicated that as compared to hearing controls, the deaf students performed equally as well on short term memory capacity and letter-processing capacity but markedly poorer on phonemic awareness and syntactic skills. This study evaluated word coding strategies of the participants when vowels were manipulated. Note that in Hebrew, vowel information is represented through diacritical marks rather than letter-graphemes, and these marks are generally left off of printed matter past the early grades. Forty nouns were presented in either unmarked (nonpointed), correctly marked, incorrectly marked but phonologically acceptable, and incorrectly marked by not phonologically acceptable forms. Subjects were shown a 10 category label on a computer screen followed by a word that was either a member of that category or not. The task was to push a button indicating a yes or no judgment regarding the membership of the second word in the category. Second words were given in one of the four forms described above. The participants were familiar with all the target words included in the analysis when in their unmarked or nonpointed form. Multivariate analyses of variance using yes/no response latencies and response accuracies across groups and within factors were examined. The orally raised students made more errors on the phonologically distorted target nouns than the signing deaf students. Both the hearing and the orally raised deaf participants categorized phonologically distorted words more slowly than the other three categories while the signing group showed no difference in rate of categorization among the other categories. The authors concluded that the deaf signers “mediated the categorization of word stimuli by means of a processing strategy that was apparently essentially different from the one used by the two other participant groups for the same purpose.” (p. 323) Part of this strategy was to ignore the linguistic (i.e., phonological) information available from the vowel markings. The authors suggest that teachers need to take into account the fact that signing deaf students may not use phonological strategies when designing instruction. Transler, C., Leybaert, J., & Gombert, J. (1999). Do deaf children use phonological syllables as reading units? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 4(2), 124143. Rating= 1 This study examined whether deaf children process written words morphophonologically, that is, at the syllable unit level. Subjects were 21 students in France who were attending schools where a combination of French Signs Language (FSL) and the French version of Cued Speech were used. All subjects had severe to profound, prelingual losses. Mean age was 10 years 6 months. They were matched on word recognition level with a hearing control group from 2nd and 3rd grades. The task was to copy written words and pseudo-words. Their attempts to copy and produce syllables based on phonological and orthographic rules were examined. When the phonological and orthographic patterns were consistent with one another, the deaf subjects performed as well as the hearing subjects, but when the orthographic and phonological segments differed (i.e., the words were irregular), the deaf children did not perform as well as the hearing children. The authors speculated that the level of difficulty of the words, the children’s lack of automaticity with phonological conversion of pseudo-words, and the impact of fingerspelling may have contributed to their lesser successes with the less consistent word patterns Waddy-Smith, B. & Wilson, V. (2003). See that sound! Visual phonics helps deaf and hard of hearing students develop reading skills. Odyssey, 5(1), 14-17. Rating =3 11 Visual phonics is a system developed in 1982 by the International Communication Learning Institute specifically for deaf children. It uses a multisensory approach including hand cues and written symbols to represent phonemic skills for the purpose of decoding and encoding sounds in English print. Wilson-Favors (1987) described the system. Although it may be appropriate as a literacy tool, Zaccagnini & Antia (1993) found that it does not improve speech production. This article described the program, its application, and its use with one student. Testimonials from teachers were also presented. Other Non-reviewed Sources LaSasso, C., Crain, K., & Leybaert, J. (2003). Rhyme generation in deaf students: The effects of exposure to Cued Speech. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 250-270. Leybaert, J. (1998). Effects of phonologically augmented lip speech on the development of phonological representations and deaf children. In M. Marschark & M. D. Clark (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness (Vol. 2, pp. 103-130). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Leybaert, J., & Charlier, B. (1996). Visual speech in the head: The effect of cued-speech on rhyming, remembering, and spelling. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1(4), 234-248. Musselman, C. (2000). How do children who can’t hear learn to read an alphabetic script? A review of the literature on reading and deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 9-31. Perfetti, C. A., & Sandak, R. (2000). Reading optimally builds on spoken language: Implications for deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 5, 3250. Olson, A. C., & Nickerson, J. F. (2001). Syllabic organization and deafness: Orthographic structure or letter frequency in reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A(2), 421-438. Schimmel, C. S, Edwards, S. G., & Prickett, H. T. (1999) Reading?...PAH! (I GOT IT!) American Annals of the Deaf, 144 (4), . Wilson-Favors, V. (November/December 1987). Using the Visual Phonics System to Improve Speech Skills: A Preliminary Study. Perspective for Teachers of the Hearing-Impaired, 6 (2), 204. Zaccagnini, C. & Antia, S. (1993). Effects of Multisensory Speech Training and Visual Phonics on Speech Production of a Hearing-Impaired Child. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders, 15 (2), 3-8. 12 III. Vocabulary Comprehension DeVilliers, P., & Pomerantz, S. (1992). Hearing-impaired students learning new words from written context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 409-431. Rating=2 Research This study investigates deaf students’ abilities to derive lexical and syntactic information about unknown words embedded in short passages of text. In study one, thirty oral upper school hearing-impaired students between 12-18 years of age participated and study two included 36 middle school students, 21 students between the ages of 9-14 years from an oral school and 15 students between the ages of 10 –15 from a TC program using Signed English. Students read passages that were lean, rich and explicit then asked what selective words mean, if the word was good or bad, to circle if the word was a noun, verb or adjective and if the word was used correctly in a sentence. Findings: The skill to derive at least partial meaning from highly informative written text is related to hearing-impaired students’ reading comprehension level. Better readers were able to generate meanings from short passages when compared to poorer readers, specifically with passages that had rich contexts. Knowledge of labels such as noun, verb, and adjective is not influenced by the context of a passage; however, context only goes so far in helping with grammatical knowledge. Conclusion Better readers gained far more from context than poorer readers when they tried to generate meanings for the unknown words based on their usage in a short passage. To enhance students’ English literacy skills and help students expand their vocabularies independent of direct instruction, teachers need to teach students how to learn vocabulary from context. Suggested by Sternberg and Powell, three basic ways to learn new words from context are 1) separate relevant from irrelevant information about the meaning of a word 2) combining relevant cues into a working definition and 3) relate new information about the word given in the text to old information about the topic already stored in memory. Gaustad, M.G. & Kelly, R.R. (2004). The relationship between reading achievement and morphological word analysis of deaf and hearing students matched for reading level. