Washington Post Opinion Editorial Rethinking the "Language Gap" Debates over the relationship between language, poverty, and school failure have been around since the 1960s, resurfacing most recently in a widely publicized study led by Stanford psychologist, Anne Fernald. Fernald argues that by the age of two, children from lower income families demonstrate inferior language processing skills and lesser cognitive aptitude than those from higher income families, primarily because poorer parents don’t talk to their children properly. Research of this kind (known as “language gap” studies) is framed as a wellintentioned attempt to assist poor children to overcome linguistic and cognitive deficits. However, both the premises and promises of these studies must be challenged because the resulting misunderstandings of such claims spread like wild fire through the media, affecting educational policy and negatively impacting the lives of millions of children. The most commonly cited study in language gap research is Betty Hart and Todd Risley’s 1995 book, Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. This book reported that by the age of three, children from affluent households are exposed to approximately 30 million more words than children from families on welfare, and that this “word gap” is largely responsible for the low academic achievement of economically impoverished students. This finding is flawed in a number of ways. First, a "linguistically stimulating environment" cannot be measured simply by counting words. Secondly, linguistic and cognitive competencies manifest in many forms not measured by this study. Finally, "school success" does not exclusively depend on the supposedly superior linguistic and cognitive capacities that result from early exposure to vocabulary-heavy environments; academic achievement is often affected by host of economic factors (e.g., access to nutrition, health care, and tutoring) unrelated to a child’s early exposure to particular linguistic environments. Though agreeing that nothing is gained from exposing babies to mere word lists, Fernald still insists that their brains will develop better “nets of meaning” if parents immerse their children in “more and richer language.” According to her study, affluent parents appear to know how to do this naturally while mothers from lower income households need to be taught how to improve their language use. These findings rely on ethnocentric methods for testing children’s linguistic and cognitive abilities, and do not consider other forms of linguistic, cognitive, and social development that take place in low-income homes. For decades, linguistic anthropologists and sociolinguists have been developing different approaches to studying language acquisition and linguistic competence. Rather than comparing and ranking children from diverse backgrounds using standardized linguistic criteria, we look at how children develop "communicative competence." This perspective encompasses both the knowledge of language as well as the knowledge of how to use language in culturally appropriate ways within specific social settings. Just as communicative features (like turntaking, register, body language, questioning strategies, and vocabulary) vary widely across social contexts and speech communities, the ways in which children acquire these features vary too. Researchers like Shirley Brice Heath and Ana Celia Zentella have demonstrated that children in non-affluent communities are engaged in socializing practices that support many alternative 1 forms of constructive thinking and communicating which reflect the appropriate norms of interaction demonstrated by members of their community. Students from affluent households tend to succeed at school since the communicative practices used by more affluent families mirror the ones expected in classrooms (i.e., not because they are linguistically superior). Conversely, students from less affluent backgrounds tend to have difficulties mastering classroom-based skills when they start school because they operate from different, NOT inferior, cultural and linguistic schemas. Blaming academic failure on a lack of early exposure to "words" glosses over the fact that schools are often insufficiently prepared to recognize and build upon the strengths of students from diverse backgrounds. If teachers are not adequately trained to integrate the aptitudes of all their students, less affluent students will continue to struggle with the new language forms expected of them in a classroom setting. The popular assumptions resulting from “language gap” research cause parents from less affluent homes to appear incompetent and irresponsible for not pumping their children full of words and bookish grammar (perpetuating the view that the linguistic and cognitive forms developed by non-affluent children are of no value). This further influences the belief that school failure can be resolved by teaching poor parents to expose their children to “more and richer language." The root problem here is not a “word gap,” but an economic gap. Given that the United States has the second highest child poverty rate among all industrialized countries (UNICEF, 2012), it is imperative that we challenge mainstream approaches to educating children from low income backgrounds. Unless educators are given the tools to identify, appreciate, and build upon the language skills of linguistically diverse students, deficit orientations and academic underachievement will continue to proliferate in public schools across the country. 800 words Contributing Authors: Eric J. Johnson, Ph.D., Washington State University Tri-Cities Ana Celia Zentella, Ph.D., University of California San Diego Kathleen C. Riley, Ph.D., Queens College, City University of New York David Cassels Johnson, Ph.D., University of Iowa Jonathan Rosa, Ph.D., University of Massachusetts Amherst Sponsoring Organizations: Society for Linguistic Anthropology/Language and Social Justice Committee American Anthropological Association Committee for Human Rights Send comments to Eric Johnson: ejj@tricity.wsu.edu . . 2