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 9, 269-285. Rating= 1 English language word meaning is based on a highly morphemic system. That is, word meanings are expanded, modified, and changed routinely by affixing single and multiple morphemes to the front or end of a root word. The word “antidisestablishmentarianism” comes to mind, where “establish” is the root word modified by two prefixes (anti-, dis-) 13 and four suffixes (-ment, -ary, -an, -ism). Others might parse this differently, but the point would be the same. In addition, rules of spelling (changing y to i) complicate the matter. In order for DHH students to read and write well, they must have utility with this system. Gaustad & Kelly compared the morphological skills of college students and hearing middle school students matched for reading achievement levels and found that, even though the older deaf students were measured to be on the same reading level as the younger hearing students, the younger hearing students were significantly superior on the ability to understand the meaning of derivational morphemes and roots and to segment words containing multiple morphemes. They concluded that although morphological awareness impacts reading ability in both groups, it impacts in different ways for the deaf students. An error analysis was conducted, and the authors presented implications for instruction. Hanson, V. L., Goodell, E. W. & Perfetti, C. A. (1991). Tongue-twister effects in the silent reading of hearing and deaf college students. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 319-330. Rating = 1 This study investigated if deaf students used phonological information during sentence comprehension using 16 profoundly deaf students from Gallaudet and 16 undergraduate hearing students. All the deaf participants had deaf parents and used ASL as their first language. To investigate use of phonological information, participants read tonguetwister sentences and control sentences. In addition, participants also read concurrent memory load numbers along with tongue-twister sentences. Findings: Deaf and hearing college students both made more errors in their acceptability judgments when reading tongue-twister than when reading control sentences. In addition, subjects, deaf and hearing, made more errors in tongue-twister sentences and concurrent memory load numbers were phonetically similar than when they were phonetically dissimilar. Conclusion: The findings support that phonological processes an important role in reading. Kelly, L. (1996). The interaction of syntactic competence and vocabulary during reading by deaf students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1(1), 75-90. Rating= 2 This study investigated the correlational interaction between vocabulary knowledge and syntactic competence and its influence on reading comprehension in three comparison groups for cross-validation: 100 adolescents from oral programs, 113 adolescents from programs using sign language, and 211 entering freshmen at Gallaudet University. The Test of Syntactic Abilities,the reading vocabulary subtest of the California Achievement Test, and the reading comprehension subtest of the SAT-HI, among others, were administered to the adolescent subjects. The English Structure, Vocabulary, and Reading components of the university’s placement test and the Degrees of Reading Power test of 14 comprehension were administered to the college students. In all three groups, the results for the interaction of syntax and vocabulary was the first predictor to enter the stepwise procedure. For the adolescent groups, vocabulary was the second most influential factor, but for the college students, syntax knowledge was the second most influential factor. Whether this difference is due to the nature of the different tests given or an actual difference in the groups needs to be examined. The author concluded that if a deaf reader’s syntactic competence is limited, this may obstruct the reader’s ability to apply stored vocabulary knowledge. Participants in the study who had higher levels of syntactic competence were better able to apply their vocabulary knowledge to a reading task.. Vocabulary is necessary but not sufficient for reading comprehension. Paul, P.V. (1996). Reading vocabulary knowledge and deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1(1), 5-15. Rating= 3 Paul reviews the knowledge base on vocabulary acquisition in this theoretical and review article and proposes that the knowledge model of vocabulary acquisition is an appropriate model for students who are deaf and hard of hearing. According to the knowledge model vocabulary development must consider how a word fits into a student's semantic repertoire rather than how it is used in a particular context. Mere memorization of a list of words in order to be able to read an upcoming assignment, which Paul refers to as the “traditional definition-and-contextual (or sentence) approach” is ineffective as it leads only to common meaning that cannot be transferred to other contexts. The model includes the following components: integration (e.g., semantic maps, word maps, and semantic features analysis), repetition, meaningful use (i.e., encounters of words in deliberate and natural learning contextual situations). Other Non-Reviewed Articles Allman, T. M. (2002). Patterns of Spelling in Young Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students. American Annals of the Deaf, 147(1), 46–64. Davey, B., & King, S. (1990). Acquisition of word meaning from context by deaf readers. American Annals of the Deaf, 135, 227-234. Gaustad, M. (2000). Morphographic analysis as a word identification strategy for deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(1), 60-80. Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Freyd, P. (1984, May). Vocabulary development: How deaf individuals can learn to use the information given. Paper presented the annual meeting of the International Reading Association, Atlanta, GA. Leybaert, J. (2000). Phonology Acquired through the Eyes and Spelling in Deaf Children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology; 75(4), 291-318. 15 Paul, P. V. (1996). Reading vocabulary knowledge and deafness. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1(1), 3-15. Sutcliffe, A., Dowker, A., & Campbell, R. (1999). Deaf children’s spelling: Does it show sensitivity to phonology? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 4(2), 111123. Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Snyder, L. S, & Mayberry, R. (1996). Can lexical/semantic skills differentiate deaf or hard-of-hearing readers and non-readers? Volta Review, 8(1), 39-61. IV. Text Comprehension (Including Reading Strategies) Andrews, J. F., & Mason, J. M. (1991). Strategy usage among deaf and hearing readers. Exceptional Children, 57, 536-545. Rating= 1 This study investigated strategies deaf elementary-school age youths used when reading expository text and filling in blanks for deleted words or phrases. The participants were 15 males. Five were deaf between the ages of 17-20, profoundly deaf from birth with hearing parents. These deaf participants had reading levels between 2nd and 6th grade. The comparison groups included 5 males between the ages of 8-11 and reading on grade level and 5 males between the ages of 14-18 with learning disabilities with a 4-year delay in their reading All the deaf participants had deaf parents and used ASL as their first language. Participants were asked to read a passage and asked to predict what the missing word or phrase might be and discuss their rationale for making the prediction. Findings: Although deaf youths used similar strategies as their hearing peers, the frequency of strategy type differed. Deaf youths relied on rereading and background knowledge while hearing made use of context clues. Deaf youths infrequently used context clues and never used the title of the passage. Conclusion: The results suggest that instruction for deaf readers should include more effective comprehension strategies. One example is reading through text with a skilled teacher who can help them identify and use appropriate strategies to eventually use on their own. Brown, P., & Brewer, L.C. (1996). Cognitive processes of deaf and hearing skilled and less skilled readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education,1(4), 263-270. Rating=1 This study looked at the impact of “on-line” or extemporaneous inference-drawing on the speed and accuracy of literal and inferential text comprehension and on the speed and accuracy of a lexical decision-making task. All subjects were college attendees. Forty control subjects had normal hearing; forty subjects were deaf and had reading skills equivalent to the hearing control group; and forty subjects were deaf and had poorer 16 reading skills. The authors were interested in determining if passages requiring inference assisted readers in speed and accuracy in making judgments about whether words were real or not real and if passages requiring inference assisted readers in speed and accuracy of comprehension. Said differently, they wanted to know if deaf readers draw inferences about what they are reading while they are reading it and, if so, does this ability differentiate between good readers and poor readers? The authors asked hearing good readers, deaf good readers, and deaf poorer readers to read a series of two sentence paragraphs and then decide if a target word was a real word or a non-word. Measures of speed and accuracy of lexical decision-making were gathered. Four types of paragraphs were used: prediction (where the second sentence did not refer to the inference but was consistent with it), coherence (where the second sentence could only be understood by drawing an appropriate inference), disconfirming (where the second sentence was antithetical to the inference), and a control condition (where all the same words were used but the content was not related to the inference). Findings: The authors found significant differences between skilled and less skilled deaf readers but not between equally skilled hearing and deaf readers on reaction time to the lexical decision. Reaction ties on non-words were slower than on real words. Although not significant, there was a pattern of slower reaction to the word decision task when followed by a control passage as opposed to the other three conditions requiring inference. Hearing students answered comprehension questions faster than skilled deaf readers; skilled deaf readers answered comprehension questions faster than lesser skilled deaf readers. Comprehension increased when students drew inferences while reading. Differences were found between skilled versus nonskilled readers rather than between deaf versus hearing readers indication that deafness, per se, does not prevent the development of word decoding or of text comprehension of both factual and inferential material. Luetke-Stahlman, B., Griffiths, C., & Montgomery, N. (1998). Development of text structure knowledge as assessed by spoken and signed retellings of a deaf secondgrade student. American Annals of the Deaf, 143(4), 337-346. Rating=3 Described development of text structure knowledge through narrative and expository retellings of one deaf second grade student in across a baseline phase, and 2 intervention phases. Findings: Mediated reading experience enhanced comprehension of text structure and affected the quantity and quality of the deaf second grade student’s story retelling over time. Conclusion: Discussion of text structure can play an important role in students comprehending text structure. Story retelling methodology is a way to document progress in text structure and reading progress. 17 Schirmer, B.R., Bailey, J., & Lockman, A.S. (2004), What verbal protocols reveal about the reading strategies of deaf students: A replication study. American Annals of the Deaf, 149(1), 5-16. Rating= 2 Research: This replica study looked at two questions: 1) Can thinking aloud during reading be used to examine reading strategies of deaf students and 2) Do verbal reports (think-aloud) of deaf readers reflect reading strategies similar to verbal reports of hearing students? The participants include 6 public school deaf children between 8 and 11 years of age. The participants read a wordless storybook and a short story. The participants also took the Gates-MacGinite Reading Tests. The authors coded the data using Pressley and Afflerbach activity analysis. Findings: 1) As found in the first study, this study also found that verbal reports can be used to examine reading strategies of deaf students 2) As a group, the deaf students used activities (constructed meaning, monitored and improved comprehension and evaluated comprehension) like their hearing peers but the deaf students did not demonstrate each of the reading strategies within the activities. Deaf students engaged more variety of reading strategies in the first activity, constructing meaning when compared to the other two activities. The findings in this replication study are consistent with the findings in the first study. Conclusion: The findings suggest the need for instruction reading strategies such as monitoring text characteristics, purpose for reading, recognizing problem of concentration, evaluating and accommodating what to read carefully, skim, or skip, improve comprehension by reading more slowly when needed, evaluate quality of writing and ideas. Strassman, B. K. (1997). Metacognition and reading in children who are deaf: A review of the research. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(3), 140-149. Rating = 3 This paper reviews literature related to metacognition and reading in deaf children. Three main issues are discussed: current instructional practices used to teach reading to deaf children might hinder metacognition: reading activities such as worksheets, answering teacher questions, or memorizing vocabulary may not promote metacognition. Low-level reading materials may not allow for practice of metacognition: deaf students reading in challenging texts engage in metacognition strategies may suggest challenging reading materials instead of low-level reading materials to promote metacognition. 18 Deaf students can benefit from metacognitive strategy instruction: teaching of reasoning skills to hearing student may work for deaf students as well. Walker, L.., Munro, J., and Rickards, F.W. (1998). Literal and inferential reading comprehension of students who are deaf or hard of hearing. VoltaReview, 100(2), 87-103. Rating = 2 Research: This study looked at types of reading comprehension (inferential, literal, functional, textual and recreational) in 3 different types of educational settings (segregated, resource room and mainstream). Participants included 195 deaf and hard-of-hearing students between the ages of 9 and 19. Forty-seven in segregated, 88 in resource room and 74 in mainstream. The Test manual for Stanford Diagnostic Test of Reading was used to analyze reading comprehension. Findings: Mainstreamed students did better with literal and inferential reading that segregated and resource room students. Students between 6 and 12th grade comprehend textual and functional print easier than recreational print. Overall mainstreamed students comprehend all types of print better than segregated students who comprehend at the lowest level for all types of print. Resource room students comprehend textual and functional print better than segregated students. Conclusion: Educational settings and teacher style may influence skill and type of reading comprehension. Mainstream students exposed to similar expectations than hearing peers while resource room and segregated students have accommodations that may lower their reading comprehension skills. Students need to be taught how to read for enjoyment and not take recreational print literally to improve their recreational print comprehension. Implication for reading comprehension instruction need to move onto teaching metacognitive strategies to improve literal and inferential reading. Other Non-Reviewed Sources Al-Hilawani, Y.A. (2003). Clinical examination of three methods of teaching reading comprehension to deaf and hard-of-hearing students: From research to classroom application. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 146-156. Akamatsu, C. T. (1988). Instruction in text structure: Metacognitive strategy instruction for literacy development in deaf students. ACEHI/ACEDA, 14, 13-32. Ewoldt, C., Israelite, N., & Dodds, R. (1992). The ability of deaf students to understand 19 text: A comparison of the perceptions of teachers and students. American Annals of the Deaf, 137, 351-361. Satchwell, S. E. (1993). Does teaching reading strategies to deaf children help increase their reading levels? ACEHI/ACEDA, 19, 38-48. Schirmer, B.R. (2003). Using verbal protocols to identify the reading strategies of students who are deaf. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 157-170. V. Reading Fluency (Spoken and Visual) Ensor, A. D., & Koller, J. R. (1997). The effect of the method of repeated readings on the reading rate and word recognition accuracy of deaf adolescents. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(2), 61-70. Rating=1 Can the method of Repeated Readings help deaf student improve reading rate and word recognition accuracy? Forty-two residential school deaf students between the ages of 15-19 participated in this study. The participants used Total Communication, had moderate-to-severe hearing loss and hearing parents. The participants were divided into two groups: 20 in the treatment group and 22 in the control group. All participants read 5 passages from Reading for Concepts. The treatment group reread the same passage 3 days in a row while the control group only read the passage twice, one the first day and last day. The control group read different Reading for Concept passages in between the pre and post readings. The pre and post readings were videotaped to analyze reading rate, reading accuracy and combined reading accuracy. Findings: Participants in the treatment group demonstrated greater improvement between pre and post measurements for reading rate, reading accuracy and combined reading accuracy. Conclusion: The Repeated Reading technique is an effective method for improving reading fluency with deaf students. Kelly, L. P. (2003). The importance of processing automaticity and temporary storage capacity to the differences in comprehension between skilled and less skilled college age deaf readers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8(3), 230-249. Rating= 1 The author defines processing automaticity as “the ability to complete certain basic operations of reading, such as word recognition and syntactic analysis, with a minimum of mental effort” (p.231) and posed the question of whether lack of linguistic knowledge, low automaticity, or limited short term memory accounted for slower reading between groups of 16 skilled and 14 less skilled college-age readers who are deaf. In an inventive series of reading tasks, Kelly applied activities that singled out and measured working memory span for reading, addition span and automaticity, facilitation (i.e., faster decision 20 time) in a lexical decision task using words and pseudo words, sentence reading automaticity, effects of automaticity on reading comprehension, and the effects of an increased temporary storage burden on comprehension. using words and pseudo words, sentence reading automaticity, effects of automaticity on reading comprehension, and the effects of an increased temporary storage burden on comprehension. He found the following: Processing automaticity distinguishes between skilled and less skills readers Processing automaticity contributes to reading comprehension Greater processing challenges impact reading speed and comprehension of poor readers more than of skilled readers, implicating grammar as a contributor to poor automaticity Processing automaticity is a primary source of the difference in comprehension between skilled and less skilled readers Shroyer, E. H., & Birch, J. (1980). Captions and reading rates of hearing-impaired students. American Annals of the Deaf, 125(7), 916-922. Rating= 2 This study investigated reading rates and fluency of 185 deaf students in residential schools using the speed and accuracy portion of the Gates McGinitie Reading Test. Participants were grouped into primary, intermediate, junior high and high school based on reading test administered by the schools and then again based on the Gates McGinitie Reading Test. Findings: Some participants were categorized in primary level by the school was recategorized into the intermediate level determined by the Gates McGinitie Reading Test. Intermediate fourth-sixth grade students reading at a mean of 145 wpm while deaf intermediate students who differ in age are reading at 142wpm. Hearing junior high reading at 188 wpm while deaf are reading at 181. Hearing high school reading at a mean of 216 wpm while deaf reading at a mean of 275 wpm. Conclusion: Keep in mind the limitations of this study because measuring reading rates depends on difficulty, interest, purpose and other factors The results suggest that instruction for deaf readers should include more effective comprehension strategies. One example is reading through text with a skilled teacher who can help them identify and use appropriate strategies to eventually use on their own. Deaf high school students reading at 110wpm will not be able to keep up with captions that run up to 120 wmp. Vaughn, S., Chard, D., Bryant, D. P., Coleman, M., Tyler, B., Thompson, S., & Kouzekanani, K. (2000). Fluency and comprehension interventions for third-grade students: Two paths to improved fluency. Remedial and Special Education, 21, 325335. 21 Rating = 2 Research: This study investigated the effects of two reading interventions: partner reading to enhance fluency by repeated readings and hearing fluent reading models and collaborative strategic reading to enhance comprehension using four strategies: preview, click and clunk, get the gist and wrap-up. Participants included 8 teachers and 111 students in the 3rd grade who either had significant reading problems or had low-to-average reading skills. Findings: All students who participated in either the CSR or PR made gains in rate of reading and correct words read per minute but made no gains in reading accuracy measure or the comprehension measure. Conclusion: CSR was a more difficult intervention to implement when compared to PR and could be the reason why difficult to teach comprehension. Need future study. Other Non-Reviewed Articles Dyer, A., MacSweeney, M., Szczerbinski, M., Green, L., & Campbell, R. (2003). Predictors of reading delay in deaf adolescents: The relative contributions of rapid automatized naming speed and phonological awareness and decoding. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 215-229. Easterbrooks, S. R., & Huston, S. G. (2001). Examining reading comprehension and fluency in students who are deaf and hard of hearing. Paper presented at the 80th Annual Meeting of the International Council for Exceptional Children, Kansas City, MO. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED454654) Hasbrouck, J. E., & Tindal, G. (1992). Curriculum-based oral reading fluency norms for students in grades 2 through 5. Teaching Exceptional Children, 24(3), 41-44. Meyer, M., & Felton, R. (1999). Repeated reading to enhance fluency: Old approaches and new directions. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 283-306. VI. Motivation Koskinen, P. S., Wilson, R. M & Jensema, C. J. (1986). Using closed-captioned television in the teaching of reading to deaf students. American Annals of the Deaf, March, 43-46. Rating= 1 22 This study looked a the effects of using closed-captioned television as a medium for reading instruction with 41 deaf students between 13 to 15 years of age who had reading levels from 1st to 3rd grade. Eight teachers who participated in the study were trained how instruct reading with closed-captioned television. Teachers collected data in four instructional settings, two with closed-caption and two without closed-caption. The percentage of retained words by the students was compared in each instructional setting. In addition, teachers were asked to share their perception of students’ motivation and learning as well as their reaction to instruction with and without closed-captioning. Findings: Approximately 10% more sight vocabulary was retained in the two instructional settings where closed-captioned TV was used when compared to the other two instructional settings where closed-caption TV was not used. Regarding the teachers’ perceptions and reactions, instruction with closed-captioning was more favorable when compared to instruction without closed-captioning. Conclusion: Severe-to-profound deaf children retain sight words better when closedcaptioned TV is used for instruction. Closed-caption TV facilitates comprehension, motivation and quality of learning. Milone, M. (2004). A teacher takes on the challenges of deaf literacy: an interview with Jennifer Herbold. Rating =3 In the interview a teacher talks about the role of a Literacy Specialist and technology in the education of deaf and hard-of-hearing students. The role of a Literacy specialist include setting up literacy resource room with multicultural books and resources, guided reading and shared reading materials and other books for classroom use. Also included is providing support to teachers by providing literacy development workshops and helping teachers plan literacy lessons. In addition, a literacy specialist holds files on each student to monitor their literacy progress. The teacher mentions teachers of deaf and hard-ofhearing students are faced with developing children’s world-knowledge and general semantics in ASL and English. Potential techniques in literacy development could include ASL storytelling, word games and semantic development in ASL and English. Another component is technology, which is seen as a fun tool for students to learn to read and write. Technology includes email journals, IM, videoconferencing, smartboards, computer software, still and motion digital cameras. A project idea using technology to help students develop their writing skills would be to make a commercial after learning about advertising. NOTE: Center for ASL/English Bilingual Education and Research (CAEBER) at the New Mexico School for the Deaf is focusing on improving literacy and learning and instruction. Rowe, L., & Allen, B. (1995). Interactive storybook reading with young deaf children in school and home settings (pp. 170-182). In P. Dreyer (Ed.), Towards multiple perspectives on literacy: Fifty-ninth Yearbook of the Claremont Reading Conference. Claremont, CA: The Claremont Reading Conference. 23 Rating= 3 Describes a two-teacher approach to interactive storybook reading with deaf, hard-ofhearing, and hearing children. The group varied in size from 20 to 30 on a day to day basis. Children ranged in age from 18 months to 2.6 years. The first teacher would read the story orally, then she would hold the book while the second teacher read the story in ASL, using appropriate techniques for gaining the children’s attention. The authors found that the children with hearing loss were motivated to take the books home and read them with their parents when they had received the model in ASL. Other Non-Reviewed Articles Calderon, R. (2000). Parental involvement in deaf children’s education programs as a predictor of child’s language, early reading, and social-emotional development. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 140-155. VII. Code-Switching (Dealing with Dual Languages) Evans, C. (2004). Literacy development in Deaf students: Case Studies in bilingual teaching and learning. American Annals of the Deaf, 149 (1), 17-27. Rating= 3 In an effort to bridge the theory to practice gap of a bilingual model to the education of deaf students’ learning ASL as their first language and written English as their second language, Evans studied a focus child in each of three classrooms (grades 4th, 5th and 6th,) the teacher in each classroom, and, the home environments for each of these students. Data were obtained via 27 classroom observations and 8 home observations of language arts and “literacy activities” respectively, employing qualitative research methods. The author suggests that teaching strategies supporting a bilingual approach such as: using ASL as the language of instruction; making translation conceptual rather than literal; giving multiple translations to a word or phrase; and presenting information through multimodal methods to illustrate the same message or meaning; contributed to literacy learning. Findings further indicate that some inconsistencies persist in applying a bilingual approach with deaf students. Padden, C., & Ramsey, C. (1998). Reading ability in signing deaf children. Topics in Language Disorders, 18, 30-46. Rating= 2 In this article, evidence is presented demonstrating that there is a relationship between ASL and reading, but the relationship involves associations between specific elements of ASL and the alphabetic writing system. Moreover, it is argued that the associations must be “cultivated” because they do not “naturally” link with written language. A 24 relationship was found between fingerspelling skills and reading skills as indicated on tests and regardless of parental hearing status. In addition, findings revealed a relationship between reading ability and the initialized signs test. Deaf teachers were found to fingerspell more than twice as often as hearing teachers. Teachers who fingerspell often were also found to use the teaching techniques of “chaining” and “sandwiching” which merit further investigation as both pedagogy and student experiences are studied. These are ways of showing matched items across systems within interactive conditions. They are practices deaf children of deaf signing families are routinely exposed to as generations of deaf readers have devised mediational systems specifically for the purposes of cracking the code of written language. Thus, the researchers stress the importance of further study of the contexts of reading instruction as well as the mediating tools and structures that enable two systems to become meaningfully related to one another. Strong, M. & Prinz, P. (1997). A study of the relationship between American sign language and English literacy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2, 37-46. Rating= 2 The authors of this article studied the relationship between American Sign Language (ASL) skills and English literacy in 160 children who were deaf. They rated ASL ability using a specially designed test of ASL the differentiated children’s signing skills into three levels, measured English literacy skills, and compared the two. They found that the children whose signing skills fell at the higher two levels of ASL had significantly better English literacy skills than the children in the lowest ASL ability level regardless of age and IQ. In addition, although deaf children with deaf mothers had better ASL and English literacy skills in general than deaf children of hearing mothers, a significant finding was that, when ASL level was held constant, there was no difference between the two groups, except in the group whose ASL skills fell in the lowest level. The authors concluded that deaf children’s English literacy skills benefit from the acquisition of even a moderate fluency in ASL. Wilbur, R. (2000). The use of ASL to support the development of English and literacy. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 81-104. Rating- 3 This article reviews the literature relating ASL to the development of English, reading skills, and social skills and presents a strong case for the use of ASL as a primary language upon which the English language and the ability to read the English language may be based as second language processes. One very important concepts upon which the author speculates is that “the most efficacious coding strategy will be the one which is congruent with the primary communication mode.” (p. 89) Taken together with Calderon’s (2000, above) findings that maternal communication is the single most important factor influencing early literacy outcomes, this should strengthen the field’s 25 resolve to support parents in becoming skilled communicators, whatever their choice of communication option. Other Non-Reviewed Articles Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students. In California Office of Bilingual Bicultural Education (Ed.), Schooling and language minority students: A theoretical framework (pp. 3-49). Los Angeles: Evaluation, Dissemination and Assessment Center, California State University. Lichtenstein, E. H. (1998). The relationships between reading processes and English skills of deaf college students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 3, 80134. Padden, C., & Hanson, V. (2000). Search for the missing link: The development of skilled reading in deaf children. In K. Emmorey & H. Lane (Eds.), The signs of language revisited: An anthology to honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima (pp. 435-447). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. VIII. Reading in the Content Areas Kelly, R.R., Albertini, J. A., & Shannon, N. B. (2001). Deaf college students' reading comprehension and strategy use. American Annals of the Deaf, 146, 385-400. Rating=2 Two comprehension studies were conducted with 46 deaf college students reading science text. The results indicate that deaf students have difficulty identifying a topically incongruent sentence in a passage. Students reading at a higher level showed improved comprehension of short passages after training while the students at a lower reading level did not. Schirmer, B. (1997). Boosting reading success: Language, literacy, and content area instruction for Deaf and hard-of-hearing students---and bilingual students, and students learning English as a second language, and everyone else who needs a little help. Teaching Exceptional Children, 30 (1), 52-55. Rating=3 Schirmer discusses strategies teachers can use to support children with and without disabilities in comprehending reading material. She describes factors contributing to text readability. She also provides some background information on the challenge of meeting a triple agenda of language acquisition, literacy development and content learning by stressing the need for consistently and consciously providing the following: a) accessible interpersonal communication, b) intrapersonal language to reflect new concepts and 26 skills, c) writing that incorporates the variety of patterns of expository writing and the purposes uniquely available in each subject area, and d) reading the bodies of information in content area subjects in various forms. Strategies for in-class text reading and independent text reading are also described. Yore, L.D. (2000) Enhancing science literacy for all students with embedded reading instruction and writing-to-learn activities. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5, 105-122. Rating=3 Deaf students need opportunities to express what they are learning and experiencing in science and to contrast their understandings with the interpretations of the science establishment. The author of this article discusses how science literacy is developed as students learn about the "big" ideas of science and how to inform and persuade others about these ideas. A framework for using science reading and science writing with deaf students is proposed, based on research and informed practice with hearing students. IX. Language Experience Helms, L. & Schleper, D. (Summer 2000). Language Experience: Fun Projects After School, Including Writing. Odyssey, 1 (3), 13-16. Rating= 3 The coordinator of literacy education and the coordinator of residence programs at a school for the deaf share their understanding and application of language experience with young adult students in the dorm as those youngsters also refine their literacy skills. With twenty pictures and the accompanying language to describe each one, readers see the approach in action. One is also reminded of how various subjects are integrated into this language-literacy experience. Whitesell, K. (May/June 1999). Language experience-Leading from behind. Perspectives in education and deafness [special literacy issue], 17 (5), 36-38. Rating=3 Describes how the language experience approach may be used in a child-centered, inquiry-oriented learning environment to facilitate authentic language and literacy learning. Shares the ten steps utilized by one teacher of the deaf and her 6th grade students as they investigate the possibility of adopting a stray dog to live at the school for the deaf. From posing initial inquiry questions, writing a letter to the school superintendent, researching data on the topic of inquiry, preparing notes, conferencing with one another, telephoning an outside expert, writing/ re-writing, illustrating and finally going public, the students worked on concepts about print, text-to-life and life-to- 27 text connections, sense of story, phonics in context, vocabulary development and beginning authorship. Was highly motivating and a confidence builder for the students. X. Reading to and with Students (Storytelling and Shared Reading and Writing) Gillespie, C.W., & Twardosz, S. (October 1997) A group storybook-reading intervention with children at a residential school for the deaf. American Annals of the Deaf, 142 (4), 320-32. Rating=1 Group storybook reading in the residences of a state-sponsored school for the deaf was investigated as a means of fostering literacy development outside of the classroom. Eighteen children, ages 4-11 years, in four cottages participated. The study followed a before/after experimental group/control group design. The nine children in the experimental group cottages participated in group storybook reading twice a week for five months. Both the experimental and the control group cottages were provided with a variety of books that were rotated biweekly. Several limitations of the study are discussed, including experimental and control groups that were not ideally matched; problems inherent in using questionnaires; and, whether or not the emergent reading task procedure used by a prominent researcher with hearing emergent literacy learners is in fact the most appropriate way to measure deaf children’s emergent reading development. Implications for practice include; commitment of the staff; consideration of competing activities; experience in managing groups of young deaf children; positioning or sitting with children and becoming engaged themselves in the reading; and, scheduling at times most conducive to engagement. The subjects were motivated when they had animated, engaging readers with appropriate texts at appropriate times in their schedule (as opposed to long books very close to bedtime) but not motivated enough to pick up texts on their own. The children were more motivated when additional counselors were present and sat down with children on their laps or with their arms around the children. They concentrated more readily and were more likely to engage in discussions with the reader about the book than if no adult was sitting with the children. The children were more motivated by certain books and requested certain books repeatedly, i.e., picture identification, counting, or animal books. They were not interested in long books or books containing story lines of any kind. Watching/listening to the book was the most frequently observed category of engagement, representing 66% of the engagement observations. Gioia, B. (2001). The emergent language and literacy experiences of three deaf preschoolers. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 48 (4), 411-428. Rating= 2 This is a multiple case study of literacy acquisition of three deaf preschoolers who demonstrated receptive vocabularies of fewer than 25 words on the Peabody Picture 28 Vocabulary Test Revised and substantial language delays at the beginning of the study. Initial classroom practices around group storybook sharing involved looking at picture and talking about them and then often tying the storybook to classroom themes. With researcher encouragement, the classroom teacher began reading the actual text rather than engaging only in picture talk or paraphrasing, and she engaged the children in an ongoing process of negotiation with the books. By the end of the 8 month intervention, all three preschoolers grew from their beginning vocabulary of fewer than 25 words to speaking and signing in five, six, and seven word sentences. They also improved from no interest in storybook reading to actually incorporating new and interesting vocabulary that had been pointed out to them when storybook reading into their own vocabularies. In addition, all parents initially reported being unable to develop satisfactory shared reading rituals with their deaf children in October. Yet, by the end of the study at the end of the school year, the children asked to be read to more frequently and on a regular basis and even read on their own at home when parents were not available, thus taking the lead themselves to improve their home literacy practices. Luetke-Stahlman, B., Hayes, L.P., & Nielson, D. (October 1996). Essential practices as adults read to meet the needs of deaf or hard of hearing students. American Annals of the Deaf, 141, 309-20 Rating=3 Based on a literature review of practices that are supported by research and linked to literacy attainment, the authors provide a rationale and description of ten practices they feel are essential to include when reading to students if they are to become proficient readers themselves. Some initial discussion is given to the language experiences and needs of deaf or hard of hearing students versus those of typically developing students prior to delineating the ten essential practices. In essence, frequent, systematic literacy experiences whereby D/hh students truly have access to the information and engage in mediated dialogue, (be it with narrative or expository texts), is what is encouraged. Maxwell, M. (1984). A deaf child’s natural development of literacy. Sign LanguageStudies, 34, 11-30. Rating= 2 Studied the emergent literacy skills of a deaf child of deaf parents, whose parents read to her using books containing illustrations, sign drawings, and print. The subject was studied from ages 2 to 6, during which time she developed concepts of print, awareness of the relationship between concepts and sign drawings, and the relationship between concepts and English print. The author indicated that her emergent literacy followed similar patterns to those of hearing children. Follow-up evaluations indicated that she was reading on grade level at first and third grade. XI. Guided Reading and Writing 29 Malik, S. (1996). Reading for Meaning: A Guided Reading Approach. Volta Review; 98 (3), 127-36 Rating=2 Discusses the author’s approach to “guided reading” to teach reading and describes a specific approach that uses a morning letter written to students to preview the day’s events. This letter uses vocabulary and syntax corresponding to students’ reading abilities.Teachers model and guide students in applying various reading strategies via group reading of the letter. Over three years results have included “developing competent reading skills” and “achieving higher test scored.” Beginning of the year test scores and end of the year retest scores on the Rhode Island Test of Language Structure showed an increase in T-scores ranging from 10-15 points, an increase of an entire standard deviation. Miller, K. & Rosenthal, L. L. (November 1995). Seeing the Big Picture: Deaf Adults' Development of Summarization through Book Discussion in American Sign Language. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy. Rating=2 Investigators discuss a classroom-based reading discussion group project involving three profoundly deaf adult learners whose primary mode of communication was sign language and whose ages ranged from 30 to 55 years of age. All were enrolled in the Adult Literacy Program at the Pennsylvania School for the Deaf and according to a basic reading inventory, their reading and writing abilities ranged from fourth to sixth grade. The purpose of the study was to determine if deaf adults could use ASL as a vehicle to access or interpret English and thereby improve their literacy skills. The study focused on the deaf adults’ first attempt at reading and discussing a novel. A three step approach to guiding the learners toward comprehending text and summarizing it rather than word by word or sentence analysis was the main focus. While the concept of summarizing was clarified, evolved and expanded over the course of the book project, at the end of the eight week project, the reading discussion group still struggled with what it means to read a text. XII. Writer’s Workshop- (Not reviewed) Several articles were scanned for inclusion in this section, but all articles found were of a descriptive or testimonial nature. No articles are reviewed as no data-based articles were found. XIII. Research Reading and Writing (Not reviewed) Several articles were scanned for inclusion in this section, but all articles found were of a descriptive or testimonial nature. No articles are reviewed as no data-based articles were found. 30 XIV. Dialogue and Other Journals and Logs Bailes, C, (1999). Dialogue journals: Fellowship, conversation, and English modeling. Perspectives in Education and Deafness, 17 (5). Rating=3 The basics of setting up and implementing dialogue journals with deaf and hard of hearing students are provided. The fact that dialogue journals are student centered; respect the writer’s privacy; are driven by the student; and the guidelines for teacher to follow in responding are all addresses. The author stresses the social, risk-free nature of dialogue journals and shares excerpts from a 4th grade deaf student’s journal. Kluwin, T.N.& Kelly, A. (1991) The effectiveness of dialogue journal writing in improving the writing skills of young deaf writers. American Annals of the Dea , 136(3), 284-291. Rating=2 The purpose of this study was to examine the claim that dialogue journal writing would improve the written English skills of a limited English language population. The project involved 204 pairs of deaf and hearing students in ten cities across the United States writing back and forth with each other exchanging interests, ideas and feelings over the span of one school year. The writing was personal, reflective and included narrations, explanations and descriptions which were written at the language and interest level of the two young people involved. In general, blind matches were done. 153 journals were available for analysis. The average age of the correspondents was 13 and a half with a range from 10 to 18 years old. Although there was observable improvement in the sentence complexity of some deaf students, not all students benefited equally from the experience, nor were all students and teachers equally positive about the outcomes. The authors conclude with three recommendations for further implementation of such an exchange. Walworth, M. (1985). Dialogue journals and the teaching of reading. Teaching English to Deaf and Second Language Students, 3 (1), 21-24. Rating= 2 Describes an intervention in which writing dialogue journals was used by college deaf students in an effort to assess their reading levels and guide them through the process of moving from basic skill reading and writing to content focused reading and writing. In this non-threatening and conversational way of approaching reading, the teacher gained insight into the students’ strengths and weaknesses, which helped in guiding them appropriately and tracking their progress. The students gained a better understanding of what they were reading and how they approached their reading assignments. They were able to identify effective ways of reading and writing without the typical judgmental feedback. XV. Independent Reading 31 Dry, E., & Earle, P.T. (1988) Can Johnny have time to read? American Annals of the Deaf, 133, 219. Rating=3 Believing that reading is over-taught and under-practiced, the authors remind readers of the benefits of providing time for students to engage in sustained silent reading. They further contend that alternating between months of traditional and sustained silent reading may be one alternative to the strict structure-bound model. Further recommendations include a) giving students time to read books of their own choice; b) allowing students to enjoy good books and stories at many different levels; c) observing, commenting on and enjoying student strategies as students reread sections or tell others what they are reading; d) allowing students time to find their own levels and discard books they become disinterested in while encouraging them to find other books; e) having students keep a simple record of reading; f) stating broad reading goals openly. Though SSR requires a firm commitment of student time, the authors remind readers that it conveys to students the extreme value the teacher places on reading as an individual pursuit and conveys a respect for student interests and resources far beyond words and lectures. XVI. Other http://clerccenter.gallaudet.edu/Literacy/about/reading.html The Laurent Clerc Center of Gallaudet University provides two charts, one for reading and one for writing, that identify skills that are critical to literacy learning in DHH students. The chart lists practices for teachers to increase on one side and practices for teachers to decrease on the other. It is a very useful tool for the instruction of future teachers of children who are deaf and hard of hearing. Overall Findings Major Finding #1 Hearing loss per se does not impose a restriction on the ability of a child to learn to read. This is evidenced by the various studies reviewed that compared lesser skilled readers with those whose skills exceeded the dire predictions generally bandied about the field. The two most prominent factors in the development of literacy are linguistic ability (whether English or ASL, but most definitely conveyed routinely by a mother well-versed in communication interactions) and opportunity to access comprehensive and cohesive instruction. All arguments made boil down to these two. Major Finding #2 Research available in the area of literacy, although improving in the last 5 or 10 years, is rife with speculation, pseudo-empirically based for the most part, deferential to a belief system, and characterized by many holes in the knowledge base. Among the more than 85 articles skimmed or reviewed for this document, only about a dozen could be categorized with a research rigor rating of 1. This rating was given to studies of true, empirical design, where a matched (or quasi-matched) control group and experimental group engaged in a particular treatment, the outcomes of which were examined with statistical rigor. Most articles that compared groups did not match them 32 carefully. Most articles that compared groups looked at existing skills based on some kind of test or rating. Few engaged in experimental design, so although we can make assumptions about what is working in literacy instruction based on existing skills of a sample, we cannot point to many programs, materials, strategies, or interventions and declare there is experimental proof of their effectiveness. In addition many of the practices that are considered sacred cows in deaf education have little or no evidence to support their efficacy. They may have exceptional sources describing their characteristics and their use, but based on the 100s of articles scanned to locate the 85 plus skimmed or reviewed, evidence is still of a descriptive nature rather than an empirical nature. Major Finding #3 Those characteristics of literacy instruction identified by the National Reading Panel as key characteristics of effective instruction are among the better researched in deaf education, although the complexity of these characteristics are not sufficiently investigated relative to the deaf and hard of hearing population. Findings include the following: Readers with hearing loss who possess better phonemic awareness and phonics skills have better literacy outcomes than those with poorer phonemic awareness and phonics skills, but there is still a debate as to whether or not audition is required in order to learn these skills. Popular visual strategies for supporting phonemic awareness and phonics point to success stories, but little empirical evidence is available comparing programs, strategies, or approaches. Readers with hearing loss possess better reading vocabulary knowledge when they have a base of world knowledge and the ability to apply strategies to discern meaning (e.g., phonemic, morphographemic, or contextual strategies). This particular finding is typical among the literature. There is a lot of information telling the field what good readers who are deaf do compared to what lesser skilled readers who are deaf do, but there is virtually do research taking random samples of deaf children, matching them to control groups, applying instructional treatments, and documenting that that strategy was, in fact, responsible for the outcome. Better readers with hearing loss possess better reading fluency skills than poorer readers when fluency when fluency is defined as reading automaticity. Automaticity includes facile linguistic ability, whereas traditional definitions of fluency refer to spoken reading expression. There is little evidence to support that speed of reading out loud, per se, influences reading comprehension in children with hearing loss. The issues involved in fluency for deaf and hard of hearing children are far more complex. Only recently is there discussion in the literature of what fluency means to non-speaking, signing deaf children. Children with hearing loss who are motivated to read will read more often than children who are not motivated to read. Being read to by skilled communicators (especially parents who are skilled communicators) whether via English, ASL, Cued Speech, or another language, and having access to engaging materials are important motivating factors. No studies were found to proved that one motivating factor was more important than another relative to reading comprehension outcomes. 33 Major Finding #3 The better a deaf or hard of hearing reader’s linguistic abilities, whether in English or ASL or both and regardless of mode of conveyance of the language, the better his or her literacy outcomes. Clearly in order to read a language one must understand a language. Sufficient evidence exists that many children who are deaf and use ASL are able to become skilled readers of English given sufficient opportunity to mediate the two languages. It appears that children who have hearing loss and their brains do not care how or what their mothers communicate with them; they just care that their mothers and important others communication with them. Their brains will respond remarkably to a variety of consistent, persistent language and literacy stimulation as long as that stimulation occurs early and effectively. Major Finding #4 Reading in the content area is an important way to promote literacy skills. Students with hearing loss benefit from specific instruction in content area reading strategies, so literacy instruction for this population should continue past the traditional grades at which instruction in reading, per se, ends. Major Finding #5 Practices associated with Gallaudet University’s Keys to Literacy perspective have varying degrees of research support. Findings include the following: The Language Experience Approach enjoys a long history of support by teachers of children who are deaf and hard of hearing, however most of the literature is of a narrative or descriptive nature, suggesting ways to incorporate multiple elements of instruction into this approach. No articles were found comparing literacy outcomes of children taught using LEAs to outcomes of children not taught using LEAs. Storytelling and Shared Reading practices have sufficient evidence to detail the elements of their application; however most of the literature is of a narrative or descriptive nature. Evidence exists that these practices improve motivation for reading as well as promoting linguistic experiences. Guided Reading practices have been shown to improve the linguistic ability of children who are deaf and hard of hearing in relation to print media. Since we know that improved linguistic ability leads to improved reading outcomes, Guided Reading seems to have the necessary support in the literature to detail its components and applications as well as documenting positive outcomes. Writer’s Workshop and Research Reading and Writing were not reviewed as no level 1 or 2 research was found among the articles considered for this project. The various forms of journaling (dialogue journals, content area journals, etc.) appear to promote motivation to read and write, but little empirical evidence is available that examines the relationship of this often-used approach to literacy development. Independent reading is both a goal of literacy instruction and an instructional strategy promoted in schools. Descriptive literature exists detailing the processes and procedures involved in designing and implementing independent reading. Summary 34 Students who are deaf and hard of hearing can learn to read, but most must be taught to do so. Both conventional wisdom and empirical evidence inform us that this is no easy task. Learning to read fluently and easily requires at least the following: Decoding, morphographemic, and/or contextual word recognition skills A working memory and the ability to store and recall information World and vocabulary knowledge Linguistic proficiency and the ability to understand the meaning conveyed through phrases, sentences and longer passages The ability to draw inferences A motivating and supportive communication environment at home and at school Committed and caring instructors who have a plan for imparting instruction in an organized, thoughtful, sustainable, and documentable manner. Further research of an empirical nature needs to be conducted across all aspects of literacy in order to sort through the many practices, approaches, and strategies that are available so that teachers know those tools that are most effective and that can impart literacy skills most efficiently.